首页 > 最新文献

Infant and Child Development最新文献

英文 中文
Dyslexia is not a gift, but it is not that simple 阅读障碍不是天赋,但也没那么简单
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-08-17 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2454
Rachelle M. Johnson

There has been relentless debate as to whether dyslexia is a gift, and specifically, if dyslexics are more creative than the average person, despite established research evidence that there is no difference in creativity between those with and without dyslexia. With this paper, I outline that this conversation is not that simple, and one must consider society's view of disability and the developmental experience of growing up in a society that devalues dyslexic people. I conclude by positing that while dyslexia is not a gift, it is a part of neurodiversity.

Highlights

  1. This paper describes the nuances as to why, while dyslexia is not a gift, it is not that simple.
  2. As dyslexics develop, they exist within an ableist society that devalues people who are struggling readers, disabled, and neurodivergent.
  3. Dyslexia does not need to come with special gifts in order for dyslexics to hold value in society. Dyslexics are part of neurodiversity, and neurodiversity is valued.
关于阅读障碍是否是一种天赋,特别是阅读障碍者是否比普通人更有创造力,一直存在着激烈的争论,尽管已有研究证据表明,有阅读障碍和没有阅读障碍的人在创造力方面没有差异。通过这篇论文,我概述了这场对话并没有那么简单,人们必须考虑到社会对残疾的看法,以及在一个贬低阅读障碍者的社会中成长的发展经历。最后,我认为阅读障碍不是天赋,而是神经多样性的一部分。亮点本文描述了为什么阅读障碍不是一种天赋,但它并没有那么简单的细微差别。随着阅读障碍的发展,他们存在于一个残疾人社会中,这个社会贬低了那些苦苦挣扎的读者、残疾人和神经分化者。阅读障碍症不需要带着特殊的天赋就能让阅读障碍者在社会中保持价值。阅读障碍是神经多样性的一部分,神经多样性受到重视。
{"title":"Dyslexia is not a gift, but it is not that simple","authors":"Rachelle M. Johnson","doi":"10.1002/icd.2454","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2454","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>There has been relentless debate as to whether dyslexia is a gift, and specifically, if dyslexics are more creative than the average person, despite established research evidence that there is no difference in creativity between those with and without dyslexia. With this paper, I outline that this conversation is not that simple, and one must consider society's view of disability and the developmental experience of growing up in a society that devalues dyslexic people. I conclude by positing that while dyslexia is not a gift, it is a part of neurodiversity.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ol>\u0000 \u0000 <li>This paper describes the nuances as to why, while dyslexia is not a gift, it is not that simple.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>As dyslexics develop, they exist within an ableist society that devalues people who are struggling readers, disabled, and neurodivergent.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Dyslexia does not need to come with special gifts in order for dyslexics to hold value in society. Dyslexics are part of neurodiversity, and neurodiversity is valued.</li>\u0000 </ol>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"32 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-08-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50151582","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Toddlers' self-regulation development from 14 to 26 months: The unique role of paternal discipline 幼儿从14岁到26岁的自我调节发展 月:父亲管教的独特作用
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-08-16 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2455
Zheyan Wang, Wei Li, Mi-lan Woudstra, Lamei Wang

Toddlerhood is a critical period for children's self-regulation development during which parenting is undoubtedly a key influencing factor. The role that parents especially fathers play in shaping the emergence of self-regulation is still under investigation. This longitudinal study was carried out in 38 families in China, in which toddlers and both parents participated in two waves of observations during home visits, one performed when the toddlers were around 14 months old and another one 12 months later. Children's self-regulation was measured by behaviour control and Stroop tasks. Parental positive and negative discipline was coded by videotaped parent–child interactions. Results indicated that the mothers showed more positive discipline than the fathers at child 14 months, and there were no significant differences in positive and negative discipline between the fathers and mothers at child 26 months, neither in their negative discipline at child 14 months. Only the fathers' positive and negative discipline at child 14 months both significantly predicted toddlers' behaviour control at 26 months—over and above maternal discipline. This study reveals fathers' unique and irreplaceable contributions to children's early self-regulation development.

