首页 > 最新文献

Infant and Child Development最新文献

英文 中文
They are the best: The significance of others to emerging adults and well-being 他们是最棒的:他人对新成人和幸福的意义
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-22 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2517
Jeffrey T. Cookston, Victoria Olivia Sherry Chou, Qudsia Khalid, Fernanda Amaya

It is surprising how little we know about the relationships that matter to emerging adults, and we propose research to bridge that gap. First, we will ask emerging adults to generate a list of the individuals who matter. The list will be submitted via open-ended text that we will code for relationship type. Our first aim is to gain information about the frequency and diversity of relationships that matter to emerging adults. We hypothesise that participants who list more relationships will have better well-being and less loneliness. Second, we will use an adapted version of the Mattering Scale to assess how much specific relationships matter to participants by asking them to focus on five common close relationships (i.e., mother figures, father figures, romantic partners, best friends and closest-in-age sibling). For each relationship that exists for the participant, how much each relationship matters will be measured. We hypothesise that failing to mention a specific relationship in the open-ended portion of the study will be related to lower perceived mattering of that relationship when participants report on mattering. Finally, we hypothesise that when specified relationships matter more to an individual, they will have less loneliness and better well-being.

令人惊讶的是,我们对新兴成人所重视的人际关系知之甚少,因此我们建议开展研究来弥补这一差距。首先,我们将要求新成人列出一份重要人物的名单。这份名单将通过开放式文本提交,我们将根据关系类型对其进行编码。我们的第一个目标是获得有关对新兴成人来说重要的关系的频率和多样性的信息。我们的假设是,列出更多关系的参与者将拥有更好的幸福感和更少的孤独感。其次,我们将使用经过改编的 "关系紧张量表"(Mattering Scale)来评估特定关系对参与者的重要程度,要求他们关注五种常见的亲密关系(即母亲形象、父亲形象、恋爱伴侣、最好的朋友和最亲密的同龄兄弟姐妹)。对于参与者存在的每种关系,我们都将衡量每种关系的重要程度。我们假设,在研究的开放式部分,如果参与者没有提及某种特定的关系,那么当参与者报告这种关系的重要性时,就会认为这种关系的重要性较低。最后,我们假设,当特定关系对个人更重要时,他们的孤独感会更少,幸福感会更高。
{"title":"They are the best: The significance of others to emerging adults and well-being","authors":"Jeffrey T. Cookston,&nbsp;Victoria Olivia Sherry Chou,&nbsp;Qudsia Khalid,&nbsp;Fernanda Amaya","doi":"10.1002/icd.2517","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2517","url":null,"abstract":"<p>It is surprising how little we know about the relationships that matter to emerging adults, and we propose research to bridge that gap. First, we will ask emerging adults to generate a list of the individuals who matter. The list will be submitted via open-ended text that we will code for relationship type. Our first aim is to gain information about the frequency and diversity of relationships that matter to emerging adults. We hypothesise that participants who list more relationships will have better well-being and less loneliness. Second, we will use an adapted version of the Mattering Scale to assess how much specific relationships matter to participants by asking them to focus on five common close relationships (i.e., mother figures, father figures, romantic partners, best friends and closest-in-age sibling). For each relationship that exists for the participant, how much each relationship matters will be measured. We hypothesise that failing to mention a specific relationship in the open-ended portion of the study will be related to lower perceived mattering of that relationship when participants report on mattering. Finally, we hypothesise that when specified relationships matter more to an individual, they will have less loneliness and better well-being.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141091861","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The health costs associated with “looking forward to success”: Educational expectations and children's health 与 "期待成功 "相关的健康成本:教育期望与儿童健康
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-19 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2524
Yiwei Liu, Yuting Su, Keshan Liu, Zhiyan Jin

This study examines the impact of the deviation between parents' educational expectations and children's educational expectations on children's health. This study based on the data from Chinese Family Panel Studies conducted in 2018 and 2020, The participants were 2340 children aged 10–15 years (1310 boys, 1030 girls) in China. We found that when parents' educational expectations are higher than their children's educational expectations, they have a negative impact on children's health status. According to the influence mechanism, the higher the parents' expectations of education are, the greater children's remedial behaviour will be, the less leisure time will be, which will adversely affect children's health. Furthermore, we found that a good parent–child relationship moderated the negative impact of the deviation in parent–child educational expectations on children's health. Therefore, in order to ensure the healthy development of children, it is necessary to resolve the adverse effects of the deviation in parent–child educational expectations by arranging tutoring and leisure time rationally and establishing a good parent–child relationship.

