首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Behavioral Decision Making最新文献

英文 中文
Correction to The Effect of a Default Nudge on Experienced and Expected Autonomy: A Field Study on Food Donation 更正《默认暗示对经验自主和预期自主的影响》:食物捐赠实地研究
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2411

Weijers, R, Wachner, J and Koning, B (2024), The Effect of a Default Nudge on Experienced and Expected Autonomy: A Field Study on Food Donation. J Behav Dec Making, 37: e2404. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2404

For the Acknowledgments, the following was not included, due to the blinding of the manuscript, which was not reverted by the authors when checking the author proofs:

The Acknowledgments should read as follows:

We would like to thank Marleen Lagendijk, Robin Luiten, Harry Wijsbroek, Sarah Puijk, Naomi Markus, Celeste Seijerlin, Eva Buiskool, Fay Olde Dubbelink, and Irissa Pape for their help with data collection.

We apologize for this error.

Weijers, R, Wachner, J 和 Koning, B (2024),《默认暗示对经验自主和预期自主的影响》:食物捐赠实地研究》。J Behav Dec Making, 37: e2404. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2404 在致谢部分,由于稿件存在盲点,作者在检查作者校样时没有改正,因此没有包括以下内容:致谢部分应为:我们感谢 Marleen Lagendijk、Robin Luiten、Harry Wijsbroek、Sarah Puijk、Naomi Markus、Celeste Seijerlin、Eva Buiskool、Fay Olde Dubbelink 和 Irissa Pape 在数据收集方面提供的帮助。
{"title":"Correction to The Effect of a Default Nudge on Experienced and Expected Autonomy: A Field Study on Food Donation","authors":"","doi":"10.1002/bdm.2411","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2411","url":null,"abstract":"<p>\u0000 <span>Weijers, R</span>, <span>Wachner, J</span> and <span>Koning, B</span> (<span>2024</span>), <span>The Effect of a Default Nudge on Experienced and Expected Autonomy: A Field Study on Food Donation</span>. <i>J Behav Dec Making</i>, <span>37</span>: e2404. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2404\u0000 </p><p>For the Acknowledgments, the following was not included, due to the blinding of the manuscript, which was not reverted by the authors when checking the author proofs:</p><p>The Acknowledgments should read as follows:</p><p>We would like to thank Marleen Lagendijk, Robin Luiten, Harry Wijsbroek, Sarah Puijk, Naomi Markus, Celeste Seijerlin, Eva Buiskool, Fay Olde Dubbelink, and Irissa Pape for their help with data collection.</p><p>We apologize for this error.</p>","PeriodicalId":48112,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/bdm.2411","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142273025","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Equivalence Framing and the Construction of Advocacy Messages 等效框架与宣传信息的构建
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-09-11 DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2409
Jiawei Liu, Douglas M. McLeod, Linqi Lu

Alternative messages that present logically equivalent information, often referred to as equivalence frames, have been shown to influence readers' opinions on public issues. While equivalence framing has been studied in the context of issue advocacy, exhibiting pervasive effects across domains of decision-making, little attention has been paid to whether the general public is able to choose these equivalence frames based on the goal of persuasion. Given that framing effects have important implications on democratic decision-making, this paper reports on experiments that manipulate the strategic goal of policy advocacy (i.e., supporting alternative policy proposals) and ask respondents to select between equivalence frames to enhance the persuasive power of the advocacy toward the specified goal. Findings across three issue topics suggest that for the general adult population, only a small proportion of people were able to select equivalence frames based on the goal of persuasion with most people failing to do so. Also, a follow-up study with a university student sample showed that familiarity with one equivalence frame over the other was a more consistent predictor of equivalence frame use than the goal of advocacy in communicating policy issues.

