首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Behavioral Decision Making最新文献

英文 中文
Collaboration during the diagnostic decision-making process: When does it help? 诊断决策过程中的合作:何时有帮助?
IF 2 3区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-09-30 DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2357
Juliane E. Kämmer, Karin Ernst, Kim Grab, Stefan K. Schauber, Stefanie C. Hautz, Dorothea Penders, Wolf E. Hautz

When making complex decisions, such as a medical diagnosis, decision makers typically gather, analyze, and synthesize (integrate) information. In a previous study, we showed that delegating such complex decisions to collaborating pairs increases decision quality substantially compared to that of individuals, without requiring different information gathering. Given the higher costs associated with teamwork, however, it is of great practical interest to understand when in the process the performance benefits of teams may arise, so that particular subtasks can be delegated to teams when most appropriate. We thus conducted an experimental study in which fourth-year medical students (n = 109) worked either in pairs or alone on two separate subtasks of the diagnostic process: (1) analyzing diagnostic test results (e.g., X-rays) and (2) integrating previously interpreted test results into diagnoses. Linear mixed-effects models revealed a small benefit of collaborating pairs over individuals in both subtasks. We conclude that collaborating with a peer may pay off both when analyzing information and when integrating it into a diagnosis as it provides the opportunity to correct each other's errors and to make use of a greater knowledge base. These findings encourage the strategic use of collaboration with a colleague when making complex decisions. Further research into the underlying processes is needed.

在做出复杂决策(如医疗诊断)时,决策者通常需要收集、分析和综合(整合)信息。在之前的一项研究中,我们发现,与个人相比,将此类复杂决策委托给合作对子会大大提高决策质量,而不需要收集不同的信息。然而,考虑到团队合作的成本较高,了解团队在整个过程中何时会产生绩效优势,从而在最合适的时候将特定的子任务委托给团队,是非常有实际意义的。因此,我们进行了一项实验研究,让四年级医学生(n = 109)结对或单独完成诊断过程中的两个独立子任务:(1)分析诊断测试结果(如 X 光片)和(2)将先前解释的测试结果整合到诊断中。线性混合效应模型显示,在这两项子任务中,两人合作比单人合作略胜一筹。我们的结论是,在分析信息和将信息整合到诊断中时,与同伴合作可能会带来回报,因为这提供了纠正彼此错误和利用更多知识库的机会。这些发现鼓励人们在做出复杂决策时,战略性地利用与同事的合作。我们需要进一步研究其基本过程。
{"title":"Collaboration during the diagnostic decision-making process: When does it help?","authors":"Juliane E. Kämmer,&nbsp;Karin Ernst,&nbsp;Kim Grab,&nbsp;Stefan K. Schauber,&nbsp;Stefanie C. Hautz,&nbsp;Dorothea Penders,&nbsp;Wolf E. Hautz","doi":"10.1002/bdm.2357","DOIUrl":"10.1002/bdm.2357","url":null,"abstract":"<p>When making complex decisions, such as a medical diagnosis, decision makers typically gather, analyze, and synthesize (integrate) information. In a previous study, we showed that delegating such complex decisions to collaborating pairs increases decision quality substantially compared to that of individuals, without requiring different information gathering. Given the higher costs associated with teamwork, however, it is of great practical interest to understand when in the process the performance benefits of teams may arise, so that particular subtasks can be delegated to teams when most appropriate. We thus conducted an experimental study in which fourth-year medical students (<i>n</i> = 109) worked either in pairs or alone on two separate subtasks of the diagnostic process: (1) analyzing diagnostic test results (e.g., X-rays) and (2) integrating previously interpreted test results into diagnoses. Linear mixed-effects models revealed a small benefit of collaborating pairs over individuals in both subtasks. We conclude that collaborating with a peer may pay off both when analyzing information <i>and</i> when integrating it into a diagnosis as it provides the opportunity to correct each other's errors and to make use of a greater knowledge base. These findings encourage the strategic use of collaboration with a colleague when making complex decisions. Further research into the underlying processes is needed.</p>","PeriodicalId":48112,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/bdm.2357","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136337085","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of time ambiguity on choice depends on delay and amount magnitude 时间模糊性对选择的影响取决于延迟和数量大小
IF 2 3区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-09-29 DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2354
Iris Ikink, Karin Roelofs, Bernd Figner

