首页 > 最新文献

Reading and Writing最新文献

英文 中文
Current writing assessment practices of kindergarten through second grade educators 幼儿园至二年级教育工作者当前的写作评估实践
IF 2.5 2区 教育学 Q2 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH Pub Date : 2024-01-13 DOI: 10.1007/s11145-023-10503-6

Abstract

This article describes the development and administration of the Kindergarten-Second Grade (K-2) Writing Data-Based Decision Making (DBDM) Survey. The K-2 Writing DBDM Survey was developed to learn more about current DBDM practices specific to early writing. A total of 376 educational professionals (175 general education classroom teachers, 42 special education teachers, 118 speech-language pathologists, and 41 specialists (e.g., interventionists, coaches, educators supporting emergent bilinguals) who work with kindergarten, first, and/or second grade students in school settings in the United States participated in this survey study. Results include participant responses to 32 closed-ended items and 2 open-ended items covering five domains: (a) demographic information; (b) assessment(s) types, (c) administration procedures, (d) scoring procedures, and (e) information gathered. Descriptive and regression analyses were applied to summarize the survey results. Findings from this survey indicated that even with limited or no guidance, some educators within all groups of professionals surveyed are assessing students. However, ongoing improvements need to be made to prioritize writing upon school entry. These results remind us of the importance of partnership with educators to ensure accurate understanding of resources available for assessment, current areas of need, and how implementation can feasibly occur in the practice setting when developing and studying approaches to assessment.

摘要 本文介绍了幼儿园至二年级(K-2)基于数据的写作决策(DBDM)调查的开发和管理。开发 K-2 写作 DBDM 调查的目的是为了更多了解当前针对早期写作的 DBDM 实践。共有 376 名教育专业人士(175 名普通教育班级教师、42 名特殊教育教师、118 名言语病理学家和 41 名专家(如干预专家、教练、支持新兴双语学生的教育工作者)参加了此次调查研究,他们在美国的学校环境中为幼儿园、一年级和/或二年级的学生提供服务。调查结果包括参与者对 32 个封闭式项目和 2 个开放式项目的回答,涵盖五个方面:(a) 人口统计信息;(b) 评估类型;(c) 管理程序;(d) 评分程序;以及 (e) 收集的信息。对调查结果进行了描述性分析和回归分析。调查结果表明,即使在指导有限或没有指导的情况下,接受调查的各组专业人员中也 有一些教育工作者正在对学生进行评估。但是,还需要不断改进,以便在学生入学时优先考虑写作。这些结果提醒我们,在开发和研究评估方法时,与教育工作者合作的重要性,以确保准确了解现有的评估资源、当前的需求领域,以及如何在实践环境中可行地实施评估。
{"title":"Current writing assessment practices of kindergarten through second grade educators","authors":"","doi":"10.1007/s11145-023-10503-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-023-10503-6","url":null,"abstract":"<h3>Abstract</h3> <p>This article describes the development and administration of the <em>Kindergarten-Second Grade (K-2) Writing Data-Based Decision Making (DBDM) Survey</em>. The <em>K-2 Writing DBDM Survey</em> was developed to learn more about current DBDM practices specific to early writing. A total of 376 educational professionals (175 general education classroom teachers, 42 special education teachers, 118 speech-language pathologists, and 41 specialists (e.g., interventionists, coaches, educators supporting emergent bilinguals) who work with kindergarten, first, and/or second grade students in school settings in the United States participated in this survey study. Results include participant responses to 32 closed-ended items and 2 open-ended items covering five domains: (a) demographic information; (b) assessment(s) types, (c) administration procedures, (d) scoring procedures, and (e) information gathered. Descriptive and regression analyses were applied to summarize the survey results. Findings from this survey indicated that even with limited or no guidance, some educators within all groups of professionals surveyed are assessing students. However, ongoing improvements need to be made to prioritize writing upon school entry. These results remind us of the importance of partnership with educators to ensure accurate understanding of resources available for assessment, current areas of need, and how implementation can feasibly occur in the practice setting when developing and studying approaches to assessment.</p>","PeriodicalId":48204,"journal":{"name":"Reading and Writing","volume":"11 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-01-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139459065","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Literacy instruction from afar: evidence for the effectiveness of a remotely delivered language-rich reading programme 远距离识字教学:远程提供语言丰富的阅读课程的有效性证据
IF 2.5 2区 教育学 Q2 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH Pub Date : 2024-01-06 DOI: 10.1007/s11145-023-10502-7