幼儿期是儿童自我调节发展的关键时期,在此期间,养育无疑是一个关键的影响因素。父母,尤其是父亲在形成自我调节方面所起的作用仍在调查中。这项纵向研究在中国的38个家庭中进行,在家访期间,学步儿童和父母双方参与了两波观察,一波是在学步儿童14个月大时进行的,另一波是12个月大 几个月后。儿童的自我调节是通过行为控制和Stroop任务来衡量的。父母的正面和负面管教是通过录像的父母与孩子的互动来编码的。结果表明,在孩子14岁时,母亲比父亲表现出更积极的管教 月,在孩子26岁时,父亲和母亲在积极和消极管教方面没有显著差异 在孩子14岁时,两人都没有受到消极的管教 月。只有父亲在孩子14岁时的积极和消极管教 两个月都显著预测了学步儿童26岁时的行为控制 几个月——超过了母亲的管教。这项研究揭示了父亲对儿童早期自我调节发展的独特和不可替代的贡献。
{"title":"Toddlers' self-regulation development from 14 to 26 months: The unique role of paternal discipline","authors":"Zheyan Wang,&nbsp;Wei Li,&nbsp;Mi-lan Woudstra,&nbsp;Lamei Wang","doi":"10.1002/icd.2455","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2455","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Toddlerhood is a critical period for children's self-regulation development during which parenting is undoubtedly a key influencing factor. The role that parents especially fathers play in shaping the emergence of self-regulation is still under investigation. This longitudinal study was carried out in 38 families in China, in which toddlers and both parents participated in two waves of observations during home visits, one performed when the toddlers were around 14 months old and another one 12 months later. Children's self-regulation was measured by behaviour control and Stroop tasks. Parental positive and negative discipline was coded by videotaped parent–child interactions. Results indicated that the mothers showed more positive discipline than the fathers at child 14 months, and there were no significant differences in positive and negative discipline between the fathers and mothers at child 26 months, neither in their negative discipline at child 14 months. Only the fathers' positive and negative discipline at child 14 months both significantly predicted toddlers' behaviour control at 26 months—over and above maternal discipline. This study reveals fathers' unique and irreplaceable contributions to children's early self-regulation development.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"32 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-08-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50134454","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Applying Research Domain Criteria to the study of emotion dysregulation in infancy and early childhood 应用研究领域标准研究婴幼儿情绪失调
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-08-07 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2452
Mindy A. Brown, Sheila E. Crowell, Elisabeth Conradt

Emotion regulation is a principal task of early development. The failure to develop effective emotion regulation skills is associated with behavioural, academic, psychological and socioemotional difficulties. Although researchers have studied emotion regulation for decades, work on emotion dysregulation—defined as the inability to maintain emotional control, engage with the environment or recover from distress in a developmentally appropriate manner—is rare and there are discrepancies in its conceptualisation among very young samples. While some scientists refer to emotion dysregulation as if it were a discrete concept, it may be more accurately depicted as a multidimensional system of developmental processes. The National Institute of Mental Health's Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) is a framework that allows researchers to track dysfunction across multiple areas of analysis and may be an ideal system for exploring the multidimensional nature of infant emotion dysregulation. In this paper, we draw on empirical studies to describe how the RDoC framework can be used to support studies of emotion dysregulation in infancy. This proposed model can also serve as a roadmap for future researchers to follow to generate the evidence base for a multidimensional conceptualisation of infant emotion dysregulation and its underlying mechanisms.

