首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied最新文献

英文 中文
Finding your roots: Do DNA ancestry tests increase racial (in)tolerance? 寻根:DNA祖先测试会增加种族(不)宽容吗?
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-12-14 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000488
Sasha Y Kimel, Kinga M Bierwiaczonek, Milan Obaidi, Anita Foeman, Bessie Lawton, James Sidanius, Jonas R Kunst

While it is often assumed that Deoxyribonucleic acid (DNA) ancestry results illuminate one's true racial or ethnic lineage, the consequence of this inference remains largely unknown. This leaves two conflictual hypotheses largely untested: Do DNA ancestry tests increase racial tolerance or, alternatively, racial intolerance? Two multiwave experiments aimed to test these hypotheses using either real or bogus DNA ancestry results in combination with random assignment and a tightly controlled repeated-measurements experimental design. Bayesian and inferential analyses on both general and student populations of majority-group members in the United States (i.e., White/European Americans) indicated no support for either hypothesis on measures including multiculturalism, essentialism, and outgroup bias, even when moderating factors such as the degree of unexpected ancestry and genetic knowledge were considered. Despite wide societal optimism as well as concern, receiving DNA ancestry results appears not to impact feelings and attitudes about other racial and ethnic groups. Implications for prospective test-takers and education are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

虽然人们通常认为脱氧核糖核酸(DNA)的祖先结果可以揭示一个人真正的种族或民族血统,但这一推断的结果在很大程度上仍然是未知的。这使得两个相互冲突的假设在很大程度上没有得到验证:DNA 祖先测试是否会增加种族宽容度,或者,是否会增加种族不宽容度?两个多波实验旨在利用真实或虚假的 DNA 血统结果,结合随机分配和严格控制的重复测量实验设计来检验这些假设。对美国多数群体成员(即白种人/欧洲裔美国人)的普通人群和学生人群进行的贝叶斯分析和推理分析表明,在多元文化主义、本质主义和外群体偏见等测量指标上,这两种假设都没有得到支持,即使考虑到意外血统和遗传知识程度等调节因素也是如此。尽管社会上存在广泛的乐观情绪和担忧情绪,但接收 DNA 祖先结果似乎并不会影响人们对其他种族和民族群体的情感和态度。本文讨论了对未来测试者和教育的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Finding your roots: Do DNA ancestry tests increase racial (in)tolerance?","authors":"Sasha Y Kimel, Kinga M Bierwiaczonek, Milan Obaidi, Anita Foeman, Bessie Lawton, James Sidanius, Jonas R Kunst","doi":"10.1037/xap0000488","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000488","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>While it is often assumed that Deoxyribonucleic acid (DNA) ancestry results illuminate one's true racial or ethnic lineage, the consequence of this inference remains largely unknown. This leaves two conflictual hypotheses largely untested: Do DNA ancestry tests increase racial tolerance or, alternatively, racial intolerance? Two multiwave experiments aimed to test these hypotheses using either real or bogus DNA ancestry results in combination with random assignment and a tightly controlled repeated-measurements experimental design. Bayesian and inferential analyses on both general and student populations of majority-group members in the United States (i.e., White/European Americans) indicated no support for either hypothesis on measures including multiculturalism, essentialism, and outgroup bias, even when moderating factors such as the degree of unexpected ancestry and genetic knowledge were considered. Despite wide societal optimism as well as concern, receiving DNA ancestry results appears not to impact feelings and attitudes about other racial and ethnic groups. Implications for prospective test-takers and education are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"120-134"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138812238","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The commission effect: Framing affects perceived magnitude of identical payouts. 佣金效应:框架影响对相同报酬的感知程度。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2022-07-21 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000441
Mathew S Isaac, Julio Sevilla, Rajesh Bagchi

In addition to their salaries, employees often receive additional variable compensation (i.e., payouts) based on the sales they generate or manage. For any single transaction, the same payout (e.g., $1,000) may be earned by a relatively high commission rate and a low sales amount (e.g., 10% commission rate on a $10,000 sale) or a relatively low commission rate and a high sales amount (e.g., 1% commission rate on a $100,000 sale). In this research, we show that individuals-including those working in sales roles and familiar with commission plans-perceive the magnitude of the same payout as larger (smaller) if it stems from a high (low) commission rate and a low (high) sales amount. Across 10 experiments with 3,484 participants, we demonstrate the robustness of this "commission effect" in a varied set of employee and consumer contexts, and we identify behavioral consequences of this bias. We also provide evidence that the effect occurs because commission rates are expressed in percentages and are therefore relatively more evaluable than sales amounts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

