首页 > 最新文献

Infancy最新文献

英文 中文
Caregiver encouragement to act on objects is related with crawling infants' receptive language 看护人鼓励婴儿对物体进行操作与爬行婴儿的接受性语言有关。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-26 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12592
Lukas D. Lopez, Eric A. Walle

The progression from crawling to walking in infancy is associated with changes in infant language development. One possible explanation for such change is the infant's language environment. Prior research indicates that caregivers use more action directives with walking infants compared to crawling infants, but the relations of such parental speech with infant vocabulary is unknown. Here, we present findings from day-long home audio recordings (Study 1) and laboratory observations (Study 2) of same-aged crawling and walking infants to explore how caregiver language, specifically action directives, were associated with parent reported infant vocabulary size. Findings in both studies indicated that caregiver action directives were associated with crawling, but not walking infants' receptive vocabulary sizes. Specifically, action directives about objects occurring when the infant and caregiver were not jointly engaged were associated with higher receptive vocabulary scores for crawling infants, but no such pattern was found for walking infants. The replication of results in distinct samples with different research methodologies strengthens the findings. Taken together, these studies demonstrate that caregiver social engagement specific to infant motoric development is related with infant language learning.

婴儿期从爬行到行走的过程与婴儿语言发展的变化有关。造成这种变化的一个可能原因是婴儿的语言环境。先前的研究表明,与爬行婴儿相比,照料者对行走婴儿使用更多的行动指令,但父母的这种言语与婴儿词汇量之间的关系尚不清楚。在此,我们展示了对同龄爬行和行走婴儿进行的长达一天的家庭录音(研究 1)和实验室观察(研究 2)的结果,以探讨照料者的语言,特别是行动指令,与家长报告的婴儿词汇量大小之间的关系。这两项研究的结果表明,照顾者的动作指令与爬行婴儿的接受词汇量有关,但与学步婴儿的接受词汇量无关。具体来说,当婴儿和照料者没有共同参与时,对物体的动作指令与爬行婴儿较高的接受词汇量得分有关,但对行走婴儿则没有发现这种模式。在采用不同研究方法的不同样本中复制的结果加强了研究结果。总之,这些研究表明,婴儿运动发展所特有的照顾者社会参与与婴儿的语言学习有关。
{"title":"Caregiver encouragement to act on objects is related with crawling infants' receptive language","authors":"Lukas D. Lopez,&nbsp;Eric A. Walle","doi":"10.1111/infa.12592","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12592","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The progression from crawling to walking in infancy is associated with changes in infant language development. One possible explanation for such change is the infant's language environment. Prior research indicates that caregivers use more action directives with walking infants compared to crawling infants, but the relations of such parental speech with infant vocabulary is unknown. Here, we present findings from day-long home audio recordings (Study 1) and laboratory observations (Study 2) of same-aged crawling and walking infants to explore how caregiver language, specifically action directives, were associated with parent reported infant vocabulary size. Findings in both studies indicated that caregiver action directives were associated with crawling, but not walking infants' receptive vocabulary sizes. Specifically, action directives about objects occurring when the infant and caregiver were not jointly engaged were associated with higher receptive vocabulary scores for crawling infants, but no such pattern was found for walking infants. The replication of results in distinct samples with different research methodologies strengthens the findings. Taken together, these studies demonstrate that caregiver social engagement specific to infant motoric development is related with infant language learning.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 4","pages":"550-570"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12592","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140289283","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
2-Year-olds’ free play during the COVID-19 pandemic COVID-19 大流行期间 2 岁儿童的自由游戏。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-26 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12591
Katharina Tisborn, Sabine Seehagen

Free play is a natural activity in toddlerhood, depending on environmental conditions like available objects and the social environment. The COVID-19 pandemic and its consequences for parents’ mental health held the potential to change toddlers’ play environment. This cross-sectional study investigated 2-year-olds’ (N = 97) free play with objects, and aspects of caregiver mental well-being in three cohorts during the pandemic in Germany. Caregivers reported their positive mental health (PMH), threat perception, perception of current family situation from negative to positive, and workload. We categorized toddlers’ behavior in free play sessions in their homes with a fixed set of objects through behavioral coding. Play behavior did not differ between cohorts and did not correlate with caregivers’ positive mental health, threat perception, and perception of family situation. A MANOVA revealed a significant main effect of cohort on PMH, threat perception and perception of family situation, qualified by two discriminant functions. Full sample analyses revealed that toddlers of caregivers perceiving a workload increase compared to the time before the pandemic showed less pretend play, and less functional and nonfunctional play. The results provide insights into 2-year-olds’ play behavior during a global pandemic and highlight the role of caregiver availability for children’s play.