Abstract

There is good evidence that high-quality instruction targeting reading-related skills in the classroom leads to gains in reading. However, considerably less is known about the possible efficacy of remote instruction. This study evaluated the efficacy of an interactive evidence-based language-rich literacy programme. 184 children were randomly allocated either to an 8-week remotely delivered language-rich literacy programme or to a wait-list control group. Children in the programme arm (n = 77 at analysis) completed 16-lessons remotely targeting vocabulary, phonemic awareness, reading, spelling, and narrative skills. Children in the wait-list arm (n = 58 at analysis) received business-as-usal from their schools. Children’s word reading accuracy and phonemic awareness was measured prior to and after the programme delivery period. Children receiving the literacy programme made significantly larger gains than the wait-list control group on reading accuracy (d = 0.32) and phonemic awareness (d = 0.63). This study demonstrates that a remotely-delivered literacy programme is effective. These findings have important implications for delivering specialist literacy instruction at scale.

摘要 有充分的证据表明,在课堂上针对与阅读有关的技能进行高质量的教学会提高学生的阅读能力。然而,人们对远程教学可能产生的效果却知之甚少。本研究评估了以证据为基础的互动式语言丰富识字课程的效果。184 名儿童被随机分配到一个为期 8 周的远程语言识字课程或一个等待名单对照组。参加课程组的儿童(分析时为 77 人)完成了 16 节远程课程,课程内容包括词汇、语音意识、阅读、拼写和叙事技能。等待组的儿童(分析时人数为 58 人)则接受学校的照常授课。在计划实施前和实施后,对儿童的单词阅读准确率和语音意识进行了测量。接受扫盲计划的儿童在阅读准确性(d = 0.32)和语音意识(d = 0.63)方面的进步明显高于等待对照组。这项研究表明,远程扫盲计划是有效的。这些研究结果对大规模开展专业识字教学具有重要意义。
{"title":"Literacy instruction from afar: evidence for the effectiveness of a remotely delivered language-rich reading programme","authors":"","doi":"10.1007/s11145-023-10502-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-023-10502-7","url":null,"abstract":"<h3>Abstract</h3> <p>There is good evidence that high-quality instruction targeting reading-related skills in the classroom leads to gains in reading. However, considerably less is known about the possible efficacy of <em>remote</em> instruction. This study evaluated the efficacy of an interactive evidence-based language-rich literacy programme. 184 children were randomly allocated either to an 8-week remotely delivered language-rich literacy programme or to a wait-list control group. Children in the programme arm (<em>n</em> = 77 at analysis) completed 16-lessons remotely targeting vocabulary, phonemic awareness, reading, spelling, and narrative skills. Children in the wait-list arm (<em>n</em> = 58 at analysis) received business-as-usal from their schools. Children’s word reading accuracy and phonemic awareness was measured prior to and after the programme delivery period. Children receiving the literacy programme made significantly larger gains than the wait-list control group on reading accuracy (<em>d</em> = 0.32) and phonemic awareness (<em>d</em> = 0.63). This study demonstrates that a remotely-delivered literacy programme is effective. These findings have important implications for delivering specialist literacy instruction at scale.</p>","PeriodicalId":48204,"journal":{"name":"Reading and Writing","volume":"73 3 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-01-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139373533","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Testing the model of a proficient academic reader (PAR) in a postsecondary context 在中学后环境中测试熟练学术阅读者(PAR)模式
IF 2.5 2区 教育学 Q2 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH Pub Date : 2024-01-03 DOI: 10.1007/s11145-023-10500-9
Gal Kaldes, Karyn Higgs, Jodi Lampi, Alecia Santuzzi, Stephen M. Tonks, Tenaha O’Reilly, John P. Sabatini, Joseph P. Magliano