本研究探讨了父母教育期望与子女教育期望之间的偏差对儿童健康的影响。本研究基于2018年和2020年进行的中国家庭面板研究数据,参与者为中国2340名10-15岁儿童(1310名男孩,1030名女孩)。我们发现,当父母的教育期望高于子女的教育期望时,会对子女的健康状况产生负面影响。根据影响机制,父母的教育期望越高,孩子的补习行为就越多,闲暇时间就越少,从而对孩子的健康产生不利影响。此外,我们还发现,良好的亲子关系可以调节亲子教育期望偏差对儿童健康的负面影响。因此,为了保证儿童的健康成长,有必要通过合理安排家教和闲暇时间,建立良好的亲子关系来化解亲子教育期望偏差的不利影响。
{"title":"The health costs associated with “looking forward to success”: Educational expectations and children's health","authors":"Yiwei Liu,&nbsp;Yuting Su,&nbsp;Keshan Liu,&nbsp;Zhiyan Jin","doi":"10.1002/icd.2524","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2524","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This study examines the impact of the deviation between parents' educational expectations and children's educational expectations on children's health. This study based on the data from Chinese Family Panel Studies conducted in 2018 and 2020, The participants were 2340 children aged 10–15 years (1310 boys, 1030 girls) in China. We found that when parents' educational expectations are higher than their children's educational expectations, they have a negative impact on children's health status. According to the influence mechanism, the higher the parents' expectations of education are, the greater children's remedial behaviour will be, the less leisure time will be, which will adversely affect children's health. Furthermore, we found that a good parent–child relationship moderated the negative impact of the deviation in parent–child educational expectations on children's health. Therefore, in order to ensure the healthy development of children, it is necessary to resolve the adverse effects of the deviation in parent–child educational expectations by arranging tutoring and leisure time rationally and establishing a good parent–child relationship.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141123440","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The ‘scars’ left on children by exclusion, but re-inclusion may ‘heal up’ 排斥给儿童留下的 "伤疤",但重新接纳可以 "愈合
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-15 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2515
Jun Zheng, Wenlu Liu, Ping Qian, Linlin Yan

Previous studies have shown that social exclusion can have negative effects on the well-being of children and adolescents. However, there is inconsistent evidence regarding how the impact of social exclusion changes with age. This study used the Cyberball paradigm to investigate the effects of exclusion and subsequent inclusion on primary needs (such as belonging, self-esteem, control and meaningful existence) and emotions (including happiness, sadness and anger) in a sample of Chinese school-aged children (163 children aged 6–8, 8–10 and 10–11). The results revealed that children aged 6–8 showed lower susceptibility to ostracism, while threats to primary needs emerged around the ages of 8–10 and persisted until 10–12. Negative effects on moods were evident from the age of 6–8 and intensified with age. Brief inclusion following exclusion was found to effectively restore the primary needs and moods of ostracized children, serving as a reliable intervention for promoting rapid recovery. However, age-related differences were observed in the restorative effects of re-inclusion, with 8- to 10-year-olds benefiting more compared to 10- to 12-year-olds. These findings provide valuable insights for educational institutions seeking to create supportive environments that help children recover from the detrimental effects of ostracism.

