首页 > 最新文献

Applied Cognitive Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Effects of internal versus external distinctive facial features on eyewitness identification 内部与外部面部特征对目击者识别的影响
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-02-22 DOI: 10.1002/acp.4186
Curt A. Carlson, William E. Pleasant, Maria A. Carlson, Alyssa R. Jones

Many criminals have distinctive facial features such as tattoos, yet the potential impact on eyewitness memory has received little research attention. Does such a feature harm memory for the face at encoding, and can police do anything about this when constructing the lineup? Does it matter whether the feature is on the interior (e.g., tattoo on face) or exterior (e.g., tattoo on neck)? These are the kinds of questions that we investigated by randomly assigning a large nationwide sample of online participants to conditions within an experimental design in which we manipulated target exposure time, presence (and location) of a distinctive feature, and whether it is replicated or removed from lineup members. Results indicate that a distinctive feature harms memory for the face regardless of location, but replicating the feature in the lineup may attenuate this effect. Fortunately, high confidence was indicative of high accuracy regardless of our manipulations.

许多罪犯都有明显的面部特征,如纹身,但其对目击者记忆的潜在影响却很少受到研究关注。这种特征是否会在编码时损害对脸部的记忆,警方在构建列队时能否对此做些什么?特征是在内部(如脸上的纹身)还是外部(如脖子上的纹身)是否重要?我们对这些问题进行了研究,在实验设计中,我们将全国范围内的大量在线参与者随机分配到不同的条件下,操纵目标暴露时间、显著特征的存在(和位置),以及是否从排队成员中复制或去除该特征。结果表明,无论位置如何,显著特征都会损害对人脸的记忆,但在队列中复制该特征可能会减轻这种影响。幸运的是,无论我们如何操作,高置信度都表明了高准确度。
{"title":"Effects of internal versus external distinctive facial features on eyewitness identification","authors":"Curt A. Carlson,&nbsp;William E. Pleasant,&nbsp;Maria A. Carlson,&nbsp;Alyssa R. Jones","doi":"10.1002/acp.4186","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.4186","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Many criminals have distinctive facial features such as tattoos, yet the potential impact on eyewitness memory has received little research attention. Does such a feature harm memory for the face at encoding, and can police do anything about this when constructing the lineup? Does it matter whether the feature is on the interior (e.g., tattoo on face) or exterior (e.g., tattoo on neck)? These are the kinds of questions that we investigated by randomly assigning a large nationwide sample of online participants to conditions within an experimental design in which we manipulated target exposure time, presence (and location) of a distinctive feature, and whether it is replicated or removed from lineup members. Results indicate that a distinctive feature harms memory for the face regardless of location, but replicating the feature in the lineup may attenuate this effect. Fortunately, high confidence was indicative of high accuracy regardless of our manipulations.</p>","PeriodicalId":48281,"journal":{"name":"Applied Cognitive Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139937304","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Susceptibility to cancer misinformation: Predictors of false belief and false memory formation 癌症误导的易感性:错误信念和错误记忆形成的预测因素
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-02-19 DOI: 10.1002/acp.4184
Nora King, Ciara M. Greene

Previous research has shown that people sometimes come to believe in misinformation presented in the form of fake news, and even form false memories for the fabricated events described. This study aimed to investigate the effects of analytical reasoning, attitudes to complementary and alternative medicine, bullshit receptivity, and previous experience with cancer on the formation of false memory and false belief for cancer related misinformation. Participants (N = 466) were exposed to four fake news stories and four true news stories relating to cancer treatment and services. Male gender, low analytical reasoning, receptivity to bullshit, and endorsement of complementary and alternative medicine were all significant predictors of belief in cancer misinformation, while participants with poor analytical reasoning or higher receptivity to bullshit reported more false memories for fake news stories. These results indicate that reflexive, intuitive thinking styles contribute to susceptibility to cancer misinformation, suggesting a potential target for public health interventions.