Time ambiguity—that is, having partially/fully incomplete information about when an outcome will occur—is common in everyday life. A recent study showed that participants preferred options with time-exact delays over options with time-ambiguous delays, a phenomenon they called time-ambiguity aversion. However, the empirical robustness and boundaries of this phenomenon remain unexplored. We conducted three online studies: Study 2 (n = 118) was a replication of Study 1 (n = 76) using preregistered analyses; Study 3 (n = 202; preregistered) was a follow-up study suggested during review. In Studies 1 and 2, participants completed hypothetical choices between €5 today versus later-but-larger (LL) rewards that systematically varied in their amount, delay, and time-ambiguity level (e.g., for a 180 day delay, time ambiguity varied from 179 to 181 to 0–360 days). Effects of time ambiguity on choice were best encoded in an absolute, dose-dependent manner and depended on delays and amounts: Increasing time ambiguity led to more time-exact LL choices at shorter delays but more time-ambiguous LL choices at longer delays. Additionally, time-ambiguity ranges including today were chosen more frequently than ranges excluding today, akin to the present bias in intertemporal choice. Lastly, evidence suggested that more time ambiguity was preferred for smaller LL amounts yet disliked for larger LL amounts. Study 3 demonstrated that time-risk and time-ambiguity preferences are differentiable by giving participants choices involving hypothetical time-exact, time-ambiguous, and time-risky options. Taken together, our results extend the nascent literature on time ambiguity by showing that (i) time-ambiguity preferences are distinguishable from both time-risk and delay preferences and (ii) time ambiguity is not generally aversive, but its impact depends on delay and amount magnitude.

在日常生活中,时间不确定性--即关于结果何时发生的信息部分/完全不完整--是很常见的。最近的一项研究表明,与时间模糊的延迟相比,参与者更喜欢时间精确的延迟选项,他们把这种现象称为时间模糊厌恶。然而,这种现象在经验上的稳健性和界限仍有待探索。我们进行了三项在线研究:研究 2(n = 118)是对研究 1(n = 76)的复制,使用了预先登记的分析方法;研究 3(n = 202;预先登记)是在审查过程中建议的后续研究。在研究 1 和研究 2 中,参与者在今天的 5 欧元奖励和稍后但更大金额(LL)奖励之间完成假设选择,这些奖励在金额、延迟时间和时间模糊性水平(例如,延迟 180 天,时间模糊性从 179 天到 181 天到 0-360 天不等)方面存在系统性差异。时间模糊性对选择的影响最好以绝对的、剂量依赖的方式进行编码,并且取决于延迟时间和数量:时间模糊性的增加会导致在较短的延迟时间内做出更多时间精确的 LL 选择,但在较长的延迟时间内做出更多时间模糊的 LL 选择。此外,包含今天的时间模糊范围比不包含今天的时间模糊范围更常被选择,这类似于时际选择中的现在偏差。最后,有证据表明,对于较小的 LL 金额,人们更喜欢时间模糊性,而对于较大的 LL 金额,人们则不喜欢时间模糊性。研究 3 通过让参与者选择假定的时间精确、时间模糊和时间风险选项,证明了时间风险和时间模糊偏好是可以区分的。综上所述,我们的研究结果表明:(i) 时间不确定性偏好与时间风险偏好和延迟偏好是可以区分的;(ii) 时间不确定性一般不具有厌恶性,但其影响取决于延迟和金额大小。
{"title":"The effect of time ambiguity on choice depends on delay and amount magnitude","authors":"Iris Ikink,&nbsp;Karin Roelofs,&nbsp;Bernd Figner","doi":"10.1002/bdm.2354","DOIUrl":"10.1002/bdm.2354","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Time ambiguity—that is, having partially/fully incomplete information about <i>when</i> an outcome will occur—is common in everyday life. A recent study showed that participants preferred options with time-exact delays over options with time-ambiguous delays, a phenomenon they called time-ambiguity aversion. However, the empirical robustness and boundaries of this phenomenon remain unexplored. We conducted three online studies: Study 2 (<i>n</i> = 118) was a replication of Study 1 (<i>n</i> = 76) using preregistered analyses; Study 3 (<i>n</i> = 202; preregistered) was a follow-up study suggested during review. In Studies 1 and 2, participants completed hypothetical choices between €5 today versus later-but-larger (LL) rewards that systematically varied in their amount, delay, and time-ambiguity level (e.g., for a 180 day delay, time ambiguity varied from 179 to 181 to 0–360 days). Effects of time ambiguity on choice were best encoded in an absolute, dose-dependent manner and depended on delays and amounts: Increasing time ambiguity led to more time-<i>exact</i> LL choices at shorter delays but more time-<i>ambiguous</i> LL choices at longer delays. Additionally, time-ambiguity ranges including today were chosen more frequently than ranges excluding today, akin to the present bias in intertemporal choice. Lastly, evidence suggested that more time ambiguity was preferred for smaller LL amounts yet disliked for larger LL amounts. Study 3 demonstrated that time-risk and time-ambiguity preferences are differentiable by giving participants choices involving hypothetical time-exact, time-ambiguous, and time-risky options. Taken together, our results extend the nascent literature on time ambiguity by showing that (i) time-ambiguity preferences are distinguishable from both time-risk and delay preferences and (ii) time ambiguity is not generally aversive, but its impact depends on delay and amount magnitude.</p>","PeriodicalId":48112,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/bdm.2354","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135199859","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of the 1-in-X numerical format on choices 1-X 数字格式对选择的影响
IF 2 3区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-09-20 DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2355
Stefania Pighin, Alessandro Bogani, Gloria Berenisse Castro Davalos, Lucia Savadori