事实证明,呈现逻辑上等价信息的替代信息(通常称为等价框架)会影响读者对公共问题的看法。虽然等效框架一直是在议题倡导的背景下进行研究的,并在决策的各个领域表现出普遍的效果,但很少有人关注普通大众是否能够根据说服的目标来选择这些等效框架。鉴于框架效应对民主决策具有重要影响,本文报告了一些实验,这些实验操纵了政策倡导的战略目标(即支持替代性政策提案),并要求受访者在等效框架之间进行选择,以增强倡导对特定目标的说服力。对三个议题的研究结果表明,对于普通成年人来说,只有一小部分人能够根据说服目标选择等效框架,而大多数人则无法做到这一点。此外,一项针对大学生样本的后续研究表明,在沟通政策问题时,对一种等效框架的熟悉程度比对另一种等效框架的熟悉程度更能稳定地预测等效框架的使用。
{"title":"Equivalence Framing and the Construction of Advocacy Messages","authors":"Jiawei Liu,&nbsp;Douglas M. McLeod,&nbsp;Linqi Lu","doi":"10.1002/bdm.2409","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2409","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Alternative messages that present logically equivalent information, often referred to as equivalence frames, have been shown to influence readers' opinions on public issues. While equivalence framing has been studied in the context of issue advocacy, exhibiting pervasive effects across domains of decision-making, little attention has been paid to whether the general public is able to choose these equivalence frames based on the goal of persuasion. Given that framing effects have important implications on democratic decision-making, this paper reports on experiments that manipulate the strategic goal of policy advocacy (i.e., supporting alternative policy proposals) and ask respondents to select between equivalence frames to enhance the persuasive power of the advocacy toward the specified goal. Findings across three issue topics suggest that for the general adult population, only a small proportion of people were able to select equivalence frames based on the goal of persuasion with most people failing to do so. Also, a follow-up study with a university student sample showed that familiarity with one equivalence frame over the other was a more consistent predictor of equivalence frame use than the goal of advocacy in communicating policy issues.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48112,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142169874","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Predicting Emotional and Behavioral Reactions to Collective Wrongdoing: Effects of Imagined Versus Experienced Collective Guilt on Moral Behavior 预测对集体不法行为的情绪和行为反应:想象中的集体内疚与经历过的集体内疚对道德行为的影响
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-09-10 DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2410
Fabian Bernhard, Udo Rudolph

Past research has shown that people are inconsistent when making predictions about emotions and moral behaviors following their own wrongdoing. However, it is less clear how people react when they did not cause the wrongdoing themselves but the group or collective they associate with. The present paper investigates people's reactions to collective wrongdoing and the question (1) whether the prediction of the experience of group-based guilt is related to actual moral behaviors and (2) whether this prediction is reliable. In three studies, we analyze collective guilt and subsequent behavioral reactions. Study 1 involved real academic situations, varying the kind of unfair treatment of others. A priori, participants overestimated their own subsequent experiences of collective guilt as well as their moral behavior. With respect to actual responses, experienced guilt was the strongest predictor of behavioral reactions, while imagined guilt, in-group identification and satisfaction did not significantly predict responses. Moreover, participants also reacted more to the direct harm caused by their group to others than to unjustified privileges granted to others. Study 2 fully replicated these results and showed relative stability in the predictions of collective guilt. Study 3 compared the responses by participants of the previous two studies with their responses 5 years later, indicating high stability of the observed effects over time. Also, we observed that making repeated predictions after experiencing the guilt-eliciting situation did not improve the accuracy of our participants' predictions. We discuss the implications of these findings for self-predictions, behavioral and affective forecasting of collective emotions, and for common assessment methods of guilt by hypothetical vignettes.

过去的研究表明,人们在预测自己犯错后的情绪和道德行为时,会出现不一致的情况。然而,当不法行为不是由他们自己造成,而是由他们所联系的群体或集体造成时,人们会做出怎样的反应就不那么清楚了。本文研究了人们对集体不法行为的反应,并探讨了以下问题:(1) 对基于群体的内疚体验的预测是否与实际道德行为相关;(2) 这种预测是否可靠。在三项研究中,我们分析了集体内疚感和随后的行为反应。研究 1 涉及真实的学术情境,改变了对他人不公平待遇的种类。参与者先验地高估了自己随后的集体内疚体验以及道德行为。在实际反应方面,体验到的内疚感对行为反应的预测作用最大,而想象中的内疚感、群体内认同感和满意度对反应的预测作用不大。此外,参与者对其群体对他人造成的直接伤害的反应也比对他人获得的不合理特权的反应更大。研究 2 完全复制了这些结果,并显示了集体内疚感预测的相对稳定性。研究 3 比较了前两次研究中参与者的反应和他们 5 年后的反应,结果表明观察到的效果随着时间的推移具有高度稳定性。此外,我们还观察到,在经历了诱发内疚的情境后,重复预测并不能提高参与者预测的准确性。我们讨论了这些发现对自我预测、集体情绪的行为和情感预测以及通过假设小故事对内疚感进行评估的常用方法的影响。
{"title":"Predicting Emotional and Behavioral Reactions to Collective Wrongdoing: Effects of Imagined Versus Experienced Collective Guilt on Moral Behavior","authors":"Fabian Bernhard,&nbsp;Udo Rudolph","doi":"10.1002/bdm.2410","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2410","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Past research has shown that people are inconsistent when making predictions about emotions and moral behaviors following their own wrongdoing. However, it is less clear how people react when they did not cause the wrongdoing themselves but the group or collective they associate with. The present paper investigates people's reactions to collective wrongdoing and the question (1) whether the prediction of the experience of group-based guilt is related to actual moral behaviors and (2) whether this prediction is reliable. In three studies, we analyze collective guilt and subsequent behavioral reactions. Study 1 involved real academic situations, varying the kind of unfair treatment of others. A priori, participants overestimated their own subsequent experiences of collective guilt as well as their moral behavior. With respect to actual responses, experienced guilt was the strongest predictor of behavioral reactions, while imagined guilt, in-group identification and satisfaction did not significantly predict responses. Moreover, participants also reacted more to the direct harm caused by their group to others than to unjustified privileges granted to others. Study 2 fully replicated these results and showed relative stability in the predictions of collective guilt. Study 3 compared the responses by participants of the previous two studies with their responses 5 years later, indicating high stability of the observed effects over time. Also, we observed that making repeated predictions after experiencing the guilt-eliciting situation did not improve the accuracy of our participants' predictions. We discuss the implications of these findings for self-predictions, behavioral and affective forecasting of collective emotions, and for common assessment methods of guilt by hypothetical vignettes.</p>","PeriodicalId":48112,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/bdm.2410","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142169860","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Relative Importance of the Contrast and Assimilation Effects in Decisions Under Risk 风险决策中对比效应和同化效应的相对重要性
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-08-26 DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2408
Eden Heilprin, Ido Erev