自由游戏是幼儿期的一种自然活动,取决于可用物品和社会环境等环境条件。COVID-19 的流行及其对父母心理健康的影响有可能改变幼儿的游戏环境。这项横断面研究调查了德国大流行期间三组 2 岁幼儿(97 人)与物品自由玩耍的情况,以及照顾者的心理健康情况。照顾者报告了他们的积极心理健康(PMH)、威胁感、对当前家庭状况从消极到积极的看法以及工作量。我们通过行为编码对幼儿在家中与固定物品自由玩耍时的行为进行了分类。游戏行为在不同组群之间没有差异,也与照顾者的积极心理健康、威胁感和家庭状况感知无关。MANOVA 分析表明,队列对积极心理健康、威胁感和家庭状况感有显著的主效应,并有两个判别函数。全样本分析表明,与大流行前相比,保育员认为工作量增加的幼儿表现出较少的装扮游戏,以及较少的功能性和非功能性游戏。研究结果为我们提供了在全球大流行期间两岁幼儿游戏行为的洞察力,并强调了照顾者的可用性对幼儿游戏的作用。
{"title":"2-Year-olds’ free play during the COVID-19 pandemic","authors":"Katharina Tisborn,&nbsp;Sabine Seehagen","doi":"10.1111/infa.12591","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12591","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Free play is a natural activity in toddlerhood, depending on environmental conditions like available objects and the social environment. The COVID-19 pandemic and its consequences for parents’ mental health held the potential to change toddlers’ play environment. This cross-sectional study investigated 2-year-olds’ (<i>N</i> = 97) free play with objects, and aspects of caregiver mental well-being in three cohorts during the pandemic in Germany. Caregivers reported their positive mental health (PMH), threat perception, perception of current family situation from negative to positive, and workload. We categorized toddlers’ behavior in free play sessions in their homes with a fixed set of objects through behavioral coding. Play behavior did not differ between cohorts and did not correlate with caregivers’ positive mental health, threat perception, and perception of family situation. A MANOVA revealed a significant main effect of cohort on PMH, threat perception and perception of family situation, qualified by two discriminant functions. Full sample analyses revealed that toddlers of caregivers perceiving a workload increase compared to the time before the pandemic showed less pretend play, and less functional and nonfunctional play. The results provide insights into 2-year-olds’ play behavior during a global pandemic and highlight the role of caregiver availability for children’s play.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 4","pages":"608-630"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12591","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140289282","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Better in sync: Temporal dynamics explain multisensory word-action-object learning in early development 更好地同步:时间动态解释早期发育中的多感官单词-动作-物体学习。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-23 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12590
Ricarda Bothe, Sarah Eiteljoerge, Leonie Trouillet, Birgit Elsner, Nivedita Mani

We investigated the temporal impact of multisensory settings on children's learning of word-object and action-object associations at 1- and 2-years of age. Specifically, we examined whether the temporal alignment of words and actions influenced the acquisition of novel word-action-object associations. We used a preferential looking and violation of expectation task in which infants and young children were first presented with two distinct word-object and action-object pairings either in a synchronous (overlapping in time) or sequential manner (one after the other). Findings revealed that 2-year-olds recognized both, action-object and word-object associations when they first saw the word-action-object combinations synchronously, but not sequentially, as evidenced by looking behavior. 1-year-olds did not show evidence for recognition for either of the word-object and action-object pairs, regardless of the initial temporal alignment of these cues. To control for individual differences, we explored factors that might influence associative learning based on parental reports of 1- and 2-year-olds development, however, developmental measures did not explain word-action-object associative learning in either group. We discuss that while young children may benefit from the temporal alignment of multisensory cues as it enables them to actively engage with the multisensory content in real-time, infants may have been overwhelmed by the complexity of this input.

