首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied最新文献

英文 中文
Disentangling the effects of message content and message sharer on students' views of political misinformation. 厘清信息内容和信息分享者对学生政治误导观点的影响。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-04 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000495
Eva M Janssen, Tamara van Gog

A consistent finding in fake news research is that people are more likely to believe content in favor of their political views. Unclear, however, is whether this political bias is moderated by contextual effects, such as politicians sharing content on their social media accounts. The present study investigated how both message content and sharer affect views of political misinformation. Participants (N = 164) evaluated eight news messages. Message content (pro-left/pro-right misinformation) and sharer (left-wing/right-wing/unknown politician) were manipulated within subjects. As expected, participants agreed more with concordant misinformation (aligned with their political orientation) and perceived it as more accurate than discordant misinformation. There was an additional, smaller effect of politician: Participants agreed more with discordant misinformation when shared by a politician representing their political viewpoint than when shared by others. Furthermore, left-oriented participants' agreement with concordant misinformation was hardly affected by message sharer, whereas right-oriented participants' agreement with concordant misinformation was-unexpectedly-positively affected by the left-wing politician. Irrespective of their political orientation, participants perceived misinformation from the left-wing politician as more accurate than misinformation from other politicians. Our findings suggest that both message content and sharer affect views of misinformation and interact in doing so, which is important for designing interventions on recognizing misinformation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

假新闻研究的一个一致发现是,人们更容易相信有利于自己政治观点的内容。然而,这种政治偏见是否会受到情境效应的影响,如政治家在其社交媒体账户上分享内容,尚不清楚。本研究调查了信息内容和分享者如何影响人们对政治错误信息的看法。参与者(N = 164)对八条新闻信息进行了评估。信息内容(支持左派/支持右派的错误信息)和分享者(左翼/右翼/不知名的政治家)在受试者内部进行操纵。不出所料,与不一致的错误信息相比,参与者更同意一致的错误信息(与他们的政治倾向一致),并认为它更准确。政治家的影响较小:与其他人相比,参与者更同意由代表其政治观点的政治家分享的不和谐错误信息。此外,左倾参与者对不一致错误信息的认同几乎不受信息分享者的影响,而右倾参与者对不一致错误信息的认同却意外地受到左翼政治家的积极影响。无论政治倾向如何,参与者都认为左翼政治家提供的错误信息比其他政治家提供的错误信息更准确。我们的研究结果表明,信息内容和分享者都会影响人们对误导信息的看法,并且在影响过程中相互影响,这对于设计识别误导信息的干预措施非常重要。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Disentangling the effects of message content and message sharer on students' views of political misinformation.","authors":"Eva M Janssen, Tamara van Gog","doi":"10.1037/xap0000495","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000495","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A consistent finding in fake news research is that people are more likely to believe content in favor of their political views. Unclear, however, is whether this political bias is moderated by contextual effects, such as politicians sharing content on their social media accounts. The present study investigated how both message content and sharer affect views of political misinformation. Participants (<i>N</i> = 164) evaluated eight news messages. Message content (pro-left/pro-right misinformation) and sharer (left-wing/right-wing/unknown politician) were manipulated within subjects. As expected, participants agreed more with concordant misinformation (aligned with their political orientation) and perceived it as more accurate than discordant misinformation. There was an additional, smaller effect of politician: Participants agreed more with discordant misinformation when shared by a politician representing their political viewpoint than when shared by others. Furthermore, left-oriented participants' agreement with concordant misinformation was hardly affected by message sharer, whereas right-oriented participants' agreement with concordant misinformation was-unexpectedly-positively affected by the left-wing politician. Irrespective of their political orientation, participants perceived misinformation from the left-wing politician as more accurate than misinformation from other politicians. Our findings suggest that both message content and sharer affect views of misinformation and interact in doing so, which is important for designing interventions on recognizing misinformation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"693-711"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10154013","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Identifying inefficient strategies in automation-aided signal detection. 识别自动化辅助信号检测中的低效策略
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-07-20 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000484
Lana Tikhomirov, Megan L Bartlett, Jackson Duncan-Reid, Jason S McCarley