The current research used the Proficient Academic Reader (PAR) framework to explore whether reading strategies, task awareness, and motivation predicted college students’ literacy skills over and above foundational skills (e.g., decoding, vocabulary). Specifically, the current research investigated the unique contribution of the PAR constructs to literacy performance across two studies with two different samples of college students. In study one, college students completed assessments of bridging and elaborative reading strategies, task awareness, motivation (intrinsic motivation and competence beliefs), foundational skills, and literacy performance at the beginning of the semester. In study two, college students completed the same assessments at the beginning and end of a reading study and strategies course. Across both studies, students’ task awareness and motivation were significantly predictive of their literacy performance over and above foundational skills. Results from study one indicated that elaborative reading strategies uniquely predicted college students’ literacy performance. Results from study two indicated that elaborative strategies did not predict literacy performance at time one, however, they predicted literacy performance at time two. Exploratory analyses showed that the relation of motivation to literacy performance was moderated by students’ enrollment in developmental education courses. Additionally, motivation, elaborative reading strategies, and task awareness partially mediated the relation of foundational skills to literacy performance, suggesting modifications to the original PAR model. These findings support using the PAR framework to understand college reading readiness. Additional randomized controlled trial intervention studies are warranted to explore if factors of the PAR framework are malleable to classroom instruction.

目前的研究使用 "熟练学术阅读"(PAR)框架来探讨阅读策略、任务意识和动机是否能预测大学生的读写技能,而不是基础技能(如解码、词汇)。具体来说,目前的研究通过两项针对两个不同大学生样本的研究,调查了 PAR 构建对识字成绩的独特贡献。在研究一中,大学生在学期开始时完成了对桥接和精细化阅读策略、任务意识、动机(内在动机和能力信念)、基础技能和识字成绩的评估。在研究二中,大学生在阅读学习和策略课程开始和结束时完成了同样的评估。在这两项研究中,学生的任务意识和动机对其读写成绩的预测作用明显高于基础技能。研究一的结果表明,精心设计的阅读策略对大学生的读写成绩有独特的预测作用。研究二的结果表明,精心设计的策略并不能预测第一阶段的识字成绩,但却能预测第二阶段的识字成绩。探索性分析表明,学习动机与识字成绩之间的关系受学生是否参加发展教育课程的影响。此外,动机、精心设计的阅读策略和任务意识在一定程度上调节了基础技能与识字成绩的关系,这表明对最初的 PAR 模型进行了修改。这些研究结果支持使用 PAR 框架来了解大学阅读准备情况。我们有必要进行更多的随机对照试验干预研究,以探索 PAR 框架中的因素是否可用于课堂教学。
{"title":"Testing the model of a proficient academic reader (PAR) in a postsecondary context","authors":"Gal Kaldes, Karyn Higgs, Jodi Lampi, Alecia Santuzzi, Stephen M. Tonks, Tenaha O’Reilly, John P. Sabatini, Joseph P. Magliano","doi":"10.1007/s11145-023-10500-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-023-10500-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The current research used the Proficient Academic Reader (PAR) framework to explore whether reading strategies, task awareness, and motivation predicted college students’ literacy skills over and above foundational skills (e.g., decoding, vocabulary). Specifically, the current research investigated the unique contribution of the PAR constructs to literacy performance across two studies with two different samples of college students. In study one, college students completed assessments of bridging and elaborative reading strategies, task awareness, motivation (intrinsic motivation and competence beliefs), foundational skills, and literacy performance at the beginning of the semester. In study two, college students completed the same assessments at the beginning and end of a reading study and strategies course. Across both studies, students’ task awareness and motivation were significantly predictive of their literacy performance over and above foundational skills. Results from study one indicated that elaborative reading strategies uniquely predicted college students’ literacy performance. Results from study two indicated that elaborative strategies did not predict literacy performance at time one, however, they predicted literacy performance at time two. Exploratory analyses showed that the relation of motivation to literacy performance was moderated by students’ enrollment in developmental education courses. Additionally, motivation, elaborative reading strategies, and task awareness partially mediated the relation of foundational skills to literacy performance, suggesting modifications to the original PAR model. These findings support using the PAR framework to understand college reading readiness. Additional randomized controlled trial intervention studies are warranted to explore if factors of the PAR framework are malleable to classroom instruction.</p>","PeriodicalId":48204,"journal":{"name":"Reading and Writing","volume":"22 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-01-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139373361","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring the frequency contours in close reading texts 探索精读课文中的频率轮廓
IF 2.5 2区 教育学 Q2 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH Pub Date : 2024-01-02 DOI: 10.1007/s11145-023-10493-5
Heidi Anne E. Mesmer

