首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied最新文献

英文 中文
Judgments of sex trafficked women: The role of emotions. 对被贩卖妇女的判断:情绪的作用。
IF 2.7 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-25 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000511
Richard L Wiener, Taylor E Petty, Megan Berry-Cohen, Julie Wertheimer-Meier

This study examined how incidental emotions influence decisions to arrest or release sex trafficking survivors. Community members (N = 984) completed an autobiographical memory task invoking disgust, sympathy, or no emotion and read case facts from United States v. Bell (2014) varying whether the survivor had a prior history of sex work and whether she came from a vulnerable or nonvulnerable background. Participants in the vulnerable condition believed that the survivor was less able to resist the trafficker's proposal. Furthermore, women but not men made to feel disgust believed that she should have resisted. Regarding arresting the survivor for prostitution versus releasing her for services, invoking either incidental disgust or sympathy, but especially disgust, triggered feelings of disgust, which in turn predicted an arrest decision. Finally, our data supported a moderated mediation model in which the belief that the survivor should have been able to resist the trafficker predicted a greater probability of an arrest judgment. Furthermore, participants in the vulnerable condition believed that the survivor had less ability to resist, and they disfavored her arrest. However, this was only true when we invoked no emotion. When we invoked disgust, vulnerability ceased to have this moderation effect. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究探讨了偶然情绪如何影响逮捕或释放性交易幸存者的决定。社区成员(N = 984)完成了一项自传体记忆任务,该任务会引发厌恶、同情或无情感,并阅读美国诉贝尔案(2014 年)中的案例事实,这些事实会根据幸存者之前是否有性工作史以及她的背景是脆弱还是非脆弱而有所不同。弱势条件下的参与者认为,幸存者抵制人贩子提议的能力较弱。此外,女性(而非男性)认为她本应反抗。关于以卖淫罪逮捕幸存者还是释放她以提供服务的问题,附带的厌恶或同情(尤其是厌恶)会引发厌恶感,进而预测逮捕决定。最后,我们的数据支持一个调节中介模型,在该模型中,认为幸存者本应能够抵制人贩子的信念预示着更大的逮捕判决概率。此外,弱势条件下的参与者认为幸存者的反抗能力较弱,因此不赞成逮捕她。然而,只有当我们不激发任何情绪时,情况才会如此。当我们调用厌恶情绪时,脆弱情绪不再具有这种调节作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Judgments of sex trafficked women: The role of emotions.","authors":"Richard L Wiener, Taylor E Petty, Megan Berry-Cohen, Julie Wertheimer-Meier","doi":"10.1037/xap0000511","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000511","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study examined how incidental emotions influence decisions to arrest or release sex trafficking survivors. Community members (<i>N</i> = 984) completed an autobiographical memory task invoking disgust, sympathy, or no emotion and read case facts from <i>United States v. Bell</i> (2014) varying whether the survivor had a prior history of sex work and whether she came from a vulnerable or nonvulnerable background. Participants in the vulnerable condition believed that the survivor was less able to resist the trafficker's proposal. Furthermore, women but not men made to feel disgust believed that she should have resisted. Regarding arresting the survivor for prostitution versus releasing her for services, invoking either incidental disgust or sympathy, but especially disgust, triggered feelings of disgust, which in turn predicted an arrest decision. Finally, our data supported a moderated mediation model in which the belief that the survivor should have been able to resist the trafficker predicted a greater probability of an arrest judgment. Furthermore, participants in the vulnerable condition believed that the survivor had less ability to resist, and they disfavored her arrest. However, this was only true when we invoked no emotion. When we invoked disgust, vulnerability ceased to have this moderation effect. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"465-481"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140856354","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Moral paragons, but crummy friends: The case of snitching. 道德楷模,但糟糕的朋友:告密的案例。
IF 2.7 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-30 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000501
Zachariah Berry, Ike Silver, Alex Shaw