The 1-in-X numerical format (e.g., 1 in 200) has been found to increase subjective probability evaluations and behavioral intentions in hypothetical scenarios compared with the N-in-NX format (e.g., 5 in 1000). However, it remains unclear whether this format can also bias choices between truly incentivized options. In four online studies (N = 1039), participants were presented with a small endowment (i.e., 1£) and an actual choice between two options: a sure loss of a part of such endowment and a lottery with the chance to lose the entire endowment, presented using either the 1-in-X or the N-in-NX format. In Studies 1–3, where the two options were equivalent in expected monetary value (EV) and the lottery was described with varying degrees of concreteness, participants preferred the lottery option to a lesser extent when the chance of losing the endowment was presented using the 1-in-X format compared with the N-in-NX format. The same effect was replicated in Study 4a when the lottery option had a higher EV than the sure loss, showing that the 1-in-X effect can also lead individuals to deviate from maximizing EV. However, the effect vanished in Study 4b when the difference in EV between the two options increased. Implications for risk communication and a possible interpretation of the results are discussed accordingly.

与 N-in-NX 格式(如 1000 分之 5)相比,1-in-X 数字格式(如 200 分之 1)被认为会提高假设情景中的主观概率评估和行为意向。然而,目前还不清楚这种形式是否也会在真正的激励选项之间产生偏差。在四项在线研究(N = 1039)中,参与者会得到一笔小额捐赠(即 1 英镑),并在两个选项中做出实际选择:一个是肯定会失去部分捐赠,另一个是有机会失去全部捐赠的彩票,这两个选项均采用 1 进 X 或 N 进 NX 的形式呈现。在研究 1-3 中,两个选项的预期货币价值(EV)相等,彩票的描述也有不同程度的具体化,与 N 进 NX 的形式相比,当采用 1 进 X 的形式呈现失去捐赠的机会时,参与者对彩票选项的偏好程度较低。在研究 4a 中,当彩票选项的 EV 值高于确定损失的 EV 值时,也出现了同样的效应,这表明 1-in-X 效应也会导致个体偏离 EV 值最大化。然而,在研究 4b 中,当两个选项之间的 EV 差值增大时,这种效应消失了。因此,我们讨论了风险交流的意义以及对结果的可能解释。
{"title":"The effect of the 1-in-X numerical format on choices","authors":"Stefania Pighin,&nbsp;Alessandro Bogani,&nbsp;Gloria Berenisse Castro Davalos,&nbsp;Lucia Savadori","doi":"10.1002/bdm.2355","DOIUrl":"10.1002/bdm.2355","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The 1-in-X numerical format (e.g., 1 in 200) has been found to increase subjective probability evaluations and behavioral intentions in hypothetical scenarios compared with the N-in-NX format (e.g., 5 in 1000). However, it remains unclear whether this format can also bias choices between truly incentivized options. In four online studies (<i>N =</i> 1039), participants were presented with a small endowment (i.e., 1£) and an actual choice between two options: a sure loss of a part of such endowment and a lottery with the chance to lose the entire endowment, presented using either the 1-in-X or the N-in-NX format. In Studies 1–3, where the two options were equivalent in expected monetary value (EV) and the lottery was described with varying degrees of concreteness, participants preferred the lottery option to a lesser extent when the chance of losing the endowment was presented using the 1-in-X format compared with the N-in-NX format. The same effect was replicated in Study 4a when the lottery option had a higher EV than the sure loss, showing that the 1-in-X effect can also lead individuals to deviate from maximizing EV. However, the effect vanished in Study 4b when the difference in EV between the two options increased. Implications for risk communication and a possible interpretation of the results are discussed accordingly.</p>","PeriodicalId":48112,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/bdm.2355","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136313549","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Poor sleep quality and stress differentially predict delay discounting for food, but not money, in college students 睡眠质量差和压力对大学生食物延迟折扣的预测不同,而对金钱的预测则不然
IF 2 3区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-09-19 DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2353
Olivia Law, Erin B. Rasmussen