Many initiatives have emphasized the importance of challenging students in text using readability formulas. Almost all formulas employ mean word frequency yet long-standing issues abound. Researchers question using a singular mean with a skewed variable like frequency. They also question the degree to which frequency pinpoints complex words, noting that infrequent words can be rare, but not rich (e.g., smock). Unfortunately, no studies have examined how often this phenomenon occurs. In fact, word frequency is a “black box” because we do not know the actual words represented at different frequency points. This study used a new frequency metric to analyze 249 texts designed for close reading. Texts averaged 640 words and were written for Grades 2–8. For each text, curves were compared by grade and type. Words at each point on the curve were identified and analyzed. Findings illustrated the curves of word frequency throughout a text and unpacked the actual words at different points along the curves. Regardless of grade or type, curves were nearly identical in shape. Curves were characterized by four frequency strata. At the lowest frequency level were the rich, rare words that advance learning. However, analyses showed that between 34 and 72% of the time these least frequent words were rare but not rich, a pattern that occurred more frequently in narrative texts. Findings also showed that narratives were characterized by one very rare word that could influence the mean word frequency. The study included a series of graphics illustrating the functions of words at different tiers and listing the words along the curves.

许多倡议都强调了在课文中使用可读性公式向学生提出挑战的重要性。几乎所有的公式都采用平均词频,但长期以来问题重重。研究人员对使用单数平均值和词频这样的倾斜变量提出了质疑。他们还质疑词频对复杂词的定位程度,指出不常见的词可能是罕见的,但并不丰富(如 smock)。遗憾的是,还没有研究探讨过这种现象发生的频率。事实上,词频是一个 "黑箱",因为我们不知道不同频率点所代表的实际词语。本研究使用了一种新的词频指标来分析 249 篇为精读而设计的文章。这些课文平均字数为 640 个,适用于二年级至八年级。对于每篇课文,按年级和类型进行曲线比较。对曲线上每个点的单词进行了识别和分析。研究结果表明了整篇文章的词频曲线,并揭示了曲线上不同点的实际词语。无论年级或类型如何,曲线的形状几乎相同。曲线有四个词频层。频率最低的是内容丰富、罕见的单词,这些单词能促进学习。然而,分析表明,在 34% 到 72% 的时间里,这些出现频率最低的词语是罕见的,但并不丰富,这种模式在记叙文中出现的频率更高。研究结果还显示,叙事文中有一个非常罕见的词,可能会影响平均词频。该研究包括一系列图表,说明了不同层级词语的功能,并沿着曲线列出了词语。
{"title":"Exploring the frequency contours in close reading texts","authors":"Heidi Anne E. Mesmer","doi":"10.1007/s11145-023-10493-5","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-023-10493-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Many initiatives have emphasized the importance of challenging students in text using readability formulas. Almost all formulas employ mean word frequency yet long-standing issues abound. Researchers question using a singular mean with a skewed variable like frequency. They also question the degree to which frequency pinpoints complex words, noting that infrequent words can be rare, but not rich (e.g., smock). Unfortunately, no studies have examined how often this phenomenon occurs. In fact, word frequency is a “black box” because we do not know the <i>actual</i> words represented at different frequency points. This study used a new frequency metric to analyze 249 texts designed for close reading. Texts averaged 640 words and were written for Grades 2–8. For each text, curves were compared by grade and type. Words at each point on the curve were identified and analyzed. Findings illustrated the curves of word frequency throughout a text and unpacked the actual words at different points along the curves. Regardless of grade or type, curves were nearly identical in shape. Curves were characterized by four frequency strata. At the lowest frequency level were the rich, rare words that advance learning. However, analyses showed that between 34 and 72% of the time these least frequent words were rare but not rich, a pattern that occurred more frequently in narrative texts. Findings also showed that narratives were characterized by one very rare word that could influence the mean word frequency. The study included a series of graphics illustrating the functions of words at different tiers and listing the words along the curves.</p>","PeriodicalId":48204,"journal":{"name":"Reading and Writing","volume":"34 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139079176","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dimensionality of writing attitude, strategic use, and confidence: an investigation on Grade 4 students in a largely Hispanic setting 写作态度、策略运用和自信心的维度:对以西班牙裔为主的四年级学生的调查
IF 2.5 2区 教育学 Q2 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH Pub Date : 2023-12-26 DOI: 10.1007/s11145-023-10499-z