Loyalty to friends is an important moral value, but does that mean snitching on friends is considered immoral? Across six preregistered studies, we examine how loyalty obligations impact people's moral evaluations of snitching (i.e., turning in others who commit transgressions). In vignette and incentivized partner choice paradigms, we find that witnesses who snitch (vs. do not snitch) are seen as more moral and as better leaders (Studies 1-6), regardless of whether they snitch on a friend or an acquaintance (Studies 1-3). We find that a willingness to turn in one's friends increases perceived morality, while an unwillingness to do so diminishes it, with the latter effect exhibiting a stronger impact than the former (Study 2). Our experiments also demonstrate that snitches receive less moral credit when snitching on nonmoral (vs. moral) transgressions (Study 3) and when snitching aligns with self-interest (Study 4). We demonstrate that although snitching is often seen as morally right, turning in transgressors entails important reputational trade-offs: Snitching makes one appear disloyal and a bad friend but boosts perceptions of morality and leadership. This reveals a context in which what is loyal is no longer considered moral. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

对朋友忠诚是一种重要的道德价值观,但这是否意味着告发朋友被认为是不道德的?在六项预先注册的研究中,我们考察了忠诚义务如何影响人们对告密(即告发他人)的道德评价。在小插曲和激励性伴侣选择范式中,我们发现告密(与不告密)的证人被视为更有道德和更好的领导者(研究1-6),无论他们告密的是朋友还是熟人(研究1-3)。我们发现,愿意交朋友会增加道德感,而不愿意交朋友则会削弱道德感,而后者的影响比前者更强(研究2)。我们的实验还表明,当告密者对非口头(与道德)违法行为进行告密时(研究3),以及当告密者与自身利益一致时(研究4),告密者获得的道德信用较少。我们证明,尽管告密通常被视为道德上正确的,但告发违规者需要进行重要的声誉权衡:告密会让人看起来不忠和坏朋友,但会增强人们对道德和领导力的看法。这揭示了一种背景,在这种背景下,忠诚不再被视为道德。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Moral paragons, but crummy friends: The case of snitching.","authors":"Zachariah Berry, Ike Silver, Alex Shaw","doi":"10.1037/xap0000501","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000501","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Loyalty to friends is an important moral value, but does that mean snitching on friends is considered immoral? Across six preregistered studies, we examine how loyalty obligations impact people's moral evaluations of snitching (i.e., turning in others who commit transgressions). In vignette and incentivized partner choice paradigms, we find that witnesses who snitch (vs. do not snitch) are seen as more moral and as better leaders (Studies 1-6), regardless of whether they snitch on a friend or an acquaintance (Studies 1-3). We find that a willingness to turn in one's friends increases perceived morality, while an unwillingness to do so diminishes it, with the latter effect exhibiting a stronger impact than the former (Study 2). Our experiments also demonstrate that snitches receive less moral credit when snitching on nonmoral (vs. moral) transgressions (Study 3) and when snitching aligns with self-interest (Study 4). We demonstrate that although snitching is often seen as morally right, turning in transgressors entails important reputational trade-offs: Snitching makes one appear disloyal and a bad friend but boosts perceptions of morality and leadership. This reveals a context in which what is loyal is no longer considered moral. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"442-464"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71414743","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Scientists, speak up! Source impacts trust in health advice across five countries. 科学家们,大声说出来!来源影响了五个国家对健康建议的信任。
IF 2.7 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-30 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000500
Natalia Zarzeczna, Paul H P Hanel, Bastiaan T Rutjens, Suzanna A Bono, Yi-Hua Chen, Geoffrey Haddock

