首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Child Language最新文献

英文 中文
Singleton consonant onset acquisition in monolingual Granada Spanish-speaking preschoolers with typical versus protracted phonological development: Impacts of word structure and feature constraints. 单语格拉纳达西班牙语学龄前儿童的单音辅音起音习得:典型与滞后语音发展:单词结构和特征限制的影响。
IF 1.7 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2023-01-31 DOI: 10.1017/S0305000922000666
Barbara May Bernhardt, Gabriela Raymond, Carmen Ávila, Pablo Cáceres Serrano, Gloria Carballo, Dolores Fresneda, Elvira Mendoza, Karen Hoang, Lydia Liu, Juana Muñoz, Denisse Pérez, Joseph P Stemberger

While consonant acquisition clearly requires mastery of different articulatory configurations (segments), sub-segmental features and suprasegmental contexts influence both order of acquisition and mismatch (error) patterns (Bérubé, Bernhardt, Stemberger & Ciocca, 2020). Constraints-based nonlinear phonology provides a comprehensive framework for investigating the impact of sub- and suprasegmental impacts on acquisition (Bernhardt & Stemberger, 1998). The current study adopted such a framework in order to investigate these questions for Granada Spanish. Single-word samples of monolingual preschoolers in Granada (29 typically developing; 30 with protracted phonological development) were transcribed by native Spanish speakers in consultation with an international team. Beta regression analyses showed significant effects of age, developmental group, and word structure variables (word length, stress, position of consonants and syllables within the word); salience, markedness and/or frequency across the phonological hierarchy accounted for many patterns. The study further demonstrates the impacts of sub- and suprasegmental constraints of the phonological system on consonant acquisition.

虽然辅音的习得显然需要掌握不同的发音配置(音段),但亚音段特征和超音段语境会影响习得顺序和错配(错误)模式(Bérubé, Bernhardt, Stemberger & Ciocca, 2020)。基于限制的非线性语音学为研究语段下和语段上对学习的影响提供了一个全面的框架(Bernhardt & Stemberger, 1998)。本研究采用了这一框架来研究格拉纳达西班牙语的这些问题。格拉纳达单语学龄前儿童的单字样本(29 个发育典型;30 个语音发育迟缓)由以西班牙语为母语的人与一个国际团队共同转录。贝塔回归分析表明,年龄、发育组别和单词结构变量(单词长度、重音、辅音和音节在单词中的位置)都有显著影响;语音层次结构中的显著性、标记性和/或频率也对许多模式有影响。该研究进一步证明了语音系统的亚音段和超音段限制对辅音学习的影响。
{"title":"Singleton consonant onset acquisition in monolingual Granada Spanish-speaking preschoolers with typical versus protracted phonological development: Impacts of word structure and feature constraints.","authors":"Barbara May Bernhardt, Gabriela Raymond, Carmen Ávila, Pablo Cáceres Serrano, Gloria Carballo, Dolores Fresneda, Elvira Mendoza, Karen Hoang, Lydia Liu, Juana Muñoz, Denisse Pérez, Joseph P Stemberger","doi":"10.1017/S0305000922000666","DOIUrl":"10.1017/S0305000922000666","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>While consonant acquisition clearly requires mastery of different articulatory configurations (segments), sub-segmental features and suprasegmental contexts influence both order of acquisition and mismatch (error) patterns (Bérubé, Bernhardt, Stemberger & Ciocca, 2020). Constraints-based nonlinear phonology provides a comprehensive framework for investigating the impact of sub- and suprasegmental impacts on acquisition (Bernhardt & Stemberger, 1998). The current study adopted such a framework in order to investigate these questions for Granada Spanish. Single-word samples of monolingual preschoolers in Granada (29 typically developing; 30 with protracted phonological development) were transcribed by native Spanish speakers in consultation with an international team. Beta regression analyses showed significant effects of age, developmental group, and word structure variables (word length, stress, position of consonants and syllables within the word); salience, markedness and/or frequency across the phonological hierarchy accounted for many patterns. The study further demonstrates the impacts of sub- and suprasegmental constraints of the phonological system on consonant acquisition.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":" ","pages":"981-1015"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10584312","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children's acquisition of word order variation: A study of subject placement in embedded clauses in Norwegian. 儿童词序变化习得:挪威语嵌入从句主语位置研究。
IF 1.7 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-29 DOI: 10.1017/S0305000923000259
Tina Ringstad, Marit Westergaard

