首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Child Language最新文献

英文 中文
Examining Dutch children's vocabularies across infancy and toddlerhood: Demographic effects are age-specific and task-specific. 研究荷兰儿童在婴儿期和学步期的词汇量:人口统计学效应具有年龄特异性和任务特异性。
IF 2.2 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-05-10 DOI: 10.1017/S0305000924000199
Anika van der Klis, Caroline Junge, Frans Adriaans, René Kager

Limited studies have examined demographic differences in children's vocabulary in longitudinal samples, while there are questions regarding the duration, direction, and magnitude of these effects across development. In this longitudinal study, we included over 400 Dutch children. Caregivers filled out N-CDIs when children were 9-11 months (measuring word comprehension, word production, and gestures) and around 2-5 years of age (measuring word production). At 2-5 years, we also administered a receptive vocabulary task in the lab. We examined demographic effects on vocabulary size across infancy and toddlerhood. We found a disadvantage for males in infants' gestures and toddlers' vocabulary production. We found a negative effect of maternal education on infants' caregiver-reported vocabulary, but a positive effect on toddlers' lab-administered receptive vocabulary. Lastly, we found a negative effect of multilingualism - but only for the lab-administered task. Examining predictors in large, longitudinal samples ensures their robustness and generalisability across development.

对纵向样本中儿童词汇量的人口统计学差异进行的研究有限,而这些影响在整个发展过程中的持续时间、方向和程度也存在问题。在这项纵向研究中,我们纳入了 400 多名荷兰儿童。在儿童 9-11 个月大时(测量词语理解能力、造词能力和手势)和 2-5 岁左右(测量造词能力),照顾者填写了 N-CDI。2-5 岁时,我们还在实验室进行了接受性词汇任务。我们研究了人口统计学对婴儿期和学步期词汇量的影响。我们发现男性在婴儿手势和学步期词汇量方面处于劣势。我们发现,母亲的教育程度对婴儿的照顾者报告词汇量有负面影响,但对幼儿的实验室接受词汇量有正面影响。最后,我们还发现了多语言的负面影响,但仅限于实验操作任务。在大型纵向样本中研究预测因素可确保其在整个发展过程中的稳健性和普遍性。
{"title":"Examining Dutch children's vocabularies across infancy and toddlerhood: Demographic effects are age-specific and task-specific.","authors":"Anika van der Klis, Caroline Junge, Frans Adriaans, René Kager","doi":"10.1017/S0305000924000199","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000924000199","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Limited studies have examined demographic differences in children's vocabulary in longitudinal samples, while there are questions regarding the duration, direction, and magnitude of these effects across development. In this longitudinal study, we included over 400 Dutch children. Caregivers filled out N-CDIs when children were 9-11 months (measuring word comprehension, word production, and gestures) and around 2-5 years of age (measuring word production). At 2-5 years, we also administered a receptive vocabulary task in the lab. We examined demographic effects on vocabulary size across infancy and toddlerhood. We found a disadvantage for males in infants' gestures and toddlers' vocabulary production. We found a negative effect of maternal education on infants' caregiver-reported vocabulary, but a positive effect on toddlers' lab-administered receptive vocabulary. Lastly, we found a negative effect of multilingualism - but only for the lab-administered task. Examining predictors in large, longitudinal samples ensures their robustness and generalisability across development.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":" ","pages":"1-20"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-05-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140899447","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Assessing two methods of webcam-based eye-tracking for child language research. 评估用于儿童语言研究的两种网络摄像头眼动跟踪方法。
IF 2.2 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-05-07 DOI: 10.1017/S0305000924000175
Margaret Kandel, Jesse Snedeker

We assess the feasibility of conducting web-based eye-tracking experiments with children using two methods of webcam-based eye-tracking: automatic gaze estimation with the WebGazer.js algorithm and hand annotation of gaze direction from recorded webcam videos. Experiment 1 directly compares the two methods in a visual-world language task with five to six year-old children. Experiment 2 more precisely investigates WebGazer.js' spatiotemporal resolution with four to twelve year-old children in a visual-fixation task. We find that it is possible to conduct web-based eye-tracking experiments with children in both supervised (Experiment 1) and unsupervised (Experiment 2) settings - however, the webcam eye-tracking methods differ in their sensitivity and accuracy. Webcam video annotation is well-suited to detecting fine-grained looking effects relevant to child language researchers. In contrast, WebGazer.js gaze estimates appear noisier and less temporally precise. We discuss the advantages and disadvantages of each method and provide recommendations for researchers conducting child eye-tracking studies online.