We examined how different types of communication influence people's responses to health advice. We tested whether presenting COVID-19 prevention advice (e.g., washing hands/distancing) as either originating from a government or scientific source would affect people's trust in and intentions to comply with the advice. We also manipulated uncertainty in communicating the advice effectiveness. To achieve this, we conducted an experiment using large samples of participants (N = 4,561) from the United Kingdom, the United States, Canada, Malaysia, and Taiwan. Across countries, participants found messages more trustworthy when the purported source was science rather than the government. This effect was moderated by political orientation in all countries except for Canada, while religiosity moderated the source effect in the United States. Although source did not directly affect intentions to act upon the advice, we found an indirect effect via trust, such that a more trusted source (i.e., science) was predictive of higher intentions to comply. However, the uncertainty manipulation was not effective. Together, our findings suggest that despite prominence of science skepticism in public discourse, people trust scientists more than governments when it comes to practical health advice. It is therefore beneficial to communicate health messages by stressing their scientific bases. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们研究了不同类型的沟通如何影响人们对健康建议的反应。我们测试了将新冠肺炎预防建议(如洗手/保持距离)作为政府或科学来源提出是否会影响人们对建议的信任和遵守建议的意愿。我们还操纵了沟通建议有效性的不确定性。为了实现这一点,我们使用了来自英国、美国、加拿大、马来西亚和台湾的大量参与者样本(N=4561)进行了一项实验。在各国,参与者发现,当所谓的来源是科学而不是政府时,信息更值得信赖。除加拿大外,所有国家的政治取向都调节了这种效应,而美国的宗教信仰调节了源效应。尽管来源并没有直接影响根据建议采取行动的意图,但我们发现了通过信任产生的间接影响,因此更可信的来源(即科学)可以预测更高的遵守意愿。然而,对不确定性的操纵并不有效。总之,我们的研究结果表明,尽管公众话语中对科学持怀疑态度,但在实际的健康建议方面,人们比政府更信任科学家。因此,通过强调健康信息的科学基础来传达健康信息是有益的。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Scientists, speak up! Source impacts trust in health advice across five countries.","authors":"Natalia Zarzeczna, Paul H P Hanel, Bastiaan T Rutjens, Suzanna A Bono, Yi-Hua Chen, Geoffrey Haddock","doi":"10.1037/xap0000500","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000500","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We examined how different types of communication influence people's responses to health advice. We tested whether presenting COVID-19 prevention advice (e.g., washing hands/distancing) as either originating from a government or scientific source would affect people's trust in and intentions to comply with the advice. We also manipulated uncertainty in communicating the advice effectiveness. To achieve this, we conducted an experiment using large samples of participants (<i>N</i> = 4,561) from the United Kingdom, the United States, Canada, Malaysia, and Taiwan. Across countries, participants found messages more trustworthy when the purported source was science rather than the government. This effect was moderated by political orientation in all countries except for Canada, while religiosity moderated the source effect in the United States. Although source did not directly affect intentions to act upon the advice, we found an indirect effect via trust, such that a more trusted source (i.e., science) was predictive of higher intentions to comply. However, the uncertainty manipulation was not effective. Together, our findings suggest that despite prominence of science skepticism in public discourse, people trust scientists more than governments when it comes to practical health advice. It is therefore beneficial to communicate health messages by stressing their scientific bases. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"430-441"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71414744","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Resolving problems with the skill retention literature: An empirical demonstration and recommendations for researchers. 解决技能保留文献中的问题:实证论证和对研究人员的建议。
IF 2.7 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2023-12-18 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000503
Phillip L Ackerman, Corey E Tatel

Questions about the degree of retention and decay for procedural skills, once acquired but not used for a period of time, have been raised repeatedly in basic and applied research. Despite widespread interest and numerous empirical investigations, definitive answers to the question "How much skill is retained after a period of disuse?" remain elusive. Shortcomings with the literature were identified that limit the ability of researchers to develop models of skill decay for various tasks, including medical/health care, military, sports, and other applications. Problems with design, measurement, analysis, and interpretation aspects of research are reviewed. An empirical study of acquisition and retention after a 1-month delay for four tasks is presented: (1) A mid fidelity air traffic control simulation, (2) a low-fidelity air traffic control task, and (3, 4) two versions of a perceptual/memory search task, with data from 150 participants. The results illustrate how different approaches to measurement and analysis lead to biased interpretations of decay, especially in the context of relearning. Recommendations are provided for research that can clarify decay functions for procedural tasks and may generate improved understanding and actionable models for refresher training programs to optimize skill retention over extended time periods. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