Norwegian embedded clauses give children two options for subject placement: preceding or following negation (S-Neg/Neg-S). In the adult language, S-Neg is the 'default' and highly frequent option, and Neg-S is infrequent in children's input. However, Neg-S may be argued to be the structurally less complex. We investigate whether children are aware of the existence of both subject positions, and if they prefer the more frequent or the less complex position. Through an elicited production task with monolingual Norwegian children (N=33, age 3;1-6;1) we find that children in general overuse the Neg-S option, and we suggest that children have an inherent preference for the less complex position, due to a principle of structural economy. We also find that a group of children display U-shaped development, first using only S-Neg, then only Neg-S and finally S-Neg again, and we relate this to structure building and economy of movement.

挪威语嵌入从句为孩子们提供了两种主语放置的选择:前否定或后否定(S-Neg/ negative - s)。在成人语言中,S-Neg是“默认的”且使用频率很高的选项,而n - s在儿童的输入中很少出现。然而,负- s可能被认为是结构较不复杂的。我们调查儿童是否意识到这两个主语位置的存在,如果他们更喜欢更频繁或不太复杂的位置。通过对单语挪威儿童(N=33, 3岁;1-6岁;1)的诱导生产任务,我们发现儿童普遍过度使用负- s选项,我们认为由于结构经济原则,儿童天生偏好较不复杂的位置。我们还发现一组儿童表现出u型发展,一开始只使用S-Neg,然后只使用- neg,最后又使用S-Neg,我们将这与结构建设和运动经济性联系起来。
{"title":"Children's acquisition of word order variation: A study of subject placement in embedded clauses in Norwegian.","authors":"Tina Ringstad, Marit Westergaard","doi":"10.1017/S0305000923000259","DOIUrl":"10.1017/S0305000923000259","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Norwegian embedded clauses give children two options for subject placement: preceding or following negation (S-Neg/Neg-S). In the adult language, S-Neg is the 'default' and highly frequent option, and Neg-S is infrequent in children's input. However, Neg-S may be argued to be the structurally less complex. We investigate whether children are aware of the existence of both subject positions, and if they prefer the more frequent or the less complex position. Through an elicited production task with monolingual Norwegian children (N=33, age 3;1-6;1) we find that children in general overuse the Neg-S option, and we suggest that children have an inherent preference for the less complex position, due to a principle of structural economy. We also find that a group of children display U-shaped development, first using only S-Neg, then only Neg-S and finally S-Neg again, and we relate this to structure building and economy of movement.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":" ","pages":"1109-1133"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9526545","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Positive Valence Contributes to Hyperarticulation in Maternal Speech to Infants and Puppies. 正效价有助于母婴语言的高清晰度。
IF 1.7 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1017/S0305000923000296
Robin Panneton, Alejandrina Cristia, Caroline Taylor, Christine Moon

Infant-directed speech often has hyperarticulated features, such as point vowels whose formants are further apart than in adult-directed speech. This increased "vowel space" may reflect the caretaker's effort to speak more clearly to infants, thus benefiting language processing. However, hyperarticulation may also result from more positive valence (e.g., speaking with positive vocal emotion) often found in mothers' speech to infants. This study was designed to replicate others who have found hyperarticulation in maternal speech to their 6-month-olds, but also to examine their speech to a non-human infant (i.e., a puppy). We rated both kinds of maternal speech for their emotional valence and recorded mothers' speech to a human adult. We found that mothers produced more positively valenced utterances and some hyperarticulation in both their infant- and puppy-directed speech, compared to their adult-directed speech. This finding promotes looking at maternal speech from a multi-faceted perspective that includes emotional state.