除工资外,员工通常还可根据其创造或管理的销售额获得额外的可变报酬(即派息)。对于任何单笔交易而言,相对较高的佣金率和较低的销售额(例如,10,000 美元销售额的佣金率为 10%)或相对较低的佣金率和较高的销售额(例如,100,000 美元销售额的佣金率为 1%)都可能获得相同的报酬(例如,1,000 美元)。在这项研究中,我们发现,包括那些从事销售工作并熟悉佣金计划的人在内,如果佣金率高(低)而销售额低(高),则个人会认为同样的报酬幅度更大(更小)。我们通过 10 个实验,3,484 名参与者,证明了这种 "佣金效应 "在不同的员工和消费者背景下的稳健性,并确定了这种偏差的行为后果。我们还提供了证据,证明之所以会出现这种效应,是因为佣金率是以百分比表示的,因此相对而言比销售金额更容易评估。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The commission effect: Framing affects perceived magnitude of identical payouts.","authors":"Mathew S Isaac, Julio Sevilla, Rajesh Bagchi","doi":"10.1037/xap0000441","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000441","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In addition to their salaries, employees often receive additional variable compensation (i.e., payouts) based on the sales they generate or manage. For any single transaction, the same payout (e.g., $1,000) may be earned by a relatively high commission rate and a low sales amount (e.g., 10% commission rate on a $10,000 sale) or a relatively low commission rate and a high sales amount (e.g., 1% commission rate on a $100,000 sale). In this research, we show that individuals-including those working in sales roles and familiar with commission plans-perceive the magnitude of the same payout as larger (smaller) if it stems from a high (low) commission rate and a low (high) sales amount. Across 10 experiments with 3,484 participants, we demonstrate the robustness of this \"commission effect\" in a varied set of employee and consumer contexts, and we identify behavioral consequences of this bias. We also provide evidence that the effect occurs because commission rates are expressed in percentages and are therefore relatively more <i>evaluable</i> than sales amounts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"187-205"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9944950","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Planning-to-binge: Time allocation for future media consumption. 计划狂欢:未来媒体消费的时间分配
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-15 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000482
Joy Lu, Uma R Karmarkar, Vinod Venkatraman

The prevalence of streaming media has led firms to embrace the phenomenon of "binge-watching" by offering entire multipart series simultaneously. Such "on-demand" availability allows consumers to choose how to allocate future viewing time, but such decisions have received little attention in the literature. Across several studies, we show that individuals can plan binging in advance by allocating time in ways that aggregate episode consumption. Thus, we expand our understanding of media consumption to a new timepoint, distinct from "in-the-moment" viewing. We demonstrate that planning-to-binge preferences are flexible and shaped by perceptions of the media of interest. In particular, they are greater for content whose episodes are perceived as more sequential and connected, as opposed to independent. Since our framework focuses on the media's structural continuity, it applies across hedonic and utilitarian time use, motivations, and content, including "binge-learning" plans for online education. Furthermore, increased plans-to-binge can be triggered by merely framing content as more sequential versus independent. Finally, consumers are willing to spend both money and time for the future opportunity to binge, and more so for sequential content. These findings suggest ways media companies may strategically emphasize content structure to influence consumer decisions and media viewing styles. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