关于程序性技能的保留和衰退程度的问题,已经在基础研究和应用研究中反复提出。尽管人们对这一问题广泛关注,并进行了大量的实证调查,但 "技能在使用一段时间后会保留多少?"这一问题仍然没有明确的答案。文献中的不足之处限制了研究人员为各种任务(包括医疗/保健、军事、体育和其他应用)开发技能衰减模型的能力。研究中存在的设计、测量、分析和解释方面的问题进行了回顾。报告介绍了对四项任务延迟 1 个月后的习得和保持情况进行的实证研究:(1) 中等逼真度空中交通管制模拟;(2) 低逼真度空中交通管制任务;(3, 4) 两个版本的感知/记忆搜索任务,数据来自 150 名参与者。结果表明,不同的测量和分析方法会导致对衰减的解释出现偏差,特别是在再学习的情况下。研究建议可以澄清程序性任务的衰减功能,并为进修培训计划提供更好的理解和可操作的模型,以优化技能在较长时间内的保持。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Resolving problems with the skill retention literature: An empirical demonstration and recommendations for researchers.","authors":"Phillip L Ackerman, Corey E Tatel","doi":"10.1037/xap0000503","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000503","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Questions about the degree of retention and decay for procedural skills, once acquired but not used for a period of time, have been raised repeatedly in basic and applied research. Despite widespread interest and numerous empirical investigations, definitive answers to the question \"How much skill is retained after a period of disuse?\" remain elusive. Shortcomings with the literature were identified that limit the ability of researchers to develop models of skill decay for various tasks, including medical/health care, military, sports, and other applications. Problems with design, measurement, analysis, and interpretation aspects of research are reviewed. An empirical study of acquisition and retention after a 1-month delay for four tasks is presented: (1) A mid fidelity air traffic control simulation, (2) a low-fidelity air traffic control task, and (3, 4) two versions of a perceptual/memory search task, with data from 150 participants. The results illustrate how different approaches to measurement and analysis lead to biased interpretations of decay, especially in the context of relearning. Recommendations are provided for research that can clarify decay functions for procedural tasks and may generate improved understanding and actionable models for refresher training programs to optimize skill retention over extended time periods. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"411-429"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138812239","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The relative effectiveness of conditioning one or two attributes to a brand. 对一个品牌的一个或两个属性进行调节的相对有效性。
IF 2.7 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-22 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000513
Lisa Eckmann, Jan R Landwehr, Fabia Högden, Christian Unkelbach

Attribute conditioning refers to the phenomenon that target stimuli acquire specific attributes by pairing them with stimuli possessing these attributes. We apply attribute conditioning to a marketing context where brands are often displayed with stimuli possessing semantic attributes to establish brand-attribute associations. In particular, we examine whether it is more effective from a brand image perspective to associate a brand with only one attribute, two related attributes, or two unrelated attributes. Across four experimental studies, we find that pairing a single attribute (e.g., athletic) with a brand is most effective for building brand-attribute associations and that pairing multiple, related attributes (athletic and healthy) is more effective than pairing multiple, unrelated attributes (athletic and smart). Supplementing this finding, an analysis of observational data from real brands suggests that attributing two orthogonal attributes to a brand is associated with negative effects on marketing-relevant outcomes. Our findings extend previous research on multiattribute conditioning and highlight the importance of the number and relationship between attributes for building effective brand associations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