Early college is a time when eating habits change and subsequent weight gain may occur. Moreover, college students report higher stress levels and poorer sleep quality while enrolled in courses. This study investigated the extent to which stress and sleep quality in college students may be related to delay discounting (DD) for food—a psychological process in which immediate outcomes are preferred over larger, more delayed outcomes. College students (N = 297) completed the Food Choice Questionnaire (FCQ) and the Monetary Choice Questionnaire (MCQ)—measures of food and monetary DD, respectively. The Perceived Stress Scale (PSS), the Pittsburg Sleep Quality Index (PSQI), and measures of subjective hunger, substance use, and demographic variables were also administered. Perceived stress was related to poor sleep quality, alcohol use, substance use, and vaping. Analyses revealed that, when controlling for subjective hunger, perceived stress and poor sleep quality contributed unique variance to food DD, though in opposing directions. Perceived stress uniquely predicted preferences for immediate food, a phenomenon consistent with stress-induced urgent eating. Poor sleep quality uniquely predicted preferences for larger amounts of delayed food, a pattern consistent with eating later in the day. Stress and sleep quality, when controlling for substance use variables, were unrelated to monetary discounting. Stress and poor sleep quality, then, predict independent and opposing discounting processes in college students that are food-specific, as opposed to more general cross-commodity processes.

大学初期是饮食习惯改变的时期,体重可能会随之增加。此外,大学生在学习期间的压力水平较高,睡眠质量较差。本研究调查了大学生的压力和睡眠质量在多大程度上可能与食物的延迟折现(DD)有关--延迟折现是一种心理过程,在这种心理过程中,即期结果优于更大、更延迟的结果。大学生(N = 297)分别完成了食物选择问卷(FCQ)和货币选择问卷(MCQ)--食物和货币延迟折现的测量。此外,还进行了感知压力量表(PSS)、匹兹堡睡眠质量指数(PSQI)以及主观饥饿感、药物使用和人口统计学变量的测量。感知压力与睡眠质量差、饮酒、使用药物和吸食毒品有关。分析表明,在控制主观饥饿感的情况下,感知到的压力和睡眠质量差会对食物摄入量产生独特的影响,但两者的影响方向相反。感知到的压力独特地预测了对即时食物的偏好,这种现象与压力引起的紧急进食一致。睡眠质量差则独特地预测了对大量延迟食物的偏好,这种模式与一天中晚些时候进食是一致的。在控制物质使用变量的情况下,压力和睡眠质量与货币贴现无关。因此,压力和睡眠质量差预示着大学生的独立和相反的贴现过程,这种过程是特定于食物的,而不是更普遍的跨商品过程。
{"title":"Poor sleep quality and stress differentially predict delay discounting for food, but not money, in college students","authors":"Olivia Law,&nbsp;Erin B. Rasmussen","doi":"10.1002/bdm.2353","DOIUrl":"10.1002/bdm.2353","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Early college is a time when eating habits change and subsequent weight gain may occur. Moreover, college students report higher stress levels and poorer sleep quality while enrolled in courses. This study investigated the extent to which stress and sleep quality in college students may be related to delay discounting (DD) for food—a psychological process in which immediate outcomes are preferred over larger, more delayed outcomes. College students (<i>N</i> = 297) completed the Food Choice Questionnaire (FCQ) and the Monetary Choice Questionnaire (MCQ)—measures of food and monetary DD, respectively. The Perceived Stress Scale (PSS), the Pittsburg Sleep Quality Index (PSQI), and measures of subjective hunger, substance use, and demographic variables were also administered. Perceived stress was related to poor sleep quality, alcohol use, substance use, and vaping. Analyses revealed that, when controlling for subjective hunger, perceived stress and poor sleep quality contributed unique variance to food DD, though in opposing directions. Perceived stress uniquely predicted preferences for immediate food, a phenomenon consistent with stress-induced urgent eating. Poor sleep quality uniquely predicted preferences for larger amounts of delayed food, a pattern consistent with eating later in the day. Stress and sleep quality, when controlling for substance use variables, were unrelated to monetary discounting. Stress and poor sleep quality, then, predict independent and opposing discounting processes in college students that are food-specific, as opposed to more general cross-commodity processes.</p>","PeriodicalId":48112,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135063101","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Decision-making styles and goal striving 决策风格和目标追求
IF 2 3区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-08-28 DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2349
Jozef Bavolar, Pavol Kacmar, Ladislav Lovas, Simona Durbisova