Automated diagnostic aids can assist human operators in signal detection tasks, providing alarms, warnings, or diagnoses. Operators often use decision aids poorly, though, falling short of best possible performance levels. Previous research has suggested that operators interact with binary signal detection aids using a sluggish contingent cutoff (CC) strategy (Robinson & Sorkin, 1985), shifting their response criterion in the direction stipulated by the aid's diagnosis each trial but making adjustments that are smaller than optimal. The present study tested this model by examining the efficiency of automation-aided signal detection under different levels of task difficulty. In a pair of experiments, participants performed a numeric decision-making task requiring them to make signal or noise judgments on the basis of probabilistic readings. The mean reading values of signal and noise states differed between groups of participants, producing two levels of task difficulty. Data were fit with the CC model and two alternative accounts of automation-aided strategy: a discrete deference (DD) model, which assumed participants defer to the aid on a subset of trials and a mixture model, which assumed that participants choose randomly between the CC and DD strategies every trial. Model fits favored the mixture model. The results indicate multiple forms of inefficiency in operators' strategies for using signal detection aids. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

自动诊断辅助设备可以协助人类操作员完成信号检测任务,提供警报、警告或诊断。不过,操作员使用决策辅助工具的效果往往不佳,达不到最佳性能水平。以往的研究表明,操作员在使用二进制信号检测辅助工具时采用的是一种迟缓的或然截止(CC)策略(Robinson & Sorkin,1985 年),每次试验都会根据辅助工具的诊断结果改变他们的反应标准,但做出的调整却小于最佳值。本研究通过考察不同任务难度下自动化辅助信号检测的效率,对这一模型进行了测试。在一对实验中,参与者进行了一项数字决策任务,要求他们根据概率读数做出信号或噪声判断。不同组别的参与者对信号和噪声状态的平均读数不同,从而产生了两种不同的任务难度。对数据进行拟合的方法有 CC 模型和两种自动化辅助策略的替代方案:离散服从(DD)模型和混合模型,前者假定参与者在部分试验中服从辅助策略,后者假定参与者每次试验都在 CC 和 DD 策略之间随机选择。模型拟合结果更倾向于混合模型。结果表明,操作员使用信号检测辅助工具的策略存在多种形式的低效。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Identifying inefficient strategies in automation-aided signal detection.","authors":"Lana Tikhomirov, Megan L Bartlett, Jackson Duncan-Reid, Jason S McCarley","doi":"10.1037/xap0000484","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000484","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Automated diagnostic aids can assist human operators in signal detection tasks, providing alarms, warnings, or diagnoses. Operators often use decision aids poorly, though, falling short of best possible performance levels. Previous research has suggested that operators interact with binary signal detection aids using a sluggish contingent cutoff (CC) strategy (Robinson & Sorkin, 1985), shifting their response criterion in the direction stipulated by the aid's diagnosis each trial but making adjustments that are smaller than optimal. The present study tested this model by examining the efficiency of automation-aided signal detection under different levels of task difficulty. In a pair of experiments, participants performed a numeric decision-making task requiring them to make signal or noise judgments on the basis of probabilistic readings. The mean reading values of signal and noise states differed between groups of participants, producing two levels of task difficulty. Data were fit with the CC model and two alternative accounts of automation-aided strategy: a discrete deference (DD) model, which assumed participants defer to the aid on a subset of trials and a mixture model, which assumed that participants choose randomly between the CC and DD strategies every trial. Model fits favored the mixture model. The results indicate multiple forms of inefficiency in operators' strategies for using signal detection aids. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"869-886"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9835218","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Playing a social dilemma game as an exploratory learning activity before instruction improves conceptual understanding. 在教学之前,把社会两难游戏作为一种探索性学习活动来玩,可以提高对概念的理解。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-03-27 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000470
Jason Bush, Marci S DeCaro, Daniel A DeCaro