Abstract

Writing attitude, strategy use, and confidence surveys help educators understand how students perceive writing and cope with writing challenges in schools. The dimensions (i.e., constructs reflected by survey items) of these surveys have been studied in general students in the U.S., and we investigated how these surveys would reflect the dimensions in Grade 4 students taught in a largely Hispanic setting. We conducted factor analyses and measurement invariant analyses to examine the dimensions in each survey and dimension consistencies between male and female and Hispanic and Non-Hispanic students. We found that attitude and strategy use are both unidimensional, and confidence can be divided into confidences in convention, ideation, and self-regulation. Strict measurement invariance evidence suggested same dimensions between male and female and Hispanic and Non-Hispanic students. Therefore, researchers can interpret results from each survey similarly across male and female and Hispanic and Non-Hispanic students.

摘要 写作态度、策略使用和信心调查有助于教育工作者了解学生如何看待写作以及如何应对学校中的写作挑战。这些调查的维度(即调查项目所反映的结构)已在美国的普通学生中进行过研究,我们研究了这些调查将如何反映在主要由西班牙裔学生任教的四年级学生中的维度。我们进行了因子分析和测量不变量分析,以研究每项调查中的维度,以及男女生、西班牙裔学生和非西班牙裔学生之间的维度一致性。我们发现,态度和策略的使用都是单维度的,而自信可分为习惯自信、思想自信和自我调节自信。严格的测量不变性证据表明,男女学生、西班牙裔学生和非西班牙裔学生的测量维度相同。因此,研究人员可以对男女学生、西班牙裔学生和非西班牙裔学生的调查结果进行类似的解释。
{"title":"Dimensionality of writing attitude, strategic use, and confidence: an investigation on Grade 4 students in a largely Hispanic setting","authors":"","doi":"10.1007/s11145-023-10499-z","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-023-10499-z","url":null,"abstract":"<h3>Abstract</h3> <p>Writing attitude, strategy use, and confidence surveys help educators understand how students perceive writing and cope with writing challenges in schools. The dimensions (i.e., constructs reflected by survey items) of these surveys have been studied in general students in the U.S., and we investigated how these surveys would reflect the dimensions in Grade 4 students taught in a largely Hispanic setting. We conducted factor analyses and measurement invariant analyses to examine the dimensions in each survey and dimension consistencies between male and female and Hispanic and Non-Hispanic students. We found that attitude and strategy use are both unidimensional, and confidence can be divided into confidences in convention, ideation, and self-regulation. Strict measurement invariance evidence suggested same dimensions between male and female and Hispanic and Non-Hispanic students. Therefore, researchers can interpret results from each survey similarly across male and female and Hispanic and Non-Hispanic students.</p>","PeriodicalId":48204,"journal":{"name":"Reading and Writing","volume":"72 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-12-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139052519","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction: Do changes in perceived teacher behaviour predict changes in intrinsic reading motivation? A five-wave analysis in German lower secondary school students 更正:教师行为感知的变化能预测内在阅读动机的变化吗?对德国初中学生进行的五波分析
IF 2.5 2区 教育学 Q2 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH Pub Date : 2023-12-21 DOI: 10.1007/s11145-023-10505-4
A. Hawrot, Ji Zhou
{"title":"Correction: Do changes in perceived teacher behaviour predict changes in intrinsic reading motivation? A five-wave analysis in German lower secondary school students","authors":"A. Hawrot, Ji Zhou","doi":"10.1007/s11145-023-10505-4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-023-10505-4","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48204,"journal":{"name":"Reading and Writing","volume":"33 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-12-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138948509","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
English reading performance by Spanish speaking children: A phonologically or semantically mediated pathway? 西班牙语儿童的英语阅读能力:以语音还是语义为中介的途径?
IF 2.5 2区 教育学 Q2 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH Pub Date : 2023-12-12 DOI: 10.1007/s11145-023-10497-1
Paz Suárez‐Coalla, Luis Castejón, Marina Vega-Harwood, Cristina Martínez-García
{"title":"English reading performance by Spanish speaking children: A phonologically or semantically mediated pathway?","authors":"Paz Suárez‐Coalla, Luis Castejón, Marina Vega-Harwood, Cristina Martínez-García","doi":"10.1007/s11145-023-10497-1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-023-10497-1","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48204,"journal":{"name":"Reading and Writing","volume":"28 6","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-12-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139007765","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sensitivity to derivational morphology as cues to lexical stress among English as second language learners 英语作为第二语言学习者对派生词词形作为词汇重音线索的敏感性
IF 2.5 2区 教育学 Q2 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH Pub Date : 2023-12-12 DOI: 10.1007/s11145-023-10496-2
Jinglei Ren, Min Wang