我们研究了多感官环境对一岁和两岁儿童学习词-物和动-物联想的时间影响。具体来说,我们研究了单词和动作的时间一致性是否会影响新的单词-动作-物体联想的习得。在这项任务中,婴幼儿首先以同步(时间上重叠)或顺序(一个接一个)的方式看到两个不同的词-对象和动作-对象配对。研究结果表明,两岁幼儿在第一次看到单词-动作-物体组合时,能同时辨认出动作-物体和单词-物体的关联,但不能依次辨认,这一点可以从幼儿的注视行为中得到证明。无论这些线索最初在时间上是否一致,1 岁幼儿都无法识别词-物和动-物对。为了控制个体差异,我们根据家长对 1 岁和 2 岁幼儿发展情况的报告,探讨了可能影响联想学习的因素,然而,发展测量并不能解释这两组幼儿的词-动作-物体联想学习。我们认为,幼儿可能会从多感官线索的时间一致性中获益,因为这能让他们积极地实时参与多感官内容,但婴儿可能会被这种输入的复杂性所淹没。
{"title":"Better in sync: Temporal dynamics explain multisensory word-action-object learning in early development","authors":"Ricarda Bothe,&nbsp;Sarah Eiteljoerge,&nbsp;Leonie Trouillet,&nbsp;Birgit Elsner,&nbsp;Nivedita Mani","doi":"10.1111/infa.12590","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12590","url":null,"abstract":"<p>We investigated the temporal impact of multisensory settings on children's learning of word-object and action-object associations at 1- and 2-years of age. Specifically, we examined whether the temporal alignment of words and actions influenced the acquisition of novel word-action-object associations. We used a preferential looking and violation of expectation task in which infants and young children were first presented with two distinct word-object and action-object pairings either in a synchronous (overlapping in time) or sequential manner (one after the other). Findings revealed that 2-year-olds recognized both, action-object and word-object associations when they first saw the word-action-object combinations synchronously, but not sequentially, as evidenced by looking behavior. 1-year-olds did not show evidence for recognition for either of the word-object and action-object pairs, regardless of the initial temporal alignment of these cues. To control for individual differences, we explored factors that might influence associative learning based on parental reports of 1- and 2-year-olds development, however, developmental measures did not explain word-action-object associative learning in either group. We discuss that while young children may benefit from the temporal alignment of multisensory cues as it enables them to actively engage with the multisensory content in real-time, infants may have been overwhelmed by the complexity of this input.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 4","pages":"482-509"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-03-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12590","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140194835","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Parental sensitivity and intrusiveness with mothers and fathers: Associations between parental behavioral activation/inhibition and infant temperament 父母对母亲和父亲的敏感性和侵入性:父母行为激活/抑制与婴儿性情之间的关联。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-21 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12589
Lauren G. Bailes, Diane M. Lickenbrock, Alyssa R. Swift, Logan J. Rios

Understanding the factors contributing to sensitive parenting is crucial to optimize infant social and emotional functioning. Research has supported the association between parents' personality and parenting quality, but findings are inconsistent when examining various global personality measures. Further, it is likely that the interaction between parent-level (e.g., personality) and infant-level characteristics (e.g., temperament) are more strongly associated with caregiving quality. Most studies examining predictors of parenting quality have only included mothers, compared to fathers. The current study examined the interaction between parental personality and infant temperament and associations with parental sensitivity and intrusiveness with mothers and fathers. The participants included families (n = 102) when the infants were 4, 6, and 8 months old. Using parent report measures and a face-to-face play task, significant main effects of maternal behavioral inhibition on parenting behaviors were observed for mothers. A Behavioral Activation X Infant Negative Reactivity interaction predicted both maternal sensitivity and intrusiveness, whereas a Behavioral Inhibition X Infant Surgency predicted paternal intrusiveness. In summary, the results revealed support for the goodness-of-fit perspective between parents' personality and infant temperament.

了解养育子女的敏感因素对于优化婴儿的社会和情感功能至关重要。研究支持父母的个性与养育质量之间的关联,但在研究各种综合个性测量时,研究结果并不一致。此外,父母层面(如性格)和婴儿层面的特征(如脾气)之间的相互作用很可能与养育质量有更密切的关系。与父亲相比,大多数研究只对母亲进行了育儿质量预测。本研究考察了父母性格与婴儿性情之间的相互作用,以及父母的敏感性和侵入性与母亲和父亲之间的关联。研究对象包括婴儿4、6和8个月大时的家庭(n = 102)。通过父母报告测量和面对面游戏任务,观察到母亲的行为抑制对养育行为有显著的主效应。行为激活 X 婴儿消极反应的交互作用预测了母亲的敏感性和干涉性,而行为抑制 X 婴儿急躁则预测了父亲的干涉性。总之,研究结果表明,父母的性格与婴儿的气质之间的拟合度很高。
{"title":"Parental sensitivity and intrusiveness with mothers and fathers: Associations between parental behavioral activation/inhibition and infant temperament","authors":"Lauren G. Bailes,&nbsp;Diane M. Lickenbrock,&nbsp;Alyssa R. Swift,&nbsp;Logan J. Rios","doi":"10.1111/infa.12589","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12589","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Understanding the factors contributing to sensitive parenting is crucial to optimize infant social and emotional functioning. Research has supported the association between parents' personality and parenting quality, but findings are inconsistent when examining various global personality measures. Further, it is likely that the interaction between parent-level (e.g., personality) and infant-level characteristics (e.g., temperament) are more strongly associated with caregiving quality. Most studies examining predictors of parenting quality have only included mothers, compared to fathers. The current study examined the interaction between parental personality and infant temperament and associations with parental sensitivity and intrusiveness with mothers and fathers. The participants included families (<i>n</i> = 102) when the infants were 4, 6, and 8 months old. Using parent report measures and a face-to-face play task, significant main effects of maternal behavioral inhibition on parenting behaviors were observed for mothers. A Behavioral Activation X Infant Negative Reactivity interaction predicted both maternal sensitivity and intrusiveness, whereas a Behavioral Inhibition X Infant Surgency predicted paternal intrusiveness. In summary, the results revealed support for the goodness-of-fit perspective between parents' personality and infant temperament.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 4","pages":"571-589"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-03-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12589","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140177174","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Prenatal influences on postnatal neuroplasticity: Integrating DOHaD and sensitive/critical period frameworks to understand biological embedding in early development 产前对产后神经可塑性的影响:整合 DOHaD 和敏感期/关键期框架,了解早期发育中的生物嵌入。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-06 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12588
Emma T. Margolis, Laurel J. Gabard-Durnam