Society's most pressing problems involve social dilemmas, yet few individuals recognize and understand their core components. We examined how a serious social dilemma game used in an educational setting impacted understanding of a classic social dilemma, the tragedy of the commons. Participants (N = 186) were randomly assigned to one of two gameplay conditions or a Lesson-Only condition without the game (traditional lesson with a reading). In the Explore-First condition, participants played the game as an exploratory learning activity before the lesson. In the Lesson-First condition, participants played the game after the lesson. Both gameplay conditions were rated as more interesting than the Lesson-Only condition. However, participants in the Explore-First condition exhibited higher conceptual understanding and spontaneous transfer to real-world dilemmas than the other conditions, which did not differ. These benefits were selective to social concepts (e.g., self-interest, interdependency) explored via gameplay. These benefits did not occur for ecological concepts (e.g., scarcity, tragedy), which were taught to everyone during the beginning instructions. Policy preferences were equal across conditions. Serious social dilemma games offer a promising educational tool for conceptual development when students can explore the complexities of social dilemmas for themselves. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

社会最紧迫的问题涉及社会困境,但很少有人认识和理解其核心内容。我们研究了在教育环境中使用严肃的社会困境游戏如何影响人们对经典社会困境--公地悲剧--的理解。参与者(N = 186)被随机分配到两种游戏条件之一或没有游戏的纯课程条件(传统的阅读课)。在 "探索先行 "条件下,参与者在上课前玩游戏,作为探索性学习活动。在 "先上课 "条件下,参与者在课后玩游戏。两种游戏条件都被评为比 "只上课 "条件更有趣。然而,与其他条件相比,"先探索 "条件下的参与者表现出更高的概念理解能力,并能自发地将其迁移到现实世界的困境中。这些益处对通过游戏探索的社会概念(如自我利益、相互依存)具有选择性。生态概念(如稀缺性、悲剧)则没有这些益处,因为在开始指导时,每个人都学到了这些概念。不同条件下的政策偏好是相同的。当学生可以自己探索社会困境的复杂性时,严肃的社会困境游戏为概念发展提供了一种很有前途的教育工具。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Playing a social dilemma game as an exploratory learning activity before instruction improves conceptual understanding.","authors":"Jason Bush, Marci S DeCaro, Daniel A DeCaro","doi":"10.1037/xap0000470","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000470","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Society's most pressing problems involve social dilemmas, yet few individuals recognize and understand their core components. We examined how a serious social dilemma game used in an educational setting impacted understanding of a classic social dilemma, the tragedy of the commons. Participants (<i>N</i> = 186) were randomly assigned to one of two gameplay conditions or a Lesson-Only condition without the game (traditional lesson with a reading). In the Explore-First condition, participants played the game as an exploratory learning activity before the lesson. In the Lesson-First condition, participants played the game after the lesson. Both gameplay conditions were rated as more interesting than the Lesson-Only condition. However, participants in the Explore-First condition exhibited higher conceptual understanding and spontaneous transfer to real-world dilemmas than the other conditions, which did not differ. These benefits were selective to social concepts (e.g., self-interest, interdependency) explored via gameplay. These benefits did not occur for ecological concepts (e.g., scarcity, tragedy), which were taught to everyone during the beginning instructions. Policy preferences were equal across conditions. Serious social dilemma games offer a promising educational tool for conceptual development when students can explore the complexities of social dilemmas for themselves. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"725-746"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9177417","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
I am (not) sorry: Interpersonal effects of neutralizations after a transgression. 我(不)感到抱歉:过失后中和的人际影响。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-17 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000483
Bastiaan T Rutjens, Coen A Ackers, Gerben A van Kleef

After a transgression, people often use neutralizations to account for their behavior, for instance, by apologizing or offering a justification. Previous research has mostly centered around the intrapersonal effects of neutralizations on actors. Consequently, we know very little of the interpersonal effects of neutralizations on observers' perceptions and judgments. Our overarching hypothesis is that neutralizations that contain an acknowledgment of wrongdoing (i.e., apologies and excuses) lead to more favorable perceptions of the transgressor and the transgression than neutralizations that do not (i.e., justifications). We report three studies (N = 800) to investigate the relationship between the type of neutralization used and observers' perceptions of actors and their behaviors. Our findings show that actor and behavior are evaluated differently depending on whether the neutralization used is an apology, an excuse, a consequentialist justification, or a deontological justification. Overall, justifications led to more negative evaluations (especially when invoking deontological reasoning), while apologies and excuses fostered more positive evaluations. We discuss the implications of these findings for understanding the social dynamics of norm violations and the social and legal implications for enforcing norm abidance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