以往的研究表明,人们有时会相信以假新闻形式出现的错误信息,甚至对所描述的捏造事件形成错误记忆。本研究旨在调查分析推理、对补充医学和替代医学的态度、废话接受能力以及以前的癌症经历对癌症相关错误信息形成错误记忆和错误信念的影响。参与者(N = 466)分别接触了四则假新闻和四则与癌症治疗和服务相关的真新闻。男性性别、低分析推理能力、对废话的接受能力以及对补充医学和替代医学的认可都是预测癌症误导信息可信度的重要因素,而分析推理能力差或对废话接受能力强的参与者则报告了更多关于假新闻的错误记忆。这些结果表明,反射性、直觉性思维方式会导致癌症误导信息的易感性,为公共健康干预提供了潜在的目标。
{"title":"Susceptibility to cancer misinformation: Predictors of false belief and false memory formation","authors":"Nora King,&nbsp;Ciara M. Greene","doi":"10.1002/acp.4184","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.4184","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Previous research has shown that people sometimes come to believe in misinformation presented in the form of fake news, and even form false memories for the fabricated events described. This study aimed to investigate the effects of analytical reasoning, attitudes to complementary and alternative medicine, bullshit receptivity, and previous experience with cancer on the formation of false memory and false belief for cancer related misinformation. Participants (<i>N</i> = 466) were exposed to four fake news stories and four true news stories relating to cancer treatment and services. Male gender, low analytical reasoning, receptivity to bullshit, and endorsement of complementary and alternative medicine were all significant predictors of belief in cancer misinformation, while participants with poor analytical reasoning or higher receptivity to bullshit reported more false memories for fake news stories. These results indicate that reflexive, intuitive thinking styles contribute to susceptibility to cancer misinformation, suggesting a potential target for public health interventions.</p>","PeriodicalId":48281,"journal":{"name":"Applied Cognitive Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/acp.4184","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139908905","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The functions of remembering the past and imagining the future during the COVID-19 pandemic 在 COVID-19 大流行期间回忆过去和想象未来的功能
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-02-19 DOI: 10.1002/acp.4181
Zeynep Adıgüzel, Demet Ay, Ezgi Bilgin, Selin Buse Coşkuner, İrem Ergen, Sami Gülgöz

In this study, we explored how the COVID-19 pandemic affected the functions of autobiographical memory and future projection over time. Between May and June 2020, we asked people (N = 286) to recall or imagine personal events from four time periods: past before the pandemic, past during the pandemic, future during the pandemic, and future after the pandemic ends. Participants rated self, social, directive, predictive, and emotion regulation functions, and the phenomenological characteristics of these events. We found that ratings for all functions decreased for the pandemic and increased for the future. Overall, this study revealed that the COVID-19 pandemic created a breaking point, with memories being less functional at the beginning of the pandemic compared to the pre-pandemic times and the future. However, imagined events that would occur after the pandemic ended were rated most functional, suggesting that people were still optimistic about a pandemic-free future.