Early environments can have significant and lasting effects on brain, body, and behavior across the lifecourse. Here, we address current research efforts to understand how experiences impact neurodevelopment with a new perspective integrating two well-known conceptual frameworks – the Developmental Origins of Health and Disease (DOHaD) and sensitive/critical period frameworks. Specifically, we consider how prenatal experiences characterized in the DOHaD model impact two key neurobiological mechanisms of sensitive/critical periods for adapting to and learning from the postnatal environment. We draw from both animal and human research to summarize the current state of knowledge on how particular prenatal substance exposures (psychoactive substances and heavy metals) and nutritional profiles (protein-energy malnutrition and iron deficiency) each differentially impact brain circuits' excitation/GABAergic inhibition balance and myelination. Finally, we highlight new research directions that emerge from this integrated framework, including testing how prenatal environments alter sensitive/critical period timing and learning and identifying potential promotional/buffering prenatal exposures to impact postnatal sensitive/critical periods. We hope this integrative framework considering prenatal influences on postnatal neuroplasticity will stimulate new research to understand how early environments have lasting consequences on our brains, behavior, and health.

早期环境会对人一生中的大脑、身体和行为产生重大而持久的影响。在此,我们将从一个新的视角,结合两个著名的概念框架--健康与疾病的发展起源(DOHaD)和敏感期/关键期框架--来探讨当前的研究工作,以了解经历如何影响神经发育。具体来说,我们将考虑 DOHaD 模型所描述的产前经历如何影响敏感期/关键期这两个关键的神经生物学机制,以适应产后环境并从中学习。我们从动物和人类研究中总结了目前关于特定产前物质接触(精神活性物质和重金属)和营养状况(蛋白质能量营养不良和缺铁)如何对大脑回路的兴奋/GABA能抑制平衡和髓鞘化产生不同影响的知识。最后,我们强调了从这一综合框架中产生的新的研究方向,包括测试产前环境如何改变敏感期/关键期的时间和学习,以及确定潜在的促进/缓冲产前暴露对产后敏感期/关键期的影响。我们希望这个考虑产前对产后神经可塑性影响的综合框架能激发新的研究,以了解早期环境如何对我们的大脑、行为和健康产生持久的影响。
{"title":"Prenatal influences on postnatal neuroplasticity: Integrating DOHaD and sensitive/critical period frameworks to understand biological embedding in early development","authors":"Emma T. Margolis,&nbsp;Laurel J. Gabard-Durnam","doi":"10.1111/infa.12588","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12588","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Early environments can have significant and lasting effects on brain, body, and behavior across the lifecourse. Here, we address current research efforts to understand how experiences impact neurodevelopment with a new perspective integrating two well-known conceptual frameworks – the Developmental Origins of Health and Disease (DOHaD) and sensitive/critical period frameworks. Specifically, we consider how prenatal experiences characterized in the DOHaD model impact two key neurobiological mechanisms of sensitive/critical periods for adapting to and learning from the postnatal environment. We draw from both animal and human research to summarize the current state of knowledge on how particular prenatal substance exposures (psychoactive substances and heavy metals) and nutritional profiles (protein-energy malnutrition and iron deficiency) each differentially impact brain circuits' excitation/GABAergic inhibition balance and myelination. Finally, we highlight new research directions that emerge from this integrated framework, including testing how prenatal environments alter sensitive/critical period timing and learning and identifying potential promotional/buffering prenatal exposures to impact postnatal sensitive/critical periods. We hope this integrative framework considering prenatal influences on postnatal neuroplasticity will stimulate new research to understand how early environments have lasting consequences on our brains, behavior, and health.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"30 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12588","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140050727","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Developmental change in English-learning children's interpretations of salient pitch contours in word learning 学习英语的儿童在单词学习中对显著音高轮廓的解释的发展变化。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-02-29 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12587
Carolyn Quam, Daniel Swingley