Highlights

  • Ostracism can be detrimental to the primary needs and moods of school-aged children.
  • There are developmental changes in the effects of ostracism on children's primary needs and moods, and 8–10 years old may be a critical period.
  • Re-inclusion can effectively restore the damaged primary needs and moods of excluded children, but its effect is also age-dependent.
以往的研究表明,社会排斥会对儿童和青少年的福祉产生负面影响。然而,关于社会排斥的影响如何随年龄变化的证据并不一致。本研究以中国学龄儿童为样本(163 名年龄分别为 6-8、8-10 和 10-11 岁的儿童),采用 "网络球 "范式研究社会排斥和社会包容对儿童主要需求(如归属感、自尊、控制和有意义的存在)和情绪(包括快乐、悲伤和愤怒)的影响。结果显示,6-8 岁的儿童对排斥的敏感度较低,而对主要需求的威胁则出现在 8-10 岁左右,并一直持续到 10-12 岁。对情绪的负面影响从 6-8 岁开始就很明显,并随着年龄的增长而加剧。研究发现,排斥后的短暂融入能有效恢复被排斥儿童的基本需求和情绪,是促进其快速康复的可靠干预措施。然而,在重新接纳的恢复效果方面,观察到了与年龄有关的差异,8 至 10 岁的儿童比 10 至 12 岁的儿童受益更多。这些研究结果为教育机构提供了宝贵的启示,这些机构正在努力创造支持性环境,帮助儿童从排斥的有害影响中恢复过来。排斥会损害学龄儿童的主要需求和情绪,排斥对儿童主要需求和情绪的影响存在发展变化,8-10 岁可能是一个关键时期。重新接纳可以有效恢复被排斥儿童受损的主要需求和情绪,但其效果也与年龄有关。
{"title":"The ‘scars’ left on children by exclusion, but re-inclusion may ‘heal up’","authors":"Jun Zheng,&nbsp;Wenlu Liu,&nbsp;Ping Qian,&nbsp;Linlin Yan","doi":"10.1002/icd.2515","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2515","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Previous studies have shown that social exclusion can have negative effects on the well-being of children and adolescents. However, there is inconsistent evidence regarding how the impact of social exclusion changes with age. This study used the Cyberball paradigm to investigate the effects of exclusion and subsequent inclusion on primary needs (such as belonging, self-esteem, control and meaningful existence) and emotions (including happiness, sadness and anger) in a sample of Chinese school-aged children (163 children aged 6–8, 8–10 and 10–11). The results revealed that children aged 6–8 showed lower susceptibility to ostracism, while threats to primary needs emerged around the ages of 8–10 and persisted until 10–12. Negative effects on moods were evident from the age of 6–8 and intensified with age. Brief inclusion following exclusion was found to effectively restore the primary needs and moods of ostracized children, serving as a reliable intervention for promoting rapid recovery. However, age-related differences were observed in the restorative effects of re-inclusion, with 8- to 10-year-olds benefiting more compared to 10- to 12-year-olds. These findings provide valuable insights for educational institutions seeking to create supportive environments that help children recover from the detrimental effects of ostracism.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Ostracism can be detrimental to the primary needs and moods of school-aged children.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>There are developmental changes in the effects of ostracism on children's primary needs and moods, and 8–10 years old may be a critical period.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Re-inclusion can effectively restore the damaged primary needs and moods of excluded children, but its effect is also age-dependent.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140972557","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Contributions of mothers' and fathers' shared book reading with infants at 9 months to language skills at 18 months in ethnically and socioeconomically diverse families 在不同种族和社会经济背景的家庭中,9 个月大时母亲和父亲与婴儿共同阅读图书对 18 个月大时语言能力的影响
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-08 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2516
Yu Chen, Natasha Cabrera, Charlotte Sudduth, Stephanie M. Reich

Using a sample of 286 mothers and fathers from ethnically and socioeconomically diverse backgrounds, we tested the associations between the frequency and quality of parents' shared book reading (SBR) with infants aged 9 months, and language skills of infants aged 18 months, and whether infants' attention during SBR at 9 months mediated these associations. Frequency of SBR was parent-report and quality of SBR (i.e., number of reading strategies) and infants' attention were coded from recorded SBR interactions at home. The majority of mothers and fathers reported reading to their 9-month-olds at least weekly, and mothers reported reading, on average, significantly more often than fathers. There was large variability in parents' SBR quality ranging from 0 to 15 strategies per minute, with labelling being the most common. Path analysis showed that mothers' SBR frequency at 9 months was significantly associated with infants' receptive and expressive language skills at 18 months, whereas, SBR quality by either parent was not significant. Infants' attention did not mediate these associations. These findings suggest that early SBR is beneficial for language development and programmes targeting early language development should encourage both mothers and fathers to read often to their infants during the first year.

我们以来自不同种族和社会经济背景的 286 位母亲和父亲为样本,测试了父母与 9 个月大婴儿共读图书(SBR)的频率和质量与 18 个月大婴儿语言技能之间的关联,以及 9 个月大婴儿在共读图书过程中的注意力是否对这些关联起到了中介作用。共读图书的频率由家长报告,共读图书的质量(即阅读策略的数量)和婴儿的注意力则根据在家中的共读互动记录进行编码。大多数母亲和父亲都表示至少每周为其 9 个月大的孩子进行一次阅读,母亲的平均阅读频率明显高于父亲。父母的 SBR 质量差异很大,从每分钟 0 个策略到 15 个策略不等,其中最常见的是贴标签。路径分析显示,母亲 9 个月时的 SBR 频率与婴儿 18 个月时的接受性和表达性语言技能有显著相关性,而父母任何一方的 SBR 质量均无显著相关性。婴儿的注意力对这些关联没有中介作用。这些研究结果表明,早期亲子阅读有利于语言发展,针对早期语言发展的计划应鼓励父母双方在婴儿出生后的第一年内经常为其朗读。
{"title":"Contributions of mothers' and fathers' shared book reading with infants at 9 months to language skills at 18 months in ethnically and socioeconomically diverse families","authors":"Yu Chen,&nbsp;Natasha Cabrera,&nbsp;Charlotte Sudduth,&nbsp;Stephanie M. Reich","doi":"10.1002/icd.2516","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2516","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Using a sample of 286 mothers and fathers from ethnically and socioeconomically diverse backgrounds, we tested the associations between the frequency and quality of parents' shared book reading (SBR) with infants aged 9 months, and language skills of infants aged 18 months, and whether infants' attention during SBR at 9 months mediated these associations. Frequency of SBR was parent-report and quality of SBR (i.e., number of reading strategies) and infants' attention were coded from recorded SBR interactions at home. The majority of mothers and fathers reported reading to their 9-month-olds at least weekly, and mothers reported reading, on average, significantly more often than fathers. There was large variability in parents' SBR quality ranging from 0 to 15 strategies per minute, with labelling being the most common. Path analysis showed that mothers' SBR frequency at 9 months was significantly associated with infants' receptive and expressive language skills at 18 months, whereas, SBR quality by either parent was not significant. Infants' attention did not mediate these associations. These findings suggest that early SBR is beneficial for language development and programmes targeting early language development should encourage both mothers and fathers to read often to their infants during the first year.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2516","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140895725","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perspectives on the impact of generative AI on early-childhood development and education 生成式人工智能对幼儿发展和教育的影响透视
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-02 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2514
Karlis Kanders, Louis Stupple-Harris, Laurie Smith, Jenny Louise Gibson