在犯错之后,人们通常会使用中和法对自己的行为做出解释,例如道歉或提供正当理由。以往的研究大多集中于中和对行为人的人际影响。因此,我们对中和对观察者感知和判断的人际影响知之甚少。我们的首要假设是,包含承认错误行为的中和(即道歉和辩解)比不包含承认错误行为的中和(即辩解)更能使人们对违法者和违法行为产生好感。我们报告了三项研究(N = 800),以调查所使用的中和类型与观察者对行为者及其行为的看法之间的关系。我们的研究结果表明,对行为人和行为的评价因其所使用的中和方式是道歉、借口、结果论辩护还是去义务论辩护而有所不同。总体而言,辩解会导致更多的负面评价(尤其是在援引去道德主义推理时),而道歉和借口则会促进更多的正面评价。我们将讨论这些发现对理解违反规范的社会动态以及强制遵守规范的社会和法律意义的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"I am (not) sorry: Interpersonal effects of neutralizations after a transgression.","authors":"Bastiaan T Rutjens, Coen A Ackers, Gerben A van Kleef","doi":"10.1037/xap0000483","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000483","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>After a transgression, people often use neutralizations to account for their behavior, for instance, by apologizing or offering a justification. Previous research has mostly centered around the intrapersonal effects of neutralizations on actors. Consequently, we know very little of the <i>interpersonal</i> effects of neutralizations on observers' perceptions and judgments. Our overarching hypothesis is that neutralizations that contain an acknowledgment of wrongdoing (i.e., apologies and excuses) lead to more favorable perceptions of the transgressor and the transgression than neutralizations that do not (i.e., justifications). We report three studies (<i>N</i> = 800) to investigate the relationship between the type of neutralization used and observers' perceptions of actors and their behaviors. Our findings show that actor and behavior are evaluated differently depending on whether the neutralization used is an apology, an excuse, a consequentialist justification, or a deontological justification. Overall, justifications led to more negative evaluations (especially when invoking deontological reasoning), while apologies and excuses fostered more positive evaluations. We discuss the implications of these findings for understanding the social dynamics of norm violations and the social and legal implications for enforcing norm abidance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"831-848"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10005536","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Science communication gets personal: Ambivalent effects of self-disclosure in science communication on trust in science. 科学传播变得私人化:科学传播中的自我披露对科学信任的矛盾影响。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-17 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000489
Marlene Sophie Altenmüller, Lorenz Kampschulte, Laura Verbeek, Mario Gollwitzer

In an attempt to display themselves as warm, approachable, and trustworthy, researchers might reveal personal details about themselves (i.e., self-disclosure) when communicating their science to the public. Here, we test whether self-disclosure in science communication can actually increase public trust in science. We present six online experiments (overall N = 2,431), integrate their results in a mini meta-analysis, and report a field experiment in a science museum (N = 480): In sum, our findings suggest that self-disclosure leads to small, but measurable increases in laypeople's feelings of closeness toward researchers and perceptions of researchers' warmth-related trustworthiness; yet, self-disclosure also leads to decreases in competence-related trustworthiness perceptions. The credibility of scientific findings was, overall, unaffected by self-disclosing communication. Findings from the field study further question whether self-disclosure in science communication has any practical relevance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

