首页 > 最新文献

Learning and Individual Differences最新文献

英文 中文
Latent profiles of Chinese students' social-emotional learning competencies and their associations with academic motivation and achievement 中国学生社会情感学习能力的潜在特征及其与学习动机和学习成绩的关系
IF 3.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-11-06 DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102580
Yi Jiang , Linjia Zhang , Chun Chen
We investigated the latent profiles of social-emotional learning (SEL) competencies among Chinese high school students and their association with academic motivation, achievement, and personal characteristics. Latent profile analysis of a sample of 1530 eleventh graders revealed four distinct profiles: high SEL competencies, average SEL competencies, low SEL competencies, and extremely low SEL competencies. There were significant differences in students' expectancy-value beliefs and achievement goals across these profiles. However, achievement levels did not significantly vary with SEL profiles, suggesting that SEL and academic success might coexist independently in Chinese high school students. The profiles also showed meaningful associations with student characteristics, including gender and growth mindset. Notably, different from studies conducted in the Western context, the underrepresentation of female students in the high SEL competencies group highlights a potential cultural disparity, warranting further investigation. The findings of this study emphasize the importance of integrating SEL curricula into Chinese educational settings to enhance students' social, emotional, and academic development.
我们研究了中国高中生社会情感学习(SEL)能力的潜在特征及其与学习动机、成绩和个人特征的关系。我们对 1530 名十一年级学生的样本进行了潜特征分析,发现了四种不同的特征:较高的社会情感学习能力、一般的社会情感学习能力、较低的社会情感学习能力和极低的社会情感学习能力。在这些特征中,学生的期望值信念和成绩目标存在明显差异。然而,学生的成绩水平并没有随着 SEL 状况的不同而发生明显的变化,这表明在中国高中生中,SEL 和学业成功可能是独立共存的。此外,这些特征还与学生的特征(包括性别和成长型思维模式)产生了有意义的关联。值得注意的是,与在西方背景下进行的研究不同,女性学生在高 SEL 能力组中所占比例较低,这凸显了潜在的文化差异,值得进一步研究。本研究的结果强调了将SEL课程融入中国教育环境以促进学生社交、情感和学业发展的重要性。
{"title":"Latent profiles of Chinese students' social-emotional learning competencies and their associations with academic motivation and achievement","authors":"Yi Jiang ,&nbsp;Linjia Zhang ,&nbsp;Chun Chen","doi":"10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102580","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102580","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>We investigated the latent profiles of social-emotional learning (SEL) competencies among Chinese high school students and their association with academic motivation, achievement, and personal characteristics. Latent profile analysis of a sample of 1530 eleventh graders revealed four distinct profiles: <em>high SEL competencies</em>, <em>average SEL competencies</em>, <em>low SEL competencies</em>, and <em>extremely low SEL competencies</em>. There were significant differences in students' expectancy-value beliefs and achievement goals across these profiles. However, achievement levels did not significantly vary with SEL profiles, suggesting that SEL and academic success might coexist independently in Chinese high school students. The profiles also showed meaningful associations with student characteristics, including gender and growth mindset. Notably, different from studies conducted in the Western context, the underrepresentation of female students in the high SEL competencies group highlights a potential cultural disparity, warranting further investigation. The findings of this study emphasize the importance of integrating SEL curricula into Chinese educational settings to enhance students' social, emotional, and academic development.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48336,"journal":{"name":"Learning and Individual Differences","volume":"116 ","pages":"Article 102580"},"PeriodicalIF":3.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142592638","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Testing the generalizability of the multiplicative effects of expectancy and value across different ages, genders, and races 测试预期和价值的倍增效应在不同年龄、性别和种族间的普遍性
IF 3.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-10-31 DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102578
Eunjin Seo , You-kyung Lee , Rebecca S. Steingut , Edna C. Alfaro , Kejin Lee
This research examines how the multiplicative effect of expectancy and value on achievement-related outcomes differ based on students' individual differences, including age, race, and gender. We analyzed longitudinal data from 10th-grade students (Study 1: n ≈ 10,700) and 3rd-grade students (Study 2: n ≈ 9680) who identified as Black or White. The results indicate that expectancy and value synergistically predict later achievement in adolescents, but not in children. We did not find evidence of different effects based on race or gender. Our findings tentatively indicate that expectancy beliefs closely aligned with their relative achievement levels, along with a clear understanding of value, could enhance the synergistic effects of expectancy and value. The findings challenge the universality of the multiplicative model of expectancy-value theory across different populations and contexts.

Educational relevance and implications statement

The multiplicative model of expectancy and value suggests that when people believe they can succeed (expectancy) and find meaning in a task (value), their achievement gains a boost that surpasses the combined gains from high expectancy and high value considered separately. The current study reveals a synergistic effect, indicating that the combined influence is greater than the individual effects when considered separately, specifically in adolescent students, but not in younger children. These findings imply that the multiplicative model of expectancy-value theory may not apply universally.