Generative artificial intelligence (GAI) is rapidly becoming ubiquitous in many contexts. There is limited scholarship, however, in the fields of Developmental Psychology and Early Childhood Education exploring the implications of generative AI for babies and young children. In this Perspectives piece, we discuss potential use cases, opportunities, and risks for the application of AI in early childhood. Our insights are informed by extensive discussion with stakeholders and by desk research carried out in our roles as academics and analysts in a social innovation foundation. Our aim is to stimulate nuanced and informed discourse on the topic of generative AI in early childhood that can inform innovation in both research and practice.

生成式人工智能(GAI)正在许多领域迅速普及。然而,在发展心理学和幼儿教育领域,探索生成式人工智能对婴幼儿的影响的学术研究还很有限。在这篇《视角》文章中,我们将讨论人工智能在幼儿期应用的潜在用例、机遇和风险。我们的见解来自与利益相关者的广泛讨论,以及我们作为学者和社会创新基金会分析师所做的案头研究。我们的目的是就幼儿期生成式人工智能这一话题展开细致入微、有理有据的讨论,为研究和实践创新提供参考。
{"title":"Perspectives on the impact of generative AI on early-childhood development and education","authors":"Karlis Kanders,&nbsp;Louis Stupple-Harris,&nbsp;Laurie Smith,&nbsp;Jenny Louise Gibson","doi":"10.1002/icd.2514","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2514","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Generative artificial intelligence (GAI) is rapidly becoming ubiquitous in many contexts. There is limited scholarship, however, in the fields of Developmental Psychology and Early Childhood Education exploring the implications of generative AI for babies and young children. In this Perspectives piece, we discuss potential use cases, opportunities, and risks for the application of AI in early childhood. Our insights are informed by extensive discussion with stakeholders and by desk research carried out in our roles as academics and analysts in a social innovation foundation. Our aim is to stimulate nuanced and informed discourse on the topic of generative AI in early childhood that can inform innovation in both research and practice.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2514","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140826395","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Longitudinal associations between attachment representations coded in the adult attachment interview in late adolescence and perceptions of romantic relationship adjustment in adulthood 青少年晚期成人依恋访谈中编码的依恋表征与成年后对恋爱关系调整的看法之间的纵向联系
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-02 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2512
Or Dagan, Marissa D. Nivison, Maria E. Bleil, Cathryn Booth-LaForce, Theodore E. A. Waters, Glenn I. Roisman

Increasingly, researchers have operationalized Adult Attachment Interview (AAI)-derived attachment representations as reflecting individual differences in secure base script knowledge (AAIsbs) – the degree to which individuals show awareness of the temporal-causal schema that summarizes the basic features of seeking and receiving effective support from caregivers when in distress. In a series of pre-registered analyses, we used AAI transcripts recently re-coded for AAIsbs and leveraged a new follow-up assessment of the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development cohort at around age 30 years (479 currently partnered participants; 59% female; 82% White/non-Hispanic) to assess and compare the links between AAIsbs and traditional AAI coding measures at around age 18 years and self-reported romantic relationship quality in adulthood. Higher AAIsbs predicted better dyadic adjustment scores in adulthood (r = 0.17) and this association remained significant controlling for other AAI-derived coding measures, as well as sociodemographic and cognitive functioning covariates. Findings extend previous evidence pointing to the predictive significance of AAIsbs for multiple adult functioning domains.