To efficiently recognize words, children learning an intonational language like English should avoid interpreting pitch-contour variation as signaling lexical contrast, despite the relevance of pitch at other levels of structure. Thus far, the developmental time-course with which English-learning children rule out pitch as a contrastive feature has been incompletely characterized. Prior studies have tested diverse lexical contrasts and have not tested beyond 30 months. To specify the developmental trajectory over a broader age range, we extended a prior study (Quam & Swingley, 2010), in which 30-month-olds and adults disregarded pitch changes, but attended to vowel changes, in newly learned words. Using the same phonological contrasts, we tested 3- to 5-year-olds, 24-month-olds, and 18-month-olds. The older two groups were tested using the language-guided-looking method. The oldest group attended to vowels but not pitch. Surprisingly, 24-month-olds ignored not just pitch but sometimes vowels as well—conflicting with prior findings of phonological constraint at 24 months. The youngest group was tested using the Switch habituation method, half with additional phonetic variability in training. Eighteen-month-olds learned both pitch-contrasted and vowel-contrasted words, whether or not additional variability was present. Thus, native-language phonological constraint was not evidenced prior to 30 months (Quam & Swingley, 2010). We contextualize our findings within other recent work in this area.

为了有效地识别单词,学习像英语这样的语调语言的儿童应避免将音高-轮廓变化解释为词汇对比的信号,尽管音高在其他结构层次上也有相关性。迄今为止,学习英语的儿童排除音高作为对比特征的发展时间过程尚未完全定性。之前的研究测试了多种词汇对比,但没有测试 30 个月以上的儿童。为了在更大的年龄范围内明确儿童的发展轨迹,我们扩展了之前的一项研究(Quam & Swingley, 2010),在这项研究中,30 个月大的儿童和成人在新学的单词中无视音高变化,但关注元音变化。我们使用相同的语音对比,对 3-5 岁儿童、24 个月儿童和 18 个月儿童进行了测试。年龄较大的两组幼儿采用语言引导观察法进行测试。年龄最大的一组注意元音,但不注意音高。令人惊讶的是,24 个月大的幼儿不仅忽略了音高,有时还忽略了元音,这与之前关于 24 个月大幼儿语音限制的研究结果相矛盾。最年幼组的测试采用了交换习惯法,其中一半在训练中增加了语音变异。18 个月大的幼儿无论是否存在额外的变异,都能学会音高对比和元音对比的单词。因此,母语语音限制在 30 个月前并不明显(Quam 和 Swingley,2010 年)。我们将我们的研究结果与该领域的其他最新研究结合起来。
{"title":"Developmental change in English-learning children's interpretations of salient pitch contours in word learning","authors":"Carolyn Quam,&nbsp;Daniel Swingley","doi":"10.1111/infa.12587","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12587","url":null,"abstract":"<p>To efficiently recognize words, children learning an intonational language like English should avoid interpreting pitch-contour variation as signaling lexical contrast, despite the relevance of pitch at other levels of structure. Thus far, the developmental time-course with which English-learning children rule out pitch as a contrastive feature has been incompletely characterized. Prior studies have tested diverse lexical contrasts and have not tested beyond 30 months. To specify the developmental trajectory over a broader age range, we extended a prior study (Quam &amp; Swingley, 2010), in which 30-month-olds and adults disregarded pitch changes, but attended to vowel changes, in newly learned words. Using the same phonological contrasts, we tested 3- to 5-year-olds, 24-month-olds, and 18-month-olds. The older two groups were tested using the language-guided-looking method. The oldest group attended to vowels but not pitch. Surprisingly, 24-month-olds ignored not just pitch but sometimes vowels as well—conflicting with prior findings of phonological constraint at 24 months. The youngest group was tested using the Switch habituation method, half with additional phonetic variability in training. Eighteen-month-olds learned both pitch-contrasted and vowel-contrasted words, whether or not additional variability was present. Thus, native-language phonological constraint was not evidenced prior to 30 months (Quam &amp; Swingley, 2010). We contextualize our findings within other recent work in this area.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 3","pages":"355-385"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-02-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12587","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139997866","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Early gesture use predicts children’s language development in South Korea: New evidence supporting the cross-cultural importance of pointing 早期手势的使用可预测韩国儿童的语言发展:支持指向的跨文化重要性的新证据
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-02-26 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12585
So Yeon Shin, Meredith L. Rowe, Hyun Suk Lee

Research in the U.S. and other Western countries shows that children’s early gesture use, which starts prior to verbal communication, is an important predictor of children’s later language development. Despite increasing efforts to study gesture use in diverse contexts, most of our knowledge on the role of gesture is largely based on populations of Western countries. In this study, we add to the growing body of international research by examining gesture use by 31 mothers and their 14-month-old infants (12 girls) in South Korea and investigate the gestures used during interaction, and whether early gesture use at 14 months predicts Korean children’s later language skills at 36 months. The results showed that in addition to using gestures observed in other cultural contexts, Korean mother-child dyads used culturally specific gesture (i.e., bowing), showing an early sign of socialization that starts with preverbal children. In addition, Korean infants’ index-finger pointing, but not showing and giving, predicted their later receptive and expressive vocabulary skills at 36 months, providing additional support for the importance of pointing in early language development.