本研究探讨了期望值和价值观对成绩相关结果的乘法效应如何因学生的个体差异(包括年龄、种族和性别)而有所不同。我们分析了十年级学生(研究 1:n ≈ 10,700 人)和三年级学生(研究 2:n ≈ 9680 人)的纵向数据,这些学生被认定为黑人或白人。研究结果表明,期望值和价值观可以协同预测青少年日后的成就,但不能预测儿童的成就。我们没有发现因种族或性别而产生不同影响的证据。我们的研究结果初步表明,与相对成就水平密切相关的期望信念,以及对价值的清晰认识,可以增强期望和价值的协同效应。研究结果对期望-价值理论的乘法模型在不同人群和环境中的普遍性提出了质疑。教育意义和影响声明期望和价值的乘法模型表明,当人们相信自己能够成功(期望)并在任务中找到意义(价值)时,他们的成就会得到提升,这种提升超过了单独考虑高期望和高价值所带来的综合收益。目前的研究揭示了一种协同效应,表明综合影响大于单独影响,特别是在青少年学生中,而不是在低年级儿童中。这些发现意味着,期望-价值理论的乘法模型可能并不普遍适用。
{"title":"Testing the generalizability of the multiplicative effects of expectancy and value across different ages, genders, and races","authors":"Eunjin Seo ,&nbsp;You-kyung Lee ,&nbsp;Rebecca S. Steingut ,&nbsp;Edna C. Alfaro ,&nbsp;Kejin Lee","doi":"10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102578","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102578","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>This research examines how the multiplicative effect of expectancy and value on achievement-related outcomes differ based on students' individual differences, including age, race, and gender. We analyzed longitudinal data from 10th-grade students (Study 1: <em>n</em> ≈ 10,700) and 3rd-grade students (Study 2: <em>n</em> ≈ 9680) who identified as Black or White. The results indicate that expectancy and value synergistically predict later achievement in adolescents, but not in children. We did not find evidence of different effects based on race or gender. Our findings tentatively indicate that expectancy beliefs closely aligned with their relative achievement levels, along with a clear understanding of value, could enhance the synergistic effects of expectancy and value. The findings challenge the universality of the multiplicative model of expectancy-value theory across different populations and contexts.</div></div><div><h3>Educational relevance and implications statement</h3><div>The multiplicative model of expectancy and value suggests that when people believe they can succeed (expectancy) and find meaning in a task (value), their achievement gains a boost that surpasses the combined gains from high expectancy and high value considered separately. The current study reveals a synergistic effect, indicating that the combined influence is greater than the individual effects when considered separately, specifically in adolescent students, but not in younger children. These findings imply that the multiplicative model of expectancy-value theory may not apply universally.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48336,"journal":{"name":"Learning and Individual Differences","volume":"116 ","pages":"Article 102578"},"PeriodicalIF":3.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142560604","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Science motivation, academic achievement, career aspirations in early adolescents 青少年的科学动机、学业成绩和职业理想
IF 3.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-10-29 DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102577
David W. Putwain , Andrea Mallaburn , Tanja Held
Studies have shown how expectancy and subjective task values (STVs) support, and perceived cost can undermine, science achievement and aspirations. Studies that combine multiple facets of STVs and cost with expectancy to explore impacts of science achievement and aspiration, in early adolescence, are lacking. The sample comprised 1240 students (498 males, mean age of 12.4 years) who self-reported expectancy, STVs, and cost. Between one and two weeks later students self-reported science aspirations and took a 30-min science test. A latent profile analysis indicated a four-profile solution was optimal. A profile comprised of high expectancy and STV, with low cost, showed the highest achievement and aspirations. In profiles where expectancy and STV were lower, or cost higher, achievement and aspirations were lower. Our findings imply classroom strategies, or interventions, to raise expectation and STV, and reduce cost, would benefit students at a critical age.

Educational implications statement

In students aged 11–14 years the highest science achievement and aspirations were found in a motivational profile where success was expected in combination with a perception of science being interesting/enjoyable, important, and useful, along with a perception that the cost of studying science (e.g., the effort involved) was low. Science achievement and aspirations were lower in motivational profiles where expectations and interest/enjoyment in science was lower, and cost was higher. Instructional strategies, interventions, and outreach activities, that raise accurate expectations of success and interest/enjoyment in science, and which reduce costs, would be possible ways to boost science aspirations and achievement.