While previous research has demonstrated the role of decision-making styles in attaining various real-life outcomes, it has neglected to explore the underlying goal-related processes in terms of goal dimensions (ways in which people appraise their goals during goal striving). The present study examines whether the most studied decision-making styles are related to self-reported effort, goal progress, and action crisis as well as other goal dimensions. We conducted 14 studies (mutual conceptual replications) with Ntotal = 2574 (70% females) which included the General Decision-Making Styles questionnaire and various goal-related scales. The results from the mini meta-analysis showed that the rational and intuitive styles were positively related to the goal dimensions associated with successful goal pursuit (e.g., goal commitment, goal attainability, positive emotions, and goal progress), while the avoidant style was mainly related to various difficulties associated with goal striving (e.g., controlled motivation, negative emotions, and action crisis). The dependent and especially spontaneous styles were found to be very weakly associated with the selected goal dimensions. When the separate studies were analyzed in the regression analyses and more process-oriented goal dimensions were accounted for, decision-making styles were only minor predictors of self-reported effort expenditure, goal progress and action crisis. The study highlights the need and usefulness of a more nuanced processual approach in the research of individual decision-making differences in goal-directed behavior.

虽然先前的研究已经证明了决策风格在实现各种现实生活结果中的作用,但它忽略了从目标维度(人们在追求目标过程中评估目标的方式)来探索潜在的目标相关过程。本研究考察了研究最多的决策风格是否与自我报告的努力、目标进展、行动危机以及其他目标维度有关。我们与Ntotal进行了14项研究(相互概念复制) = 2574人(70%为女性),包括一般决策风格问卷和各种目标相关量表。迷你元分析的结果表明,理性和直觉风格与成功追求目标的目标维度(如目标承诺、目标可达性、积极情绪和目标进展)呈正相关,而回避型风格主要与目标争取相关的各种困难有关(如控制动机、负面情绪和行动危机)。依赖性风格,尤其是自发风格,被发现与所选目标维度的相关性非常弱。当在回归分析中对单独的研究进行分析,并考虑到更多以过程为导向的目标维度时,决策风格只是自我报告的努力支出、目标进展和行动危机的次要预测因素。这项研究强调了在研究目标导向行为中的个人决策差异时,更细致的过程方法的必要性和有用性。
{"title":"Decision-making styles and goal striving","authors":"Jozef Bavolar,&nbsp;Pavol Kacmar,&nbsp;Ladislav Lovas,&nbsp;Simona Durbisova","doi":"10.1002/bdm.2349","DOIUrl":"10.1002/bdm.2349","url":null,"abstract":"<p>While previous research has demonstrated the role of decision-making styles in attaining various real-life outcomes, it has neglected to explore the underlying goal-related processes in terms of goal dimensions (ways in which people appraise their goals during goal striving). The present study examines whether the most studied decision-making styles are related to self-reported effort, goal progress, and action crisis as well as other goal dimensions. We conducted 14 studies (mutual conceptual replications) with <i>N</i><sub>total</sub> = 2574 (70% females) which included the General Decision-Making Styles questionnaire and various goal-related scales. The results from the mini meta-analysis showed that the rational and intuitive styles were positively related to the goal dimensions associated with successful goal pursuit (e.g., goal commitment, goal attainability, positive emotions, and goal progress), while the avoidant style was mainly related to various difficulties associated with goal striving (e.g., controlled motivation, negative emotions, and action crisis). The dependent and especially spontaneous styles were found to be very weakly associated with the selected goal dimensions. When the separate studies were analyzed in the regression analyses and more process-oriented goal dimensions were accounted for, decision-making styles were only minor predictors of self-reported effort expenditure, goal progress and action crisis. The study highlights the need and usefulness of a more nuanced processual approach in the research of individual decision-making differences in goal-directed behavior.</p>","PeriodicalId":48112,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-08-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/bdm.2349","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45804581","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The power of the lens: Filming increases honesty in children as young as five 镜头的力量:拍摄能让5岁的孩子变得诚实
IF 2 3区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-08-28 DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2351
Liyang Sai, Yue Bi, Chengfei Yu, Xiao Pan Ding

As early as 5 years of age, children begin to manage their reputations strategically. We investigated whether the reputation concern elicited by filming affected children's mental cheating, which is a form of cheating that cannot be detected even if someone else is watching. During the test, the experimenter was in the room with children, and we operationalized reputational cues as whether the video camera was actively recording or not. We compared the self-reported accuracy of a filmed group versus a non-filmed group in a mental cheating game, under two motivational contexts: self-interest and other-interest. A total of 320 children aged 3 to 6 years played a mind game in which they were asked to predict the outcome of a dice roll and to report whether their prediction was correct. We found that 5- to 6-year-olds were less likely to cheat for their self-interests when being filmed than when not being filmed. However, filming did not reduce 5- to 6-year-olds' other-interested cheating. Furthermore, we found that filming did not influence the self-interested or other-interested cheating of 3- to 4-year-old children. This study highlights how reputation concern elicited by filming motivates children to appear honest to others, even in purely mental cheating scenarios. Additionally, our results suggest that young children are sophisticated in their early reputation management and that prosocial justifications can alleviate concerns about dishonesty.