美国和其他西方国家的研究表明,儿童在语言交流之前开始使用的早期手势是儿童日后语言发展的重要预测指标。尽管我们越来越努力地研究儿童在不同情境中使用手势的情况,但我们对手势作用的了解大多基于西方国家的人群。在本研究中,我们对韩国 31 名母亲及其 14 个月大的婴儿(12 名女孩)的手势使用情况进行了研究,调查了她们在互动过程中使用的手势,以及 14 个月大时的早期手势使用是否能预测韩国儿童日后 36 个月大时的语言技能,从而为日益增多的国际研究增添了新的内容。结果表明,除了使用在其他文化背景下观察到的手势外,韩国母子二人组还使用了文化特有的手势(即鞠躬),显示了从语言前儿童开始的社会化的早期迹象。此外,韩国婴儿的食指指向(而非显示和给予)可以预测他们日后在 36 个月大时的接受和表达词汇能力,这为指向在早期语言发展中的重要性提供了更多支持。
{"title":"Early gesture use predicts children’s language development in South Korea: New evidence supporting the cross-cultural importance of pointing","authors":"So Yeon Shin,&nbsp;Meredith L. Rowe,&nbsp;Hyun Suk Lee","doi":"10.1111/infa.12585","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12585","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Research in the U.S. and other Western countries shows that children’s early gesture use, which starts prior to verbal communication, is an important predictor of children’s later language development. Despite increasing efforts to study gesture use in diverse contexts, most of our knowledge on the role of gesture is largely based on populations of Western countries. In this study, we add to the growing body of international research by examining gesture use by 31 mothers and their 14-month-old infants (12 girls) in South Korea and investigate the gestures used during interaction, and whether early gesture use at 14 months predicts Korean children’s later language skills at 36 months. The results showed that in addition to using gestures observed in other cultural contexts, Korean mother-child dyads used culturally specific gesture (i.e., bowing), showing an early sign of socialization that starts with preverbal children. In addition, Korean infants’ index-finger pointing, but not showing and giving, predicted their later receptive and expressive vocabulary skills at 36 months, providing additional support for the importance of pointing in early language development.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 3","pages":"327-354"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-02-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12585","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139968897","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sustained looking at faces at 5 months of age is associated with socio-communicative skills in the second year of life 5 个月大时持续看脸与第二年的社会交流能力有关。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-02-15 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12586
Charlotte Viktorsson, Ana Maria Portugal, Mark J. Taylor, Angelica Ronald, Terje Falck-Ytter

Efficiently processing information from faces in infancy is foundational for nonverbal communication. We studied individual differences in 5-month-old infants' (N = 517) sustained attention to faces and preference for emotional faces. We assessed the contribution of genetic and environmental influences to individual differences in these gaze behaviors, and the association between these traits and other concurrent and later phenotypes. We found an association between the mean duration of looking at a face (before looking away from it) at 5 months and socio-communicative abilities at 14 months (β = 0.17, 95% CI: 0.08; 0.26, p < 0.001). Sustained attention to faces predicted socio-communicative abilities over and above variance captured by mean fixation duration. We also found a statistically significant but weak tendency to prefer looking at smiling faces (relative to neutral faces), but no indication that variability in this behavior was explained by genetic effects. Moderate heritability was found for sustained attention to faces (A = 0.23, CI: 0.06; 0.38), while shared environmental influences were non-significant for both phenotypes. These findings suggest that sustained looking at individual faces before looking away is a developmentally significant ‘social attention’ phenotype in infancy, characterized by moderate heritability and a specific relation to later socio-communicative abilities.