研究表明,期望值和主观任务价值(STVs)会支持科学成就和抱负,而感知到的成本则会削弱科学成就和抱负。目前还缺乏将主观任务价值和成本的多个方面与期望值相结合,以探讨对青少年科学成就和愿望的影响的研究。样本包括 1240 名学生(498 名男生,平均年龄为 12.4 岁),他们自我报告了期望值、STV 和成本。一至两周后,学生们自我报告了对科学的渴望,并参加了 30 分钟的科学测试。潜在特征分析表明,四种特征解决方案最为理想。由高期望值和 STV 以及低成本组成的方案显示出最高的成就和愿望。在期望值和 STV 值较低或成本较高的情况下,成绩和愿望都较低。我们的研究结果表明,提高期望值和 STV 值并降低成本的课堂策略或干预措施将使处于关键年龄段的学生受益匪浅。在对科学的期望和兴趣/乐趣较低、成本较高的动机特征中,科学成就和愿望较低。教学策略、干预措施和外联活动,如果能提高对成功的准确预期和对科学的兴趣/乐趣,并降低成本,将是提高科学志向和成就的可能途径。
{"title":"Science motivation, academic achievement, career aspirations in early adolescents","authors":"David W. Putwain ,&nbsp;Andrea Mallaburn ,&nbsp;Tanja Held","doi":"10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102577","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102577","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Studies have shown how expectancy and subjective task values (STVs) support, and perceived cost can undermine, science achievement and aspirations. Studies that combine multiple facets of STVs and cost with expectancy to explore impacts of science achievement and aspiration, in early adolescence, are lacking. The sample comprised 1240 students (498 males, mean age of 12.4 years) who self-reported expectancy, STVs, and cost. Between one and two weeks later students self-reported science aspirations and took a 30-min science test. A latent profile analysis indicated a four-profile solution was optimal. A profile comprised of high expectancy and STV, with low cost, showed the highest achievement and aspirations. In profiles where expectancy and STV were lower, or cost higher, achievement and aspirations were lower. Our findings imply classroom strategies, or interventions, to raise expectation and STV, and reduce cost, would benefit students at a critical age.</div></div><div><h3>Educational implications statement</h3><div>In students aged 11–14 years the highest science achievement and aspirations were found in a motivational profile where success was expected in combination with a perception of science being interesting/enjoyable, important, and useful, along with a perception that the cost of studying science (e.g., the effort involved) was low. Science achievement and aspirations were lower in motivational profiles where expectations and interest/enjoyment in science was lower, and cost was higher. Instructional strategies, interventions, and outreach activities, that raise accurate expectations of success and interest/enjoyment in science, and which reduce costs, would be possible ways to boost science aspirations and achievement.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48336,"journal":{"name":"Learning and Individual Differences","volume":"116 ","pages":"Article 102577"},"PeriodicalIF":3.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142537512","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Parents' implicit intelligence beliefs about children's intelligence: Implications for children's academic self-concept and achievement in Maths, English, and French 父母对子女智力的内隐智力信念:对儿童数学、英语和法语学习自我概念和成绩的影响
IF 3.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-10-19 DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102576
Frédéric Guay, Patrick Racine, Richard Bradet
In this study, we hypothesized that a higher parents' fixed mindset will lead to lower achievement in math, French and English (MEF) school subjects, but also that lower achievement in MEF will lead to a higher parents' fixed mindset. We have also tested the mediational role of academic self-concept (ASC) in the parents' fixed mindset-children's achievement relation. Participants (n = 1046 students and their parents) have filled twice a questionnaire. Results indicate that parents' fixed mindset significantly and negatively predicted subsequent achievement in French and English, but the magnitude of these coefficients was quite low (β < | 0.07|). Moreover, most relations connecting prior ASC to subsequent achievement in MEF were not supported thereby discarding the possibility that ASC mediates the parents' fixed mindset-children's achievement relation. Overall, these results cast some doubts about the importance of parental fixed mindset for children's academic achievement and ASC.

Educational relevance and implications of the research

The present study tests if the effect of parental fixed mindset on achievement in Math, English, and French school subjects is explained by students' academic self-concept in these school subjects. The results showed that parental fixed mindset weakly predicts subsequent achievement in French and English and that academic self-concept does not mediate this small association. Thus, the safest conclusion is that parents' mindsets are related to their children's achievement, but so weakly that it is difficult to recommend any intervention based on parental mindsets with this research.
在本研究中,我们假设父母的固定型思维模式越高,孩子在数学、法语和英语(MEF)学校科目中的成绩就越低,同时,MEF成绩越低,父母的固定型思维模式就越高。我们还测试了学业自我概念(ASC)在父母的固定思维模式与子女成绩关系中的中介作用。参与者(n = 1046 名学生及其家长)填写了两次问卷。结果表明,父母的固定心态对孩子随后的法语和英语成绩有显著的负向预测作用,但这些系数的大小相当低(β < | 0.07|)。此外,大多数先前的 ASC 与后来的 MEF 成绩之间的关系都没有得到支持,从而排除了 ASC 调解父母的固定心态与子女成绩之间关系的可能性。总体而言,这些结果让人对父母的固定心态对儿童学业成绩和 ASC 的重要性产生了一些怀疑。本研究的教育意义和启示本研究检验了父母的固定心态对数学、英语和法语学科成绩的影响是否可以通过学生在这些学科中的学业自我概念来解释。结果表明,父母的固定心态对学生随后的法语和英语成绩有微弱的预测作用,而学业自我概念并不能调节这种微小的关联。因此,最稳妥的结论是,父母的思维方式与子女的学习成绩有关,但程度很弱,因此很难通过这项研究建议采取任何基于父母思维方式的干预措施。
{"title":"Parents' implicit intelligence beliefs about children's intelligence: Implications for children's academic self-concept and achievement in Maths, English, and French","authors":"Frédéric Guay,&nbsp;Patrick Racine,&nbsp;Richard Bradet","doi":"10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102576","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102576","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>In this study, we hypothesized that a higher parents' fixed mindset will lead to lower achievement in math, French and English (MEF) school subjects, but also that lower achievement in MEF will lead to a higher parents' fixed mindset. We have also tested the mediational role of academic self-concept (ASC) in the parents' fixed mindset-children's achievement relation. Participants (<em>n</em> = 1046 students and their parents) have filled twice a questionnaire. Results indicate that parents' fixed mindset significantly and negatively predicted subsequent achievement in French and English, but the magnitude of these coefficients was quite low (β &lt; | 0.07|). Moreover, most relations connecting prior ASC to subsequent achievement in MEF were not supported thereby discarding the possibility that ASC mediates the parents' fixed mindset-children's achievement relation. Overall, these results cast some doubts about the importance of parental fixed mindset for children's academic achievement and ASC.</div></div><div><h3>Educational relevance and implications of the research</h3><div>The present study tests if the effect of parental fixed mindset on achievement in Math, English, and French school subjects is explained by students' academic self-concept in these school subjects. The results showed that parental fixed mindset weakly predicts subsequent achievement in French and English and that academic self-concept does not mediate this small association. Thus, the safest conclusion is that parents' mindsets are related to their children's achievement, but so weakly that it is difficult to recommend any intervention based on parental mindsets with this research.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48336,"journal":{"name":"Learning and Individual Differences","volume":"116 ","pages":"Article 102576"},"PeriodicalIF":3.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142526398","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What is the link between an early university entry offer and the academic and personal wellbeing outcomes of students in their final year of school? 提前进入大学与学生在最后一年的学业和个人幸福之间有什么联系?