在这项研究中,我们探讨了 COVID-19 大流行如何随着时间的推移影响自传体记忆和未来预测的功能。在 2020 年 5 月至 6 月期间,我们要求参与者(人数 = 286)回忆或想象四个时间段内的个人事件:大流行之前的过去、大流行期间的过去、大流行期间的未来以及大流行结束后的未来。受试者对自我、社会、指令、预测和情绪调节功能以及这些事件的现象特征进行评分。我们发现,所有功能的评分在大流行时都有所下降,而在未来则有所上升。总之,这项研究表明,COVID-19 大流行造成了一个断裂点,与大流行前和未来相比,大流行初期的记忆功能较弱。然而,人们对大流行结束后发生的事件的想象被评为功能最强,这表明人们对没有大流行的未来仍持乐观态度。
{"title":"The functions of remembering the past and imagining the future during the COVID-19 pandemic","authors":"Zeynep Adıgüzel,&nbsp;Demet Ay,&nbsp;Ezgi Bilgin,&nbsp;Selin Buse Coşkuner,&nbsp;İrem Ergen,&nbsp;Sami Gülgöz","doi":"10.1002/acp.4181","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.4181","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In this study, we explored how the COVID-19 pandemic affected the functions of autobiographical memory and future projection over time. Between May and June 2020, we asked people (<i>N</i> = 286) to recall or imagine personal events from four time periods: past before the pandemic, past during the pandemic, future during the pandemic, and future after the pandemic ends. Participants rated self, social, directive, predictive, and emotion regulation functions, and the phenomenological characteristics of these events. We found that ratings for all functions decreased for the pandemic and increased for the future. Overall, this study revealed that the COVID-19 pandemic created a breaking point, with memories being less functional at the beginning of the pandemic compared to the pre-pandemic times and the future. However, imagined events that would occur after the pandemic ended were rated most functional, suggesting that people were still optimistic about a pandemic-free future.</p>","PeriodicalId":48281,"journal":{"name":"Applied Cognitive Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/acp.4181","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139908906","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Enhanced detection efficiency in reaction time-based concealed information test through response preparation interference 通过反应准备干扰提高基于反应时间的隐藏信息测试的检测效率
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-02-18 DOI: 10.1002/acp.4180
Jing Liang, Yu-Hsin Chen, Wen-Jing Yan, Yun-Feng He

This study investigated the influence of cognitive interference on the detectability of concealed information in a reaction time (RT)-based concealed information test (CIT). The classical RT-based CIT was modified to interfere with response preparation, hypothesizing that this interference would enhance detection efficiency. A total of 120 participants were randomly assigned as “guilty” or “innocent” and were instructed to deny any knowledge related to a mock crime. The results revealed that guilty participants exhibited significantly longer RTs for probes than for irrelevant stimuli in both the classical and modified-CIT conditions (aka CIT effect). In contrast, innocent participants showed no difference in RTs between the probe and irrelevant stimuli under either condition. Furthermore, the modified-CIT yielded significantly higher detection efficiency than the classical-CIT. These findings signify promising advancements in the application of cognitive interventions in deception detection methodologies.

本研究调查了认知干扰对基于反应时间(RT)的隐藏信息测试(CIT)中隐藏信息可探测性的影响。研究人员对基于反应时间的经典 CIT 进行了修改,以干扰反应准备,并假设这种干扰会提高检测效率。共有 120 名参与者被随机分配为 "有罪 "或 "无罪",并被要求否认任何与模拟犯罪有关的知识。结果显示,在经典和修正的 CIT 条件下(又称 CIT 效应),有罪的参与者在探测时的反应时间明显长于无关刺激。与此相反,在这两种条件下,无辜的被试者对探究刺激和无关刺激的反应时间均无差异。此外,改良 CIT 的检测效率明显高于经典 CIT。这些发现标志着认知干预在欺骗检测方法中的应用有望取得进展。
{"title":"Enhanced detection efficiency in reaction time-based concealed information test through response preparation interference","authors":"Jing Liang,&nbsp;Yu-Hsin Chen,&nbsp;Wen-Jing Yan,&nbsp;Yun-Feng He","doi":"10.1002/acp.4180","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.4180","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This study investigated the influence of cognitive interference on the detectability of concealed information in a reaction time (RT)-based concealed information test (CIT). The classical RT-based CIT was modified to interfere with response preparation, hypothesizing that this interference would enhance detection efficiency. A total of 120 participants were randomly assigned as “guilty” or “innocent” and were instructed to deny any knowledge related to a mock crime. The results revealed that guilty participants exhibited significantly longer RTs for probes than for irrelevant stimuli in both the classical and modified-CIT conditions (aka CIT effect). In contrast, innocent participants showed no difference in RTs between the probe and irrelevant stimuli under either condition. Furthermore, the modified-CIT yielded significantly higher detection efficiency than the classical-CIT. These findings signify promising advancements in the application of cognitive interventions in deception detection methodologies.</p>","PeriodicalId":48281,"journal":{"name":"Applied Cognitive Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139901661","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Testing the efficacy of the cognitive interview to road traffic accident investigations 测试认知访谈对道路交通事故调查的有效性
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-02-15 DOI: 10.1002/acp.4177
Dewi Maulina, Diandra Yasmine Irwanda,  Guritnaningsih, Henry Otgaar, Dela Septiana Nurfajriah, La Ode Muhammad Alwi Armas, Aliqa Impuni Dewi