早在5岁时,孩子们就开始有策略地管理自己的声誉。我们调查了拍摄引起的名誉问题是否影响了儿童的心理欺骗,这是一种即使有人在观看也无法被发现的欺骗形式。在测试过程中,实验者和孩子们在一个房间里,我们将声誉线索操作为摄像机是否在主动记录。在自我利益和他人利益两种动机背景下,我们比较了心理作弊游戏中被拍摄组和未被拍摄组的自我报告准确性。共有320名3到6岁的儿童参与了一项心理游戏,他们被要求预测掷骰子的结果,并报告他们的预测是否正确。我们发现,5 - 6岁的孩子在被拍摄时比不被拍摄时更不可能为了自己的利益而作弊。然而,拍摄并没有减少5 - 6岁孩子的其他兴趣作弊。此外,我们发现拍摄对3 - 4岁儿童的自我利益或他人利益作弊行为没有影响。这项研究强调了通过拍摄引起的声誉担忧如何促使孩子们对他人表现得诚实,即使是在纯粹的心理欺骗场景中。此外,我们的研究结果表明,幼儿在早期的声誉管理方面是成熟的,亲社会的辩护可以减轻对不诚实的担忧。
{"title":"The power of the lens: Filming increases honesty in children as young as five","authors":"Liyang Sai,&nbsp;Yue Bi,&nbsp;Chengfei Yu,&nbsp;Xiao Pan Ding","doi":"10.1002/bdm.2351","DOIUrl":"10.1002/bdm.2351","url":null,"abstract":"<p>As early as 5 years of age, children begin to manage their reputations strategically. We investigated whether the reputation concern elicited by filming affected children's mental cheating, which is a form of cheating that cannot be detected even if someone else is watching. During the test, the experimenter was in the room with children, and we operationalized reputational cues as whether the video camera was actively recording or not. We compared the self-reported accuracy of a filmed group versus a non-filmed group in a mental cheating game, under two motivational contexts: self-interest and other-interest. A total of 320 children aged 3 to 6 years played a mind game in which they were asked to predict the outcome of a dice roll and to report whether their prediction was correct. We found that 5- to 6-year-olds were less likely to cheat for their self-interests when being filmed than when not being filmed. However, filming did not reduce 5- to 6-year-olds' other-interested cheating. Furthermore, we found that filming did not influence the self-interested or other-interested cheating of 3- to 4-year-old children. This study highlights how reputation concern elicited by filming motivates children to appear honest to others, even in purely mental cheating scenarios. Additionally, our results suggest that young children are sophisticated in their early reputation management and that prosocial justifications can alleviate concerns about dishonesty.</p>","PeriodicalId":48112,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-08-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/bdm.2351","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45622636","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Thou shalt be safe: Risk preferences in choice for sad others 你应该是安全的:为悲伤的他人选择风险偏好
IF 2 3区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-08-23 DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2350
Gerri Spassova, Mauricio Palmeira

This research takes the first step in exploring how the emotions of choice recipients influence the riskiness of decisions made for them by others. In particular, we focus on the role of sadness—an emotion that has been shown to prompt risk-seeking in choices for self. Across five studies, in monetary and social decisions, participants prefer safer options for sad, relative to neutral-affect others, even when the expected value of the safer option is significantly lower than that of the riskier option. The effect is driven by a desire to protect sad others from further loss or disappointment.

这项研究迈出了探索选择接受者的情绪如何影响他人为他们做出决定的风险的第一步。特别是,我们关注悲伤的作用——这种情绪已被证明会促使人们在自我选择中寻求风险。在五项研究中,在货币和社会决策中,相对于中性影响的其他人,参与者更喜欢悲伤的更安全的选择,即使更安全的选项的预期值明显低于风险更高的选项。这种效果是由保护悲伤的他人免受进一步损失或失望的愿望所驱动的。
{"title":"Thou shalt be safe: Risk preferences in choice for sad others","authors":"Gerri Spassova,&nbsp;Mauricio Palmeira","doi":"10.1002/bdm.2350","DOIUrl":"10.1002/bdm.2350","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This research takes the first step in exploring how the emotions of choice recipients influence the riskiness of decisions made for them by others. In particular, we focus on the role of sadness—an emotion that has been shown to prompt risk-seeking in choices for self. Across five studies, in monetary and social decisions, participants prefer safer options for sad, relative to neutral-affect others, even when the expected value of the safer option is significantly lower than that of the riskier option. The effect is driven by a desire to protect sad others from further loss or disappointment.</p>","PeriodicalId":48112,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-08-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46758873","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Information search processing affects social decisions 信息搜索过程影响社会决策
IF 2 3区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-08-23 DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2352
Zi-Han Wei, Yan Liang, Ci-Juan Liang, Hong-Zhi Liu