越来越多的研究人员将成人依恋访谈(AAI)得出的依恋表征操作化,以反映安全基础脚本知识(AAIsbs)的个体差异,即个体对时间-因果图式的认识程度,该图式总结了个体在遇到困难时寻求和接受照顾者有效支持的基本特征。在一系列预先登记的分析中,我们使用了最近为 AAIsbs 重新编码的 AAI 成绩单,并利用对 30 岁左右的 NICHD 早期儿童保育和青少年发展研究(NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development)队列(479 名目前有伴侣的参与者;59% 为女性;82% 为白人/非西班牙裔)进行的新的后续评估,来评估和比较 18 岁左右 AAIsbs 与传统 AAI 编码测量之间的联系,以及成年后自我报告的恋爱关系质量。较高的 AAIsbs 预测了成年后较好的双向适应得分(r = 0.17),并且在控制其他 AAI 派生编码测量以及社会人口和认知功能协变量的情况下,这种关联仍然显著。研究结果扩展了之前的证据,表明 AAIsbs 对多个成人功能领域具有预测意义。
{"title":"Longitudinal associations between attachment representations coded in the adult attachment interview in late adolescence and perceptions of romantic relationship adjustment in adulthood","authors":"Or Dagan,&nbsp;Marissa D. Nivison,&nbsp;Maria E. Bleil,&nbsp;Cathryn Booth-LaForce,&nbsp;Theodore E. A. Waters,&nbsp;Glenn I. Roisman","doi":"10.1002/icd.2512","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2512","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Increasingly, researchers have operationalized Adult Attachment Interview (AAI)-derived attachment representations as reflecting individual differences in secure base script knowledge (AAI<sub>sbs</sub>) – the degree to which individuals show awareness of the temporal-causal schema that summarizes the basic features of seeking and receiving effective support from caregivers when in distress. In a series of pre-registered analyses, we used AAI transcripts recently re-coded for AAI<sub>sbs</sub> and leveraged a new follow-up assessment of the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development cohort at around age 30 years (479 currently partnered participants; 59% female; 82% White/non-Hispanic) to assess and compare the links between AAI<sub>sbs</sub> and traditional AAI coding measures at around age 18 years and self-reported romantic relationship quality in adulthood. Higher AAI<sub>sbs</sub> predicted better dyadic adjustment scores in adulthood (<i>r</i> = 0.17) and this association remained significant controlling for other AAI-derived coding measures, as well as sociodemographic and cognitive functioning covariates. Findings extend previous evidence pointing to the predictive significance of AAI<sub>sbs</sub> for multiple adult functioning domains.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140826387","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Early risk for child externalising symptoms: Examining genetic, prenatal, temperamental and parental influences 儿童外部化症状的早期风险:研究遗传、产前、脾气和父母的影响因素
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-30 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2508
Sohee Lee, Olivia C. Robertson, Kristine Marceau, Valerie S. Knopik, Misaki N. Natsuaki, Daniel S. Shaw, Leslie D. Leve, Jody M. Ganiban, Jenae M. Neiderhiser

This study utilised the Early Growth and Development Study (N = 561 adoptive children; 57.2% male, 55.3% White), a study of children adopted at birth, to examine heritable (birth parent psychopathology) and prenatal risk (prenatal maternal distress and smoking during pregnancy), infant negative affectivity, adoptive parent over-reactivity and warmth as independent predictors of childhood externalising symptoms. The current study evaluated if: (1) infant negative affectivity and over-reactive parenting are candidate mediators for the effects of heritable and prenatal risk on externalising symptoms and (2) parental warmth weakens the influence of heritable risk, prenatal risk, negative affectivity and over-reactive parenting on externalising symptoms. There were main effects of heritable risk, infant negative affectivity and over-reactive parenting on child externalising symptoms. The study found no support for the hypothesised mediation and moderation effects, suggesting that targeting parental over-reactivity rather than warmth would be more effective in reducing the risk for childhood externalising symptoms.

本研究利用 "早期成长与发展研究"(N = 561 名领养儿童;57.2% 为男性,55.3% 为白人)(一项针对出生时被领养儿童的研究),对遗传(亲生父母的精神病理学)、产前风险(产前母亲的痛苦和怀孕期间的吸烟)、婴儿的消极情绪、领养父母的过度反应和温暖作为儿童外化症状的独立预测因素进行了研究。本研究对以下因素进行了评估(1) 婴儿的负性情感和过度反应的养育方式是否是遗传风险和产前风险对外向化症状影响的候选中介,以及 (2) 父母的温暖是否会削弱遗传风险、产前风险、负性情感和过度反应的养育方式对外向化症状的影响。遗传风险、婴儿负性情感和过度反应的养育方式对儿童外化症状有主效应。研究发现,假设的中介效应和调节效应并不成立,这表明,针对父母的过度反应而非温情会更有效地降低儿童外化症状的风险。
{"title":"Early risk for child externalising symptoms: Examining genetic, prenatal, temperamental and parental influences","authors":"Sohee Lee,&nbsp;Olivia C. Robertson,&nbsp;Kristine Marceau,&nbsp;Valerie S. Knopik,&nbsp;Misaki N. Natsuaki,&nbsp;Daniel S. Shaw,&nbsp;Leslie D. Leve,&nbsp;Jody M. Ganiban,&nbsp;Jenae M. Neiderhiser","doi":"10.1002/icd.2508","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2508","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This study utilised the Early Growth and Development Study (<i>N</i> = 561 adoptive children; 57.2% male, 55.3% White), a study of children adopted at birth, to examine heritable (birth parent psychopathology) and prenatal risk (prenatal maternal distress and smoking during pregnancy), infant negative affectivity, adoptive parent over-reactivity and warmth as independent predictors of childhood externalising symptoms. The current study evaluated if: (1) infant negative affectivity and over-reactive parenting are candidate mediators for the effects of heritable and prenatal risk on externalising symptoms and (2) parental warmth weakens the influence of heritable risk, prenatal risk, negative affectivity and over-reactive parenting on externalising symptoms. There were main effects of heritable risk, infant negative affectivity and over-reactive parenting on child externalising symptoms. The study found no support for the hypothesised mediation and moderation effects, suggesting that targeting parental over-reactivity rather than warmth would be more effective in reducing the risk for childhood externalising symptoms.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2508","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140820000","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Adults' memories of parental acceptance–rejection in childhood and psychological (mal)adjustment predict forgiveness and vengeance 成年人对童年时期父母接受-拒绝的记忆和心理(不良)适应可预测宽恕和报复行为
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-29 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2511
Muhammad Mussaffa Butt, Ronald P. Rohner, Nour M. Zaki