In the current set of experiments, we examined the effectiveness of the Cognitive Interview (CI) on memory for traffic accidents. Three online experiments were conducted among Indonesian young motorcyclists who had an accident experience. Participants were interviewed about their most memorable accident experience, either with a CI or a Standard Interview (SI) method (Experiment 1). We added the time delay as the second independent variable (less than a month versus between 6 and 10 months) in Experiment 2. In Experiment 3, the interview methods were compared across two age groups (adolescents versus young adults). In all experiments, the CI led to more event-related and peripheral recall than the SI. Furthermore, this effect persisted even when the event happened a long time ago and was replicated in both age groups. Our results show empirical evidence for the effectiveness of the CI in a traffic accident context, especially in a non-WEIRD country.

在本组实验中,我们考察了认知访谈(CI)对交通事故记忆的效果。我们在印度尼西亚的年轻摩托车手中进行了三项在线实验。我们采用认知访谈(CI)或标准访谈(SI)的方法对参与者进行了访谈,询问他们印象最深的事故经历(实验 1)。在实验 2 中,我们加入了时间延迟作为第二个自变量(少于一个月与 6 至 10 个月)。在实验 3 中,我们对两个年龄组(青少年和青年)的访谈方法进行了比较。在所有实验中,CI 比 SI 更能导致事件相关回忆和周边回忆。此外,即使事件发生在很久以前,这种效果也会持续存在,并且在两个年龄组中都得到了验证。我们的研究结果证明了 CI 在交通事故背景下的有效性,尤其是在一个非 WEIRD 国家。
{"title":"Testing the efficacy of the cognitive interview to road traffic accident investigations","authors":"Dewi Maulina,&nbsp;Diandra Yasmine Irwanda,&nbsp; Guritnaningsih,&nbsp;Henry Otgaar,&nbsp;Dela Septiana Nurfajriah,&nbsp;La Ode Muhammad Alwi Armas,&nbsp;Aliqa Impuni Dewi","doi":"10.1002/acp.4177","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.4177","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In the current set of experiments, we examined the effectiveness of the Cognitive Interview (CI) on memory for traffic accidents. Three online experiments were conducted among Indonesian young motorcyclists who had an accident experience. Participants were interviewed about their most memorable accident experience, either with a CI or a Standard Interview (SI) method (Experiment 1). We added the time delay as the second independent variable (less than a month versus between 6 and 10 months) in Experiment 2. In Experiment 3, the interview methods were compared across two age groups (adolescents versus young adults). In all experiments, the CI led to more event-related and peripheral recall than the SI. Furthermore, this effect persisted even when the event happened a long time ago and was replicated in both age groups. Our results show empirical evidence for the effectiveness of the CI in a traffic accident context, especially in a non-WEIRD country.</p>","PeriodicalId":48281,"journal":{"name":"Applied Cognitive Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139744954","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The efficacy of the Self-Administered Interview in minimising the misinformation effect 自填式访谈在减少误导效应方面的功效
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-02-13 DOI: 10.1002/acp.4179
Emily Bird, Jan Wiener, Janice Attard-Johnson

Two experiments investigated the efficacy of the Self-Administered Interview (SAI) in immediate recall and minimising misinformation into later recall. In Experiment 1, participants (N = 114) viewed a reconstructed crime video and completed the SAI, Written Free Recall (WFR), or no recall for immediate recall, followed by a Delayed Recall questionnaire 1-to-2 weeks later. Experiment 2 replicated the procedure, except after the delay, participants (N = 141) were exposed to misinformation before Delayed Recall. Across both experiments, the SAI enhanced immediate memory, particularly for person details, compared to WFR. However, this initial enhancement did not lead to more correct information after the delay, although it reduced incorrect responses compared to no immediate recall. Surprisingly, neither the SAI nor the WFR reduced susceptibility to misinformation. The findings suggest completing an SAI immediately or soon after witnessing an event could enhance memory recall, reinforcing its potential utility for witnesses.