我们使用两种基于网络摄像头的眼动跟踪方法对儿童进行网络眼动跟踪实验的可行性进行了评估:WebGazer.js 算法的自动注视估计和从录制的网络摄像头视频中对注视方向的手工标注。实验 1 直接比较了这两种方法在五至六岁儿童视觉世界语言任务中的应用。实验 2 更精确地研究了 WebGazer.js 的时空分辨率,实验对象是四至十二岁的儿童,进行的是视觉定格任务。我们发现,在有监督(实验 1)和无监督(实验 2)的情况下,对儿童进行基于网络的眼动跟踪实验是可行的--然而,网络摄像头眼动跟踪方法在灵敏度和准确性方面存在差异。网络摄像头视频注释非常适合检测与儿童语言研究人员相关的细粒度注视效果。相比之下,WebGazer.js 的注视估计值显得噪音较大,时间精确度较低。我们讨论了每种方法的优缺点,并为在线进行儿童眼动跟踪研究的研究人员提供了建议。
{"title":"Assessing two methods of webcam-based eye-tracking for child language research.","authors":"Margaret Kandel, Jesse Snedeker","doi":"10.1017/S0305000924000175","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000924000175","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We assess the feasibility of conducting web-based eye-tracking experiments with children using two methods of webcam-based eye-tracking: automatic gaze estimation with the WebGazer.js algorithm and hand annotation of gaze direction from recorded webcam videos. Experiment 1 directly compares the two methods in a visual-world language task with five to six year-old children. Experiment 2 more precisely investigates WebGazer.js' spatiotemporal resolution with four to twelve year-old children in a visual-fixation task. We find that it is possible to conduct web-based eye-tracking experiments with children in both supervised (Experiment 1) and unsupervised (Experiment 2) settings - however, the webcam eye-tracking methods differ in their sensitivity and accuracy. Webcam video annotation is well-suited to detecting fine-grained looking effects relevant to child language researchers. In contrast, WebGazer.js gaze estimates appear noisier and less temporally precise. We discuss the advantages and disadvantages of each method and provide recommendations for researchers conducting child eye-tracking studies online.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":" ","pages":"1-34"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-05-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140873705","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Development of derivational morphological knowledge in monolingual and bilingual children: Effects of modality and lexicality. 单语和双语儿童派生形态知识的发展:模态和词性的影响。
IF 2.2 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-05-07 DOI: 10.1017/S0305000924000126
Tamar Michaly, Anat Prior

This study mapped the trajectory of developing derivational morphological knowledge in Hebrew monolingual and Russian-Hebrew bilingual children. We investigated 2nd and 4th graders, using a two-by-two structure along the dimensions of modality (comprehension, production) and type of word (real-word, pseudo-word). Performance in the morphological analogies comprehension tasks improved with grade, and monolingual and bilingual children performed equally well. A different pattern was evident in production tasks. In real-word production, monolingual children were more accurate than bilingual children, but this group difference narrowed with age. In pseudo-word production, monolingual children used more morphological elements than bilingual children, and there was also a tendency towards group differences narrowing with age. Detailed error analyses across all tasks revealed that monolingual children recruited more morphological elements than bilingual children. We present implications for assessment of morphological knowledge, and suggest that morphological intervention is a promising avenue for promoting bilingual children's success.