流媒体的盛行使公司开始接受 "狂欢式观看 "现象,同时提供整部多集连续剧。这种 "按需 "提供的方式允许消费者选择如何分配未来的观看时间,但这种决策在文献中很少受到关注。通过几项研究,我们发现,个人可以通过分配时间的方式提前计划 "狂欢",从而实现剧集消费的聚合。因此,我们将对媒体消费的理解扩展到了一个新的时间点,有别于 "即时 "观看。我们证明,"计划-狂欢 "偏好是灵活的,是由对感兴趣的媒体的看法决定的。特别是,对于那些被认为更有连续性和关联性(而非独立性)的内容,这种偏好会更大。由于我们的框架侧重于媒体的结构连续性,因此它适用于享乐型和功利型的时间使用、动机和内容,包括在线教育的 "狂欢学习 "计划。此外,只需将内容框定为更有连续性而非独立性,就能触发更多的 "狂欢 "计划。最后,消费者愿意为未来的狂欢机会花费金钱和时间,而对于连续性的内容来说更是如此。这些发现表明,媒体公司可以从战略上强调内容结构,从而影响消费者的决定和媒体观看方式。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Planning-to-binge: Time allocation for future media consumption.","authors":"Joy Lu, Uma R Karmarkar, Vinod Venkatraman","doi":"10.1037/xap0000482","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000482","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The prevalence of streaming media has led firms to embrace the phenomenon of \"binge-watching\" by offering entire multipart series simultaneously. Such \"on-demand\" availability allows consumers to choose how to allocate <i>future</i> viewing time, but such decisions have received little attention in the literature. Across several studies, we show that individuals can <i>plan</i> binging in advance by allocating time in ways that aggregate episode consumption. Thus, we expand our understanding of media consumption to a new timepoint, distinct from \"in-the-moment\" viewing. We demonstrate that planning-to-binge preferences are flexible and shaped by perceptions of the media of interest. In particular, they are greater for content whose episodes are perceived as more sequential and connected, as opposed to independent. Since our framework focuses on the media's structural continuity, it applies across hedonic and utilitarian time use, motivations, and content, including \"binge-learning\" plans for online education. Furthermore, increased plans-to-binge can be triggered by merely framing content as more sequential versus independent. Finally, consumers are willing to spend both money and time for the future opportunity to binge, and more so for sequential content. These findings suggest ways media companies may strategically emphasize content structure to influence consumer decisions and media viewing styles. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"169-186"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9640403","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Less biased yet more defensive: The impact of control processes. 减少偏见,加强防卫:控制程序的影响。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-03-09 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000468
Joseph A Vitriol, Brian A O'Shea, Jimmy Calanchini

Educational and training programs designed to reduce racial bias often focus on increasing people's awareness of psychological sources of their biases. However, when people learn about their biases, they often respond defensively, which can undermine the effectiveness of antibias interventions and the success of prejudice regulation. Using process (Quad) modeling, we provide one of the first investigations of the relationships between (a) controlled and automatic cognitive processes that underpin performance on the Implicit Association Test and (b) defensive reactions to unflattering implicit racial bias feedback. In two correlational samples (one preregistered; N = 8,000) and one experiment in which the provision of bias feedback was manipulated (N = 547), we find racially biased associations and some control over these associations among White people. Nonetheless, more defensiveness to bias feedback consistently predicted weaker ability to control biased associations. We also find correlational evidence that lower levels of biased associations predict more defensiveness, but did not replicate this observation in the experimental study. These results are critical for theories of implicit attitudes, models of prejudice regulation, and strategies for antibias interventions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

旨在减少种族偏见的教育和培训项目通常侧重于提高人们对其偏见心理根源的认识。然而,当人们了解到自己的偏见时,他们往往会做出防御性反应,这可能会影响抗偏见干预的效果和偏见调节的成功。利用过程(四元)建模,我们首次研究了(a)支持内隐联想测验成绩的受控和自动认知过程与(b)对不光彩的内隐种族偏见反馈的防御性反应之间的关系。在两个相关样本(一个是预先登记的样本;N = 8,000)和一个实验(N = 547)中,我们发现了白人的种族偏见联想和对这些联想的一些控制。然而,对偏见反馈的防御性越强,对偏见联想的控制能力就越弱。我们还发现相关证据表明,偏差联想水平越低,防御性越强,但在实验研究中并没有重复这一观察结果。这些结果对于内隐态度理论、偏见调节模型和抗偏见干预策略至关重要。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Less biased yet more defensive: The impact of control processes.","authors":"Joseph A Vitriol, Brian A O'Shea, Jimmy Calanchini","doi":"10.1037/xap0000468","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000468","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Educational and training programs designed to reduce racial bias often focus on increasing people's awareness of psychological sources of their biases. However, when people learn about their biases, they often respond defensively, which can undermine the effectiveness of antibias interventions and the success of prejudice regulation. Using process (Quad) modeling, we provide one of the first investigations of the relationships between (a) controlled and automatic cognitive processes that underpin performance on the Implicit Association Test and (b) defensive reactions to unflattering implicit racial bias feedback. In two correlational samples (one preregistered; <i>N</i> = 8,000) and one experiment in which the provision of bias feedback was manipulated (<i>N</i> = 547), we find racially biased associations and some control over these associations among White people. Nonetheless, more defensiveness to bias feedback consistently predicted weaker ability to control biased associations. We also find correlational evidence that lower levels of biased associations predict more defensiveness, but did not replicate this observation in the experimental study. These results are critical for theories of implicit attitudes, models of prejudice regulation, and strategies for antibias interventions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"108-119"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10870943","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Choose as much as you wish: Freedom cues in the marketplace help consumers feel more satisfied with what they choose and improve customer experience. 尽情选择:市场中的自由提示有助于消费者对自己的选择感到更加满意,并改善客户体验。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-07-27 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000481
Barbara Fasolo, Raffaella Misuraca, Elena Reutskaja