Derivational suffixes are known to play a crucial role in assigning stress to multi-syllabic words among native English speakers. However, it is unclear whether second language (L2) learners of English can effectively use derivational suffixes as stress cues in written words. To address this gap, we studied if native Chinese-speaking adults learning English as an L2 can use derivational cues to correctly assign lexical stress, and whether the frequency of these suffixes and their L2 language proficiency moderate this sensitivity. Utilizing a written stress assignment and a stress production task, participants saw a series of written stems and their derived forms and were asked to either choose a syllable to assign the stress (Experiment 1) or read it aloud (Experiment 2). Results showed that L2 learners are sensitive to derivational cues to lexical stress in English. Moreover, the strength of this sensitivity is dependent upon the suffix class, frequency, and L2 language proficiency. Specifically, in both experiments, participants made more correct stress shifts in derived forms with high compared to low nonneutral suffix frequency; however, it did not differ between high and low neutral suffix frequency. In addition, as participants’ English proficiency increased, their correct stress shifts increased significantly greater in those derived nonwords with nonneutral than neutral suffixes in the stress assignment task. These findings make a significant contribution to the limited literature on L2 learners' sensitivity to morpho-orthographic cues to lexical stress and support the proposed statistical learning mechanism, that is, L2 learners can implicitly learn statistical regularities from linguistic materials.