本研究描绘了希伯来语单语儿童和俄语-希伯来语双语儿童派生词形态知识的发展轨迹。我们对二年级和四年级的学生进行了调查,采用的是一种两两结构,即模式(理解、制作)和词的类型(实词、假词)。形态类比理解任务的成绩随年级的提高而提高,单语和双语儿童的成绩相当。在造词任务中,则出现了不同的模式。在实词拼写中,单语儿童比双语儿童更准确,但随着年龄的增长,这一群体差异逐渐缩小。在假词生成中,单语儿童比双语儿童使用更多的形态要素,而且随着年龄的增长,组间差异也有缩小的趋势。对所有任务的详细错误分析表明,单语儿童比双语儿童使用了更多的形态要素。我们介绍了形态学知识评估的意义,并建议形态学干预是促进双语儿童成功的一个有前途的途径。
{"title":"Development of derivational morphological knowledge in monolingual and bilingual children: Effects of modality and lexicality.","authors":"Tamar Michaly, Anat Prior","doi":"10.1017/S0305000924000126","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000924000126","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study mapped the trajectory of developing derivational morphological knowledge in Hebrew monolingual and Russian-Hebrew bilingual children. We investigated 2<sup>nd</sup> and 4<sup>th</sup> graders, using a two-by-two structure along the dimensions of modality (comprehension, production) and type of word (real-word, pseudo-word). Performance in the morphological analogies comprehension tasks improved with grade, and monolingual and bilingual children performed equally well. A different pattern was evident in production tasks. In real-word production, monolingual children were more accurate than bilingual children, but this group difference narrowed with age. In pseudo-word production, monolingual children used more morphological elements than bilingual children, and there was also a tendency towards group differences narrowing with age. Detailed error analyses across all tasks revealed that monolingual children recruited more morphological elements than bilingual children. We present implications for assessment of morphological knowledge, and suggest that morphological intervention is a promising avenue for promoting bilingual children's success.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":" ","pages":"1-26"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-05-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140852393","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Revisiting frequencies of phonological sound classes in speech input: Change over time in child-directed speech. 重新审视语音输入中语音类的频率:儿童定向语音随时间的变化。
IF 2.2 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-18 DOI: 10.1017/S0305000923000570
Sue Ann S Lee, Jaehoon Lee, Barbara L Davis

The purpose of the current study was to revisit a controversial topic: whether frequencies of phonological consonant and vowel classes differ in speech directed to children and to adults. In addition, the current study investigated whether the frequency of phonological consonant and vowel classes changes with children's increasing chronological and/or developmental age. This study analyzed speech input from 44 adults to four different age groups of listeners (i.e., three groups of children at 6, 18, and 36 months of age and one group of adult listeners) in terms of frequency of occurrence of consonant and vowel classes. Results revealed that consonant stop, nasal, fricative and glide manner classes as well as all four consonant place classes were significantly different in speech directed to the four different age groups. A perspective is discussed to better understand the nature of frequency input of phonological sound classes.

当前研究的目的是重新审视一个有争议的话题:语音辅音和元音类的频率在针对儿童和成人的语音中是否不同。此外,目前的研究调查了语音辅音和元音类别的频率是否随着儿童年龄的增长而变化。本研究分析了44名成年人对四个不同年龄组听众(即三组6、18和36个月大的儿童和一组成年听众)的语音输入,分析了辅音和元音类别的出现频率。结果表明,在针对四个不同年龄组的语音中,辅音停止、鼻音、擦音和滑音方式类别以及所有四个辅音位置类别都有显著差异。讨论了一个更好地理解语音类频率输入性质的视角。
{"title":"Revisiting frequencies of phonological sound classes in speech input: Change over time in child-directed speech.","authors":"Sue Ann S Lee, Jaehoon Lee, Barbara L Davis","doi":"10.1017/S0305000923000570","DOIUrl":"10.1017/S0305000923000570","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The purpose of the current study was to revisit a controversial topic: whether frequencies of phonological consonant and vowel classes differ in speech directed to children and to adults. In addition, the current study investigated whether the frequency of phonological consonant and vowel classes changes with children's increasing chronological and/or developmental age. This study analyzed speech input from 44 adults to four different age groups of listeners (i.e., three groups of children at 6, 18, and 36 months of age and one group of adult listeners) in terms of frequency of occurrence of consonant and vowel classes. Results revealed that consonant stop, nasal, fricative and glide manner classes as well as all four consonant place classes were significantly different in speech directed to the four different age groups. A perspective is discussed to better understand the nature of frequency input of phonological sound classes.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":" ","pages":"550-572"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41239902","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of masks on infants' ability to fast-map and generalize new words. 面具对婴儿快速映射和概括新词能力的影响。
IF 2.2 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-08 DOI: 10.1017/S0305000923000697
Siying Liu, Xun Li, Renji Sun

Young children today are exposed to masks on a regular basis. However, there is limited empirical evidence on how masks may affect word learning. The study explored the effect of masks on infants' abilities to fast-map and generalize new words. Seventy-two Chinese infants (43 males, Mage = 18.26 months) were taught two novel word-object pairs by a speaker with or without a mask. They then heard the words and had to visually identify the correct objects and also generalize words to a different speaker and objects from the same category. Eye-tracking results indicate that infants looked longer at the target regardless of whether a speaker wore a mask. They also looked longer at the speaker's eyes than at the mouth only when words were taught through a mask. Thus, fast-mapping and generalization occur in both masked and not masked conditions as infants can flexibly access different visual cues during word-learning.