婴儿指向语通常具有高清晰度的特征,例如点元音的共振峰比成人指向语更远。这种增加的“元音空间”可能反映了看护人对婴儿说话更清楚的努力,从而有利于语言处理。然而,高清晰度也可能是由于更多的积极效价(例如,用积极的声音情绪说话)经常在母亲对婴儿的讲话中发现。这项研究的目的是复制其他发现母亲对6个月大的婴儿说话时发音异常的研究,但也研究了她们对非人类婴儿(即小狗)说话的情况。我们根据母亲的情感效价对这两种言语进行了评级,并记录了母亲对成年人的言语。我们发现,与成人指导的语言相比,母亲在婴儿和小狗指导的语言中产生了更多积极的话语和一些高清晰度。这一发现促使人们从包括情绪状态在内的多角度来看待母亲的言语。
{"title":"Positive Valence Contributes to Hyperarticulation in Maternal Speech to Infants and Puppies.","authors":"Robin Panneton, Alejandrina Cristia, Caroline Taylor, Christine Moon","doi":"10.1017/S0305000923000296","DOIUrl":"10.1017/S0305000923000296","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Infant-directed speech often has hyperarticulated features, such as point vowels whose formants are further apart than in adult-directed speech. This increased \"vowel space\" may reflect the caretaker's effort to speak more clearly to infants, thus benefiting language processing. However, hyperarticulation may also result from more positive valence (e.g., speaking with positive vocal emotion) often found in mothers' speech to infants. This study was designed to replicate others who have found hyperarticulation in maternal speech to their 6-month-olds, but also to examine their speech to a non-human infant (i.e., a puppy). We rated both kinds of maternal speech for their emotional valence and recorded mothers' speech to a human adult. We found that mothers produced more positively valenced utterances and some hyperarticulation in both their infant- and puppy-directed speech, compared to their adult-directed speech. This finding promotes looking at maternal speech from a multi-faceted perspective that includes emotional state.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":" ","pages":"1230-1240"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9724954","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Enhancing adolescent parent interactions in communication through facilitative playgroups. 通过促进性游戏小组加强青少年父母在交流中的互动。
IF 1.7 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2023-01-12 DOI: 10.1017/S030500092200071X
Diana Abarca, Kasey Allen, Jacqueline A Towson, Katherine B Green

Adolescent pregnancy can impact the educational attainment of adolescent mothers and language development of their children. However, support services and interventions can mitigate these risk factors. Adolescent mothers have shown success in implementing various language facilitation strategies (LFS) with their children when coached. We developed a triadic language intervention in a facilitated playgroup context to support adolescent mothers' use of language strategies when interacting with their children. The current pilot study utilized a pre-test, post-test descriptive approach to analyze differences in adolescent mothers' language use and parenting behaviors and their children's language use after participation in facilitated playgroups. All adolescent mothers demonstrated enhanced parenting practices. Changes in mothers' and children's language from pre- to post-test were variable across participants. While this pilot study shows promising evidence of the utility of a triadic intervention situated in playgroups with adolescent families, further research is required to address the limitations of this study.