Social decisions often require individuals to balance conflicts between their own selfish interests and the need for equality. The way information about available options is presented can have an impact on how people process information and make social decisions. In this study, we examined the effect of information presentation on social decisions in a mini-dictator game, where participants must make binary choices about how to allocate money between themselves and another participant. We conducted two tasks with different presentation styles: the attribute-based task, where attributes such as own payoffs or others' payoffs were displayed sequentially, and the alternative-based task, where alternatives were displayed sequentially. We found that participants in the attribute-based task made more selfish choices and were less sensitive to equality than those in the alternative-based task. The direction of information search and the complexity level of information processing played a mediating role in the effect of task on social decisions. Our findings highlight the relationship between information search and social decisions, shedding light on the mechanisms and processes that underlie social decision-making.

社会决策往往要求个人平衡自身私利和平等需求之间的冲突。关于可用选项的信息呈现方式可能会对人们处理信息和做出社会决策的方式产生影响。在这项研究中,我们检验了迷你独裁者游戏中信息呈现对社会决策的影响,在这个游戏中,参与者必须对如何在自己和另一个参与者之间分配金钱做出二元选择。我们进行了两项具有不同表现风格的任务:基于属性的任务,其中按顺序显示自己或他人的报酬等属性,以及基于备选方案的任务,按顺序显示备选方案。我们发现,与基于替代任务的参与者相比,基于属性任务的参与者做出了更多自私的选择,对平等的敏感度更低。信息搜索的方向和信息处理的复杂程度在任务对社会决策的影响中起着中介作用。我们的研究结果强调了信息搜索和社会决策之间的关系,揭示了社会决策的机制和过程。
{"title":"Information search processing affects social decisions","authors":"Zi-Han Wei,&nbsp;Yan Liang,&nbsp;Ci-Juan Liang,&nbsp;Hong-Zhi Liu","doi":"10.1002/bdm.2352","DOIUrl":"10.1002/bdm.2352","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Social decisions often require individuals to balance conflicts between their own selfish interests and the need for equality. The way information about available options is presented can have an impact on how people process information and make social decisions. In this study, we examined the effect of information presentation on social decisions in a mini-dictator game, where participants must make binary choices about how to allocate money between themselves and another participant. We conducted two tasks with different presentation styles: the attribute-based task, where attributes such as own payoffs or others' payoffs were displayed sequentially, and the alternative-based task, where alternatives were displayed sequentially. We found that participants in the attribute-based task made more selfish choices and were less sensitive to equality than those in the alternative-based task. The direction of information search and the complexity level of information processing played a mediating role in the effect of task on social decisions. Our findings highlight the relationship between information search and social decisions, shedding light on the mechanisms and processes that underlie social decision-making.</p>","PeriodicalId":48112,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-08-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44854536","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Comparison-specific preferences: The attentional dilution effect for delay and risk 比较特定偏好:延迟和风险的注意稀释效应
IF 2 3区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-08-17 DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2348
Daniel Read, Rebecca McDonald, Robin Cubitt

In cross-modal decisions, the options differ on many attributes, and in uni-modal decisions, they differ on few. We supply new theory and data to understand how discounting for both delay and risk differs between cross-modal and uni-modal decisions. We propose the attentional dilution effect in decision making in which (a) allocation of limited attention to an attribute determines that attribute's decision weight and (b) the attention an attribute receives is increasing in the difference between options on that attribute and decreasing in the number of other attributes that differ between options. We introduce the random order delayed compensation method and conduct two experiments focusing on delayed and risky receipt of consumer goods. Consistent with the attentional dilution effect, we find that in this domain, patience and risk tolerance are generally higher in cross-modal than uni-modal decisions. We suggest that, since many real-world choices are cross-modal, people may be more patient and risk-tolerant in their everyday life than is suggested by standard lab experiments.