现在的幼儿经常接触面具。然而,关于面具如何影响单词学习的实证证据却很有限。本研究探讨了面具对婴儿快速映射和概括新词能力的影响。72名中国婴儿(43名男婴,年龄=18.26个月)在有或没有面具的情况下接受了由说话者教授的两对新词-物的学习。然后,他们在听到单词后必须用视觉识别出正确的物体,并将单词归纳为不同的说话者和同一类别的物体。眼动跟踪结果显示,无论说话者是否戴面具,婴儿看目标物的时间都更长。当通过面具教授单词时,他们看说话者眼睛的时间也比只看嘴的时间长。因此,在戴面具和不戴面具的情况下都会出现快速映射和泛化,因为婴儿可以在单词学习过程中灵活地获取不同的视觉线索。
{"title":"The effect of masks on infants' ability to fast-map and generalize new words.","authors":"Siying Liu, Xun Li, Renji Sun","doi":"10.1017/S0305000923000697","DOIUrl":"10.1017/S0305000923000697","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Young children today are exposed to masks on a regular basis. However, there is limited empirical evidence on how masks may affect word learning. The study explored the effect of masks on infants' abilities to fast-map and generalize new words. Seventy-two Chinese infants (43 males, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 18.26 months) were taught two novel word-object pairs by a speaker with or without a mask. They then heard the words and had to visually identify the correct objects and also generalize words to a different speaker and objects from the same category. Eye-tracking results indicate that infants looked longer at the target regardless of whether a speaker wore a mask. They also looked longer at the speaker's eyes than at the mouth only when words were taught through a mask. Thus, fast-mapping and generalization occur in both masked and not masked conditions as infants can flexibly access different visual cues during word-learning.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":" ","pages":"637-655"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139378566","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cognates are advantaged over non-cognates in early bilingual expressive vocabulary development - ERRATUM. 在早期双语表达词汇发展过程中,认知词汇比非认知词汇更具优势 - ERRATUM.
IF 1.7 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-22 DOI: 10.1017/S0305000924000059
Lori Mitchell, Rachel Ka-Ying Tsui, Krista Byers-Heinlein
{"title":"Cognates are advantaged over non-cognates in early bilingual expressive vocabulary development - ERRATUM.","authors":"Lori Mitchell, Rachel Ka-Ying Tsui, Krista Byers-Heinlein","doi":"10.1017/S0305000924000059","DOIUrl":"10.1017/S0305000924000059","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":" ","pages":"720"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139933640","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Comprehension of indirect answers: Developmental trajectory for preschool- and early elementary school-aged children with typical development. 理解间接答案:学龄前和小学低年级典型发育儿童的发展轨迹。
IF 2.2 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-30 DOI: 10.1017/S0305000923000442
Timothy Huang, Lizbeth H Finestack

Indirect answers are a common type of non-literal language that do not provide an explicit "yes" or "no" to a question (e.g., "I have to work late" indirectly answered "Are you going to the party?" with a negative response). In the current study, we examined the developmental trajectory of comprehension of indirect answers among 5- to 10-year-old children with typical development. Forty-eight children, 23 boys and 25 girls, between the ages of 5 years; 0 months and 10 years; 11 months (M = 8;2, SD = 19.77 months) completed an experimental task to judge whether a verbally presented indirect answer meant yes or no (Comprehension Task) and then explain their choice (Explanation Task). Responses were scored for accuracy and coded for error analysis. On the Comprehension Task, the 5- to 8-year-olds performed with approximately 85% accuracy, while the 9- and 10-year-olds achieved 95% accuracy. On the Explanation Task, the cross-sectional trajectory revealed three stages: the 5- and 6-year-olds adequately explained indirect answers 32% of the time, the 7- and 8-year-olds performed significantly higher at 55%, and the 9- and 10-year-olds made significant gains than the younger children at 66%. Error analysis revealed that when children fail to interpret speaker intentions appropriately, they repeat the speaker's utterance or provide an insufficient explanation 80% of the time. Other responses, such as those irrelevant to the context, indicating "I don't know" or no response, or that were made-up interpretations each accounted for 2%-10% of total inadequate explanations. Study findings indicate discrepancies between task performances and offer two separate sets of baseline data for future comparisons that investigate comprehension or explanation of indirect answers by children with different cultural and linguistic backgrounds and by those with varying cognitive and language profiles.