情绪调节是早期发育的主要任务。未能培养有效的情绪调节技能与行为、学业、心理和社会情绪困难有关。尽管研究人员已经研究情绪调节几十年了,但对情绪失调的研究却很少,情绪失调的定义是无法保持情绪控制、无法与环境相处或无法以适合发展的方式从痛苦中恢复,而且在非常年轻的样本中,其概念存在差异。虽然一些科学家将情绪失调视为一个离散的概念,但它可能更准确地描述为一个发育过程的多维系统。美国国家心理健康研究所的研究领域标准(RDoC)是一个框架,允许研究人员追踪多个分析领域的功能障碍,可能是探索婴儿情绪失调多维本质的理想系统。在本文中,我们利用实证研究来描述如何使用RDoC框架来支持婴儿期情绪失调的研究。这一提出的模型也可以作为未来研究人员的路线图,为婴儿情绪失调及其潜在机制的多维概念化提供证据基础。
{"title":"Applying Research Domain Criteria to the study of emotion dysregulation in infancy and early childhood","authors":"Mindy A. Brown,&nbsp;Sheila E. Crowell,&nbsp;Elisabeth Conradt","doi":"10.1002/icd.2452","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2452","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Emotion regulation is a principal task of early development. The failure to develop effective emotion regulation skills is associated with behavioural, academic, psychological and socioemotional difficulties. Although researchers have studied emotion regulation for decades, work on emotion dysregulation—defined as the inability to maintain emotional control, engage with the environment or recover from distress in a developmentally appropriate manner—is rare and there are discrepancies in its conceptualisation among very young samples. While some scientists refer to emotion dysregulation as if it were a discrete concept, it may be more accurately depicted as a multidimensional system of developmental processes. The National Institute of Mental Health's Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) is a framework that allows researchers to track dysfunction across multiple areas of analysis and may be an ideal system for exploring the multidimensional nature of infant emotion dysregulation. In this paper, we draw on empirical studies to describe how the RDoC framework can be used to support studies of emotion dysregulation in infancy. This proposed model can also serve as a roadmap for future researchers to follow to generate the evidence base for a multidimensional conceptualisation of infant emotion dysregulation and its underlying mechanisms.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"32 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-08-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50123788","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
I copy you as I believe you know about our culture: Combining imitation and selective trust literatures 我复制你对我们文化的了解:结合模仿和选择性信任文献
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-08-02 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2453
Frankie T. K. Fong, Mark Nielsen, Kathleen H. Corriveau

Empirical findings and theorizations of both imitation and selective trust offer different views on and interpretations of children's social learning mechanisms. The imitation literature provides ample documentation of children's behavioural patterns in the acquisition of socially appropriate norms and practices. The selective trust literature provides insights into children's cognitive processes of choosing credible informants and what information to learn in future interactions. In this paper, we place together findings from both fields and note that they share analogically similar theoretical underpinnings and offer explanations that are complementary to each other. We contend that children's imitative tendency may be due to their selection of in-group members as cultural experts, who serve as reliable sources of conventional information. Moving forward, we note the importance of evaluating individual differences and cultural factors to provide a more holistic understanding of universality and variation in children's social learning mechanisms.

模仿和选择性信任的经验发现和理论对儿童的社会学习机制提供了不同的观点和解释。模仿文献提供了大量关于儿童在获得适合社会的规范和做法方面的行为模式的文献。选择性信任文献深入了解了儿童选择可靠线人的认知过程,以及在未来的互动中要学习什么信息。在本文中,我们将这两个领域的研究结果放在一起,并注意到它们有着相似的理论基础,并提供了相互补充的解释。我们认为,儿童的模仿倾向可能是由于他们选择了作为文化专家的小组成员,他们是传统信息的可靠来源。展望未来,我们注意到评估个体差异和文化因素的重要性,以更全面地了解儿童社会学习机制的普遍性和变异性。
{"title":"I copy you as I believe you know about our culture: Combining imitation and selective trust literatures","authors":"Frankie T. K. Fong,&nbsp;Mark Nielsen,&nbsp;Kathleen H. Corriveau","doi":"10.1002/icd.2453","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2453","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Empirical findings and theorizations of both imitation and selective trust offer different views on and interpretations of children's social learning mechanisms. The imitation literature provides ample documentation of children's behavioural patterns in the acquisition of socially appropriate norms and practices. The selective trust literature provides insights into children's cognitive processes of choosing credible informants and what information to learn in future interactions. In this paper, we place together findings from both fields and note that they share analogically similar theoretical underpinnings and offer explanations that are complementary to each other. We contend that children's imitative tendency may be due to their selection of in-group members as <i>cultural experts</i>, who serve as reliable sources of conventional information. Moving forward, we note the importance of evaluating individual differences and cultural factors to provide a more holistic understanding of universality and variation in children's social learning mechanisms.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"32 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-08-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2453","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50117605","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children's preferences of the colour composition of art paintings 儿童对艺术绘画色彩构成的偏好
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-07-24 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2450
Tomoko Imura, Nobu Shirai, Taisei Kondo, Shigeki Nakauchi