衍生后缀在多音节单词的重音分配中起着至关重要的作用。然而,第二语言(L2)英语学习者是否能够有效地使用衍生后缀作为书面单词的重音线索尚不清楚。为了解决这一差距,我们研究了母语为汉语的成年人在学习英语作为第二语言时是否可以使用衍生线索来正确分配词汇重音,以及这些后缀的频率和他们的第二语言水平是否会调节这种敏感性。利用书面重音分配和重音产生任务,参与者看到一系列书面词干及其衍生形式,并被要求选择一个音节来分配重音(实验1)或大声朗读(实验2)。结果表明,第二语言学习者对衍生线索敏感。此外,这种敏感性的强弱取决于后缀的类别、使用频率和第二语言的熟练程度。具体来说,在这两个实验中,参与者在衍生形式中高非中性词缀频率比低词缀频率做出更正确的应力转移;中性词词频高低无明显差异。此外,当被试的英语熟练程度提高时,他们在重音分配任务中对非中性衍生非词的正确重音转移的提高显著高于中性衍生非词。这些研究结果对目前有限的关于二语学习者对词法正字法线索对词汇重音的敏感性的研究做出了重大贡献,并支持了提出的统计学习机制,即二语学习者可以隐性地从语言材料中学习统计规律。
{"title":"Sensitivity to derivational morphology as cues to lexical stress among English as second language learners","authors":"Jinglei Ren, Min Wang","doi":"10.1007/s11145-023-10496-2","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-023-10496-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Derivational suffixes are known to play a crucial role in assigning stress to multi-syllabic words among native English speakers. However, it is unclear whether second language (L2) learners of English can effectively use derivational suffixes as stress cues in written words. To address this gap, we studied if native Chinese-speaking adults learning English as an L2 can use derivational cues to correctly assign lexical stress, and whether the frequency of these suffixes and their L2 language proficiency moderate this sensitivity. Utilizing a written stress assignment and a stress production task, participants saw a series of written stems and their derived forms and were asked to either choose a syllable to assign the stress (Experiment 1) or read it aloud (Experiment 2). Results showed that L2 learners are sensitive to derivational cues to lexical stress in English. Moreover, the strength of this sensitivity is dependent upon the suffix class, frequency, and L2 language proficiency. Specifically, in both experiments, participants made more correct stress shifts in derived forms with high compared to low nonneutral suffix frequency; however, it did not differ between high and low neutral suffix frequency. In addition, as participants’ English proficiency increased, their correct stress shifts increased significantly greater in those derived nonwords with nonneutral than neutral suffixes in the stress assignment task. These findings make a significant contribution to the limited literature on L2 learners' sensitivity to morpho-orthographic cues to lexical stress and support the proposed statistical learning mechanism, that is, L2 learners can implicitly learn statistical regularities from linguistic materials.</p>","PeriodicalId":48204,"journal":{"name":"Reading and Writing","volume":"68 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-12-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138629796","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of mind wandering on the recall of central ideas 思维游离对回忆中心思想的影响
IF 2.5 2区 教育学 Q2 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH Pub Date : 2023-12-08 DOI: 10.1007/s11145-023-10495-3
Amanda C. Miller, Irene Adjei, Hannah Christensen

Mind wandering occurs when a reader’s thoughts are unrelated to the text’s ideas. We examined the relation between mind wandering and readers’ memory for text. More specifically, we assessed whether mind wandering inhibits the reader’s development of the situation model and thus their ability to identify and recall the text’s most central ideas. Undergraduate participants (M = 18.92 years; SD = 1.32) read and recalled three expository passages. Participants responded to intermittent probes to report mind wandering frequency. We examined how mind wandering impacted the readers’ situation model, indicated by the proportion of central and peripheral ideas recalled. Using path analysis models, we found that mind wandering negatively predicted the recall of central, but not peripheral, ideas. The effect of mind wandering on the recall of central ideas was not explained by working memory span (measured by WAIS-IV digit span backward and letter-number sequencing), word reading skill (measured by Letter-Word Identification and Word Attack), or general reading comprehension skill (measured by the Gates-MacGinitie Reading Test). These results indicate that mind wandering hinders the recognition and recall of a text’s most central ideas and suggest that mind wandering impacts the development of a coherent situation model. This effect seems to be independent of working memory, word reading, and general reading comprehension skill. Future studies should test approaches to decrease mind wandering among adult readers.