少女怀孕会影响少女母亲的受教育程度及其子女的语言发展。然而,支持服务和干预措施可以减轻这些风险因素。有研究表明,少女母亲在接受指导后,能成功地与孩子一起实施各种语言促进策略(LFS)。我们在促进性游戏小组的背景下开发了一种三元语言干预方法,以支持未成年母亲在与孩子互动时使用语言策略。目前的试点研究采用了前测、后测的描述性方法,以分析青少年母亲在参加游戏小组后在语言使用、养育行为及其子女的语言使用方面的差异。所有未成年母亲的养育行为都有所改善。从测试前到测试后,不同参与者的母亲和孩子的语言变化各不相同。虽然这项试点研究证明了在青少年家庭游戏小组中进行三元干预的有效性,但还需要进一步的研究来解决本研究的局限性。
{"title":"Enhancing adolescent parent interactions in communication through facilitative playgroups.","authors":"Diana Abarca, Kasey Allen, Jacqueline A Towson, Katherine B Green","doi":"10.1017/S030500092200071X","DOIUrl":"10.1017/S030500092200071X","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Adolescent pregnancy can impact the educational attainment of adolescent mothers and language development of their children. However, support services and interventions can mitigate these risk factors. Adolescent mothers have shown success in implementing various language facilitation strategies (LFS) with their children when coached. We developed a triadic language intervention in a facilitated playgroup context to support adolescent mothers' use of language strategies when interacting with their children. The current pilot study utilized a pre-test, post-test descriptive approach to analyze differences in adolescent mothers' language use and parenting behaviors and their children's language use after participation in facilitated playgroups. All adolescent mothers demonstrated enhanced parenting practices. Changes in mothers' and children's language from pre- to post-test were variable across participants. While this pilot study shows promising evidence of the utility of a triadic intervention situated in playgroups with adolescent families, further research is required to address the limitations of this study.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":" ","pages":"965-980"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10574554","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Internal state language factor structure and development in toddlerhood: Insights from WordBank. 幼儿期的内部状态语言因素结构和发展:来自词库的启示。
IF 1.7 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-11 DOI: 10.1017/S0305000924000060
Tyler C McFayden, Madeleine Bruce

Internal state language (ISL) research contains knowledge gaps, including dimensionality and predictors of growth, addressed here in a two-aim study. Parent-reported expressive language from N = 6,373 monolingual, English-speaking toddlers (Mage = 23.5mos, 46% male, 57% white) was collected using cross-sectional and longitudinal data in WordBank. Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses suggested a best-fitting one-factor model of ISL. The single-factor model of ISL was then submitted to hierarchical linear modeling to evaluate predictors of ISL development. Age 2 ISL production was predicted by child sex, wherein females outperform males, and maternal education, wherein higher education contributes to higher ISL. Only maternal education emerged as a significant predictor of ISL growth. These results provide support to theory suggesting a unitary construct of ISL, as opposed to considering ISL as categorical, and further illustrate linear growth through the second postnatal year that varies as a function of child sex and maternal education.

内部状态语言(ISL)研究存在知识空白,包括维度和成长预测因素,本研究通过双目标研究解决了这一问题。通过 WordBank 中的横向和纵向数据,收集了 N = 6,373 名单语英语幼儿(年龄 = 23.5 个月,46% 为男性,57% 为白人)的家长报告语言表达能力。探索性和确认性因素分析表明,ISL 的单因素模型最为拟合。随后,对 ISL 的单因素模型进行了分层线性建模,以评估 ISL 发展的预测因素。预测 2 岁儿童 ISL 发展的因素包括儿童性别(女性优于男性)和母亲教育程度(教育程度越高,ISL 越高)。只有母亲的教育程度对 ISL 的增长有显著的预测作用。这些结果表明,ISL是一个统一的概念,而不是将ISL视为一个分类概念,并进一步说明了ISL在出生后第二年的线性增长随儿童性别和母亲教育程度的变化而变化。
{"title":"Internal state language factor structure and development in toddlerhood: Insights from WordBank.","authors":"Tyler C McFayden, Madeleine Bruce","doi":"10.1017/S0305000924000060","DOIUrl":"10.1017/S0305000924000060","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Internal state language (ISL) research contains knowledge gaps, including dimensionality and predictors of growth, addressed here in a two-aim study. Parent-reported expressive language from <i>N</i> = 6,373 monolingual, English-speaking toddlers (<i>M<sub>age</sub></i> = 23.5mos, 46% male, 57% white) was collected using cross-sectional and longitudinal data in WordBank. Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses suggested a best-fitting one-factor model of ISL. The single-factor model of ISL was then submitted to hierarchical linear modeling to evaluate predictors of ISL development. Age 2 ISL production was predicted by child sex, wherein females outperform males, and maternal education, wherein higher education contributes to higher ISL. Only maternal education emerged as a significant predictor of ISL growth. These results provide support to theory suggesting a unitary construct of ISL, as opposed to considering ISL as categorical, and further illustrate linear growth through the second postnatal year that varies as a function of child sex and maternal education.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":" ","pages":"721-740"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11387952/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140094918","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sign advantage: Both children and adults' spatial expressions in sign are more informative than those in speech and gestures combined. 手势优势:儿童和成人的手势空间表达都比语言和手势的空间表达信息量更大。
IF 1.7 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2022-12-13 DOI: 10.1017/S0305000922000642
Dilay Z Karadöller, Beyza Sümer, Ercenur Ünal, Aslı Özyürek