属性条件反射是指目标刺激物通过与具有特定属性的刺激物配对而获得这些属性的现象。我们将属性条件反射应用于市场营销中,在市场营销中,品牌通常与具有语义属性的刺激物一起展示,以建立品牌-属性关联。特别是,我们研究了从品牌形象的角度来看,将品牌与一个属性、两个相关属性或两个不相关属性联系起来是否更有效。通过四项实验研究,我们发现将单一属性(如运动)与品牌配对对于建立品牌属性关联最为有效,而将多个相关属性(运动和健康)配对比将多个不相关属性(运动和聪明)配对更为有效。对真实品牌观察数据的分析表明,为一个品牌赋予两个正交属性会对营销相关结果产生负面影响,这是对上述发现的补充。我们的研究结果扩展了以往关于多属性条件的研究,并强调了属性数量和属性之间的关系对于建立有效品牌联想的重要性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The relative effectiveness of conditioning one or two attributes to a brand.","authors":"Lisa Eckmann, Jan R Landwehr, Fabia Högden, Christian Unkelbach","doi":"10.1037/xap0000513","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000513","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Attribute conditioning refers to the phenomenon that target stimuli acquire specific attributes by pairing them with stimuli possessing these attributes. We apply attribute conditioning to a marketing context where brands are often displayed with stimuli possessing semantic attributes to establish brand-attribute associations. In particular, we examine whether it is more effective from a brand image perspective to associate a brand with only one attribute, two related attributes, or two unrelated attributes. Across four experimental studies, we find that pairing a single attribute (e.g., athletic) with a brand is most effective for building brand-attribute associations and that pairing multiple, related attributes (athletic and healthy) is more effective than pairing multiple, unrelated attributes (athletic and smart). Supplementing this finding, an analysis of observational data from real brands suggests that attributing two orthogonal attributes to a brand is associated with negative effects on marketing-relevant outcomes. Our findings extend previous research on multiattribute conditioning and highlight the importance of the number and relationship between attributes for building effective brand associations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"496-509"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139933632","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Acceleration neglect in trend extrapolations. 趋势推断中的加速忽略。
IF 2.7 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-11 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000510
Massimiliano Ostinelli, David Luna

Many decisions rely on intuitive predictions based on time series data showing a trend. For instance, the current upward trend in global temperatures might lead to specific predictions about the extent to which global temperatures will rise in the future, and these predictions might be used to inform judgments about the urgency with which climate change must be addressed. However, those predictions often need to be revised to incorporate the effects of unexpected events that might accelerate a trend (i.e., increase its rate of change), such as an unanticipated increase in CO₂ emissions, or decelerate a trend (i.e., decrease its rate of change), such as an unanticipated reduction in CO₂ emissions. In this work, we uncover a new cognitive bias by which people neglect how much a trend can accelerate (vs. decelerate) due to unexpected events. We explain this bias in terms of momentum theory and a naive understanding of physics. These findings have important implications for businesses and policymakers seeking to communicate information about topics such as climate change, stock market prices, or disease prevention. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

许多决策都依赖于基于显示趋势的时间序列数据的直观预测。例如,当前全球气温的上升趋势可能会导致对未来全球气温上升程度的具体预测,这些预测可能会被用于判断解决气候变化问题的紧迫性。然而,这些预测往往需要进行修正,以纳入意外事件的影响,这些意外事件可能会加速趋势(即增加其变化率),例如二氧化碳排放量的意外增加,或减缓趋势(即降低其变化率),例如二氧化碳排放量的意外减少。在这项研究中,我们发现了一种新的认知偏差,即人们忽视了趋势因突发事件而加速(或减速)的程度。我们用动量理论和对物理学的天真理解来解释这种偏差。这些发现对于企业和政策制定者传播气候变化、股票市场价格或疾病预防等主题的信息具有重要意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Acceleration neglect in trend extrapolations.","authors":"Massimiliano Ostinelli, David Luna","doi":"10.1037/xap0000510","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000510","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many decisions rely on intuitive predictions based on time series data showing a trend. For instance, the current upward trend in global temperatures might lead to specific predictions about the extent to which global temperatures will rise in the future, and these predictions might be used to inform judgments about the urgency with which climate change must be addressed. However, those predictions often need to be revised to incorporate the effects of unexpected events that might accelerate a trend (i.e., increase its rate of change), such as an unanticipated increase in CO₂ emissions, or decelerate a trend (i.e., decrease its rate of change), such as an unanticipated reduction in CO₂ emissions. In this work, we uncover a new cognitive bias by which people neglect how much a trend can accelerate (vs. decelerate) due to unexpected events. We explain this bias in terms of momentum theory and a naive understanding of physics. These findings have important implications for businesses and policymakers seeking to communicate information about topics such as climate change, stock market prices, or disease prevention. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"482-495"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140870477","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
From surviving to thriving: How preferences shift in helping resource allocation. 从生存到繁荣:偏好如何在帮助资源分配中发生转变。
IF 2.7 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-08-08 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000516
Tianhong Wang, Shaojing Shen, Zipeng Cheng, Xiaofei Xie