This study investigated relations between emerging adults' memories of parental acceptance–rejection during childhood and the adults' dispositions towards forgiveness and vengeance, as mediated by psychological (mal)adjustment. South Asian (Pakistani) participants, including 242 (25.6%) men (Mage = 21.69, SD = 2.34) and 704 (74.4%) women (Mage = 21.14, SD = 2.09), responded to the short forms of the maternal and paternal Adult Parental Acceptance–Rejection Questionnaires, the Adult Personality Assessment Questionnaire, the Heartland Forgiveness Scale, the Vengeance Scale and a Personal Information Form. Results revealed that memories of both maternal and paternal rejection were correlated with current psychological maladjustment. Paternal rejection amongst women and maternal rejection amongst men predicted the disposition towards vengeance. Maternal acceptance amongst both men and women predicted the disposition towards forgiveness. Paternal acceptance, however, did not predict the disposition towards forgiveness amongst either men or women. Psychological adjustment amongst both men and women mediated relations between maternal and paternal acceptance and the disposition towards forgiveness. However, psychological maladjustment was a significant mediator of the relation between maternal and paternal rejection and the disposition towards vengeance amongst men and women. Implications of the findings and future directions of research are discussed.

本研究调查了新成人对童年时期父母接受-拒绝的记忆与成人的宽恕和报复倾向之间的关系,以及这种关系在心理(不良)适应方面的中介作用。南亚(巴基斯坦)参与者包括 242 名男性(25.6%)(年龄=21.69,标准差=2.34)和 704 名女性(74.4%)(年龄=21.14,标准差=2.09),他们回答了母系和父系成人接受-拒绝问卷简表、成人人格评估问卷、心境宽恕量表、复仇量表和个人信息表。结果显示,母亲和父亲的拒绝记忆与当前的心理不适应相关。女性对父亲的排斥和男性对母亲的排斥预示着报复倾向。而男性和女性对母亲的接纳则预示着对宽恕的倾向。然而,父亲的接纳并不能预测男性或女性的宽恕倾向。男性和女性的心理调适在母亲和父亲的接纳与宽恕倾向之间起到了中介作用。然而,心理不适应则是母亲和父亲的拒绝与男性和女性的报复倾向之间关系的重要中介。本文讨论了研究结果的意义和未来的研究方向。
{"title":"Adults' memories of parental acceptance–rejection in childhood and psychological (mal)adjustment predict forgiveness and vengeance","authors":"Muhammad Mussaffa Butt,&nbsp;Ronald P. Rohner,&nbsp;Nour M. Zaki","doi":"10.1002/icd.2511","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2511","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This study investigated relations between emerging adults' memories of parental acceptance–rejection during childhood and the adults' dispositions towards forgiveness and vengeance, as mediated by psychological (mal)adjustment. South Asian (Pakistani) participants, including 242 (25.6%) men (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 21.69, SD = 2.34) and 704 (74.4%) women (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 21.14, SD = 2.09), responded to the short forms of the maternal and paternal Adult Parental Acceptance–Rejection Questionnaires, the Adult Personality Assessment Questionnaire, the Heartland Forgiveness Scale, the Vengeance Scale and a Personal Information Form. Results revealed that memories of both maternal and paternal rejection were correlated with current psychological maladjustment. <i>Paternal</i> rejection amongst women and <i>maternal</i> rejection amongst men predicted the disposition towards vengeance. <i>Maternal</i> acceptance amongst both men and women predicted the disposition towards forgiveness. <i>Paternal</i> acceptance, however, did not predict the disposition towards forgiveness amongst either men or women. Psychological <i>adjustment</i> amongst both men and women mediated relations between maternal and paternal acceptance and the disposition towards forgiveness. However, psychological <i>maladjustment</i> was a significant mediator of the relation between maternal and paternal rejection and the disposition towards vengeance amongst men and women. Implications of the findings and future directions of research are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140820018","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do mother's socialization goals in early childhood predict children's later self? 母亲在幼儿期的社会化目标能否预测孩子日后的自我?
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-25 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2513
Pirko Tõugu, Anni Tamm, Tiia Tulviste