Expressing Left-Right relations is challenging for speaking-children. Yet, this challenge was absent for signing-children, possibly due to iconicity in the visual-spatial modality of expression. We investigate whether there is also a modality advantage when speaking-children's co-speech gestures are considered. Eight-year-old child and adult hearing monolingual Turkish speakers and deaf signers of Turkish-Sign-Language described pictures of objects in various spatial relations. Descriptions were coded for informativeness in speech, sign, and speech-gesture combinations for encoding Left-Right relations. The use of co-speech gestures increased the informativeness of speakers' spatial expressions compared to speech-only. This pattern was more prominent for children than adults. However, signing-adults and children were more informative than child and adult speakers even when co-speech gestures were considered. Thus, both speaking- and signing-children benefit from iconic expressions in visual modality. Finally, in each modality, children were less informative than adults, pointing to the challenge of this spatial domain in development.

对于会说话的儿童来说,表达左右关系具有挑战性。然而,手语儿童却不存在这种挑战,这可能是由于表达的视觉空间模态具有标志性。我们研究了在考虑说话儿童的共同言语手势时是否也存在模态优势。八岁的儿童和成人听力单语土耳其语者和聋人土耳其手语者描述了各种空间关系中的物体图片。对描述的信息量进行了编码,包括语言、手势和语言-手势组合对左右关系的编码。与纯语音相比,使用联合语音手势会增加说话者空间表达的信息量。这种模式在儿童中比在成人中更为突出。然而,即使考虑到共同言语手势,手势成人和儿童也比儿童和成人说话者的信息量更大。因此,会说话和会手势的儿童都能从视觉模式的图标表达中获益。最后,在每种模式中,儿童提供的信息都少于成人,这表明了这一空间领域在发展过程中面临的挑战。
{"title":"Sign advantage: Both children and adults' spatial expressions in sign are more informative than those in speech and gestures combined.","authors":"Dilay Z Karadöller, Beyza Sümer, Ercenur Ünal, Aslı Özyürek","doi":"10.1017/S0305000922000642","DOIUrl":"10.1017/S0305000922000642","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Expressing Left-Right relations is challenging for speaking-children. Yet, this challenge was absent for signing-children, possibly due to iconicity in the visual-spatial modality of expression. We investigate whether there is also a modality advantage when speaking-children's co-speech gestures are considered. Eight-year-old child and adult hearing monolingual Turkish speakers and deaf signers of Turkish-Sign-Language described pictures of objects in various spatial relations. Descriptions were coded for informativeness in speech, sign, and speech-gesture combinations for encoding Left-Right relations. The use of co-speech gestures increased the informativeness of speakers' spatial expressions compared to speech-only. This pattern was more prominent for children than adults. However, signing-adults and children were more informative than child and adult speakers even when co-speech gestures were considered. Thus, both speaking- and signing-children benefit from iconic expressions in visual modality. Finally, in each modality, children were less informative than adults, pointing to the challenge of this spatial domain in development.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":" ","pages":"876-902"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10332493","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Developing early lexical composition in Mandarin-speaking children: A longitudinal study. 普通话儿童早期词汇构成的发展:纵向研究。
IF 1.7 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2023-01-06 DOI: 10.1017/S0305000922000654
Yi-Fang Hung, Chien-Ju Chang