Past research on decisions under risk has documented two contradictory context effects: the contrast effect, where risk preferences in “target” tasks diverge from those in previous “surrounding” tasks, and the assimilation effect that implies the opposite bias. We present four web experiments (three preregistered) that clarify the conditions determining the relative prominence of these opposing effects. Our experiments focus on choice patterns in “target” tasks where participants choose between the status quo and a risky mixed gamble with an expected value of zero. Study 1 examines the impact of surroundings that differ from the target task with respect to the expected benefit from risk-taking. The findings reveal a strong contrast effect: Decreasing the attractiveness of risk-taking in the surrounding tasks increased the risk-taking rate in the target tasks from 53.2% to 79.7%. Study 2 investigates the impact of surroundings that differ in the payoff domain. The findings indicate a strong assimilation effect: Decreasing the attractiveness of risk-taking in the surrounding tasks decreased the risk-taking rate in the target tasks from 74.7% to 36.5%. Additionally, the results revealed unpredicted and robust reversed loss aversion patterns which Studies 3 and 4 further clarify. Our findings (1) suggest that the isolated within-task computations assumed by leading descriptive models overlook substantial contextual considerations, (2) clarify the factors determining the impact of the contrast and assimilation effects in decisions under risk, and (3) provide a theoretical framework for making useful predictions in various scenarios.

过去有关风险决策的研究记录了两种相互矛盾的情境效应:一种是对比效应,即 "目标 "任务中的风险偏好与之前 "周围 "任务中的风险偏好不同;另一种是同化效应,即相反的偏好。我们介绍了四项网络实验(其中三项是预先注册的),这些实验阐明了决定这些对立效应相对突出的条件。我们的实验侧重于 "目标 "任务中的选择模式,即参与者在维持现状和预期值为零的高风险混合赌博之间做出选择。研究 1 考察了与目标任务不同的环境对冒险预期收益的影响。研究结果显示了强烈的对比效应:降低周围环境任务中冒险的吸引力,会使目标任务中的冒险率从 53.2% 提高到 79.7%。研究 2 调查了在报酬领域不同的周围环境的影响。研究结果表明,同化效应很强:降低周围任务中冒险的吸引力会使目标任务中的冒险率从 74.7% 降至 36.5%。此外,研究结果还揭示了无法预测的、稳健的反向损失规避模式,研究 3 和研究 4 进一步阐明了这一点。我们的研究结果(1)表明,主要的描述性模型所假设的孤立的任务内计算忽略了大量的情境因素,(2)澄清了决定风险决策中对比效应和同化效应影响的因素,以及(3)提供了在各种情景下进行有用预测的理论框架。
{"title":"The Relative Importance of the Contrast and Assimilation Effects in Decisions Under Risk","authors":"Eden Heilprin,&nbsp;Ido Erev","doi":"10.1002/bdm.2408","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2408","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Past research on decisions under risk has documented two contradictory context effects: the contrast effect, where risk preferences in “target” tasks diverge from those in previous “surrounding” tasks, and the assimilation effect that implies the opposite bias. We present four web experiments (three preregistered) that clarify the conditions determining the relative prominence of these opposing effects. Our experiments focus on choice patterns in “target” tasks where participants choose between the status quo and a risky mixed gamble with an expected value of zero. Study 1 examines the impact of surroundings that differ from the target task with respect to the expected benefit from risk-taking. The findings reveal a strong contrast effect: <i>Decreasing</i> the attractiveness of risk-taking in the surrounding tasks <i>increased</i> the risk-taking rate in the target tasks from 53.2% to 79.7%. Study 2 investigates the impact of surroundings that differ in the payoff domain. The findings indicate a strong assimilation effect: <i>Decreasing</i> the attractiveness of risk-taking in the surrounding tasks <i>decreased</i> the risk-taking rate in the target tasks from 74.7% to 36.5%. Additionally, the results revealed unpredicted and robust reversed loss aversion patterns which Studies 3 and 4 further clarify. Our findings (1) suggest that the isolated within-task computations assumed by leading descriptive models overlook substantial contextual considerations, (2) clarify the factors determining the impact of the contrast and assimilation effects in decisions under risk, and (3) provide a theoretical framework for making useful predictions in various scenarios.</p>","PeriodicalId":48112,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-08-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/bdm.2408","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142077998","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reference-Dependent Risk-Taking in the NBA NBA 中依赖参照物的冒险行为
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-08-26 DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2407
Daniel Mochon