IF 3.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-10-17 DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102571
Andrew J. Martin , Helen Tam
There has been growth in the number of final year school students applying for an offer of a place at university prior to completing their last year of school. This study investigated the role of early entry offer status in 1512 final year (Year 12) Australian students' academic performance—and also in a sub-sample's (n = 525) self-reported academic motivation and engagement, academic stress responses, and personal wellbeing. We found no significant effects on final year performance, academic motivation and engagement, or personal wellbeing as a function of early entry offer status—thus, most final year school outcomes were accounted for by factors unrelated to a student's early entry offer status. However, there was a small but significant positive effect for academic buoyancy among students who had applied for and received an early entry offer—thus, in part assisting their capacity to navigate academic challenge.
在完成最后一年学业之前申请大学录取通知书的应届毕业生人数不断增加。本研究调查了提前录取资格对 1512 名澳大利亚毕业班(12 年级)学生学业成绩的影响,以及对子样本(n = 525)自我报告的学习动机和参与度、学习压力反应和个人健康的影响。我们发现,提前录取情况对最后一年的学习成绩、学习动机和参与度以及个人幸福感没有明显影响,因此,最后一年的大部分学习成绩都是由与学生提前录取情况无关的因素造成的。然而,在申请并获得提前录取通知的学生中,学业积极性有微小但显著的积极影响--这在一定程度上有助于他们应对学业挑战的能力。
{"title":"What is the link between an early university entry offer and the academic and personal wellbeing outcomes of students in their final year of school?","authors":"Andrew J. Martin ,&nbsp;Helen Tam","doi":"10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102571","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102571","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>There has been growth in the number of final year school students applying for an offer of a place at university prior to completing their last year of school. This study investigated the role of early entry offer status in 1512 final year (Year 12) Australian students' academic performance—and also in a sub-sample's (<em>n</em> = 525) self-reported academic motivation and engagement, academic stress responses, and personal wellbeing. We found no significant effects on final year performance, academic motivation and engagement, or personal wellbeing as a function of early entry offer status—thus, most final year school outcomes were accounted for by factors unrelated to a student's early entry offer status. However, there was a small but significant positive effect for academic buoyancy among students who had applied for and received an early entry offer—thus, in part assisting their capacity to navigate academic challenge.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48336,"journal":{"name":"Learning and Individual Differences","volume":"116 ","pages":"Article 102571"},"PeriodicalIF":3.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142445202","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Is intelligence necessary and sufficient for creativity? An analysis of convergent and divergent thinking 智力是创造力的必要条件和充分条件吗?聚合思维和发散思维分析
IF 3.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-10-17 DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102575
Merav Dechaume , Maxence Mercier , Cyrille Feybesse , Todd Lubart , Gaëlle Chouvelon , Solenn Kermarrec , Sylvie Tordjman
This study examined the relationship between intelligence and creativity using divergent-exploratory and convergent-integrative thinking as key concepts. Researchers analyzed secondary data from 854 children, including 238 children with high intellectual potential (IQ > 130). Intelligence was measured using WISC (Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children), and creativity was assessed through EPoC (Evaluation of Potential for Creativity). Segmented Regression Analysis (SRA) and Necessary Condition Analysis (NCA) explored these relationships. Positive correlations were found between convergent-integrative thinking and verbal comprehension or processing speed. However, no significant breakpoints were detected between Total IQ and creative potential sub-dimensions. A sufficient level of processing speed was necessary for achieving high divergent-exploratory thinking scores, while minimum levels of Total IQ or fluid reasoning were required for convergent-integrative thinking. The study suggested using a Generalized Additive Model (GAM) to capture complex, non-linear relationships. This approach offers educational insights by highlighting cognitive strengths and weaknesses in assessments.