Colour composition is acknowledged as a key contributor to aesthetic evaluation in visual art. Adult observers tend to prefer colour compositions that are particularly close to the original, regardless of the painting's category or the observer's cultural background. In this study, we examined whether children aged 5–12 years expressed a preference for the colour composition of original paintings using three scene types: abstract, figure, and botanical. The children each selected one of their preferred images from the paintings with four different colour compositions, including the original hue. A preference for the colour compositions of original figure paintings emerged between the ages of 5 and 8, and a similar preference for abstract and botanical paintings emerged between the ages of 9 and 12. Thus, preferences regarding colour composition in paintings are gradually acquired from childhood to adulthood.

Highlights

  • Children aged 9–12 years showed a preference for colour compositions closer to the originals, as did adults.
  • Children aged 5–8 years showed no particular colour preference for abstract or botanical paintings but did show a preference for figure paintings that were closest to the original with respect to colour composition.
  • Preferences for original colour composition in painting develop gradually from childhood to adulthood.
色彩构成被公认为视觉艺术审美评价的关键因素。无论绘画类别或观察者的文化背景如何,成年观察者往往更喜欢与原作特别接近的色彩构成。在这项研究中,我们检查了5-12岁的儿童 years表达了对使用三种场景类型的原画色彩构成的偏好:抽象、人物和植物。孩子们每人从画中选择一幅他们喜欢的图像,用四种不同的颜色组成,包括原始色调。5岁至8岁之间出现了对原始人物画色彩构成的偏好,9岁至12岁之间也出现了对抽象和植物画的类似偏好。因此,从童年到成年,人们对绘画色彩构成的偏好是逐渐形成的。亮点9-12岁的儿童 几岁的人和成年人一样,更喜欢接近原作的色彩。5-8岁的儿童 几年来,人们对抽象或植物画没有特别的色彩偏好,但在色彩构成方面,他们确实对最接近原作的人物画表现出了偏好。绘画中对原色构图的偏好从童年到成年逐渐发展。
{"title":"Children's preferences of the colour composition of art paintings","authors":"Tomoko Imura,&nbsp;Nobu Shirai,&nbsp;Taisei Kondo,&nbsp;Shigeki Nakauchi","doi":"10.1002/icd.2450","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2450","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Colour composition is acknowledged as a key contributor to aesthetic evaluation in visual art. Adult observers tend to prefer colour compositions that are particularly close to the original, regardless of the painting's category or the observer's cultural background. In this study, we examined whether children aged 5–12 years expressed a preference for the colour composition of original paintings using three scene types: abstract, figure, and botanical. The children each selected one of their preferred images from the paintings with four different colour compositions, including the original hue. A preference for the colour compositions of original figure paintings emerged between the ages of 5 and 8, and a similar preference for abstract and botanical paintings emerged between the ages of 9 and 12. Thus, preferences regarding colour composition in paintings are gradually acquired from childhood to adulthood.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Children aged 9–12 years showed a preference for colour compositions closer to the originals, as did adults.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Children aged 5–8 years showed no particular colour preference for abstract or botanical paintings but did show a preference for figure paintings that were closest to the original with respect to colour composition.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Preferences for original colour composition in painting develop gradually from childhood to adulthood.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"32 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-07-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50153954","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Use them or lose them: Are manipulatives needed to assess numeracy and geometry performance in preschool? 使用或丢失它们:是否需要操纵器来评估学前班的算术和几何表现?
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-07-19 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2444
Connor D. O'Rear, Erica L. Zippert, Patrick Ehrman, Lauren Westerberg, Christopher J. Lonigan, David J. Purpura

In two studies, we investigated whether using three-dimensional (3D) manipulatives during assessment aided performance on a variety of preschool mathematics tasks compared to pictorial representations. On measures of children's understanding of counting and cardinality (n = 103), there was no difference in performance between manipulatives and pictures, with Bayes factors suggesting moderate evidence in favor of the null hypothesis. On a measure of children's shape identification (n = 93), there was no difference in performance between objects and pictures, with Bayes factors suggesting moderate evidence in favor of the null hypothesis. These results suggest flexibility in the materials that can be used during assessment. Pictures, or 2D renderings of 3D objects, which can be easily printed and reproduced, may be sufficient for assessing counting and shape knowledge without the need for more cumbersome concrete manipulatives.