This paper examines whether risk preferences in the NBA are reference-dependent and attempts to identify the reference point. Using data from 10 NBA seasons (12,890 games), I find that teams are more likely to attempt a riskier three-point shot (vs. a less risky two-point shot) when below the reference point than above it, consistent with Prospect Theory. The results further show that teams are not influenced by a single fixed reference point, but instead, their choices depend on the score difference, most recent score change, and pregame expectations. Additionally, the weight given to the reference point changes over the course of the game. Teams show a breakeven effect, such that they are more likely to attempt a three-point shot when doing so can tie the game. They also show behavior consistent with mental accounting, as the reference point carries more weight at the end of a quarter than at the beginning. These results provide further real-world evidence for reference-dependent risk preferences while highlighting the challenge of applying reference-dependent models to real-world settings.

本文研究了 NBA 的风险偏好是否取决于参考点,并试图确定参考点。利用 10 个 NBA 赛季(12,890 场比赛)的数据,我发现球队在低于参考点时比高于参考点时更有可能尝试风险较高的三分球(相对于风险较低的两分球),这与前景理论是一致的。结果进一步表明,球队并不受单一固定参考点的影响,相反,他们的选择取决于比分差距、最近的比分变化和赛前预期。此外,参考点的权重在比赛过程中也会发生变化。球队表现出一种收支平衡效应,即如果投三分球可以扳平比分,那么他们就更有可能尝试投三分球。他们还表现出与心理核算一致的行为,因为参考点在一节结束时比在开始时具有更大的权重。这些结果为依赖参考点的风险偏好提供了进一步的现实证据,同时也凸显了将依赖参考点的模型应用于现实环境所面临的挑战。
{"title":"Reference-Dependent Risk-Taking in the NBA","authors":"Daniel Mochon","doi":"10.1002/bdm.2407","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2407","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This paper examines whether risk preferences in the NBA are reference-dependent and attempts to identify the reference point. Using data from 10 NBA seasons (12,890 games), I find that teams are more likely to attempt a riskier three-point shot (vs. a less risky two-point shot) when below the reference point than above it, consistent with Prospect Theory. The results further show that teams are not influenced by a single fixed reference point, but instead, their choices depend on the score difference, most recent score change, and pregame expectations. Additionally, the weight given to the reference point changes over the course of the game. Teams show a breakeven effect, such that they are more likely to attempt a three-point shot when doing so can tie the game. They also show behavior consistent with mental accounting, as the reference point carries more weight at the end of a quarter than at the beginning. These results provide further real-world evidence for reference-dependent risk preferences while highlighting the challenge of applying reference-dependent models to real-world settings.</p>","PeriodicalId":48112,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-08-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/bdm.2407","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142077997","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Progress Decisions Involving Time: Sunk Cost or Completion Effects 涉及时间的进度决策:沉没成本或完工效应
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-08-18 DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2405
Todd J. Thorsteinson, Christian G. Billings