Consumer satisfaction and customer experience are key predictors of an organization's future market growth, long-term customer loyalty, and profitability but are hard to maintain in marketplaces with abundance of choice. Building on self-determination theory, we experimentally test a novel intervention that leverages consumer need for autonomy. The intervention is a message called a "freedom cue" (FC) which makes it salient that consumers can "choose as much as they wish." A 4-week field experiment in a sporting gear store establishes that FCs lead to greater consumer satisfaction compared to when the store displays no FC. A large (N = 669) preregistered process-tracing experiment run with a consumer panel and a global e-commerce company shows that FCs at point-of-sale improve consumer satisfaction and customer experience compared to an equivalent message that does not make freedom to choose any amount salient. Perceived freedom mediates the effect. FCs do not change the time spent or clicks on the website overall but do change the focus of the choice process. FCs lead to greater focus on what is chosen than on what is not chosen. We discuss practical implications for organizations and future research in consumer choice. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

消费者满意度和客户体验是预测企业未来市场增长、长期客户忠诚度和盈利能力的关键因素,但在选择丰富的市场中却难以维持。在自我决定理论的基础上,我们通过实验测试了一种利用消费者自主需求的新型干预措施。这种干预是一种被称为 "自由提示"(FC)的信息,它让消费者明白 "可以随心所欲地选择"。在一家体育用品商店进行的为期 4 周的实地实验表明,与商店没有展示自由提示相比,自由提示能提高消费者的满意度。一项由消费者小组和一家全球电子商务公司进行的大型(N = 669)预先登记的过程追踪实验表明,与不突出自由选择任意数量的同等信息相比,销售点的自由选择功能提高了消费者满意度和客户体验。感知到的自由对这一效果起到了中介作用。自由选择不会改变消费者在网站上花费的时间或点击次数,但会改变选择过程的重点。自由选择会使人们更加关注所选择的内容,而不是未选择的内容。我们讨论了消费者选择对组织和未来研究的实际意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Choose as much as you wish: Freedom cues in the marketplace help consumers feel more satisfied with what they choose and improve customer experience.","authors":"Barbara Fasolo, Raffaella Misuraca, Elena Reutskaja","doi":"10.1037/xap0000481","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000481","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Consumer satisfaction and customer experience are key predictors of an organization's future market growth, long-term customer loyalty, and profitability but are hard to maintain in marketplaces with abundance of choice. Building on self-determination theory, we experimentally test a novel intervention that leverages consumer need for autonomy. The intervention is a message called a \"freedom cue\" (FC) which makes it salient that consumers can \"choose as much as they wish.\" A 4-week field experiment in a sporting gear store establishes that FCs lead to greater consumer satisfaction compared to when the store displays no FC. A large (<i>N</i> = 669) preregistered process-tracing experiment run with a consumer panel and a global e-commerce company shows that FCs at point-of-sale improve consumer satisfaction and customer experience compared to an equivalent message that does not make freedom to choose any amount salient. Perceived freedom mediates the effect. FCs do not change the time spent or clicks on the website overall but do change the focus of the choice process. FCs lead to greater focus on what is chosen than on what is not chosen. We discuss practical implications for organizations and future research in consumer choice. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"156-168"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10259362","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Spatial alignment supports comparison of life science images. 空间配准支持生命科学图像的比较。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-04-06 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000471
Nina K Simms, Bryan J Matlen, Benjamin D Jee, Dedre Gentner