The helper's allocation of helping resources to multiple recipients often involves a trade-off between equality and efficiency. This research examines how the condition of potential recipients ("survival" or "development") influences the preferences for helping resources allocation in terms of equality and efficiency. Through seven studies, including a field study (Study 6), we discovered that helpers show a higher preference for equality over efficiency when recipients are in a survival situation (i.e., below the survival line) as opposed to in development situation (i.e., above the survival line). This phenomenon is attributed to the different priorities of deontological and utilitarian perspectives in survival and development situations (Studies 3 and 4). Our findings offer insights into the existing research on helping decisions and enhance the understanding of the trade-off between efficiency and equality among helpers. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

帮扶者在向多个受助者分配帮扶资源时,往往需要在平等与效率之间做出权衡。本研究探讨了潜在受助者的状况("生存 "或 "发展")如何影响求助者在平等和效率方面对帮助资源分配的偏好。通过七项研究,包括一项实地研究(研究 6),我们发现,当受助者处于生存状况(即生存线以下)与发展状况(即生存线以上)时,助人者更倾向于平等而非效率。这一现象归因于在生存和发展情况下,义务观和功利观的优先次序不同(研究 3 和 4)。我们的研究结果为现有的助人决策研究提供了启示,并加深了人们对助人者在效率与平等之间权衡的理解。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"From surviving to thriving: How preferences shift in helping resource allocation.","authors":"Tianhong Wang, Shaojing Shen, Zipeng Cheng, Xiaofei Xie","doi":"10.1037/xap0000516","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000516","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The helper's allocation of helping resources to multiple recipients often involves a trade-off between equality and efficiency. This research examines how the condition of potential recipients (\"survival\" or \"development\") influences the preferences for helping resources allocation in terms of equality and efficiency. Through seven studies, including a field study (Study 6), we discovered that helpers show a higher preference for equality over efficiency when recipients are in a survival situation (i.e., below the survival line) as opposed to in development situation (i.e., above the survival line). This phenomenon is attributed to the different priorities of deontological and utilitarian perspectives in survival and development situations (Studies 3 and 4). Our findings offer insights into the existing research on helping decisions and enhance the understanding of the trade-off between efficiency and equality among helpers. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141903220","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mapping the traits desired in followers and leaders onto fundamental dimensions of social evaluation. 将追随者和领导者所需的特质映射到社会评价的基本维度上。
IF 2.7 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-07-25 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000514
Alex J Benson, Hayden J R Woodley, Lynden Jensen, James Hardy