Five studies involving seven samples were conducted to examine the effects of sunk time in progress decisions. Previous research on sunk time in progress decisions has failed to control for completion effects. Studies 1a and 1b found strong evidence for completion effects and weaker effects for sunk time effects on probability of continuing. In Studies 2a and 2b, we expanded our sunk time scenarios to use multiple types. We found evidence for sunk time and completion effects on probability of continuing. An additional analysis, however, found that the sunk time effect was larger when the progress decision involved a goal focused on the accomplishment of a project (e.g., writing a paper for class), as opposed to a goal focused on enjoyment (e.g., watching a television show). Study 3 found that both sunk time and completion effects were present in progress decisions focused on the accomplishment of a project. These effects were present even when participants were informed that there was sufficient time to complete an alternative project. Study 4 failed to find a sunk time effect in a behavioral study, and Study 5 replicated Study 4 using a vignette format. Overall, these results provide some evidence of sunk time effects in progress decisions, but the effect appears to be small and may be due, at least partially, to completion effects.

我们进行了五项研究,涉及七个样本,以考察沉没时间对进度决策的影响。以往关于沉没时间对进展决策的影响的研究未能控制完成效应。研究 1a 和 1b 发现了完成效应的有力证据,而沉没时间对继续决策概率的影响较弱。在研究 2a 和 2b 中,我们扩展了沉没时间情景,使用了多种类型。我们发现了沉没时间和完成效应对继续学习概率的影响。然而,额外的分析发现,当进度决策涉及的目标侧重于完成一个项目(如为课堂撰写论文),而不是侧重于享受(如观看电视节目)时,沉没时间效应更大。研究 3 发现,沉没时间效应和完成效应都存在于以完成项目为重点的进度决策中。即使参与者被告知有足够的时间完成另一个项目,这些效应仍然存在。研究 4 未能在行为研究中发现沉没时间效应,研究 5 采用小故事的形式重复了研究 4。总之,这些结果提供了一些证据,证明在进度决策中存在沉没时间效应,但这种效应似乎很小,而且可能至少部分是由于完成效应造成的。
{"title":"Progress Decisions Involving Time: Sunk Cost or Completion Effects","authors":"Todd J. Thorsteinson,&nbsp;Christian G. Billings","doi":"10.1002/bdm.2405","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2405","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Five studies involving seven samples were conducted to examine the effects of sunk time in progress decisions. Previous research on sunk time in progress decisions has failed to control for completion effects. Studies 1a and 1b found strong evidence for completion effects and weaker effects for sunk time effects on probability of continuing. In Studies 2a and 2b, we expanded our sunk time scenarios to use multiple types. We found evidence for sunk time and completion effects on probability of continuing. An additional analysis, however, found that the sunk time effect was larger when the progress decision involved a goal focused on the accomplishment of a project (e.g., writing a paper for class), as opposed to a goal focused on enjoyment (e.g., watching a television show). Study 3 found that both sunk time and completion effects were present in progress decisions focused on the accomplishment of a project. These effects were present even when participants were informed that there was sufficient time to complete an alternative project. Study 4 failed to find a sunk time effect in a behavioral study, and Study 5 replicated Study 4 using a vignette format. Overall, these results provide some evidence of sunk time effects in progress decisions, but the effect appears to be small and may be due, at least partially, to completion effects.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48112,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-08-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142002577","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Clean Slate: Adapting the Realization Effect to Online Gambling and Its Effectiveness in People With Gambling Problems 一片净土:将 "实现效应 "适用于网络赌博及其对有赌博问题的人的有效性
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-08-18 DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2406
Ke Zhang, Alex Imas, Luke Clark

Betting more after losses (i.e., “loss-chasing”) is a central clinical feature of disordered gambling. According to prospect theory, increasing risk-seeking following losses could arise from a failure to “re-reference.” By contrast, successful re-referencing between successive decisions closes the mental account, and any losses are regarded as final or realized; gamblers should not chase realized losses. The present study sought to test this “realization effect” among gamblers using an ecologically valid online gambling task. We were further interested in whether the effectiveness of the loss realization varied as a function of problem gambling severity. Using online recruitment of past-year gamblers stratified on the Problem Gambling Severity Index, we tested a group without gambling problems (n = 227), a group with at-risk gambling (n = 239), and a group with gambling problems (n = 223). Over a sequence of nine bets, after the sixth bet, half of the participants underwent a simulated realization procedure that entailed cashing out from the gambling website and redepositing their remaining funds on another website. The feedback comparison group were shown their account balance after the sixth bet but did not withdraw or transfer their funds. In line with the realization effect, the group with non-problem gambling significantly reduced their bet after cashing out. The realization procedure did not significantly ameliorate loss-chasing in the groups with at-risk gambling or gambling problems. We conclude that the realization effect can be elicited in an online gambling context but that stronger interventions for realizing losses may be required for people experiencing gambling problems.