In this study, maternal socialization goals in early childhood were linked to qualities of children's self in middle childhood, while also considering maternal education and child gender. Estonian mothers (N = 209; Mage = 33.6; 52.2% had university education) provided ratings of their socialization goals for children (52.2% girls; Mage = 6.2), children's self was gauged 4 years later. The more mothers endorsed social conformity-related socialization goals, the fewer different autonomy-related (β = −0.15) and relatedness-related qualities of self (β = −0.21) their children had in their descriptions of themselves and their autobiographical memories. Mothers' self-maximization socialization goals were not linked to the qualities of children's self. Mothers without a university degree endorsed social conformity-related socialization goals more than mothers with a university degree (β = −0.20). Girls referred to more relatedness-related qualities than boys (β = 0.23). The results show that maternal socialization goals do not always have a straightforward effect on different qualities of children's self. Yet, conformity-related goals by mothers predict the qualities of children's later self.

Highlights

  • Children's self is predicted by gender and earlier maternal socialization goals.
  • Mother's social conformity-related socialization goals predict children's later self.
  • Self-maximization related socialization goals are not linked to children's self.
在这项研究中,母亲在幼儿期的社会化目标与儿童在中年期的自我品质相关联,同时也考虑了母亲的教育和儿童的性别。爱沙尼亚母亲(人数 = 209;年龄 = 33.6;52.2% 接受过大学教育)对其子女(52.2% 为女孩;年龄 = 6.2)的社会化目标进行了评分,4 年后对子女的自我进行了测量。母亲越赞同与社会一致性相关的社会化目标,其子女在自我描述和自传记忆中与自主性(β = -0.15)和相关性(β = -0.21)相关的自我品质就越少。母亲的自我最大化社会化目标与子女的自我品质无关。没有大学学历的母亲比有大学学历的母亲更赞同与社会一致性相关的社会化目标(β = -0.20)。与男孩相比,女孩更多提及与相关性有关的品质(β = 0.23)。结果表明,母亲的社会化目标并不总是对儿童不同的自我品质产生直接影响。然而,母亲与顺应相关的目标却能预测儿童日后的自我特质。亮点 儿童的自我特质是由性别和母亲早期的社会化目标预测的。母亲与社会一致性相关的社会化目标可预测儿童日后的自我。与自我最大化相关的社会化目标与儿童的自我无关。
{"title":"Do mother's socialization goals in early childhood predict children's later self?","authors":"Pirko Tõugu,&nbsp;Anni Tamm,&nbsp;Tiia Tulviste","doi":"10.1002/icd.2513","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2513","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>In this study, maternal socialization goals in early childhood were linked to qualities of children's self in middle childhood, while also considering maternal education and child gender. Estonian mothers (<i>N</i> = 209; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 33.6; 52.2% had university education) provided ratings of their socialization goals for children (52.2% girls; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 6.2), children's self was gauged 4 years later. The more mothers endorsed social conformity-related socialization goals, the fewer different autonomy-related (<i>β</i> = −0.15) and relatedness-related qualities of self (<i>β</i> = −0.21) their children had in their descriptions of themselves and their autobiographical memories. Mothers' self-maximization socialization goals were not linked to the qualities of children's self. Mothers without a university degree endorsed social conformity-related socialization goals more than mothers with a university degree (<i>β</i> = −0.20). Girls referred to more relatedness-related qualities than boys (<i>β</i> = 0.23). The results show that maternal socialization goals do not always have a straightforward effect on different qualities of children's self. Yet, conformity-related goals by mothers predict the qualities of children's later self.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Children's self is predicted by gender and earlier maternal socialization goals.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Mother's social conformity-related socialization goals predict children's later self.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Self-maximization related socialization goals are not linked to children's self.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2513","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140651822","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Verification report: Egalitarianism in young children 核查报告:幼儿的平等主义
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-15 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2505
Georgia Clift, Jennifer Beaudry, Sumie Leung, Jordy Kaufman