This study investigated the developmental pattern of early lexical production and composition in Mandarin-speaking children. Forty Mandarin-speaking children and their parents participated in this one-and-a-half-year longitudinal study, and naturalistic samples of parent-to-child speech in toy play were collected when the children were 1;8, 2;2, and 3;0. The results showed that children's lexical production increased significantly between ages 1;8 and 3;0. The proportion of closed-class words increased significantly with age, whereas the proportion of common nouns showed the inverse pattern, indicating the role of grammatical words increased as the children grew. Furthermore, nouns and verbs were predominant in Mandarin-speaking children between ages 1;8 and 3;0, and Mandarin-speaking children used more verbs than nouns at 2;2 and 3;0 in the toy play context. The longitudinal study clarifies early lexical development in Mandarin-speaking children, which provides a valuable contrast for different language systems.

本研究调查了普通话儿童早期词汇产生和构成的发展模式。这项为期一年半的纵向研究收集了 40 名讲普通话的儿童及其父母在儿童 1;8、2;2 和 3;0 岁时在玩具游戏中亲子间的自然言语样本。结果表明,儿童的词汇量在 1.8 岁至 3.0 岁期间显著增加。随着年龄的增长,封闭类词的比例明显增加,而普通名词的比例则呈反比,这表明语法词的作用随着儿童的成长而增加。此外,说普通话的儿童在1;8至3;0岁期间以名词和动词为主,而在2;2和3;0岁时,说普通话的儿童在玩具游戏情境中使用的动词多于名词。这项纵向研究阐明了普通话儿童的早期词汇发展情况,为不同语言系统提供了宝贵的对比资料。
{"title":"Developing early lexical composition in Mandarin-speaking children: A longitudinal study.","authors":"Yi-Fang Hung, Chien-Ju Chang","doi":"10.1017/S0305000922000654","DOIUrl":"10.1017/S0305000922000654","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study investigated the developmental pattern of early lexical production and composition in Mandarin-speaking children. Forty Mandarin-speaking children and their parents participated in this one-and-a-half-year longitudinal study, and naturalistic samples of parent-to-child speech in toy play were collected when the children were 1;8, 2;2, and 3;0. The results showed that children's lexical production increased significantly between ages 1;8 and 3;0. The proportion of closed-class words increased significantly with age, whereas the proportion of common nouns showed the inverse pattern, indicating the role of grammatical words increased as the children grew. Furthermore, nouns and verbs were predominant in Mandarin-speaking children between ages 1;8 and 3;0, and Mandarin-speaking children used more verbs than nouns at 2;2 and 3;0 in the toy play context. The longitudinal study clarifies early lexical development in Mandarin-speaking children, which provides a valuable contrast for different language systems.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":" ","pages":"903-924"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10489079","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emergent bilingual children during the silent period: A scoping review of their communication strategies and classroom environments. 沉默期的新兴双语儿童:对他们的交流策略和课堂环境进行的范围审查。
IF 2.2 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-05-27 DOI: 10.1017/S0305000924000151
Pui Fong Kan, Morgan Jones, Christina Meyers-Denman, Natalya Sparks

This scoping review aimed to investigate the communication strategies utilized by children who acquire a minority language (L1) and subsequently learn a community language (L2) during what is commonly referred to as the "silent period." Electronic database searches were conducted using keywords such as "silent period" and "bilingual children," resulting in the inclusion of 40 studies in the review. The findings revealed that emergent bilingual children utilize various communication strategies, including nonverbal communication, private speech, and their L1, to communicate within classroom environments. The findings shed light on the adaptability of emergent bilingual children during early stage of L2 acquisition. Furthermore, our review provides information about the classroom contexts such as teacher support and peer interactions where children develop their L2 skills. From a clinical perspective, recognizing these strategies and classroom contexts could significantly enhance the screening process for emergent bilingual children.