在两项研究中,我们调查了与图形表示相比,在评估过程中使用三维(3D)操作是否有助于各种学前数学任务的表现。关于儿童对计数和基数(n = 103),操纵词和图片之间的表现没有差异,贝叶斯因素表明有适度的证据支持零假设。关于儿童形状识别的一个测度(n = 93),物体和图片之间的表现没有差异,贝叶斯因素表明有适度的证据支持零假设。这些结果表明,在评估过程中可以使用的材料具有灵活性。可以容易地打印和复制的3D物体的图片或2D渲染图可能足以评估计数和形状知识,而不需要更繁琐的具体操作。
{"title":"Use them or lose them: Are manipulatives needed to assess numeracy and geometry performance in preschool?","authors":"Connor D. O'Rear,&nbsp;Erica L. Zippert,&nbsp;Patrick Ehrman,&nbsp;Lauren Westerberg,&nbsp;Christopher J. Lonigan,&nbsp;David J. Purpura","doi":"10.1002/icd.2444","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2444","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In two studies, we investigated whether using three-dimensional (3D) manipulatives during assessment aided performance on a variety of preschool mathematics tasks compared to pictorial representations. On measures of children's understanding of counting and cardinality (<i>n</i> = 103), there was no difference in performance between manipulatives and pictures, with Bayes factors suggesting moderate evidence in favor of the null hypothesis. On a measure of children's shape identification (<i>n</i> = 93), there was no difference in performance between objects and pictures, with Bayes factors suggesting moderate evidence in favor of the null hypothesis. These results suggest flexibility in the materials that can be used during assessment. Pictures, or 2D renderings of 3D objects, which can be easily printed and reproduced, may be sufficient for assessing counting and shape knowledge without the need for more cumbersome concrete manipulatives.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"32 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-07-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2444","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50138012","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Moulding environmental contexts to optimise neurodiverse executive function performance and development: A goodness-of-fit account 塑造环境背景以优化神经多样性执行功能的表现和发展:一个适合度的账户
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-07-19 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2448
Alexandra Hendry, Gaia Scerif

Executive functions (EFs) provide a top-down response to stimuli and events in pursuit of a goal. We argue that the extent to which an individual's environment is enriching and a good fit for them influences whether their performance at that moment is towards their upper- or lower-limit of EF ability. We outline the implications of this for interpreting measures of EF. We next argue that a child's sensitivity to the environment, and their caregivers' ability to modulate the environment to improve goodness-of-fit, influences the cumulative effects of the environment in shaping that child's actualised EF ability (the performance level shown in day-to-day situations), and thus their skill development. We therefore recommend that EF interventions be designed to improve children's actualised EF ability by improving their day-to-day environment, while simultaneously helping children modulate their physiological response to environmental challenges, and providing opportunities to practise EF skills in ecologically-valid contexts.

执行功能(EF)为追求目标提供自上而下的刺激和事件反应。我们认为,个人的环境在多大程度上丰富和适合他们,会影响他们当时的表现是达到EF能力的上限还是下限。我们概述了这对解释EF指标的影响。接下来,我们认为,儿童对环境的敏感性,以及他们的照顾者调节环境以提高适合度的能力,会影响环境在塑造儿童实际EF能力(日常情况下表现出的表现水平)方面的累积效应,从而影响他们的技能发展。因此,我们建议设计EF干预措施,通过改善儿童的日常环境来提高他们的实际EF能力,同时帮助儿童调节对环境挑战的生理反应,并提供在生态有效的环境中练习EF技能的机会。
{"title":"Moulding environmental contexts to optimise neurodiverse executive function performance and development: A goodness-of-fit account","authors":"Alexandra Hendry,&nbsp;Gaia Scerif","doi":"10.1002/icd.2448","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2448","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Executive functions (EFs) provide a top-down response to stimuli and events in pursuit of a goal. We argue that the extent to which an individual's environment is enriching and a good fit for them influences whether their performance at that moment is towards their upper- or lower-limit of EF ability. We outline the implications of this for interpreting measures of EF. We next argue that a child's sensitivity to the environment, and their caregivers' ability to modulate the environment to improve goodness-of-fit, influences the cumulative effects of the environment in shaping that child's actualised EF ability (the performance level shown in day-to-day situations), and thus their skill development. We therefore recommend that EF interventions be designed to improve children's actualised EF ability by improving their day-to-day environment, while simultaneously helping children modulate their physiological response to environmental challenges, and providing opportunities to practise EF skills in ecologically-valid contexts.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"32 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-07-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2448","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50138007","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Predictors of child student engagement in elementary school: A mixed-methods study exploring the role of externalising problems 小学生参与度的预测因素:探索外在化问题作用的混合方法研究
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-07-17 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2449
Ana Raquel Ribeiro, Ana Isabel Pereira, Marta Pedro, Magda Sofia Roberto