为了展示自己热情、平易近人和值得信赖的形象,研究人员在向公众传播科学知识时可能会透露自己的个人细节(即自我披露)。在此,我们将检验科学传播中的自我披露是否真的能提高公众对科学的信任。我们介绍了六项在线实验(总人数 = 2,431),在一项小型荟萃分析中整合了实验结果,并报告了一项在科学博物馆进行的实地实验(人数 = 480):总之,我们的研究结果表明,自我披露会导致非专业人士对研究人员的亲近感以及对研究人员与热情相关的可信度的感知出现微小但可测量的增长;然而,自我披露也会导致与能力相关的可信度感知出现下降。总体而言,科学发现的可信度没有受到自我披露交流的影响。实地研究的结果进一步质疑了科学传播中的自我披露是否具有实际意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Science communication gets personal: Ambivalent effects of self-disclosure in science communication on trust in science.","authors":"Marlene Sophie Altenmüller, Lorenz Kampschulte, Laura Verbeek, Mario Gollwitzer","doi":"10.1037/xap0000489","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000489","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In an attempt to display themselves as warm, approachable, and trustworthy, researchers might reveal personal details about themselves (i.e., self-disclosure) when communicating their science to the public. Here, we test whether self-disclosure in science communication can actually increase public trust in science. We present six online experiments (overall <i>N</i> = 2,431), integrate their results in a mini meta-analysis, and report a field experiment in a science museum (<i>N</i> = 480): In sum, our findings suggest that self-disclosure leads to small, but measurable increases in laypeople's feelings of closeness toward researchers and perceptions of researchers' warmth-related trustworthiness; yet, self-disclosure also leads to decreases in competence-related trustworthiness perceptions. The credibility of scientific findings was, overall, unaffected by self-disclosing communication. Findings from the field study further question whether self-disclosure in science communication has any practical relevance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"793-812"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10005537","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Automated decision aids: When are they advisors and when do they take control of human decision making? 自动决策辅助工具:它们何时是顾问,何时控制人类决策?
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-03-06 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000463
Luke Strickland, Russell J Boag, Andrew Heathcote, Vanessa Bowden, Shayne Loft

We applied a computational model to examine the extent to which participants used an automated decision aid as an advisor, as compared to a more autonomous trigger of responding, at varying levels of decision aid reliability. In an air traffic control conflict detection task, we found higher accuracy when the decision aid was correct, and more errors when the decision aid was incorrect, as compared to a manual condition (no decision aid). Responses that were correct despite incorrect automated advice were slower than matched manual responses. Decision aids set at lower reliability (75%) had smaller effects on choices and response times, and were subjectively trusted less, than decision aids set at higher reliability (95%). We fitted an evidence accumulation model to choices and response times to measure how information processing was affected by decision aid inputs. Participants primarily treated low-reliability decision aids as an advisor rather than directly accumulating evidence based on its advice. Participants directly accumulated evidence based upon the advice of high-reliability decision aids, consistent with granting decision aids more autonomous influence over decisions. Individual differences in the level of direct accumulation correlated with subjective trust, suggesting a cognitive mechanism by which trust impacts human decisions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

我们应用了一个计算模型来研究在不同的决策辅助可靠性水平下,与更自主的触发响应相比,参与者在多大程度上使用自动决策辅助作为顾问。在一项空中交通管制冲突检测任务中,我们发现与手动条件(无决策辅助)相比,当决策辅助正确时,准确率更高,而当决策辅助不正确时,错误率更高。尽管自动建议不正确,但正确的反应却比匹配的人工反应慢。与可靠性较高(95%)的辅助决策相比,可靠性较低(75%)的辅助决策对选择和反应时间的影响较小,主观信任度也较低。我们对选择和反应时间建立了一个证据积累模型,以衡量决策辅助工具输入对信息处理的影响。参与者主要将可靠性低的决策辅助工具视为顾问,而不是根据其建议直接积累证据。参与者根据高可靠性决策辅助工具的建议直接积累证据,这与赋予决策辅助工具对决策更多自主影响是一致的。直接积累水平的个体差异与主观信任度相关,这表明信任度影响人类决策的认知机制。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Automated decision aids: When are they advisors and when do they take control of human decision making?","authors":"Luke Strickland, Russell J Boag, Andrew Heathcote, Vanessa Bowden, Shayne Loft","doi":"10.1037/xap0000463","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000463","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We applied a computational model to examine the extent to which participants used an automated decision aid as an advisor, as compared to a more autonomous trigger of responding, at varying levels of decision aid reliability. In an air traffic control conflict detection task, we found higher accuracy when the decision aid was correct, and more errors when the decision aid was incorrect, as compared to a manual condition (no decision aid). Responses that were correct despite incorrect automated advice were slower than matched manual responses. Decision aids set at lower reliability (75%) had smaller effects on choices and response times, and were subjectively trusted less, than decision aids set at higher reliability (95%). We fitted an evidence accumulation model to choices and response times to measure how information processing was affected by decision aid inputs. Participants primarily treated low-reliability decision aids as an advisor rather than directly accumulating evidence based on its advice. Participants directly accumulated evidence based upon the advice of high-reliability decision aids, consistent with granting decision aids more autonomous influence over decisions. Individual differences in the level of direct accumulation correlated with subjective trust, suggesting a cognitive mechanism by which trust impacts human decisions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"849-868"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10821255","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Understanding implicit bias (UIB): Experimental evaluation of an online bias education program. 了解隐性偏见(UIB):在线偏见教育项目的实验评估。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-04-06 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000469
Carlee Beth Hawkins, Nicole Lofaro, Emily Umansky, Kate A Ratliff