本范围界定综述旨在调查学习少数民族语言(L1)并随后学习社区语言(L2)的儿童在通常所说的 "沉默期 "中使用的交流策略。我们使用 "沉默期 "和 "双语儿童 "等关键词对电子数据库进行了搜索,最终将 40 项研究纳入了综述。研究结果表明,初学双语的儿童利用各种交流策略,包括非语言交流、私人语言和他们的母语,在课堂环境中进行交流。这些发现揭示了初学二语儿童在学习第二语言早期阶段的适应能力。此外,我们的综述还提供了有关课堂环境的信息,如教师支持和同伴互动,儿童在这些环境中发展他们的第二语言技能。从临床角度来看,认识到这些策略和课堂情境可以大大加强对初学二语儿童的筛选过程。
{"title":"Emergent bilingual children during the silent period: A scoping review of their communication strategies and classroom environments.","authors":"Pui Fong Kan, Morgan Jones, Christina Meyers-Denman, Natalya Sparks","doi":"10.1017/S0305000924000151","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000924000151","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This scoping review aimed to investigate the communication strategies utilized by children who acquire a minority language (L1) and subsequently learn a community language (L2) during what is commonly referred to as the \"silent period.\" Electronic database searches were conducted using keywords such as \"silent period\" and \"bilingual children,\" resulting in the inclusion of 40 studies in the review. The findings revealed that emergent bilingual children utilize various communication strategies, including nonverbal communication, private speech, and their L1, to communicate within classroom environments. The findings shed light on the adaptability of emergent bilingual children during early stage of L2 acquisition. Furthermore, our review provides information about the classroom contexts such as teacher support and peer interactions where children develop their L2 skills. From a clinical perspective, recognizing these strategies and classroom contexts could significantly enhance the screening process for emergent bilingual children.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":" ","pages":"1-34"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-05-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141155787","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Comprehension of complex sentences containing temporal connectives: How children are led down the event-semantic kindergarten-path. 理解包含时间连接词的复杂句子:儿童如何被引入事件-语义幼儿园之路。
IF 2.2 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-05-16 DOI: 10.1017/S0305000924000205
Christos Makrodimitris, Petra Schulz

Children up to school age are known to have difficulty comprehending complex sentences with temporal connectives, but the reasons remain controversial. We tested six- to twelve-year-old children to assess how the iconicity of event-language mapping, type of connective, and clause order mediate the comprehension of temporal sentences. Sixty monolingual Greek-speaking children and 15 adult controls completed a picture-sequence selection task in which they judged after- and before-sentences in iconic and non-iconic order. Up to age twelve, children did not reach full adult-like comprehension of the connectives; performance in non-iconic after-sentences was significantly lower than in the other three conditions across all ages. We conclude that neither iconicity, connective, nor clause order can fully explain these findings and propose an account based on the interaction of iconicity and clause order: non-iconic, sentence-medial after requires revision of the initial event representation, resulting in an event-semantic kindergarten-path that children find difficult to overcome.