在输钱之后下更多的赌注(即 "追逐损失")是赌博失调的一个核心临床特征。根据前景理论,输钱后寻求更多风险可能是由于 "重新参照 "失败所致。与此相反,在连续决策之间成功地重新参照会关闭心理账户,任何损失都会被视为最终或已实现的损失;赌徒不应该追逐已实现的损失。本研究试图通过一项生态学上有效的在线赌博任务来检验赌徒的这种 "实现效应"。我们还想进一步了解,损失实现的效果是否会随着赌博问题的严重程度而变化。我们在网上招募了根据问题赌博严重程度指数分层的上一年赌徒,对无赌博问题组(n = 227)、有赌博风险组(n = 239)和有赌博问题组(n = 223)进行了测试。在一连九次下注中,第六次下注后,半数参与者进行了模拟变现程序,即从赌博网站提现,并将剩余资金转存到另一个网站。反馈对比组则在第六次下注后显示其账户余额,但不提取或转移资金。与 "意识到 "效应一致的是,无问题赌博组在提现后明显减少了赌注。在有赌博风险或赌博问题的组别中,变现程序并没有明显改善追逐损失的情况。我们的结论是,实现效应可以在网络赌博中产生,但对于有赌博问题的人来说,可能需要更有力的干预措施来实现损失。
{"title":"A Clean Slate: Adapting the Realization Effect to Online Gambling and Its Effectiveness in People With Gambling Problems","authors":"Ke Zhang,&nbsp;Alex Imas,&nbsp;Luke Clark","doi":"10.1002/bdm.2406","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2406","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Betting more after losses (i.e., “loss-chasing”) is a central clinical feature of disordered gambling. According to prospect theory, increasing risk-seeking following losses could arise from a failure to “re-reference.” By contrast, successful re-referencing between successive decisions closes the mental account, and any losses are regarded as final or <i>realized</i>; gamblers should not chase realized losses. The present study sought to test this “realization effect” among gamblers using an ecologically valid online gambling task. We were further interested in whether the effectiveness of the loss realization varied as a function of problem gambling severity. Using online recruitment of past-year gamblers stratified on the Problem Gambling Severity Index, we tested a group without gambling problems (<i>n</i> = 227), a group with at-risk gambling (<i>n</i> = 239), and a group with gambling problems (<i>n</i> = 223). Over a sequence of nine bets, after the sixth bet, half of the participants underwent a simulated realization procedure that entailed cashing out from the gambling website and redepositing their remaining funds on another website. The feedback comparison group were shown their account balance after the sixth bet but did not withdraw or transfer their funds. In line with the realization effect, the group with non-problem gambling significantly reduced their bet after cashing out. The realization procedure did not significantly ameliorate loss-chasing in the groups with at-risk gambling or gambling problems. We conclude that the realization effect can be elicited in an online gambling context but that stronger interventions for realizing losses may be required for people experiencing gambling problems.</p>","PeriodicalId":48112,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-08-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/bdm.2406","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142002591","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Choice Bolstering Changes Attribute Importance and Affects Future Choices 选择支撑改变属性重要性并影响未来选择
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-08-08 DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2401
Martin R. Zemborain, Gita Venkataramani Johar, Anne L. Roggeveen, Asim Ansari

Prior research has established that decision-makers engage in a bolstering process by magnifying the value of a previously made choice in order to justify their choices. The current research examines the impact of bolstering on attribute importance weights and future choices. Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrate that after making a choice between a priori comparable options, consumers prefer attributes (e.g., sunlight in an apartment) that they associate with positive features of their chosen option (lot of sunlight) more than attributes they associate (a) with negative features of their chosen option or (b) with positive features of their nonchosen option. These postchoice associations and altered attribute importance weights drive subsequent preferences. In an incentive-compatible study, Experiment 3 demonstrates that bolstering goes beyond choice justification and impacts subsequent choice. After an initial choice, participants choose new products that have positive features consistent with their original chosen option rather than products with positive features consistent with their original nonchosen option. This research contributes to the literature on preference construction by examining the impact of justification for one's previous choices on constructed attribute preferences and subsequent choices.