在跨模态决策中,选项在许多属性上不同,而在单模态决策中则在少数属性上不同。我们提供了新的理论和数据,以了解跨模态决策和单模态决策之间延迟和风险的贴现有何不同。我们提出了决策中的注意力稀释效应,其中(a)对一个属性的有限注意力分配决定了该属性的决策权重,(b)一个属性受到的注意力在该属性的选项之间的差异中增加,而在选项之间不同的其他属性的数量中减少。我们引入了随机订单延迟补偿方法,并针对消费品的延迟和风险接收进行了两个实验。与注意力稀释效应一致,我们发现在这一领域,跨模态决策的耐心和风险承受能力通常高于单模态决策。我们认为,由于许多现实世界的选择是跨模式的,人们在日常生活中可能比标准实验室实验更具耐心和风险承受能力。
{"title":"Comparison-specific preferences: The attentional dilution effect for delay and risk","authors":"Daniel Read,&nbsp;Rebecca McDonald,&nbsp;Robin Cubitt","doi":"10.1002/bdm.2348","DOIUrl":"10.1002/bdm.2348","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In cross-modal decisions, the options differ on many attributes, and in uni-modal decisions, they differ on few. We supply new theory and data to understand how discounting for both delay and risk differs between cross-modal and uni-modal decisions. We propose the attentional dilution effect in decision making in which (a) allocation of limited attention to an attribute determines that attribute's decision weight and (b) the attention an attribute receives is increasing in the difference between options on that attribute and decreasing in the number of other attributes that differ between options. We introduce the <i>random order delayed compensation method</i> and conduct two experiments focusing on delayed and risky receipt of consumer goods. Consistent with the attentional dilution effect, we find that in this domain, patience and risk tolerance are generally higher in cross-modal than uni-modal decisions. We suggest that, since many real-world choices are cross-modal, people may be more patient and risk-tolerant in their everyday life than is suggested by standard lab experiments.</p>","PeriodicalId":48112,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-08-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41685339","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Incidentally elicited multiple, discrete emotions have differential effects on risky behavior: The action priming perspective 偶然引发的多重、离散的情绪对冒险行为有不同的影响:行动启动的观点
IF 2 3区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2023-08-16 DOI: 10.1002/bdm.2346
David Matsumoto, Matthew Wilson

We present a novel theoretical framework called the Action Priming Perspective to predict effects of discrete emotions on judgment and decision-making and report results from two studies examining five discrete emotions (anger, disgust, fear, happiness, and sadness) and neutral on a behavioral task of risky decision-making. We tested two hypotheses concerning single and combinatorial effects of the emotions based on previous theoretical and empirical work delineating the action priming functions of discrete emotions. As predicted, a fear–sadness combination, elicited separately but combined for analyses, produced the highest risk-taking behavior, higher than an anger–disgust combination (also elicited separately but combined for analyses). Sadness also produced more risky behavior than did disgust, as predicted. These effects, however, did not occur when the task was less uncertain. These findings were discussed vis-à-vis understanding implications of specific, discrete emotions on risky, ambiguous judgment and decision-making.

我们提出了一个新的理论框架,称为行动启动视角来预测离散情绪对判断和决策的影响,并报告了两项研究的结果,研究了五种离散情绪(愤怒、厌恶、恐惧、快乐和悲伤)和中性情绪在风险决策行为任务中的作用。我们基于先前描述离散情绪的动作启动功能的理论和实证工作,测试了关于情绪的单一和组合效应的两个假设。正如预测的那样,恐惧-悲伤组合(单独引出但结合分析)产生了最高的冒险行为,高于愤怒-厌恶组合(也单独引出但结合分析)。正如预测的那样,悲伤也会比厌恶产生更多的冒险行为。然而,当任务的不确定性降低时,这些影响就不会发生。这些研究结果针对特定的、离散的情绪对风险、模糊的判断和决策的影响进行了讨论。
{"title":"Incidentally elicited multiple, discrete emotions have differential effects on risky behavior: The action priming perspective","authors":"David Matsumoto,&nbsp;Matthew Wilson","doi":"10.1002/bdm.2346","DOIUrl":"10.1002/bdm.2346","url":null,"abstract":"<p>We present a novel theoretical framework called the Action Priming Perspective to predict effects of discrete emotions on judgment and decision-making and report results from two studies examining five discrete emotions (anger, disgust, fear, happiness, and sadness) and neutral on a behavioral task of risky decision-making. We tested two hypotheses concerning single and combinatorial effects of the emotions based on previous theoretical and empirical work delineating the action priming functions of discrete emotions. As predicted, a fear–sadness combination, elicited separately but combined for analyses, produced the highest risk-taking behavior, higher than an anger–disgust combination (also elicited separately but combined for analyses). Sadness also produced more risky behavior than did disgust, as predicted. These effects, however, did not occur when the task was less uncertain. These findings were discussed vis-à-vis understanding implications of specific, discrete emotions on risky, ambiguous judgment and decision-making.</p>","PeriodicalId":48112,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Behavioral Decision Making","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-08-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41438688","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Behavioral Decision Making
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1