Visual comparisons are pervasive in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) instruction and practice. In previous work, adults' visual comparisons of simple stimuli were faster and more accurate when the layout of a display facilitated alignment of corresponding elements-the spatial alignment principle (Matlen et al., 2020). Here, we asked whether the spatial alignment principle extends to rich, educationally relevant stimuli, and how prior experience and spatial skill relate to spatial alignment effects. Participants were asked to find an incorrect bone within a skeleton, presented individually or paired with a correct skeleton in a layout that did (direct placement) or did not (impeded placement) support alignment (Kurtz & Gentner, 2013). Consistent with the spatial alignment principle, undergraduates (Study 1) showed an advantage of direct over impeded placement. Middle schoolers (Study 2) showed a direct advantage on items presented in atypical orientations. That atypical items showed the strongest effects suggests that direct placement may help most when materials are less familiar. However, neither individual differences in undergraduates' STEM course history, nor undergraduates' or middle schoolers' spatial skills moderated spatial alignment effects. Thus, applying the spatial alignment principle in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics has potential to improve visual comparisons, especially those that are challenging, for students of all spatial skill levels. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

视觉比较在科学、技术、工程和数学(STEM)教学和实践中非常普遍。在之前的研究中,如果显示屏的布局有利于相应元素的对齐--即空间对齐原则(Matlen 等人,2020 年),成人对简单刺激物的视觉比较会更快更准确。在此,我们想知道空间对齐原则是否适用于丰富的、与教育相关的刺激,以及先前的经验和空间技能与空间对齐效应的关系。我们要求参与者在一个骨架中找到一个错误的骨头,该骨架可单独呈现,也可与一个正确的骨架配对,其布局支持(直接放置)或不支持(阻碍放置)对齐(Kurtz & Gentner, 2013)。与空间对齐原则一致,本科生(研究 1)表现出直接摆放比阻碍摆放更有优势。初中生(研究 2)在以非典型方向呈现的项目上表现出直接优势。非典型项目显示出最强的效果,这表明当材料不太熟悉时,直接放置可能最有帮助。然而,无论是本科生的 STEM 课程历史个体差异,还是本科生或初中生的空间技能个体差异,都没有调节空间排列效应。因此,在科学、技术、工程和数学中应用空间对齐原则有可能改善视觉比较,尤其是那些具有挑战性的视觉比较,适合所有空间技能水平的学生。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Spatial alignment supports comparison of life science images.","authors":"Nina K Simms, Bryan J Matlen, Benjamin D Jee, Dedre Gentner","doi":"10.1037/xap0000471","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000471","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Visual comparisons are pervasive in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) instruction and practice. In previous work, adults' visual comparisons of simple stimuli were faster and more accurate when the layout of a display facilitated alignment of corresponding elements-the <i>spatial alignment principle</i> (Matlen et al., 2020). Here, we asked whether the spatial alignment principle extends to rich, educationally relevant stimuli, and how prior experience and spatial skill relate to spatial alignment effects. Participants were asked to find an incorrect bone within a skeleton, presented individually or paired with a correct skeleton in a layout that did (direct placement) or did not (impeded placement) support alignment (Kurtz & Gentner, 2013). Consistent with the spatial alignment principle, undergraduates (Study 1) showed an advantage of direct over impeded placement. Middle schoolers (Study 2) showed a direct advantage on items presented in atypical orientations. That atypical items showed the strongest effects suggests that direct placement may help most when materials are less familiar. However, neither individual differences in undergraduates' STEM course history, nor undergraduates' or middle schoolers' spatial skills moderated spatial alignment effects. Thus, applying the spatial alignment principle in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics has potential to improve visual comparisons, especially those that are challenging, for students of all spatial skill levels. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"747-760"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9263701","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Can conflict cultivate collaboration? The positive impact of mild versus intense task conflict via perceived openness rather than emotions. 冲突能促进合作吗?温和与激烈的任务冲突通过感知开放性而非情绪产生的积极影响。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2022-08-25 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000448
Ming-Hong Tsai