Student engagement promotes school learning and adaptation and can be a protective factor for children who are vulnerable to school failure. A longitudinal mixed-methods study was conducted to identify individual, family and school predictors of children's school engagement as well as to explore the facilitators of and obstacles to student engagement at school amongst children with externalising problems. The sample consisted of 369 elementary school children (53.7% girls) and their parents as well as 35 teachers. Participants completed several measures to evaluate emotional and behavioural engagement at school, externalising behaviours, parental emotional support, parental involvement with the school and student–teacher relationships. Semi structured interviews were conducted with a subsample of 17 parents of children with high levels of externalising problems. Problems with externalising behaviours and student–teacher relationships were significant predictors of school engagement. Parents’ reports showed that family support, positive student–teacher relationships, and positive relationships with peers are the main facilitators of school engagement in children with high levels of externalising problems. Our study highlights the role played by externalising problems in school engagement. Limitations of the study and implications for prevention research and practice are discussed.

Highlights

  • This study aimed to analyse individual, family and school predictors of children's school engagement and to explore the facilitators of and obstacles to school engagement in a subsample of children with high levels of externalising problems.
  • A mixed method design consisting of a longitudinal quantitative and cross-sectional qualitative approach was used.
  • The quantitative results show that problems with externalising behaviours and student–teacher relationships were significant predictors of school engagement.
  • The qualitative results show that family support, positive student–teacher relationships and positive relationships with peers are the main facilitators of school engagement in children with high levels of externalising problems
学生的参与促进了学校的学习和适应,可以成为易受学业失败影响的儿童的保护因素。进行了一项纵向混合方法研究,以确定儿童学校参与的个人、家庭和学校预测因素,并探讨有外在问题的儿童在学校参与的促进因素和障碍。样本包括369名小学生(53.7%为女孩)及其父母以及35名教师。参与者完成了几项评估学校情感和行为参与、外在行为、父母情感支持、父母对学校的参与以及师生关系的措施。对17名有高度外在化问题的孩子的父母进行了半结构化访谈。外在行为和师生关系问题是学校参与度的重要预测因素。家长报告显示,家庭支持、积极的师生关系以及与同龄人的积极关系是有高度外在问题的儿童参与学校活动的主要促进因素。我们的研究强调了外在化问题在学校参与中所起的作用。讨论了该研究的局限性以及对预防研究和实践的启示。亮点本研究旨在分析儿童学校参与的个人、家庭和学校预测因素,并在具有高度外部化问题的儿童的子样本中探索学校参与的促进因素和障碍。采用由纵向定量和横断面定性方法组成的混合方法设计。定量结果表明,外在行为和师生关系问题是学校参与度的重要预测因素。定性结果表明,家庭支持、积极的师生关系和与同龄人的积极关系是有高度外部化问题的儿童参与学校活动的主要促进因素
{"title":"Predictors of child student engagement in elementary school: A mixed-methods study exploring the role of externalising problems","authors":"Ana Raquel Ribeiro,&nbsp;Ana Isabel Pereira,&nbsp;Marta Pedro,&nbsp;Magda Sofia Roberto","doi":"10.1002/icd.2449","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2449","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Student engagement promotes school learning and adaptation and can be a protective factor for children who are vulnerable to school failure. A longitudinal mixed-methods study was conducted to identify individual, family and school predictors of children's school engagement as well as to explore the facilitators of and obstacles to student engagement at school amongst children with externalising problems. The sample consisted of 369 elementary school children (53.7% girls) and their parents as well as 35 teachers. Participants completed several measures to evaluate emotional and behavioural engagement at school, externalising behaviours, parental emotional support, parental involvement with the school and student–teacher relationships. Semi structured interviews were conducted with a subsample of 17 parents of children with high levels of externalising problems. Problems with externalising behaviours and student–teacher relationships were significant predictors of school engagement. Parents’ reports showed that family support, positive student–teacher relationships, and positive relationships with peers are the main facilitators of school engagement in children with high levels of externalising problems. Our study highlights the role played by externalising problems in school engagement. Limitations of the study and implications for prevention research and practice are discussed.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>This study aimed to analyse individual, family and school predictors of children's school engagement and to explore the facilitators of and obstacles to school engagement in a subsample of children with high levels of externalising problems.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>A mixed method design consisting of a longitudinal quantitative and cross-sectional qualitative approach was used.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>The quantitative results show that problems with externalising behaviours and student–teacher relationships were significant predictors of school engagement.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>The qualitative results show that family support, positive student–teacher relationships and positive relationships with peers are the main facilitators of school engagement in children with high levels of externalising problems</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"32 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-07-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50135831","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Variability in parenting behaviours during play and during mealtimes with toddlers 幼儿玩耍和用餐期间育儿行为的可变性
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-07-17 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2447
Bharathi J. Zvara, Sarah A. Keim, Rebecca Andridge, Sarah E. Anderson