本研究以发散-探索思维和聚合-整合思维为关键概念,探讨了智力与创造力之间的关系。研究人员分析了 854 名儿童的二手数据,其中包括 238 名高智力潜能儿童(IQ 130)。智力通过韦氏儿童智力量表(WISC)进行测量,创造力通过创造力潜能评估(EPoC)进行评估。分段回归分析(SRA)和必要条件分析(NCA)探讨了这些关系。发现聚合-整合思维与言语理解能力或处理速度之间存在正相关。然而,在总智商和创造潜能子维度之间没有发现明显的分界点。要获得较高的发散-探索性思维分数,必须有足够的处理速度,而收敛-整合思维则需要最低水平的总智商或流畅推理能力。研究建议使用广义相加模型(GAM)来捕捉复杂的非线性关系。这种方法通过突出评估中的认知强项和弱项,为教育提供了启示。
{"title":"Is intelligence necessary and sufficient for creativity? An analysis of convergent and divergent thinking","authors":"Merav Dechaume ,&nbsp;Maxence Mercier ,&nbsp;Cyrille Feybesse ,&nbsp;Todd Lubart ,&nbsp;Gaëlle Chouvelon ,&nbsp;Solenn Kermarrec ,&nbsp;Sylvie Tordjman","doi":"10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102575","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102575","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>This study examined the relationship between intelligence and creativity using divergent-exploratory and convergent-integrative thinking as key concepts. Researchers analyzed secondary data from 854 children, including 238 children with high intellectual potential (IQ &gt; 130). Intelligence was measured using WISC (Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children), and creativity was assessed through EPoC (Evaluation of Potential for Creativity). Segmented Regression Analysis (SRA) and Necessary Condition Analysis (NCA) explored these relationships. Positive correlations were found between convergent-integrative thinking and verbal comprehension or processing speed. However, no significant breakpoints were detected between Total IQ and creative potential sub-dimensions. A sufficient level of processing speed was necessary for achieving high divergent-exploratory thinking scores, while minimum levels of Total IQ or fluid reasoning were required for convergent-integrative thinking. The study suggested using a Generalized Additive Model (GAM) to capture complex, non-linear relationships. This approach offers educational insights by highlighting cognitive strengths and weaknesses in assessments.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48336,"journal":{"name":"Learning and Individual Differences","volume":"116 ","pages":"Article 102575"},"PeriodicalIF":3.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142444753","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotional intelligence and reading comprehension: Examining mediating roles of word reading, vocabulary, and working memory 情商与阅读理解:研究单词阅读、词汇量和工作记忆的中介作用
IF 3.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-10-17 DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102574
Liyan Yu , Xiuhong Tong
This study examined the potential pathways connecting emotional intelligence to reading comprehension of narrative and non-narrative texts, and the influence of family socioeconomic status (SES) on these pathways. We tracked 689 Chinese third-grade students (341 female; Mage = 9.23 years, SD = 0.66) from wave 1 (grade three) to wave 3 (grade five). Results revealed that after controlling for gender, reading comprehension, and non-verbal intelligence at wave 1, word reading and vocabulary knowledge at wave 2 were significant mediators linking emotional intelligence at wave 1 to reading comprehension of both narrative and non-narrative texts at wave 3. In contrast, working memory at wave 2 served as a mediator only between emotional intelligence at wave 1 and reading comprehension of non-narrative texts at wave 3. Additionally, family SES moderated the pathways linking emotional intelligence to reading comprehension. These findings provide a comprehensive understanding of mechanisms linking emotional intelligence to reading comprehension.

Educational relevance and implications statement

Using both narrative and non-narrative texts, our study found that for Chinese primary school students, word reading (β = 0.04, 95 % CI [0.03, 0.07] for narrative texts; β = 0.04, 95 % CI [0.02, 0.06] for non-narrative texts) and vocabulary knowledge at age 10 (β = 0.03, 95 % CI [0.01, 0.05] for narrative texts; β = 0.02, 95 % CI [0.10, 0.04] for non-narrative texts) significantly mediate the relationship between emotional intelligence at age 9 and reading comprehension at age 11. These results highlight the importance of combining word reading, vocabulary knowledge, and emotional intelligence during intervention to improve children's reading comprehension. We also found that working memory at age 10 significantly mediates the relationship between emotional intelligence at age 9 and reading comprehension at age 11 for non-narrative (β = 0.01, 95 % CI [0.01, 0.03]), but not narrative, texts (β = 0.01, 95 % CI [−0.004, 0.02]). These findings underscore the importance of considering text genre when examining the development of reading comprehension, designing reading instruction, and establishing reading interventions. Additionally, emotional intelligence at age 9 is more strongly associated with word reading, vocabulary knowledge, and working memory at age 10 for children from low socioeconomic status (SES) families compared to those from high-SES families, highlighting the need for more attention to the emotional intelligence development in children from low-SES backgrounds.