Previous research has demonstrated negative associations between task-relevant conflicts and collaboration. To supplement the previous findings and explore the potential benefits of conflicts, we differentiate between two types of task conflict expressions (i.e., mild vs. intense task conflicts, such as debates vs. disagreements regarding work-related issues) in dyad interactions and propose the differential effects of mild versus intense task conflicts on collaboration based on the theory of conflict expression. In three studies with experimental manipulations and surveys on working adults, the results demonstrated that perceptions of debates versus disagreements (in Studies 1 and 2) or mild versus intense task conflicts (in Study 3) enhanced perceivers' collaboration with others via the perceivers' assessments of others' openness rather than emotions. The findings regarding positive associations between mild task conflicts and collaboration implicate the coexistence of conflict and collaboration. Moreover, the results showed that debates versus disagreements (in Study 1) or mild versus intense task conflicts (in Study 3) achieved high task performance by enhancing perceptions of others' openness that subsequently increased collaboration. These findings clarify why conflicts inconsistently influence interpersonal interactions and task performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

以往的研究表明,任务相关冲突与协作之间存在负相关。为了补充之前的研究结果并探索冲突的潜在益处,我们区分了二人互动中两种类型的任务冲突表达(即温和与激烈的任务冲突,如关于工作相关问题的辩论与分歧),并基于冲突表达理论提出了温和与激烈的任务冲突对合作的不同影响。在三项针对工作成年人的实验操作和调查研究中,结果表明,对辩论与分歧的感知(研究 1 和研究 2)或对轻微与激烈任务冲突的感知(研究 3)通过感知者对他人开放性的评估而不是情绪,增强了感知者与他人的合作。关于轻微任务冲突与协作之间正相关的研究结果表明,冲突与协作是共存的。此外,研究结果表明,辩论与分歧(研究 1)或轻微任务冲突与激烈任务冲突(研究 3)通过增强对他人开放性的感知来提高任务绩效,进而增强协作。这些研究结果澄清了为什么冲突会不一致地影响人际互动和任务绩效。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Can conflict cultivate collaboration? The positive impact of mild versus intense task conflict via perceived openness rather than emotions.","authors":"Ming-Hong Tsai","doi":"10.1037/xap0000448","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000448","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous research has demonstrated negative associations between task-relevant conflicts and collaboration. To supplement the previous findings and explore the potential benefits of conflicts, we differentiate between two types of task conflict expressions (i.e., mild vs. intense task conflicts, such as debates vs. disagreements regarding work-related issues) in dyad interactions and propose the differential effects of mild versus intense task conflicts on collaboration based on the theory of conflict expression. In three studies with experimental manipulations and surveys on working adults, the results demonstrated that perceptions of debates versus disagreements (in Studies 1 and 2) or mild versus intense task conflicts (in Study 3) enhanced perceivers' collaboration with others via the perceivers' assessments of others' openness rather than emotions. The findings regarding positive associations between mild task conflicts and collaboration implicate the coexistence of conflict and collaboration. Moreover, the results showed that debates versus disagreements (in Study 1) or mild versus intense task conflicts (in Study 3) achieved high task performance by enhancing perceptions of others' openness that subsequently increased collaboration. These findings clarify why conflicts inconsistently influence interpersonal interactions and task performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"813-830"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9996455","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Comparing the effectiveness of two theory-based strategies to promote cognitive training adherence. 比较两种基于理论的促进认知训练坚持性策略的有效性。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-07-20 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000485
Erin R Harrell, Nelson A Roque, Walter R Boot