间接回答是一种常见的非字面语言,它不对问题作出明确的 "是 "或 "否 "的回答(例如,"我要加班 "间接回答了 "你会去参加聚会吗?",并作出了否定的回答)。在本研究中,我们考察了 5-10 岁典型发育儿童理解间接回答的发展轨迹。48 名年龄在 5 岁 0 个月至 10 岁 11 个月之间的儿童(23 名男孩和 25 名女孩,中位数 = 8;2,标准差 = 19.77 个月)完成了一项实验任务,即判断口头给出的间接答案是 "是 "还是 "否"(理解任务),然后解释他们的选择(解释任务)。对回答的准确性进行评分,并对错误进行编码分析。在理解任务中,5-8 岁儿童的准确率约为 85%,而 9-10 岁儿童的准确率则达到了 95%。在解释任务中,横向轨迹显示出三个阶段:5 至 6 岁儿童在 32% 的时间里充分解释了间接答案,7 至 8 岁儿童的解释正确率显著提高,达到 55%,而 9 至 10 岁儿童的解释正确率显著提高,达到 66%。错误分析表明,当儿童不能正确理解说话者的意图时,他们会在 80% 的情况下重复说话者的话语或提供不充分的解释。其他回答,如与上下文无关的回答、表示 "我不知道 "或没有回答的回答,或者胡编乱造的解释,各占不充分解释总数的 2%-10%。研究结果表明了任务表现之间的差异,并提供了两组独立的基线数据,以便今后进行比较,研究具有不同文化和语言背景的儿童以及具有不同认知和语言特征的儿童对间接答案的理解或解释。
{"title":"Comprehension of indirect answers: Developmental trajectory for preschool- and early elementary school-aged children with typical development.","authors":"Timothy Huang, Lizbeth H Finestack","doi":"10.1017/S0305000923000442","DOIUrl":"10.1017/S0305000923000442","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Indirect answers are a common type of non-literal language that do not provide an explicit \"yes\" or \"no\" to a question (e.g., \"I have to work late\" indirectly answered \"Are you going to the party?\" with a negative response). In the current study, we examined the developmental trajectory of comprehension of indirect answers among 5- to 10-year-old children with typical development. Forty-eight children, 23 boys and 25 girls, between the ages of 5 years; 0 months and 10 years; 11 months (<i>M</i> = 8;2, <i>SD</i> = 19.77 months) completed an experimental task to judge whether a verbally presented indirect answer meant yes or no (Comprehension Task) and then explain their choice (Explanation Task). Responses were scored for accuracy and coded for error analysis. On the Comprehension Task, the 5- to 8-year-olds performed with approximately 85% accuracy, while the 9- and 10-year-olds achieved 95% accuracy. On the Explanation Task, the cross-sectional trajectory revealed three stages: the 5- and 6-year-olds adequately explained indirect answers 32% of the time, the 7- and 8-year-olds performed significantly higher at 55%, and the 9- and 10-year-olds made significant gains than the younger children at 66%. Error analysis revealed that when children fail to interpret speaker intentions appropriately, they repeat the speaker's utterance or provide an insufficient explanation 80% of the time. Other responses, such as those irrelevant to the context, indicating \"I don't know\" or no response, or that were made-up interpretations each accounted for 2%-10% of total inadequate explanations. Study findings indicate discrepancies between task performances and offer two separate sets of baseline data for future comparisons that investigate comprehension or explanation of indirect answers by children with different cultural and linguistic backgrounds and by those with varying cognitive and language profiles.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":" ","pages":"526-549"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10110486","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Acquisition of the feature [+spread glottis] in Icelandic. 获得冰岛语的[+扩展声门]特征。
IF 2.2 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-20 DOI: 10.1017/S0305000923000582
Thora Másdóttir, Barbara May Bernhardt, Joseph Paul Stemberger, Gunnar Ólafur Hansson