众所周知,学龄前儿童很难理解带有时间连接词的复杂句子,但其原因仍存在争议。我们对六到十二岁的儿童进行了测试,以评估事件-语言映射的标志性、连接词的类型和分句顺序如何影响对时间句的理解。60 名说希腊语的单语儿童和 15 名成人对照组完成了一项图片序列选择任务,在这项任务中,他们按照标志性和非标志性顺序对后句和前句进行判断。12 岁以下的儿童对连接词的理解能力还没有完全达到成人的水平;在所有年龄段,非标志性后句子的表现明显低于其他三种情况。我们的结论是,图标性、连接词和分句顺序都不能完全解释这些发现,因此我们提出了一种基于图标性和分句顺序交互作用的解释:非图标性、句子中间的后句需要修正最初的事件表征,从而形成儿童难以克服的事件-语义幼儿园路径。
{"title":"Comprehension of complex sentences containing temporal connectives: How children are led down the event-semantic kindergarten-path.","authors":"Christos Makrodimitris, Petra Schulz","doi":"10.1017/S0305000924000205","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000924000205","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Children up to school age are known to have difficulty comprehending complex sentences with temporal connectives, but the reasons remain controversial. We tested six- to twelve-year-old children to assess how the iconicity of event-language mapping, type of connective, and clause order mediate the comprehension of temporal sentences. Sixty monolingual Greek-speaking children and 15 adult controls completed a picture-sequence selection task in which they judged <i>after</i>- and <i>before</i>-sentences in iconic and non-iconic order. Up to age twelve, children did not reach full adult-like comprehension of the connectives; performance in non-iconic <i>after</i>-sentences was significantly lower than in the other three conditions across all ages. We conclude that neither iconicity, connective, nor clause order can fully explain these findings and propose an account based on the interaction of iconicity and clause order: non-iconic, sentence-medial <i>after</i> requires revision of the initial event representation, resulting in an event-semantic kindergarten-path that children find difficult to overcome.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":" ","pages":"1-33"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-05-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140946270","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
That kid is a grasshopper! Metaphor development from 3 to 9 years of age. 那孩子是只蚱蜢3 至 9 岁的隐喻发展。
IF 2.2 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-05-13 DOI: 10.1017/S0305000924000187
Isabel Martín-González, Camilo R Ronderos, Elena Castroviejo, Kristen Schroeder, Ingrid Lossius-Falkum, Agustín Vicente

Two major trends on children's skills to comprehend metaphors have governed the literature on the subject: the literal stage hypothesis vs. the early birds hypothesis (Falkum, 2022). We aim to contribute to this debate by testing children's capability to comprehend novel metaphors ('X is a Y') in Spanish with a child-friendly, picture selection task, while also tracking their gaze. Further, given recent findings on the development of metonymy comprehension suggesting a U-shaped developmental curve for this phenomenon (Köder & Falkum, 2020), we aimed to determine the shape of the developmental trajectory of novel metaphor comprehension, and to explore how both types of data (picture selection and gaze behavior) relate to each other. Our results suggest a linear developmental trajectory with 6-year-olds significantly succeeding in picture selection and consistently looking at the metaphorical target even after question onset.

关于儿童理解隐喻技能的文献主要有两种趋势:文字阶段假说和早期鸟类假说(Falkum,2022 年)。我们旨在通过一项儿童友好型图片选择任务测试儿童理解西班牙语新隐喻("X 是 Y")的能力,同时跟踪他们的视线,从而为这一争论做出贡献。此外,鉴于最近关于隐喻理解能力发展的研究结果表明这一现象呈 U 型发展曲线(Köder & Falkum,2020 年),我们旨在确定新隐喻理解能力发展轨迹的形状,并探索这两类数据(图片选择和注视行为)之间的关系。我们的研究结果表明,6 岁儿童在图片选择和持续注视隐喻目标方面取得了显著成功,甚至在问题开始后也是如此。
{"title":"That kid is a grasshopper! Metaphor development from 3 to 9 years of age.","authors":"Isabel Martín-González, Camilo R Ronderos, Elena Castroviejo, Kristen Schroeder, Ingrid Lossius-Falkum, Agustín Vicente","doi":"10.1017/S0305000924000187","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000924000187","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Two major trends on children's skills to comprehend metaphors have governed the literature on the subject: the <i>literal stage</i> hypothesis vs. the <i>early birds</i> hypothesis (Falkum, 2022). We aim to contribute to this debate by testing children's capability to comprehend novel metaphors ('X is a Y') in Spanish with a child-friendly, picture selection task, while also tracking their gaze. Further, given recent findings on the development of metonymy comprehension suggesting a U-shaped developmental curve for this phenomenon (Köder & Falkum, 2020), we aimed to determine the shape of the developmental trajectory of novel metaphor comprehension, and to explore how both types of data (picture selection and gaze behavior) relate to each other. Our results suggest a linear developmental trajectory with 6-year-olds significantly succeeding in picture selection and consistently looking at the metaphorical target even after question onset.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":" ","pages":"1-26"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-05-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140913135","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Child Language
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1