This study compared the effectiveness of two theory-based strategies to promote cognitive training adherence among older adults (Mage = 70 years, SD = 4.42, range = 64-84). Strategies incorporated either (a) elements of implementation intention formation or (b) positive message framing, both of which have been found to promote adherence to health behaviors in other domains. Participants (N = 120) were asked to engage in technology-based cognitive training at home comprised of seven gamified neuropsychological tasks. In Phase 1 (structured), participants were provided a schedule that required engagement in 1 hr of cognitive training 5 days each week over 2 months. In Phase 2 (unstructured), participants were instructed to engage with the intervention as much as they desired for 1 month. Contrary to expectations, neither the implementation intention nor positive message framing produced greater adherence relative to control as measured by the total number of training sessions completed in each phase. However, exploratory analysis indicated a greater likelihood of intervention engagement for participants assigned to the implementation intention condition on many days of the intervention, though the trajectory of engagement decline was similar for all three groups. Measures of cognition, attitudes/personality, and technology proficiency also did not predict adherence over either phase. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究比较了两种基于理论的策略对促进老年人(年龄 = 70 岁,标准差 = 4.42,范围 = 64-84)坚持认知训练的效果。这两种策略都包含(a)实施意向形成要素或(b)积极的信息框架,这两种方法都被发现在其他领域促进健康行为的坚持。参与者(N = 120)被要求在家进行基于技术的认知训练,包括七项游戏化神经心理学任务。在第一阶段(结构化),参与者需要在 2 个月内每周 5 天参加 1 小时的认知训练。在第 2 阶段(非结构化),参与者被要求在 1 个月内随心所欲地参与干预。与预期相反的是,与对照组相比,无论是实施意向还是积极信息框架都没有产生更大的依从性(以每个阶段完成的训练总次数来衡量)。然而,探索性分析表明,在干预的许多天里,被分配到实施意向条件下的参与者参与干预的可能性更大,尽管三组参与者参与度下降的轨迹相似。对认知、态度/个性和技术熟练程度的测量也不能预测两个阶段的坚持情况。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Comparing the effectiveness of two theory-based strategies to promote cognitive training adherence.","authors":"Erin R Harrell, Nelson A Roque, Walter R Boot","doi":"10.1037/xap0000485","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000485","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study compared the effectiveness of two theory-based strategies to promote cognitive training adherence among older adults (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 70 years, <i>SD</i> = 4.42, range = 64-84). Strategies incorporated either (a) elements of implementation intention formation or (b) positive message framing, both of which have been found to promote adherence to health behaviors in other domains. Participants (<i>N</i> = 120) were asked to engage in technology-based cognitive training at home comprised of seven gamified neuropsychological tasks. In Phase 1 (structured), participants were provided a schedule that required engagement in 1 hr of cognitive training 5 days each week over 2 months. In Phase 2 (unstructured), participants were instructed to engage with the intervention as much as they desired for 1 month. Contrary to expectations, neither the implementation intention nor positive message framing produced greater adherence relative to control as measured by the total number of training sessions completed in each phase. However, exploratory analysis indicated a greater likelihood of intervention engagement for participants assigned to the implementation intention condition on many days of the intervention, though the trajectory of engagement decline was similar for all three groups. Measures of cognition, attitudes/personality, and technology proficiency also did not predict adherence over either phase. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"782-792"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9840886","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
People are worse at detecting fake news in their foreign language. 人们用外语识别假新闻的能力更差。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-08 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000475
Rafał Muda, Gordon Pennycook, Damian Hamerski, Michał Białek

Across two preregistered within-subject experiments (N = 570), we found that when using their foreign language, proficient bilinguals discerned true from false news less accurately. This was the case for international news (Experiment 1) and more local news (Experiment 2). When using a foreign (as opposed to native) language, false news headlines were always judged more believable, while true news headlines were judged equally (Experiment 2) or less believable (Experiment 1). In contrast to past theorizing, the foreign language effect interacted neither with perceived arousal of news (Experiment 1) nor with individual differences in cognitive reflection (Experiments 1 and 2). Finally, using signal detection theory modeling, we showed that the negative effects of using a foreign language were not caused by adopting different responding strategies (e.g., preferring omissions to false alarms) but rather by decreased sensitivity to the truth. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

在两个预先登记的被试内实验(N = 570)中,我们发现,在使用外语时,精通双语者辨别真假新闻的准确性较低。国际新闻(实验 1)和更多本地新闻(实验 2)的情况都是如此。当使用外语(而非母语)时,虚假新闻标题总是被判定为更可信,而真实新闻标题则被判定为同样可信(实验 2)或更不可信(实验 1)。与过去的理论不同,外语效应既不与新闻的感知唤醒(实验 1)相互作用,也不与认知反思的个体差异(实验 1 和 2)相互作用。最后,通过信号检测理论模型,我们证明了使用外语的负面影响并不是由于采取了不同的反应策略(例如,更倾向于遗漏而不是错误警报),而是由于对真相的敏感度下降造成的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"People are worse at detecting fake news in their foreign language.","authors":"Rafał Muda, Gordon Pennycook, Damian Hamerski, Michał Białek","doi":"10.1037/xap0000475","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000475","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Across two preregistered within-subject experiments (<i>N</i> = 570), we found that when using their foreign language, proficient bilinguals discerned true from false news less accurately. This was the case for international news (Experiment 1) and more local news (Experiment 2). When using a foreign (as opposed to native) language, false news headlines were always judged more believable, while true news headlines were judged equally (Experiment 2) or less believable (Experiment 1). In contrast to past theorizing, the foreign language effect interacted neither with perceived arousal of news (Experiment 1) nor with individual differences in cognitive reflection (Experiments 1 and 2). Finally, using signal detection theory modeling, we showed that the negative effects of using a foreign language were not caused by adopting different responding strategies (e.g., preferring omissions to false alarms) but rather by decreased sensitivity to the truth. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"712-724"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9486739","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Disentangling the effects of message content and message sharer on students' views of political misinformation. 厘清信息内容和信息分享者对学生政治误导观点的影响。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-04 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000495
Eva M Janssen, Tamara van Gog