Can people learn about implicit bias through an online course? We developed a brief (∼30 min) online educational program called Understanding Implicit Bias (UIB) consisting of four modules: (a) what is implicit bias? (b) the Implicit Association Test, (c) implicit bias and behavior, and (d) what can you do? In Experiment 1, we randomly assigned 6,729 college students across three separate samples to complete dependent measures before (control group) or after (intervention group) the UIB program. In Experiment 2, we randomly assigned 389 college students to complete the UIB program (intervention group) or two TED talks (control group) before dependent measures. Compared to control groups, the intervention groups had significantly higher objective knowledge about bias (ds = 0.39, 1.49) and subjective knowledge about bias (ds = 1.43, 2.61), awareness of bias (ds = 0.10, 0.54), and behavioral intentions to reduce bias (ds = 0.19, 0.84). These differences were again observed at a 2-week follow-up. These results suggest that brief online education about bias can affect knowledge and awareness of bias, as well as intentions to change behavior. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

人们可以通过在线课程了解内隐偏见吗?我们开发了一个名为 "理解内隐偏见"(Understanding Implicit Bias,UIB)的简短(∼30 分钟)在线教育课程,由四个模块组成:(a)什么是内隐偏见?(b) 内隐关联测试;(c) 内隐偏见与行为;(d) 你能做些什么?在实验 1 中,我们在三个独立样本中随机分配了 6729 名大学生,让他们在 UIB 项目之前(对照组)或之后(干预组)完成依赖性测量。在实验 2 中,我们随机分配了 389 名大学生,让他们在依赖性测量前完成 UIB 计划(干预组)或两次 TED 演讲(对照组)。与对照组相比,干预组的客观偏见知识(ds = 0.39,1.49)、主观偏见知识(ds = 1.43,2.61)、偏见意识(ds = 0.10,0.54)和减少偏见的行为意向(ds = 0.19,0.84)都明显高于对照组。这些差异在两周的随访中再次出现。这些结果表明,有关偏见的简短在线教育可以影响对偏见的了解和认识,以及改变行为的意愿。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Understanding implicit bias (UIB): Experimental evaluation of an online bias education program.","authors":"Carlee Beth Hawkins, Nicole Lofaro, Emily Umansky, Kate A Ratliff","doi":"10.1037/xap0000469","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000469","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Can people learn about implicit bias through an online course? We developed a brief (∼30 min) online educational program called Understanding Implicit Bias (UIB) consisting of four modules: (a) what is implicit bias? (b) the Implicit Association Test, (c) implicit bias and behavior, and (d) what can you do? In Experiment 1, we randomly assigned 6,729 college students across three separate samples to complete dependent measures before (control group) or after (intervention group) the UIB program. In Experiment 2, we randomly assigned 389 college students to complete the UIB program (intervention group) or two TED talks (control group) before dependent measures. Compared to control groups, the intervention groups had significantly higher objective knowledge about bias (<i>d</i>s = 0.39, 1.49) and subjective knowledge about bias (<i>d</i>s = 1.43, 2.61), awareness of bias (<i>d</i>s = 0.10, 0.54), and behavioral intentions to reduce bias (<i>d</i>s = 0.19, 0.84). These differences were again observed at a 2-week follow-up. These results suggest that brief online education about bias can affect knowledge and awareness of bias, as well as intentions to change behavior. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"887-902"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9258368","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
"It was not mentioned": Improving responses to unanswerable questions using retrieval instructions. "没有提到":利用检索指令改进对无法回答的问题的回答。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-04 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000473
Ewa Skopicz-Radkiewicz, Monika Derda, Agnieszka Niedźwieńska