本研究探讨了连接情商与叙事和非叙事文本阅读理解的潜在途径,以及家庭社会经济地位(SES)对这些途径的影响。我们对689名中国三年级学生(341名女生;年龄=9.23岁,SD=0.66)进行了从第一波(三年级)到第三波(五年级)的跟踪调查。结果显示,在控制了第一阶段的性别、阅读理解能力和非语言智能后,第二阶段的单词阅读和词汇知识是第一阶段情商与第三阶段叙事和非叙事文本阅读理解能力之间的重要中介。相比之下,第二阶段的工作记忆仅是第一阶段情商与第三阶段非叙事文本阅读理解之间的中介。此外,家庭经济状况也调节了情商与阅读理解之间的联系。这些发现为我们全面了解情商与阅读理解之间的关联机制提供了依据。教育相关性及启示声明通过对叙事文本和非叙事文本的研究,我们发现对于中国小学生而言,单词阅读(β = 0.04,95 % CI [0.03, 0.07]; β = 0.04, 95 % CI [0.02, 0.06] for non-narrative texts)和10岁时的词汇知识(β = 0.03, 95 % CI [0.01, 0.05] for narrative texts; β = 0.02, 95 % CI [0.10, 0.04] for non-narrative texts)显著地调节了9岁时的情绪智力和11岁时的阅读理解能力之间的关系。这些结果凸显了在干预过程中将单词阅读、词汇知识和情商结合起来以提高儿童阅读理解能力的重要性。我们还发现,对于非叙事文本(β = 0.01,95 % CI [0.01,0.03])而非叙事文本(β = 0.01,95 % CI [-0.004,0.02]),10 岁时的工作记忆对 9 岁时的情商和 11 岁时的阅读理解能力之间的关系有显著的中介作用。这些发现强调了在研究阅读理解能力的发展、设计阅读教学和制定阅读干预措施时考虑文本体裁的重要性。此外,与来自高社会经济地位家庭的儿童相比,来自低社会经济地位家庭的儿童在9岁时的情商与10岁时的单词阅读、词汇知识和工作记忆有更密切的关系,这突出表明需要更多地关注来自低社会经济地位家庭的儿童的情商发展。
{"title":"Emotional intelligence and reading comprehension: Examining mediating roles of word reading, vocabulary, and working memory","authors":"Liyan Yu ,&nbsp;Xiuhong Tong","doi":"10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102574","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102574","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>This study examined the potential pathways connecting emotional intelligence to reading comprehension of narrative and non-narrative texts, and the influence of family socioeconomic status (SES) on these pathways. We tracked 689 Chinese third-grade students (341 female; Mage = 9.23 years, SD = 0.66) from wave 1 (grade three) to wave 3 (grade five). Results revealed that after controlling for gender, reading comprehension, and non-verbal intelligence at wave 1, word reading and vocabulary knowledge at wave 2 were significant mediators linking emotional intelligence at wave 1 to reading comprehension of both narrative and non-narrative texts at wave 3. In contrast, working memory at wave 2 served as a mediator only between emotional intelligence at wave 1 and reading comprehension of non-narrative texts at wave 3. Additionally, family SES moderated the pathways linking emotional intelligence to reading comprehension. These findings provide a comprehensive understanding of mechanisms linking emotional intelligence to reading comprehension.</div></div><div><h3>Educational relevance and implications statement</h3><div>Using both narrative and non-narrative texts, our study found that for Chinese primary school students, word reading (<em>β</em> = 0.04, 95 % CI [0.03, 0.07] for narrative texts; <em>β</em> = 0.04, 95 % CI [0.02, 0.06] for non-narrative texts) and vocabulary knowledge at age 10 (<em>β</em> = 0.03, 95 % CI [0.01, 0.05] for narrative texts; <em>β</em> = 0.02, 95 % CI [0.10, 0.04] for non-narrative texts) significantly mediate the relationship between emotional intelligence at age 9 and reading comprehension at age 11. These results highlight the importance of combining word reading, vocabulary knowledge, and emotional intelligence during intervention to improve children's reading comprehension. We also found that working memory at age 10 significantly mediates the relationship between emotional intelligence at age 9 and reading comprehension at age 11 for non-narrative (<em>β</em> = 0.01, 95 % CI [0.01, 0.03]), but not narrative, texts (<em>β</em> = 0.01, 95 % CI [−0.004, 0.02]). These findings underscore the importance of considering text genre when examining the development of reading comprehension, designing reading instruction, and establishing reading interventions. Additionally, emotional intelligence at age 9 is more strongly associated with word reading, vocabulary knowledge, and working memory at age 10 for children from low socioeconomic status (SES) families compared to those from high-SES families, highlighting the need for more attention to the emotional intelligence development in children from low-SES backgrounds.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48336,"journal":{"name":"Learning and Individual Differences","volume":"116 ","pages":"Article 102574"},"PeriodicalIF":3.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142444754","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A longitudinal quasi-experiment of leaderboard effectiveness on learner behaviors and course performance 排行榜对学习者行为和课程成绩影响的纵向准实验
IF 3.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-10-15 DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102572
Nga Do, Tao Jin , Reed Priest , Liza N. Meredith, Richard N. Landers
Leaderboards are increasingly viewed as a useful gamification technique to improve learning, partly due to their ease of implementation into common learning management systems. However, past research has revealed mixed results in terms of their effectiveness. This inconsistency may be attributable to a lack of attention to research design, conflation of multiple design factors, or overreliance on correlational studies. In the present study, a longitudinal quasi-experimental design incorporating random assignment of groups to leaderboard conditions was used to examine the effect of leaderboards on optional practice behaviors and course performance in a college course. Although practice behaviors were linked to course performance, the leaderboard did not meaningfully affect those practice behaviors and also negatively affected course performance through other mediators. These results highlight the critical role of rigorous theory-based design of gamified systems and also demonstrate the need for rigorous research designs to understand actual impacts.