A consistent finding in fake news research is that people are more likely to believe content in favor of their political views. Unclear, however, is whether this political bias is moderated by contextual effects, such as politicians sharing content on their social media accounts. The present study investigated how both message content and sharer affect views of political misinformation. Participants (N = 164) evaluated eight news messages. Message content (pro-left/pro-right misinformation) and sharer (left-wing/right-wing/unknown politician) were manipulated within subjects. As expected, participants agreed more with concordant misinformation (aligned with their political orientation) and perceived it as more accurate than discordant misinformation. There was an additional, smaller effect of politician: Participants agreed more with discordant misinformation when shared by a politician representing their political viewpoint than when shared by others. Furthermore, left-oriented participants' agreement with concordant misinformation was hardly affected by message sharer, whereas right-oriented participants' agreement with concordant misinformation was-unexpectedly-positively affected by the left-wing politician. Irrespective of their political orientation, participants perceived misinformation from the left-wing politician as more accurate than misinformation from other politicians. Our findings suggest that both message content and sharer affect views of misinformation and interact in doing so, which is important for designing interventions on recognizing misinformation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

假新闻研究的一个一致发现是,人们更容易相信有利于自己政治观点的内容。然而,这种政治偏见是否会受到情境效应的影响,如政治家在其社交媒体账户上分享内容,尚不清楚。本研究调查了信息内容和分享者如何影响人们对政治错误信息的看法。参与者(N = 164)对八条新闻信息进行了评估。信息内容(支持左派/支持右派的错误信息)和分享者(左翼/右翼/不知名的政治家)在受试者内部进行操纵。不出所料,与不一致的错误信息相比,参与者更同意一致的错误信息(与他们的政治倾向一致),并认为它更准确。政治家的影响较小:与其他人相比,参与者更同意由代表其政治观点的政治家分享的不和谐错误信息。此外,左倾参与者对不一致错误信息的认同几乎不受信息分享者的影响,而右倾参与者对不一致错误信息的认同却意外地受到左翼政治家的积极影响。无论政治倾向如何,参与者都认为左翼政治家提供的错误信息比其他政治家提供的错误信息更准确。我们的研究结果表明,信息内容和分享者都会影响人们对误导信息的看法,并且在影响过程中相互影响,这对于设计识别误导信息的干预措施非常重要。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Disentangling the effects of message content and message sharer on students' views of political misinformation.","authors":"Eva M Janssen, Tamara van Gog","doi":"10.1037/xap0000495","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000495","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A consistent finding in fake news research is that people are more likely to believe content in favor of their political views. Unclear, however, is whether this political bias is moderated by contextual effects, such as politicians sharing content on their social media accounts. The present study investigated how both message content and sharer affect views of political misinformation. Participants (<i>N</i> = 164) evaluated eight news messages. Message content (pro-left/pro-right misinformation) and sharer (left-wing/right-wing/unknown politician) were manipulated within subjects. As expected, participants agreed more with concordant misinformation (aligned with their political orientation) and perceived it as more accurate than discordant misinformation. There was an additional, smaller effect of politician: Participants agreed more with discordant misinformation when shared by a politician representing their political viewpoint than when shared by others. Furthermore, left-oriented participants' agreement with concordant misinformation was hardly affected by message sharer, whereas right-oriented participants' agreement with concordant misinformation was-unexpectedly-positively affected by the left-wing politician. Irrespective of their political orientation, participants perceived misinformation from the left-wing politician as more accurate than misinformation from other politicians. Our findings suggest that both message content and sharer affect views of misinformation and interact in doing so, which is important for designing interventions on recognizing misinformation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"693-711"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10154013","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1