Previous research shows that posing many questions about an event may lead to asking questions about unwitnessed details and that people sometimes provide substantive and erroneous answers to them. Therefore, two experiments investigated the role of the problem-solving and judgment processes, which are unrelated to memory access, in improving responding to unanswerable questions. Experiment 1 compared the effects of a brief retrieval training with the effects of an instruction to increase the criterion of reporting. As expected, the two manipulations had different effects on participants' answers, which demonstrates that training can do more than just instigate more cautious responding. However, we found evidence against our prediction that an enhancement in metacognitive ability underlies improved responding after training. Experiment 2 investigated, for the first time, the role of constant awareness that questions can be unanswerable and that such questions should be rejected. We compared the effects of training with the effects of a small change in response format that ensured such awareness. The effects of the two manipulations were similar, which supports our prediction that the constant awareness of unanswerable questions is a key factor behind improved responding. Practical implications for the eyewitness memory domain are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

以往的研究表明,对某一事件提出许多问题可能会导致人们对未经目击的细节提出问题,而且人们有时会对这些问题做出实质性的错误回答。因此,我们通过两项实验研究了与记忆获取无关的问题解决和判断过程在改善对无法回答的问题的回答中的作用。实验 1 比较了简短检索训练的效果和提高报告标准的指令的效果。不出所料,这两种操作对被试的回答产生了不同的影响,这表明训练不仅仅能促使被试更谨慎地回答问题。不过,我们也发现了与我们的预测相反的证据,即元认知能力的提高是训练后反应能力提高的基础。实验 2 首次研究了不断意识到问题可能无法回答以及应该拒绝这类问题的作用。我们比较了训练的效果和稍微改变回答格式以确保这种意识的效果。两种操作的效果相似,这支持了我们的预测,即持续意识到无法回答的问题是改进回答的关键因素。本研究还讨论了对目击者记忆领域的实际影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"\"It was not mentioned\": Improving responses to unanswerable questions using retrieval instructions.","authors":"Ewa Skopicz-Radkiewicz, Monika Derda, Agnieszka Niedźwieńska","doi":"10.1037/xap0000473","DOIUrl":"10.1037/xap0000473","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous research shows that posing many questions about an event may lead to asking questions about unwitnessed details and that people sometimes provide substantive and erroneous answers to them. Therefore, two experiments investigated the role of the problem-solving and judgment processes, which are unrelated to memory access, in improving responding to unanswerable questions. Experiment 1 compared the effects of a brief retrieval training with the effects of an instruction to increase the criterion of reporting. As expected, the two manipulations had different effects on participants' answers, which demonstrates that training can do more than just instigate more cautious responding. However, we found evidence against our prediction that an enhancement in metacognitive ability underlies improved responding after training. Experiment 2 investigated, for the first time, the role of constant awareness that questions can be unanswerable and that such questions should be rejected. We compared the effects of training with the effects of a small change in response format that ensured such awareness. The effects of the two manipulations were similar, which supports our prediction that the constant awareness of unanswerable questions is a key factor behind improved responding. Practical implications for the eyewitness memory domain are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":" ","pages":"761-781"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9410444","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for Market Mindset Can Increase Allocations in the Trust Game Through Proportional Thinking 市场思维补充材料可以通过比例思维增加信任博弈中的配置
3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2023-11-02 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000499.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Market Mindset Can Increase Allocations in the Trust Game Through Proportional Thinking","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/xap0000499.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000499.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":"51 9","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135874977","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for Weighting Ratings: Are People Adjusting for Bias in Extreme Reviews? 加权评级补充材料:人们是否在极端评价中调整偏差?
3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2023-10-30 DOI: 10.1037/xap0000497.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Weighting Ratings: Are People Adjusting for Bias in Extreme Reviews?","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/xap0000497.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000497.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48003,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied","volume":"65 3","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136018709","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1