We applied the social evaluation framework to investigate the traits desired in an "ideal" follower, which were compared to the traits desired in an "ideal" leader. Across three studies and five samples, both differences and similarities in role-specific preferences mapped onto the Vertical-Horizontal dimensions of the social evaluation framework in ways that aligned with the demands of each role. Traits higher on the Horizontal-Morality facet (e.g., cooperative, dutiful) and lower on the Vertical-Assertiveness facet (e.g., confident, ambitious) differentiated ideal follower preferences from ideal leader preferences. Focusing on the traits most strongly desired in relation to each role, traits that supported social coordination and collective goal attainment (i.e., work ethic, cooperativeness) were prioritized in relation to ideal followers, whereas intelligence was prioritized for ideal leaders. Trustworthiness was equally valued across both roles. Moreover, we differentiated between necessary and luxury traits by adjusting the budget individuals could allocate toward the desired traits. Investments in necessary versus luxury traits further supported the social evaluation framework and highlighted the need to account for the facet-level distinctions within the Vertical (assertiveness, ability) and Horizontal (morality, friendliness) dimensions. Further, these findings were found to be robust across manipulations (e.g., the target's gender and hierarchical level). (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们运用社会评价框架调查了 "理想 "追随者所需的特质,并将其与 "理想 "领导者所需的特质进行了比较。在三项研究和五个样本中,特定角色偏好的差异和相似性都映射到了社会评价框架的纵向-横向维度上,与每个角色的要求相一致。在 "水平-道德 "维度上较高的特质(如合作、尽职)和在 "垂直-傲慢 "维度上较低的特质(如自信、雄心勃勃)将理想追随者的偏好与理想领导者的偏好区分开来。从每个角色最需要的特质来看,支持社会协调和集体目标实现的特质(如职业道德、合作性)在理想追随者中被优先考虑,而智力在理想领导者中被优先考虑。在这两种角色中,信任度同样受到重视。此外,我们还通过调整个人分配给理想特质的预算来区分必要特质和奢侈特质。对必要特质和奢侈特质的投资进一步支持了社会评价框架,并强调了在纵向(自信、能力)和横向(道德、友善)维度中考虑面级区别的必要性。此外,这些研究结果在不同的操作(如目标的性别和等级)下都是稳健的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Mapping the traits desired in followers and leaders onto fundamental dimensions of social evaluation.","authors":"Alex J Benson, Hayden J R Woodley, Lynden Jensen, James Hardy","doi":"10.1037/xap0000514","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000514","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We applied the social evaluation framework to investigate the traits desired in an \"ideal\" follower, which were compared to the traits desired in an \"ideal\" leader. Across three studies and five samples, both differences and similarities in role-specific preferences mapped onto the Vertical-Horizontal dimensions of the social evaluation framework in ways that aligned with the demands of each role. Traits higher on the Horizontal-Morality facet (e.g., cooperative, dutiful) and lower on the Vertical-Assertiveness facet (e.g., confident, ambitious) differentiated ideal follower preferences from ideal leader preferences. Focusing on the traits most strongly desired in relation to each role, traits that supported social coordination and collective goal attainment (i.e., work ethic, cooperativeness) were prioritized in relation to ideal followers, whereas intelligence was prioritized for ideal leaders. Trustworthiness was equally valued across both roles. Moreover, we differentiated between necessary and luxury traits by adjusting the budget individuals could allocate toward the desired traits. Investments in necessary versus luxury traits further supported the social evaluation framework and highlighted the need to account for the facet-level distinctions within the Vertical (assertiveness, ability) and Horizontal (morality, friendliness) dimensions. Further, these findings were found to be robust across manipulations (e.g., the target's gender and hierarchical level). (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141761699","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Metacognition guides intention offloading and fulfillment of real-world plans. 元认知指导意向卸载和现实世界计划的实现。
IF 2.7 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000515
Ava E Scott, Sam J Gilbert

Reminders such as calendar entries and digital alerts are highly effective at helping people remember what they need to do. Recent work has explored the triggers of reminder setting, also known as intention offloading, finding that low confidence is associated with more offloading in laboratory tasks. This supports a metacognitive model of cognitive offloading. Here, we investigated whether this model generalizes to real-world intentions. We asked 112 participants about their upcoming plans and subsequent fulfillment of those plans in two online surveys conducted in 2022. Participants were more likely to set reminders for plans that (a) they were less confident they would remember and (b) they judged to be more important. Participants with lower confidence that they would remember to return for the second survey were more likely to set a reminder for this plan. This, in turn, predicted greater fulfillment. Therefore, ironically, lower confidence predicted greater success. These findings show that intention offloading predicts fulfillment of real-world intentions. They also point toward metacognition as a target for interventions to facilitate this. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