Educational relevance and implications statement

Our research explored the impact of using leaderboards, a common gamification strategy, in an educational setting to see if they would motivate students to engage in optional practice activities, with the aim of improving their course performance. Surprisingly, we found that leaderboards did not encourage additional practice and, unexpectedly, led to lower exam scores. This suggests that leaderboards, while intended to boost motivation and learning through competition, might have the opposite effect by potentially undermining students' motivation or altering their perception of the course's value. These findings underscore the complexity of integrating gamification into education and highlight the necessity for careful design and testing of such strategies to ensure they align with educational goals. Our study is crucial for educators and instructional designers as it provides evidence that not all gamification techniques work as intended and stresses the importance of understanding the underlying psychological impacts on students.
排行榜被越来越多的人视为提高学习效果的一种有用的游戏化技术,部分原因是排行榜易于在普通的学习管理系统中实施。然而,过去的研究显示,排行榜的有效性参差不齐。造成这种不一致的原因可能是缺乏对研究设计的关注,混淆了多种设计因素,或者过度依赖相关研究。本研究采用纵向准实验设计,将各组随机分配到排行榜条件下,以考察排行榜对大学课程中选修练习行为和课程成绩的影响。尽管练习行为与课程成绩相关联,但排行榜并没有对这些练习行为产生有意义的影响,而且还通过其他中介因素对课程成绩产生了负面影响。这些结果凸显了基于严谨理论设计游戏化系统的关键作用,同时也证明了严谨的研究设计对了解实际影响的必要性。我们的研究探索了在教育环境中使用排行榜(一种常见的游戏化策略)的影响,以了解排行榜是否会激励学生参与选修实践活动,从而提高他们的课程成绩。令人惊讶的是,我们发现排行榜并不能鼓励学生进行额外的练习,反而会导致考试成绩下降。这表明,排行榜虽然旨在通过竞争来提高学习积极性和学习效果,但可能会产生相反的效果,因为它可能会削弱学生的学习积极性或改变他们对课程价值的看法。这些发现凸显了将游戏化融入教育的复杂性,并强调了精心设计和测试此类策略以确保其符合教育目标的必要性。我们的研究对教育工作者和教学设计者来说至关重要,因为它提供了证据,证明并非所有游戏化技术都能达到预期效果,并强调了了解学生潜在心理影响的重要性。
{"title":"A longitudinal quasi-experiment of leaderboard effectiveness on learner behaviors and course performance","authors":"Nga Do,&nbsp;Tao Jin ,&nbsp;Reed Priest ,&nbsp;Liza N. Meredith,&nbsp;Richard N. Landers","doi":"10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102572","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102572","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Leaderboards are increasingly viewed as a useful gamification technique to improve learning, partly due to their ease of implementation into common learning management systems. However, past research has revealed mixed results in terms of their effectiveness. This inconsistency may be attributable to a lack of attention to research design, conflation of multiple design factors, or overreliance on correlational studies. In the present study, a longitudinal quasi-experimental design incorporating random assignment of groups to leaderboard conditions was used to examine the effect of leaderboards on optional practice behaviors and course performance in a college course. Although practice behaviors were linked to course performance, the leaderboard did not meaningfully affect those practice behaviors and also negatively affected course performance through other mediators. These results highlight the critical role of rigorous theory-based design of gamified systems and also demonstrate the need for rigorous research designs to understand actual impacts.</div></div><div><h3>Educational relevance and implications statement</h3><div>Our research explored the impact of using leaderboards, a common gamification strategy, in an educational setting to see if they would motivate students to engage in optional practice activities, with the aim of improving their course performance. Surprisingly, we found that leaderboards did not encourage additional practice and, unexpectedly, led to lower exam scores. This suggests that leaderboards, while intended to boost motivation and learning through competition, might have the opposite effect by potentially undermining students' motivation or altering their perception of the course's value. These findings underscore the complexity of integrating gamification into education and highlight the necessity for careful design and testing of such strategies to ensure they align with educational goals. Our study is crucial for educators and instructional designers as it provides evidence that not all gamification techniques work as intended and stresses the importance of understanding the underlying psychological impacts on students.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48336,"journal":{"name":"Learning and Individual Differences","volume":"116 ","pages":"Article 102572"},"PeriodicalIF":3.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142438129","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The highs and lows of mathematical ability: Shared and distinct longitudinal predictors of mathematical ability grouping 数学能力的高低:数学能力分组的共同和独特纵向预测因素
IF 3.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-10-15 DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102570
Stanley Cheng , Rebecca Bull , Emma C. Burns , David Muñez
The current study examines how kindergarten domain-general and domain-specific factors, mathematical skills at primary 1 (P1), and socioeconomic status (SES) predict high versus low (top and bottom 13th percentile) mathematical ability at primary 6 (P6). Four-hundred-and-seven P6 students had previously completed the measures. Logistic path analyses showed that SES, P1 mathematics, and fluid intelligence showed direct significant effects on P6 high achievement group membership. Visuospatial working memory, SES, reading, and P1 mathematics showed direct effects on P6 low group membership. Number sense, fine motor skills, and reading demonstrated indirect effects on both group memberships through P1 mathematics. No significant effect was found for executive functioning or verbal working memory. Findings suggest that there is a qualitative difference in mathematical ability across the performance spectrum. Assessment and instruction should be differentiated to account for predictors that are specific to either end of the ability spectrum.