Few empirical studies have explored variability in parenting behaviours observed in a mealtime setting as well as in a play setting with young children. We analysed data from 282 parent–toddler dyads who participated in the Play & Grow study in the United States in 2017–2019. Parent–child interactions were video recorded during play in the laboratory when the child was 18 months old and then during mealtime in the home 6 months later. Standardised coding procedures were used to rate parenting behaviours (sensitivity, cognitive stimulation, positive regard, detachment, intrusive control and negative regard). We describe correlations and differences and variability in parenting behaviour between the two assessments and relative to child sex, gestational age, parent education level, household composition and household income. Detachment was lower on average during play at 18 months compared to during mealtime at 24 months (Cohen's d = −0.40), and the other five parenting behaviours were greater on average during play than during mealtime (d = 0.18–0.43). Sociodemographic characteristics were unrelated to the magnitude of individual differences across contexts in any of the parenting behaviours. Implications for research on associations between parent–child interactions and risk for childhood obesity are discussed.

很少有实证研究探讨在用餐和幼儿玩耍环境中观察到的育儿行为的可变性。我们分析了282名参加Play&;2017年至2019年在美国进行增长研究。当孩子18岁时,父母和孩子的互动是在实验室玩耍时录制的视频 几个月大,6个月后在家吃饭。使用标准化的编码程序对养育行为(敏感性、认知刺激、积极关注、超然、侵入性控制和消极关注)进行评分。我们描述了两种评估之间育儿行为的相关性、差异性和可变性,以及与儿童性别、胎龄、父母教育水平、家庭组成和家庭收入的关系。在18岁的比赛中,分离度平均较低 与24岁的用餐时间相比 月(Cohen的d = −0.40),其他五种育儿行为在玩耍期间的平均值高于用餐期间(d = 0.18–0.43)。社会形态特征与任何育儿行为中不同背景下的个体差异程度无关。讨论了亲子互动与儿童肥胖风险之间关系的研究意义。
{"title":"Variability in parenting behaviours during play and during mealtimes with toddlers","authors":"Bharathi J. Zvara,&nbsp;Sarah A. Keim,&nbsp;Rebecca Andridge,&nbsp;Sarah E. Anderson","doi":"10.1002/icd.2447","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2447","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Few empirical studies have explored variability in parenting behaviours observed in a mealtime setting as well as in a play setting with young children. We analysed data from 282 parent–toddler dyads who participated in the Play &amp; Grow study in the United States in 2017–2019. Parent–child interactions were video recorded during play in the laboratory when the child was 18 months old and then during mealtime in the home 6 months later. Standardised coding procedures were used to rate parenting behaviours (sensitivity, cognitive stimulation, positive regard, detachment, intrusive control and negative regard). We describe correlations and differences and variability in parenting behaviour between the two assessments and relative to child sex, gestational age, parent education level, household composition and household income. Detachment was lower on average during play at 18 months compared to during mealtime at 24 months (Cohen's <i>d</i> = −0.40), and the other five parenting behaviours were greater on average during play than during mealtime (<i>d</i> = 0.18–0.43). Sociodemographic characteristics were unrelated to the magnitude of individual differences across contexts in any of the parenting behaviours. Implications for research on associations between parent–child interactions and risk for childhood obesity are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"32 5","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-07-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2447","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50136131","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pre-K attendance and social development: The moderating role of kindergarten classroom experiences 幼儿园出勤率与社会发展:课堂体验的调节作用
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-07-16 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2445
Kathryn Zimmermann, Qingqing Yang, Kelly Purtell, Arya Ansari