The present study sought to evaluate the reproducibility of prominent findings stated by Fehr et al. in their developmental resource allocation experiment "Egalitarianism in Young Children", published in 2008. The experiment involved children making decisions about distributing sweets between themselves and either an in-group or an out-group recipient. Fehr et al. found that (1) inequity aversion develops with age; (2) 3- to 4-year-old children are inclined toward self-advantageous allocations, whereas 7- to 8-year-olds distribute sweets more evenly in divisions, and (3) the influence of group status increases as children age. The original article stated that 229 Swiss school students aged 3 to 8 years (102 boys, 127 girls) participated in the study. However, no further demographics were reported. In our attempts to reproduce Fehr et al.'s original analyses and reanalyse the raw dataset, we found that one of the key variables was miscoded. After rectifying the miscoded variable, the reproduction results revealed only one ambiguously irreproducible result regarding a group status main effect in the sharing mini-game—with three other tests exhibiting either strong reproducibility or ambiguous reproducibility. Reanalysis results indicated that Fehr et al.'s conclusions are robust when tested with alternative analytical tests.

Highlights

  • We evaluated the reproducibility of Fehr et al.'s (2008) highly cited developmental resource allocation findings.
  • After identifying an inversely coded variable, we found all tests—bar one probit regression not related to major findings—to be reproducible. Reanalysis confirmed the robustness of Fehr et al.'s conclusions when using alternate analytical methods.
  • Fehr et al.'s developmental resource allocation findings are reproducible and have theoretical implications for understanding inequity aversion and group status effects on resource allocation decisions in young children.
本研究旨在评估费尔等人在 2008 年发表的发展资源分配实验 "幼儿的平等主义 "中得出的重要结论的可重复性。实验中,儿童决定在自己和组内或组外接受者之间分配糖果。费尔等人发现:(1) 不公平厌恶会随着年龄的增长而发展;(2) 3 至 4 岁的儿童倾向于对自己有利的分配,而 7 至 8 岁的儿童在分配糖果时会更加平均;(3) 随着儿童年龄的增长,群体地位的影响也会增加。原文称,229 名 3 至 8 岁的瑞士小学生(102 名男孩,127 名女孩)参与了这项研究。但是,没有报告更多的人口统计数据。我们试图重现 Fehr 等人的原始分析并重新分析原始数据集,结果发现其中一个关键变量被误码。在纠正了误码变量后,重现结果只发现了一个关于分享小游戏中群体地位主效应的模糊不可再现结果,其他三个测试要么表现出很强的可再现性,要么表现出模糊的可再现性。重新分析的结果表明,Fehr 等人的结论在使用其他分析测试时是稳健的。亮点 我们评估了 Fehr 等人(2008 年)被高度引用的发展资源分配结论的可重复性。在确定了一个反向编码变量后,我们发现除了一个与主要发现无关的 probit 回归外,所有的检验结果都是可重复的。重新分析证实了 Fehr 等人的结论在使用其他分析方法时的稳健性。Fehr等人的发展性资源分配研究结果具有可重复性,对于理解不公平厌恶和群体地位对幼儿资源分配决策的影响具有理论意义。
{"title":"Verification report: Egalitarianism in young children","authors":"Georgia Clift,&nbsp;Jennifer Beaudry,&nbsp;Sumie Leung,&nbsp;Jordy Kaufman","doi":"10.1002/icd.2505","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2505","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>The present study sought to evaluate the reproducibility of prominent findings stated by Fehr et al. in their developmental resource allocation experiment \"Egalitarianism in Young Children\", published in 2008. The experiment involved children making decisions about distributing sweets between themselves and either an in-group or an out-group recipient. Fehr et al. found that (1) inequity aversion develops with age; (2) 3- to 4-year-old children are inclined toward self-advantageous allocations, whereas 7- to 8-year-olds distribute sweets more evenly in divisions, and (3) the influence of group status increases as children age. The original article stated that 229 Swiss school students aged 3 to 8 years (102 boys, 127 girls) participated in the study. However, no further demographics were reported. In our attempts to reproduce Fehr et al.'s original analyses and reanalyse the raw dataset, we found that one of the key variables was miscoded. After rectifying the miscoded variable, the reproduction results revealed only one ambiguously irreproducible result regarding a group status main effect in the sharing mini-game—with three other tests exhibiting either strong reproducibility or ambiguous reproducibility. Reanalysis results indicated that Fehr et al.'s conclusions are robust when tested with alternative analytical tests.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>We evaluated the reproducibility of Fehr et al.'s (2008) highly cited developmental resource allocation findings.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>After identifying an inversely coded variable, we found all tests—bar one probit regression not related to major findings—to be reproducible. Reanalysis confirmed the robustness of Fehr et al.'s conclusions when using alternate analytical methods.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Fehr et al.'s developmental resource allocation findings are reproducible and have theoretical implications for understanding inequity aversion and group status effects on resource allocation decisions in young children.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2505","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140557283","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Infant and Child Development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1