有效处理婴儿期的面孔信息是非语言交流的基础。我们研究了 5 个月大婴儿(N = 517)对面孔的持续关注和对情感面孔的偏好的个体差异。我们评估了遗传和环境对这些注视行为个体差异的影响,以及这些特征与其他同时出现的表型和后期表型之间的关联。我们发现,5 个月大时注视一张面孔(在移开视线之前)的平均持续时间与 14 个月大时的社会交流能力之间存在关联(β = 0.17,95% CI: 0.08; 0.26,p
{"title":"Sustained looking at faces at 5 months of age is associated with socio-communicative skills in the second year of life","authors":"Charlotte Viktorsson,&nbsp;Ana Maria Portugal,&nbsp;Mark J. Taylor,&nbsp;Angelica Ronald,&nbsp;Terje Falck-Ytter","doi":"10.1111/infa.12586","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12586","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Efficiently processing information from faces in infancy is foundational for nonverbal communication. We studied individual differences in 5-month-old infants' (<i>N</i> = 517) sustained attention to faces and preference for emotional faces. We assessed the contribution of genetic and environmental influences to individual differences in these gaze behaviors, and the association between these traits and other concurrent and later phenotypes. We found an association between the mean duration of looking at a face (before looking away from it) at 5 months and socio-communicative abilities at 14 months (<i>β</i> = 0.17, 95% CI: 0.08; 0.26, <i>p</i> &lt; 0.001). Sustained attention to faces predicted socio-communicative abilities over and above variance captured by mean fixation duration. We also found a statistically significant but weak tendency to prefer looking at smiling faces (relative to neutral faces), but no indication that variability in this behavior was explained by genetic effects. Moderate heritability was found for sustained attention to faces (<i>A</i> = 0.23, CI: 0.06; 0.38), while shared environmental influences were non-significant for both phenotypes. These findings suggest that sustained looking at individual faces before looking away is a developmentally significant ‘social attention’ phenotype in infancy, characterized by moderate heritability and a specific relation to later socio-communicative abilities.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 3","pages":"459-478"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-02-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12586","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139736427","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A longitudinal study examining the associations between prenatal and postnatal maternal distress and toddler socioemotional developmental during the COVID-19 pandemic 一项纵向研究,探讨了 COVID-19 大流行期间产前和产后产妇痛苦与幼儿社会情感发展之间的关联。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-02-08 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12584
Jennifer E. Khoury, Leslie Atkinson, Andrea Gonzalez

Elevated psychological distress, experienced by pregnant women and parents, has been well-documented during the COVID-19 pandemic. Most research focuses on the first 6-months postpartum, with single or limited repeated measures of perinatal distress. The present longitudinal study examined how perinatal distress, experienced over nearly 2 years of the COVID-19 pandemic, impacted toddler socioemotional development. A sample of 304 participants participated during pregnancy, 6-weeks, 6-months, and 15-months postpartum. Mothers reported their depressive, anxiety, and stress symptoms, at each timepoint. Mother-reported toddler socioemotional functioning (using the Brief Infant–Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment) was measured at 15-months. Results of structural equation mediation models indicated that (1) higher prenatal distress was associated with elevated postpartum distress, from 6-weeks to 15-months postpartum; (2) associations between prenatal distress and toddler socioemotional problems became nonsignificant after accounting for postpartum distress; and (3) higher prenatal distress was indirectly associated with greater socioemotional problems, and specifically elevated externalizing problems, through higher maternal distress at 6 weeks and 15 months postpartum. Findings suggest that the continued experience of distress during the postpartum period plays an important role in child socioemotional development during the COVID-19 pandemic.

在 COVID-19 大流行期间,孕妇和父母所经历的高心理压力已得到充分证实。大多数研究都集中在产后的前 6 个月,对围产期心理压力进行单一或有限的重复测量。本纵向研究探讨了在 COVID-19 大流行的近两年时间里,围产期困扰如何影响幼儿的社会情感发展。304 名参与者分别在孕期、6 周、6 个月和产后 15 个月参加了这项研究。母亲们报告了她们在每个时间点的抑郁、焦虑和压力症状。母亲报告的幼儿社会情感功能(使用简易婴幼儿社会情感评估)在 15 个月时进行了测量。结构方程调解模型的结果表明:(1) 从产后 6 周到 15 个月,较高的产前困扰与较高的产后困扰相关;(2) 在考虑到产后困扰后,产前困扰与幼儿社会情感问题之间的关联变得不显著;(3) 较高的产前困扰通过产后 6 周和 15 个月较高的产妇困扰间接与较高的社会情感问题,特别是较高的外化问题相关。研究结果表明,在 COVID-19 大流行期间,产后持续经历痛苦对儿童的社会情感发展起着重要作用。
{"title":"A longitudinal study examining the associations between prenatal and postnatal maternal distress and toddler socioemotional developmental during the COVID-19 pandemic","authors":"Jennifer E. Khoury,&nbsp;Leslie Atkinson,&nbsp;Andrea Gonzalez","doi":"10.1111/infa.12584","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12584","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Elevated psychological distress, experienced by pregnant women and parents, has been well-documented during the COVID-19 pandemic. Most research focuses on the first 6-months postpartum, with single or limited repeated measures of perinatal distress. The present longitudinal study examined how perinatal distress, experienced over nearly 2 years of the COVID-19 pandemic, impacted toddler socioemotional development. A sample of 304 participants participated during pregnancy, 6-weeks, 6-months, and 15-months postpartum. Mothers reported their depressive, anxiety, and stress symptoms, at each timepoint. Mother-reported toddler socioemotional functioning (using the Brief Infant–Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment) was measured at 15-months. Results of structural equation mediation models indicated that (1) higher prenatal distress was associated with elevated postpartum distress, from 6-weeks to 15-months postpartum; (2) associations between prenatal distress and toddler socioemotional problems became nonsignificant after accounting for postpartum distress; and (3) higher prenatal distress was indirectly associated with greater socioemotional problems, and specifically elevated externalizing problems, through higher maternal distress at 6 weeks and 15 months postpartum. Findings suggest that the continued experience of distress during the postpartum period plays an important role in child socioemotional development during the COVID-19 pandemic.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 3","pages":"412-436"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-02-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12584","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139708123","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
COVID-19 pandemic effects: Examining prenatal internalizing symptoms and infant temperament COVID-19 大流行的影响:研究产前内化症状和婴儿气质。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-20 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12583
Jennifer A. Mattera, Nora L. Erickson, Celestina Barbosa-Leiker, Maria A. Gartstein