Although academic benefits of pre-K are well established, the associations between pre-K attendance and social and learning behaviours are less clear. Some research suggests that pre-K attenders may enter and exit kindergarten exhibiting less optimal social and learning behaviours than their non-attending peers, and little attention has been paid to how aspects of kindergarten experiences, like academic rigour, activity settings and teacher–student relationships, may contribute to these patterns of development. The current study addresses these gaps in knowledge by using nationally representative data from the ECLSK:2011 (n = 14,260) to examine the social and learning behaviours of pre-K attenders and nonattenders at kindergarten entry and exit. Our results reveal that pre-K attenders are not consistently underperforming their non-attending peers on social and learning behaviours in kindergarten. However, teachers reported that pre-K attenders exhibited more externalising behaviour problems at kindergarten entry and exit, and non-attenders made greater gains in learning behaviours over the kindergarten year. We also found no evidence of moderation when examining our focal classroom experience variables.

虽然学前教育在学业上的好处是公认的,但学前教育出勤率与社会和学习行为之间的联系却不太清楚。一些研究表明,学龄前儿童在进入和离开幼儿园时,表现出的社交和学习行为可能比不参加幼儿园的同龄人更不理想,而且很少有人关注幼儿园经历的各个方面,如学术严谨性、活动环境和师生关系,可能对这些发展模式有何影响。目前的研究通过使用ECLSK:2011年的全国代表性数据(n = 14,260)来解决这些知识差距,以检查幼儿园入学和退学时学龄前儿童和非学龄前儿童的社会和学习行为。我们的研究结果显示,学龄前儿童在幼儿园的社交和学习行为上并不总是表现不如非学龄前儿童。然而,老师们报告说,幼儿园前参加的孩子在幼儿园入学和毕业时表现出更多的外化行为问题,而非参加的孩子在幼儿园的学习行为上取得了更大的进步。在检查我们的重点课堂体验变量时,我们也没有发现适度的证据。
{"title":"Pre-K attendance and social development: The moderating role of kindergarten classroom experiences","authors":"Kathryn Zimmermann,&nbsp;Qingqing Yang,&nbsp;Kelly Purtell,&nbsp;Arya Ansari","doi":"10.1002/icd.2445","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2445","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Although academic benefits of pre-K are well established, the associations between pre-K attendance and social and learning behaviours are less clear. Some research suggests that pre-K attenders may enter and exit kindergarten exhibiting less optimal social and learning behaviours than their non-attending peers, and little attention has been paid to how aspects of kindergarten experiences, like academic rigour, activity settings and teacher–student relationships, may contribute to these patterns of development. The current study addresses these gaps in knowledge by using nationally representative data from the ECLSK:2011 (<i>n</i> = 14,260) to examine the social and learning behaviours of pre-K attenders and nonattenders at kindergarten entry and exit. Our results reveal that pre-K attenders are not consistently underperforming their non-attending peers on social and learning behaviours in kindergarten. However, teachers reported that pre-K attenders exhibited more externalising behaviour problems at kindergarten entry and exit, and non-attenders made greater gains in learning behaviours over the kindergarten year. We also found no evidence of moderation when examining our focal classroom experience variables.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 2","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-07-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2445","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138538854","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Infant and Child Development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1