先前的研究已经证实,决策者会通过放大先前所做选择的价值来证明自己的选择是正确的,从而参与支持过程。当前的研究探讨了 "支持 "对属性重要性权重和未来选择的影响。实验 1 和 2 表明,在对先验可比选项做出选择后,消费者更偏好与所选选项(大量阳光)的积极特征相关联的属性(如公寓中的阳光),而不是与(a)所选选项的消极特征或(b)非所选选项的积极特征相关联的属性。这些选择后的联想和属性重要性权重的改变推动了随后的偏好。在一项与激励相容的研究中,实验 3 证明了支持超出了选择理由的范围,并影响了后续选择。在最初的选择之后,参与者会选择具有与其最初选择一致的积极特征的新产品,而不是具有与其最初非选择一致的积极特征的产品。这项研究通过考察一个人之前选择的合理性对构建属性偏好和后续选择的影响,为偏好构建方面的文献做出了贡献。
{"title":"Choice Bolstering Changes Attribute Importance and Affects Future Choices","authors":"Martin R. Zemborain,&nbsp;Gita Venkataramani Johar,&nbsp;Anne L. Roggeveen,&nbsp;Asim Ansari","doi":"10.1002/bdm.2401","DOIUrl":"10.1002/bdm.2401","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Prior research has established that decision-makers engage in a bolstering process by magnifying the value of a previously made choice in order to justify their choices. The current research examines the impact of bolstering on attribute importance weights and future choices. Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrate that after making a choice between a priori comparable options, consumers prefer attributes (e.g., sunlight in an apartment) that they associate with positive features of their chosen option (lot of sunlight) more than attributes they associate (a) with negative features of their chosen option or (b) with positive features of their nonchosen option. These postchoice associations and altered attribute importance weights drive subsequent preferences. In an incentive-compatible study, Experiment 3 demonstrates that bolstering goes beyond choice justification and impacts subsequent choice. After an initial choice, participants choose new products that have positive features consistent with their original chosen option rather than products with positive features consistent with their original nonchosen option. This research contributes to the literature on preference construction by examining the impact of justification for one's previous choices on constructed attribute preferences and subsequent choices.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48112,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-08-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141926838","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do We Really Believe That “More Is Better”? Mapping Implicit and Explicit Associations Between Quantity and Quality 我们真的相信 "多多益善 "吗?绘制数量与质量之间的隐性和显性关联图
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-08-02 DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2403
Mariana Vences, Filipe Loureiro, Teresa Garcia-Marques

The dimensions of quantity and quality play a crucial role in shaping our judgments and decisions. How these dimensions are perceived in relation to each other is of extreme importance when it comes to those decisions and judgments. The presumed positive link between them, embodied in the notion that “the more, the better,” is a common thread in decision-making. However, owing to the diverse contexts within which decisions unfold, individuals appear to acquire the understanding that such relationship is not universally applicable, leading to profess the belief that “quantity is not quality.” This dichotomy establishes a dissociation between the implicit and explicit associations formed regarding the connection between quantity and quality. In two studies, we put this hypothesis to the test. Initially, we explore the nature of this association through an Implicit Association Test (Study 1), followed by an investigation into the modulation of this association within an ecological context (Study 2). The results show that, explicitly, participants assert no inherent relationship between quantity and quality. However, at an implicit level, with consequential impacts on behavior, a robust positive association between quantity and quality persists, providing challenging to overturn.

数量和质量对我们的判断和决策起着至关重要的作用。如何看待这两个维度之间的关系,对我们的决策和判断极为重要。在决策过程中,"越多越好 "这一概念所体现的两者之间的积极联系是一条共同的主线。然而,由于决策所处的环境各不相同,个人似乎逐渐认识到这种关系并非放之四海而皆准,从而宣称 "数量不代表质量"。这种二分法在数量和质量之间形成的隐性和显性联想之间建立了一种分离。在两项研究中,我们对这一假设进行了验证。首先,我们通过内隐联想测试(研究 1)来探索这种联想的性质,然后在生态背景下调查这种联想的调节情况(研究 2)。研究结果表明,在显性层面上,参与者认为数量和质量之间没有内在联系。然而,在内隐层面上,随着对行为的影响,数量和质量之间持续存在着强有力的正关联,这给推翻这种关联带来了挑战。
{"title":"Do We Really Believe That “More Is Better”? Mapping Implicit and Explicit Associations Between Quantity and Quality","authors":"Mariana Vences,&nbsp;Filipe Loureiro,&nbsp;Teresa Garcia-Marques","doi":"10.1002/bdm.2403","DOIUrl":"10.1002/bdm.2403","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>The dimensions of quantity and quality play a crucial role in shaping our judgments and decisions. How these dimensions are perceived in relation to each other is of extreme importance when it comes to those decisions and judgments. The presumed positive link between them, embodied in the notion that “the more, the better,” is a common thread in decision-making. However, owing to the diverse contexts within which decisions unfold, individuals appear to acquire the understanding that such relationship is not universally applicable, leading to profess the belief that “quantity is not quality.” This dichotomy establishes a dissociation between the implicit and explicit associations formed regarding the connection between quantity and quality. In two studies, we put this hypothesis to the test. Initially, we explore the nature of this association through an Implicit Association Test (Study 1), followed by an investigation into the modulation of this association within an ecological context (Study 2). The results show that, explicitly, participants assert no inherent relationship between quantity and quality. However, at an implicit level, with consequential impacts on behavior, a robust positive association between quantity and quality persists, providing challenging to overturn.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48112,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-08-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141939770","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Effect of a Default Nudge on Experienced and Expected Autonomy: A Field Study on Food Donation 默认暗示对经验自主和预期自主的影响:食物捐赠实地研究
IF 1.8 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, APPLIED Pub Date : 2024-08-02 DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2404
Robert J. Weijers, Jonas Wachner, Björn B. de Koning