有两项实验调查了自控访谈(SAI)在即时回忆和尽量减少误导后期回忆方面的功效。在实验 1 中,参与者(114 人)观看了一段重建的犯罪视频,并完成了 SAI、书面自由回忆(WFR)或无回忆的即时回忆,然后在 1-2 周后进行了延迟回忆问卷调查。实验 2 复制了这一过程,但在延迟之后,参与者(N = 141)在延迟回忆之前会接触到错误信息。在这两项实验中,与 WFR 相比,SAI 增强了即时记忆,尤其是对人物细节的记忆。然而,这种最初的增强并没有在延迟后带来更多的正确信息,尽管与没有即时回忆相比,它减少了错误的反应。令人惊讶的是,SAI 和 WFR 都没有降低对错误信息的敏感性。研究结果表明,在目睹事件发生后立即或很快完成 SAI 可以增强记忆回忆,从而加强其对目击者的潜在作用。
{"title":"The efficacy of the Self-Administered Interview in minimising the misinformation effect","authors":"Emily Bird,&nbsp;Jan Wiener,&nbsp;Janice Attard-Johnson","doi":"10.1002/acp.4179","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.4179","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Two experiments investigated the efficacy of the Self-Administered Interview (SAI) in immediate recall and minimising misinformation into later recall. In Experiment 1, participants (<i>N</i> = 114) viewed a reconstructed crime video and completed the SAI, Written Free Recall (WFR), or no recall for immediate recall, followed by a Delayed Recall questionnaire 1-to-2 weeks later. Experiment 2 replicated the procedure, except after the delay, participants (<i>N</i> = 141) were exposed to misinformation before Delayed Recall. Across both experiments, the SAI enhanced immediate memory, particularly for person details, compared to WFR. However, this initial enhancement did not lead to more correct information after the delay, although it reduced incorrect responses compared to no immediate recall. Surprisingly, neither the SAI nor the WFR reduced susceptibility to misinformation. The findings suggest completing an SAI immediately or soon after witnessing an event could enhance memory recall, reinforcing its potential utility for witnesses.</p>","PeriodicalId":48281,"journal":{"name":"Applied Cognitive Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/acp.4179","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139732331","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Characteristics and functions of predictive and directive memories and forecasts 预测性和指令性记忆与预报的特点和功能
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-02-11 DOI: 10.1002/acp.4174
Demet Ay, Sami Gülgöz

Earlier research focused on three functions of recollecting the past self, social, and directive functions, but few studies examined the characteristics of events serving these functions. Moreover, research has neglected the function of prediction, which refers to predicting the future by recollecting the past. The current study distinguished the predictive function from the directive function and aimed to characterize memories and future events serving different functions by employing function prompts as cues. In addition, the frequency of using function-cued memories for the other functions was measured. Results showed that predictive and directive function ratings of the predictive function cued events were significantly different. However, directive events served the predictive function as frequently as the directive function, indicating that the predictive function is a prerequisite for future planning conceptualized under the directive function. The results are indicative of a distinct predictive function and considerable overlap between functions of memories.