The feature [+spread glottis] ([+s.g.]) denotes that a speech sound is produced with a wide glottal aperture with audible voiceless airflow. Icelandic is unusual in the degree to which [+spread glottis] is involved in the phonology: in /h/, pre-aspirated and post-aspirated stops, voiceless fricatives and voiceless sonorants. The ubiquitousness of the feature could potentially affect the rate and process of its acquisition. This paper investigates the development of [+s.g.] in Icelandic, both in general and in a range of contexts, in a cross-sectional study of 433 typically developing Icelandic-speaking children aged two to seven years. As a feature, [+s.g.] is acquired early in Icelandic, although specific sound classes lag behind due to other output constraints. Children reach mastery of [+s.g.] by age three except in word-initial post-aspirated stops and voiceless nasals. Findings are interpreted in light of the literature on the feature and its development.

特征[+扩展声门]([+s.g.])表示语音是用宽声门孔和可听见的无声气流产生的。冰岛语的[+扩展声门]在音韵学中的参与程度是不寻常的:in/h/、送气前和送气后塞音、无声擦音和无声声符。该特征的普遍性可能会影响其获取的速度和过程。本文对433名2至7岁的冰岛语儿童进行了横断面研究,调查了冰岛语[+s.g.]在一般情况下和各种情况下的发展情况。作为一个功能,[+s.g.]在冰岛语中很早就获得了,尽管由于其他输出限制,特定的声音类别落后了。孩子们在三岁时就掌握了[+s.g.],但单词首字母后送气塞音和无声鼻音除外。研究结果是根据有关该特征及其发展的文献进行解释的。
{"title":"Acquisition of the feature [+spread glottis] in Icelandic.","authors":"Thora Másdóttir, Barbara May Bernhardt, Joseph Paul Stemberger, Gunnar Ólafur Hansson","doi":"10.1017/S0305000923000582","DOIUrl":"10.1017/S0305000923000582","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The feature [+spread glottis] ([+s.g.]) denotes that a speech sound is produced with a wide glottal aperture with audible voiceless airflow. Icelandic is unusual in the degree to which [+spread glottis] is involved in the phonology: in /h/, pre-aspirated and post-aspirated stops, voiceless fricatives and voiceless sonorants. The ubiquitousness of the feature could potentially affect the rate and process of its acquisition. This paper investigates the development of [+s.g.] in Icelandic, both in general and in a range of contexts, in a cross-sectional study of 433 typically developing Icelandic-speaking children aged two to seven years. As a feature, [+s.g.] is acquired early in Icelandic, although specific sound classes lag behind due to other output constraints. Children reach mastery of [+s.g.] by age three except in word-initial post-aspirated stops and voiceless nasals. Findings are interpreted in light of the literature on the feature and its development.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":" ","pages":"573-595"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49683665","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Structure and acoustics of the speech of verbal autistic preschoolers. 言语自闭症学龄前儿童的语音结构和声学。
IF 2.2 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-07-10 DOI: 10.1017/S0305000923000417
Pauline Maes, Marielle Weyland, Mikhail Kissine

In this study, we report an extensive investigation of the structural language and acoustical specificities of the spontaneous speech of ten three- to five-year-old verbal autistic children. The autistic children were compared to a group of ten typically developing children matched pairwise on chronological age, nonverbal IQ and socioeconomic status, and groupwise on verbal IQ and gender on various measures of structural language (phonetic inventory, lexical diversity and morpho-syntactic complexity) and a series of acoustical measures of speech (mean and range fundamental frequency, a formant dispersion index, syllable duration, jitter and shimmer). Results showed that, overall, the structure and acoustics of the verbal autistic children's speech were highly similar to those of the TD children. Few remaining atypicalities in the speech of autistic children lay in a restricted use of different vocabulary items, a somewhat diminished morpho-syntactic complexity, and a slightly exaggerated syllable duration.