For pregnant women, the COVID-19 pandemic has resulted in unprecedented stressors, including uncertainty regarding prenatal care and the long-term consequences of perinatal infection. However, few studies have examined the role of this adverse event on maternal wellbeing and infant socioemotional development following the initial wave of the pandemic when less stringent public health restrictions were in place. The current study addressed these gaps in the literature by first comparing prenatal internalizing symptoms and infant temperament collected after the first wave of the pandemic to equivalent measures in a pre-pandemic sample. Second, associations between prenatal pandemic-related stress and infant temperament were examined. Women who were pregnant during the COVID-19 pandemic endorsed higher pregnancy-specific anxiety relative to the pre-pandemic sample. They also reported greater infant negative emotionality and lower positive affectivity and regulatory capacity at 2 months postpartum. Prenatal infection stress directly predicted infant negative affect. Both prenatal infection and preparedness stress were indirectly related to infant negative emotionality through depression symptoms during pregnancy and at 2 months postpartum. These results have implications for prenatal mental health screening procedures during the pandemic and the development of early intervention programs for infants born to mothers during this adverse event.

COVID-19 大流行给孕妇带来了前所未有的压力,包括产前护理的不确定性和围产期感染的长期后果。然而,很少有研究探讨了这一不利事件在大流行初期公共卫生限制较为宽松的情况下对孕产妇健康和婴儿社会情感发展的影响。针对这些文献空白,本研究首先将第一波大流行后收集的产前内化症状和婴儿性情与大流行前样本中的同等指标进行了比较。其次,研究了产前大流行相关压力与婴儿气质之间的关联。与大流行前的样本相比,在 COVID-19 大流行期间怀孕的妇女表现出更高的妊娠焦虑。她们还报告说,产后两个月的婴儿负面情绪更多,正面情绪和调节能力更低。产前感染压力可直接预测婴儿的负面情绪。产前感染和备孕压力通过孕期和产后 2 个月时的抑郁症状与婴儿的负面情绪间接相关。这些结果对大流行病期间的产前心理健康筛查程序以及在这一不利事件中为母亲所生婴儿制定早期干预计划具有重要意义。
{"title":"COVID-19 pandemic effects: Examining prenatal internalizing symptoms and infant temperament","authors":"Jennifer A. Mattera,&nbsp;Nora L. Erickson,&nbsp;Celestina Barbosa-Leiker,&nbsp;Maria A. Gartstein","doi":"10.1111/infa.12583","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12583","url":null,"abstract":"<p>For pregnant women, the COVID-19 pandemic has resulted in unprecedented stressors, including uncertainty regarding prenatal care and the long-term consequences of perinatal infection. However, few studies have examined the role of this adverse event on maternal wellbeing and infant socioemotional development following the initial wave of the pandemic when less stringent public health restrictions were in place. The current study addressed these gaps in the literature by first comparing prenatal internalizing symptoms and infant temperament collected after the first wave of the pandemic to equivalent measures in a pre-pandemic sample. Second, associations between prenatal pandemic-related stress and infant temperament were examined. Women who were pregnant during the COVID-19 pandemic endorsed higher pregnancy-specific anxiety relative to the pre-pandemic sample. They also reported greater infant negative emotionality and lower positive affectivity and regulatory capacity at 2 months postpartum. Prenatal infection stress directly predicted infant negative affect. Both prenatal infection and preparedness stress were indirectly related to infant negative emotionality through depression symptoms during pregnancy and at 2 months postpartum. These results have implications for prenatal mental health screening procedures during the pandemic and the development of early intervention programs for infants born to mothers during this adverse event.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 3","pages":"386-411"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-01-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12583","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139513965","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Infancy
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1