早期的研究主要集中在回忆过去的自我功能、社会功能和指令功能这三种功能上,但很少有研究探讨服务于这些功能的事件的特点。此外,研究还忽视了预测功能,即通过回忆过去来预测未来。本研究将预测功能与指示功能区分开来,旨在通过使用功能提示作为线索来描述具有不同功能的记忆和未来事件的特征。此外,还测量了其他功能使用功能提示记忆的频率。结果表明,预测性功能提示事件的预测性和指导性功能评分有显著差异。然而,指令事件在预测功能方面的使用频率与在指令功能方面的使用频率相同,这表明预测功能是在指令功能概念下进行未来规划的先决条件。这些结果表明,记忆具有独特的预测功能,而且记忆功能之间有相当多的重叠。
{"title":"Characteristics and functions of predictive and directive memories and forecasts","authors":"Demet Ay,&nbsp;Sami Gülgöz","doi":"10.1002/acp.4174","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.4174","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Earlier research focused on three functions of recollecting the past self, social, and directive functions, but few studies examined the characteristics of events serving these functions. Moreover, research has neglected the function of prediction, which refers to predicting the future by recollecting the past. The current study distinguished the predictive function from the directive function and aimed to characterize memories and future events serving different functions by employing function prompts as cues. In addition, the frequency of using function-cued memories for the other functions was measured. Results showed that predictive and directive function ratings of the predictive function cued events were significantly different. However, directive events served the predictive function as frequently as the directive function, indicating that the predictive function is a prerequisite for future planning conceptualized under the directive function. The results are indicative of a distinct predictive function and considerable overlap between functions of memories.</p>","PeriodicalId":48281,"journal":{"name":"Applied Cognitive Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/acp.4174","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139720123","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
“What if…?”: Vividness and frequency of counterfactual thinking in survivors of terrorism "如果......会怎样?恐怖主义幸存者反事实思维的生动性和频率
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-02-11 DOI: 10.1002/acp.4176
Andrea Undset, Tine K. Jensen, Grete Dyb, Tore Wentzel-Larsen, Ines Blix

After traumatic experiences, it is common to think about alternative scenarios or outcomes of the event. This is often referred to as counterfactual thoughts (CFT), and CFT after trauma have been associated with posttraumatic stress reactions (PTSR). In this study, we aimed to: (1) investigate the relationship between the vividness and frequency of CFT and PTSR, and to (2) examine the associations between exposure, peri-traumatic reactions, physical injury and the loss of someone close, and the subsequent vividness and frequency of CFT. The participants (N = 289) were survivors of a terror attack in Norway. More vivid and frequent CFT were significantly and independently related to more PTSR. Exposure during trauma was significantly associated with more vivid CFT in the unadjusted regression model. No further significant relationships were found between exposure, peri-traumatic reactions, physical injury and the loss of someone close, and the vividness and frequency of CFT. As CFT are common after trauma and potentially cause distress, clinicians should identify and validate CFT, and provide coping assistance.

在经历创伤后,人们通常会思考事件的其他情景或结果。这通常被称为反事实想法(CFT),创伤后的反事实想法与创伤后应激反应(PTSR)有关。本研究旨在(1) 调查反事实想法的生动性和频率与创伤后应激反应之间的关系;(2) 调查暴露、创伤前反应、身体伤害和失去亲人与随后的反事实想法的生动性和频率之间的关系。参与者(N = 289)是挪威恐怖袭击事件的幸存者。更生动、更频繁的CFT与更多的创伤后应激反应有明显的独立关系。在未经调整的回归模型中,创伤期间的暴露与更生动的CFT显著相关。在暴露、创伤前反应、身体伤害和失去亲人与 CFT 的生动性和频率之间没有发现进一步的重要关系。由于CFT在创伤后很常见,并可能造成痛苦,临床医生应识别和验证CFT,并提供应对援助。
{"title":"“What if…?”: Vividness and frequency of counterfactual thinking in survivors of terrorism","authors":"Andrea Undset,&nbsp;Tine K. Jensen,&nbsp;Grete Dyb,&nbsp;Tore Wentzel-Larsen,&nbsp;Ines Blix","doi":"10.1002/acp.4176","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.4176","url":null,"abstract":"<p>After traumatic experiences, it is common to think about alternative scenarios or outcomes of the event. This is often referred to as counterfactual thoughts (CFT), and CFT after trauma have been associated with posttraumatic stress reactions (PTSR). In this study, we aimed to: (1) investigate the relationship between the vividness and frequency of CFT and PTSR, and to (2) examine the associations between exposure, peri-traumatic reactions, physical injury and the loss of someone close, and the subsequent vividness and frequency of CFT. The participants (<i>N</i> = 289) were survivors of a terror attack in Norway. More vivid and frequent CFT were significantly and independently related to more PTSR. Exposure during trauma was significantly associated with more vivid CFT in the unadjusted regression model. No further significant relationships were found between exposure, peri-traumatic reactions, physical injury and the loss of someone close, and the vividness and frequency of CFT. As CFT are common after trauma and potentially cause distress, clinicians should identify and validate CFT, and provide coping assistance.</p>","PeriodicalId":48281,"journal":{"name":"Applied Cognitive Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/acp.4176","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139720124","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cognitive effects of humorous drawings on learning: An eye-tracking study 幽默图画对学习的认知影响:眼动追踪研究
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-02-07 DOI: 10.1002/acp.4178
Mathilde Cazes, Audrey Noël, Eric Jamet