当读者的思维与文章的思想无关时,就会出现思维游离。我们研究了思维游离与读者对文本记忆之间的关系。更具体地说,我们评估了思维游离是否会抑制读者对情境模型的发展,从而抑制他们识别和回忆文章最核心思想的能力。本科生参与者(中=18.92岁;标度=1.32)阅读并回忆了三个说明性段落。参与者在回答间歇性问题时报告了思维游离的频率。我们研究了思维游离如何影响读者的情境模型,该模型通过回忆中心思想和边缘思想的比例来表示。通过路径分析模型,我们发现思维游离对中心思想的回忆有负面影响,但对边缘思想的回忆没有影响。工作记忆跨度(通过 WAIS-IV 数字跨度倒序和字母-数字排序测量)、单词阅读技能(通过字母-单词识别和单词攻击测量)或一般阅读理解技能(通过盖茨-麦金尼蒂阅读测试测量)均无法解释思维游离对中心思想回忆的影响。这些结果表明,思维游离会阻碍对文章最中心思想的识别和回忆,并表明思维游离会影响连贯情境模型的发展。这种影响似乎与工作记忆、单词阅读和一般阅读理解能力无关。未来的研究应测试减少成年读者思维游离的方法。
{"title":"The impact of mind wandering on the recall of central ideas","authors":"Amanda C. Miller, Irene Adjei, Hannah Christensen","doi":"10.1007/s11145-023-10495-3","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-023-10495-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Mind wandering occurs when a reader’s thoughts are unrelated to the text’s ideas. We examined the relation between mind wandering and readers’ memory for text. More specifically, we assessed whether mind wandering inhibits the reader’s development of the situation model and thus their ability to identify and recall the text’s most central ideas. Undergraduate participants (<i>M</i> = 18.92 years; <i>SD</i> = 1.32) read and recalled three expository passages. Participants responded to intermittent probes to report mind wandering frequency. We examined how mind wandering impacted the readers’ situation model, indicated by the proportion of central and peripheral ideas recalled. Using path analysis models, we found that mind wandering negatively predicted the recall of central, but not peripheral, ideas. The effect of mind wandering on the recall of central ideas was not explained by working memory span (measured by WAIS-IV digit span backward and letter-number sequencing), word reading skill (measured by Letter-Word Identification and Word Attack), or general reading comprehension skill (measured by the Gates-MacGinitie Reading Test). These results indicate that mind wandering hinders the recognition and recall of a text’s most central ideas and suggest that mind wandering impacts the development of a coherent situation model. This effect seems to be independent of working memory, word reading, and general reading comprehension skill. Future studies should test approaches to decrease mind wandering among adult readers.</p>","PeriodicalId":48204,"journal":{"name":"Reading and Writing","volume":"18 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-12-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138560265","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Methods for studying the writing time-course 研究写作时间进程的方法
IF 2.5 2区 教育学 Q2 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH Pub Date : 2023-12-08 DOI: 10.1007/s11145-023-10490-8
Mark Torrance, Rianne Conijn

The understanding of the cognitive processes that underlie written composition requires analysis of moment-by-moment fluctuation in the rate of output that go beyond traditional approaches to writing time-course analysis based on, for example, counting pauses. This special issue includes 10 papers that provide important new tools and methods for extracting and analyzing writing timecourse data that go beyond traditional approaches. The papers in this special issue divide into three groups: papers that describe methods for capturing and coding writing timecourse data from writers producing text either by hand or by keyboard, papers that describe new statistical approaches to describing and drawing inferences from these data, and papers that focus on analysis of how a text develops over time as the writer makes changes to what they have already written.

要了解支撑书面写作的认知过程,就必须对每时每刻的输出率波动进行分析,这超越了基于停顿计数等传统写作时程分析方法。本特刊收录了 10 篇论文,为提取和分析写作时程数据提供了超越传统方法的重要新工具和新方法。本特刊中的论文分为三类:描述从手写或键盘输入文本的写作者那里获取写作时程数据并对其进行编码的方法的论文;描述描述这些数据并从中得出推论的新统计方法的论文;以及重点分析写作者在对已写的内容进行修改时文本如何随时间推移而发展的论文。
{"title":"Methods for studying the writing time-course","authors":"Mark Torrance, Rianne Conijn","doi":"10.1007/s11145-023-10490-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-023-10490-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The understanding of the cognitive processes that underlie written composition requires analysis of moment-by-moment fluctuation in the rate of output that go beyond traditional approaches to writing time-course analysis based on, for example, counting pauses. This special issue includes 10 papers that provide important new tools and methods for extracting and analyzing writing timecourse data that go beyond traditional approaches. The papers in this special issue divide into three groups: papers that describe methods for capturing and coding writing timecourse data from writers producing text either by hand or by keyboard, papers that describe new statistical approaches to describing and drawing inferences from these data, and papers that focus on analysis of how a text develops over time as the writer makes changes to what they have already written.</p>","PeriodicalId":48204,"journal":{"name":"Reading and Writing","volume":"8 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-12-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138564040","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Reading and Writing
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1