日历条目和数字提醒等提醒方式能非常有效地帮助人们记住需要做的事情。最近的研究探索了设置提醒的触发因素,也就是所谓的 "意图卸载",发现在实验室任务中,低自信与更多的卸载相关。这支持了认知卸载的元认知模型。在此,我们研究了这一模型是否适用于现实世界中的意图。在 2022 年进行的两次在线调查中,我们向 112 名参与者询问了他们即将实施的计划以及随后这些计划的实现情况。参与者更有可能为以下计划设置提醒:(a) 他们不太相信自己会记住的计划;(b) 他们认为更重要的计划。如果参与者对自己是否会记得参加第二次调查的信心较低,则更有可能为该计划设置提醒。这反过来又预示着更大的满足感。因此,具有讽刺意味的是,较低的信心预示着更大的成功。这些发现表明,意向卸载预示着现实世界中意向的实现。他们还指出,元认知是促进实现这一目标的干预措施的目标。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Metacognition guides intention offloading and fulfillment of real-world plans.","authors":"Ava E Scott, Sam J Gilbert","doi":"10.1037/xap0000515","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000515","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Reminders such as calendar entries and digital alerts are highly effective at helping people remember what they need to do. Recent work has explored the triggers of reminder setting, also known as intention offloading, finding that low confidence is associated with more offloading in laboratory tasks. This supports a metacognitive model of cognitive offloading. Here, we investigated whether this model generalizes to real-world intentions. We asked 112 participants about their upcoming plans and subsequent fulfillment of those plans in two online surveys conducted in 2022. Participants were more likely to set reminders for plans that (a) they were less confident they would remember and (b) they judged to be more important. Participants with lower confidence that they would remember to return for the second survey were more likely to set a reminder for this plan. This, in turn, predicted greater fulfillment. Therefore, ironically, lower confidence predicted greater success. These findings show that intention offloading predicts fulfillment of real-world intentions. They also point toward metacognition as a target for interventions to facilitate this. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141634975","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effects of generating examples on comprehension and metacomprehension. 生成示例对理解和元理解的影响
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-04 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000490
Tricia A Guerrero, Thomas D Griffin, Jennifer Wiley

Teachers and students often report using examples to support learning. Research has shown benefits of viewing provided examples and generating examples during declarative concept learning; however, there is less work showing clear benefits when learners generate their own examples on comprehension measures while students are attempting to learn from expository science texts. The present study tested whether generating examples would be useful for improving comprehension and comprehension monitoring in the context of an undergraduate science course. In a pre-post design, students completed an initial reading activity, followed by taking practice tests on each topic. Some students were assigned to complete an additional example generation activity after taking the practice tests. Some students also evaluated the quality of generated examples and received explanatory feedback. While there was an overall improvement in comprehension for all students, those who generated examples without the opportunity to evaluate the quality had the smallest overall learning gains. Students who evaluated the quality of examples showed the greatest learning gains on application-based test questions. And, although overall overconfidence decreased, there were no differences between conditions. These results suggest that example generation may not always be as helpful for improving learning as many students and teachers assume. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

教师和学生经常报告使用实例来支持学习。研究表明,在陈述性概念学习过程中,查看提供的示例和生成示例都有好处;然而,当学生试图从说明性科学课文中学习时,很少有研究表明学习者生成自己的示例对理解测量有明显的好处。本研究测试了在本科科学课程中,生成示例是否有助于提高理解能力和理解监控能力。在前-后设计中,学生完成了初步的阅读活动,然后参加了每个题目的练习测试。一些学生在参加完练习测试后,还被指定完成额外的示例生成活动。一些学生还对生成的示例进行了质量评估,并获得了解释性反馈。虽然所有学生的理解能力都有了整体提高,但那些没有机会对质量进行评估的生成示例的学生的整体学习收益最小。对示例质量进行评估的学生在应用型测试题上的学习收获最大。虽然总体过度自信有所减少,但不同情况下没有差异。这些结果表明,范例的生成并不总是像许多学生和教师认为的那样有助于提高学习效果。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The effects of generating examples on comprehension and metacomprehension.","authors":"Tricia A Guerrero, Thomas D Griffin, Jennifer Wiley","doi":"10.1037/xap0000490","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000490","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Teachers and students often report using examples to support learning. Research has shown benefits of viewing provided examples and generating examples during declarative concept learning; however, there is less work showing clear benefits when learners generate their own examples on comprehension measures while students are attempting to learn from expository science texts. The present study tested whether generating examples would be useful for improving comprehension and comprehension monitoring in the context of an undergraduate science course. In a pre-post design, students completed an initial reading activity, followed by taking practice tests on each topic. Some students were assigned to complete an additional example generation activity after taking the practice tests. Some students also evaluated the quality of generated examples and received explanatory feedback. While there was an overall improvement in comprehension for all students, those who generated examples without the opportunity to evaluate the quality had the smallest overall learning gains. Students who evaluated the quality of examples showed the greatest learning gains on application-based test questions. And, although overall overconfidence decreased, there were no differences between conditions. These results suggest that example generation may not always be as helpful for improving learning as many students and teachers assume. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"318-330"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10154014","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1