Although humor is frequently used in face-to-face courses and computer-based training, there is no consensus in the literature on its effects on students' learning. The aim of the present study was therefore to assess the cognitive effects of adding humorous drawings in a computer-based course on both learning outcomes and learning behaviors (eye movements). It was assumed that humorous drawings would improve learning through the active resolution of incongruity. To isolate this effect from those of the illustrations, a nonhumorous drawings condition was also compared with the text-only condition. Eye-movement data showed that humor increased visiting duration for the drawings. Learning outcomes were not affected by the nonhumorous drawings, whereas humor had a positive effect on comprehension, though not on retention. This effect of humor was not mediated by visiting time for the slides. Our results argue in favor of the use of content-relevant cartoons to improve students' learning.

虽然幽默经常被用于面授课程和基于计算机的培训中,但关于幽默对学生学习的影响,文献中还没有达成共识。因此,本研究旨在评估在计算机课程中加入幽默图画对学习效果和学习行为(眼球运动)的认知影响。我们假设幽默图画会通过积极解决不协调问题来提高学习效果。为了将这种效果与插图的效果区分开来,还将非幽默图画条件与纯文本条件进行了比较。眼动数据显示,幽默感增加了参观图画的时间。学习效果没有受到非幽默图画的影响,而幽默对理解力有积极影响,但对保持力没有影响。幽默的这种效果并不受幻灯片浏览时间的影响。我们的研究结果支持使用与内容相关的漫画来提高学生的学习效果。
{"title":"Cognitive effects of humorous drawings on learning: An eye-tracking study","authors":"Mathilde Cazes,&nbsp;Audrey Noël,&nbsp;Eric Jamet","doi":"10.1002/acp.4178","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.4178","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Although humor is frequently used in face-to-face courses and computer-based training, there is no consensus in the literature on its effects on students' learning. The aim of the present study was therefore to assess the cognitive effects of adding humorous drawings in a computer-based course on both learning outcomes and learning behaviors (eye movements). It was assumed that humorous drawings would improve learning through the active resolution of incongruity. To isolate this effect from those of the illustrations, a nonhumorous drawings condition was also compared with the text-only condition. Eye-movement data showed that humor increased visiting duration for the drawings. Learning outcomes were not affected by the nonhumorous drawings, whereas humor had a positive effect on comprehension, though not on retention. This effect of humor was not mediated by visiting time for the slides. Our results argue in favor of the use of content-relevant cartoons to improve students' learning.</p>","PeriodicalId":48281,"journal":{"name":"Applied Cognitive Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139704677","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Introducing a new era 引入新时代
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-02-05 DOI: 10.1002/acp.4175
Michael E. Lamb
{"title":"Introducing a new era","authors":"Michael E. Lamb","doi":"10.1002/acp.4175","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.4175","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48281,"journal":{"name":"Applied Cognitive Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-02-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139695176","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Applied Cognitive Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1