在本研究中,我们报告了对十名三至五岁会说话的自闭症儿童自发言语的语言结构和声学特异性的广泛调查。我们将这些自闭症儿童与十名发育正常的儿童进行了比较,这十名儿童在年龄、非语言智商和社会经济地位方面是配对的,在语言智商和性别方面则是分组的,比较的内容包括各种语言结构测量(语音清单、词汇多样性和形态句法复杂性)和一系列语音声学测量(基频的平均值和范围、共格分散指数、音节持续时间、抖动和颤音)。结果表明,总体而言,言语自闭症儿童的语音结构和声学特征与言语发育障碍儿童高度相似。自闭症儿童言语中仅存的一些非典型特征包括对不同词汇的使用受到限制、形态句法的复杂性略有降低以及音节持续时间略显夸张。
{"title":"Structure and acoustics of the speech of verbal autistic preschoolers.","authors":"Pauline Maes, Marielle Weyland, Mikhail Kissine","doi":"10.1017/S0305000923000417","DOIUrl":"10.1017/S0305000923000417","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In this study, we report an extensive investigation of the structural language and acoustical specificities of the spontaneous speech of ten three- to five-year-old verbal autistic children. The autistic children were compared to a group of ten typically developing children matched pairwise on chronological age, nonverbal IQ and socioeconomic status, and groupwise on verbal IQ and gender on various measures of structural language (phonetic inventory, lexical diversity and morpho-syntactic complexity) and a series of acoustical measures of speech (mean and range fundamental frequency, a formant dispersion index, syllable duration, jitter and shimmer). Results showed that, overall, the structure and acoustics of the verbal autistic children's speech were highly similar to those of the TD children. Few remaining atypicalities in the speech of autistic children lay in a restricted use of different vocabulary items, a somewhat diminished morpho-syntactic complexity, and a slightly exaggerated syllable duration.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":" ","pages":"509-525"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9765045","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
"Mom said it in quotation marks!" Irony comprehension and metapragmatic awareness in 8-year-olds. "妈妈用引号说的!"8 岁儿童的反讽理解能力和隐喻意识。
IF 2.2 2区 文学 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-07-10 DOI: 10.1017/S0305000923000399
Sarah Garfinkel, Meredith L Rowe, Sandra Bosacki, Natalia Banasik-Jemielniak

This study investigated links between the development of children's understanding of ironic comments and their metapragmatic knowledge. Forty-six 8-year-olds completed the short version of the Irony Comprehension Task, during which they were presented with ironic comments in three stories and asked to provide reasons for why the speaker in a story uttered an ironic comment. We coded their responses and compared the results to similar data collected previously with 5-year-olds. Results showed that compared to younger children, 8-year-olds frequently refer to interlocutors' emotions, intentions, and to metapragmatics. These results support the view that comprehension of verbal irony is an emerging skill in children.

本研究调查了儿童对讽刺性评论的理解能力的发展与他们的元语用知识之间的联系。46 名 8 岁儿童完成了简短版的 "反讽理解任务",在该任务中,我们向他们展示了三个故事中的反讽性评论,并要求他们提供故事中说话者为何发表反讽性评论的原因。我们对他们的回答进行了编码,并将结果与之前收集的 5 岁儿童的类似数据进行了比较。结果显示,与年龄较小的儿童相比,8 岁儿童经常提到对话者的情绪、意图和隐喻。这些结果支持了这样一种观点,即对言语反讽的理解是儿童的一种新兴技能。
{"title":"\"Mom said it in quotation marks!\" Irony comprehension and metapragmatic awareness in 8-year-olds.","authors":"Sarah Garfinkel, Meredith L Rowe, Sandra Bosacki, Natalia Banasik-Jemielniak","doi":"10.1017/S0305000923000399","DOIUrl":"10.1017/S0305000923000399","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study investigated links between the development of children's understanding of ironic comments and their metapragmatic knowledge. Forty-six 8-year-olds completed the short version of the Irony Comprehension Task, during which they were presented with ironic comments in three stories and asked to provide reasons for why the speaker in a story uttered an ironic comment. We coded their responses and compared the results to similar data collected previously with 5-year-olds. Results showed that compared to younger children, 8-year-olds frequently refer to interlocutors' emotions, intentions, and to metapragmatics. These results support the view that comprehension of verbal irony is an emerging skill in children.</p>","PeriodicalId":48132,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Child Language","volume":" ","pages":"485-508"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9765043","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Child Language
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1