Default nudges—making the desired option the standard option—are often criticized for hampering autonomy. However, laboratory research suggests this expectation of autonomy loss is not reflected in nudgees' experienced autonomy. In this study, we investigated whether this finding translates to a real-world setting by implementing a default nudge to increase food donations in the supermarket (Experiment 1). Upon entering the supermarket, customers were asked to donate a food item for charity and were handed a shopping cart/basket that was empty (control) or contained a food item meant for donation (default nudge). Donations were collected after checkout, and customers rated their experienced autonomy of their choice to donate. We replicated this study and added a vignette condition portraying the default nudge to measure expected autonomy for the same situation (Experiment 2). The results show the default nudge reduced participants' expected autonomy but not their experienced autonomy, shedding new light on the relation between nudging and autonomy. Furthermore, this novel form of the default nudge was found to be successful in increasing food donations in the supermarket, further expanding the field where this nudge has been successful.

默认推断使期望选项成为标准选项,常常被批评为妨碍自主性。然而,实验室研究表明,这种自主性丧失的预期并没有反映在被劝导者的自主性体验中。在本研究中,我们通过在超市中实施增加食物捐赠的默认劝导(实验 1),来研究这一发现是否可以转化到现实世界中。进入超市后,顾客被要求为慈善机构捐赠一件食品,并被递给一个空的购物车/篮子(对照组)或装有捐赠食品的购物车/篮子(默认暗示)。结账后收集捐赠物品,顾客对其选择捐赠的自主性进行评分。我们重复了这一研究,并增加了一个描述默认劝诱的小故事,以测量相同情况下的预期自主性(实验 2)。结果显示,默认暗示降低了参与者的预期自主性,但没有降低他们的体验自主性,这为我们揭示暗示与自主性之间的关系提供了新的线索。此外,实验还发现这种新颖形式的默认暗示成功地增加了超市中的食物捐赠,进一步拓展了这种暗示的成功领域。
{"title":"The Effect of a Default Nudge on Experienced and Expected Autonomy: A Field Study on Food Donation","authors":"Robert J. Weijers,&nbsp;Jonas Wachner,&nbsp;Björn B. de Koning","doi":"10.1002/bdm.2404","DOIUrl":"10.1002/bdm.2404","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Default nudges—making the desired option the standard option—are often criticized for hampering autonomy. However, laboratory research suggests this expectation of autonomy loss is not reflected in nudgees' experienced autonomy. In this study, we investigated whether this finding translates to a real-world setting by implementing a default nudge to increase food donations in the supermarket (Experiment 1). Upon entering the supermarket, customers were asked to donate a food item for charity and were handed a shopping cart/basket that was empty (control) or contained a food item meant for donation (default nudge). Donations were collected after checkout, and customers rated their experienced autonomy of their choice to donate. We replicated this study and added a vignette condition portraying the default nudge to measure expected autonomy for the same situation (Experiment 2). The results show the default nudge reduced participants' <i>expected</i> autonomy but not their <i>experienced</i> autonomy, shedding new light on the relation between nudging and autonomy. Furthermore, this novel form of the default nudge was found to be successful in increasing food donations in the supermarket, further expanding the field where this nudge has been successful.</p>","PeriodicalId":48112,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-08-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/bdm.2404","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141939769","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Behavioral Decision Making
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1