Educational relevance and implications of the research

Mathematical ability plays a critical role in future academic success and quality of life, and also provides the foundational knowledge for engagement in STEM school subjects and careers. While we know there are some early cognitive and environmental factors that predict future mathematical outcomes, little is known about whether they predict both mathematical high and low achievers differently. This study is one of very few that uses a large-scale longitudinal design to track how early factors predict students' mathematical performance across the two ends of the achievement continuum over eight years. The findings add to the existing literature that cognitive factors predict mathematical competencies differently across developmental time, and that there are some common and unique predictors of performance at the two ends of the achievement continuum. The outcomes of the study inform educators that fine motor skills, basic reading skills, and number sense are likely to be important fundamental skills for early mathematical development, which may have a carry-effect on both mathematical success and failure in late primary school. Visuospatial working memory and basic reading skills are likely to be particularly important to prevent students from falling behind, whereas logical reasoning skills appear to be particularly important for mathematically talented students.
本研究探讨了幼儿园领域一般因素和特定领域因素、小学一年级(P1)数学技能和社会经济地位(SES)如何预测小学六年级(P6)数学能力的高低(前13百分位和后13百分位)。四百零七名小六学生之前完成了这些测量。逻辑路径分析显示,社会经济地位、小学一年级数学和流体智力对小学六年级高成就组的成员资格有直接的显著影响。视觉空间工作记忆、社会经济地位、阅读和 P1 数学对 P6 低成绩组的成员有直接影响。数感、精细动作技能和阅读通过 P1 数学对两个小组的成员资格都有间接影响。在执行功能和言语工作记忆方面没有发现明显的影响。研究结果表明,不同成绩的学生在数学能力上存在质的差异。研究的教育意义和影响数学能力对未来的学业成功和生活质量起着至关重要的作用,同时也为从事 STEM 学科和职业提供了基础知识。虽然我们知道有一些早期认知和环境因素可以预测未来的数学成绩,但对于这些因素是否会对数学成绩优秀和数学成绩较差的学生产生不同的影响却知之甚少。这项研究是为数不多的采用大规模纵向设计来追踪早期因素如何预测学生八年来在成绩连续体两端的数学表现的研究之一。研究结果补充了现有的文献,即认知因素在不同的发展时期对数学能力的预测是不同的,在成绩连续体的两端有一些共同的和独特的预测因素。研究结果告诉教育工作者,精细动作技能、基本阅读技能和数感可能是早期数学发展的重要基本技能,可能对小学后期的数学成败产生影响。视觉空间工作记忆和基本阅读技能可能对防止学生落后尤为重要,而逻辑推理技能似乎对有数学天赋的学生尤为重要。
{"title":"The highs and lows of mathematical ability: Shared and distinct longitudinal predictors of mathematical ability grouping","authors":"Stanley Cheng ,&nbsp;Rebecca Bull ,&nbsp;Emma C. Burns ,&nbsp;David Muñez","doi":"10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102570","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102570","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>The current study examines how kindergarten domain-general and domain-specific factors, mathematical skills at primary 1 (P1), and socioeconomic status (SES) predict high versus low (top and bottom 13th percentile) mathematical ability at primary 6 (P6). Four-hundred-and-seven P6 students had previously completed the measures. Logistic path analyses showed that SES, P1 mathematics, and fluid intelligence showed direct significant effects on P6 high achievement group membership. Visuospatial working memory, SES, reading, and P1 mathematics showed direct effects on P6 low group membership. Number sense, fine motor skills, and reading demonstrated indirect effects on both group memberships through P1 mathematics. No significant effect was found for executive functioning or verbal working memory. Findings suggest that there is a qualitative difference in mathematical ability across the performance spectrum. Assessment and instruction should be differentiated to account for predictors that are specific to either end of the ability spectrum.</div></div><div><h3>Educational relevance and implications of the research</h3><div>Mathematical ability plays a critical role in future academic success and quality of life, and also provides the foundational knowledge for engagement in STEM school subjects and careers. While we know there are some early cognitive and environmental factors that predict future mathematical outcomes, little is known about whether they predict both mathematical high and low achievers differently. This study is one of very few that uses a large-scale longitudinal design to track how early factors predict students' mathematical performance across the two ends of the achievement continuum over eight years. The findings add to the existing literature that cognitive factors predict mathematical competencies differently across developmental time, and that there are some common and unique predictors of performance at the two ends of the achievement continuum. The outcomes of the study inform educators that fine motor skills, basic reading skills, and number sense are likely to be important fundamental skills for early mathematical development, which may have a carry-effect on both mathematical success and failure in late primary school. Visuospatial working memory and basic reading skills are likely to be particularly important to prevent students from falling behind, whereas logical reasoning skills appear to be particularly important for mathematically talented students.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48336,"journal":{"name":"Learning and Individual Differences","volume":"116 ","pages":"Article 102570"},"PeriodicalIF":3.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142438200","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Opportunities and obstacles: Using profile analysis to examine sources of reading self-efficacy in early adolescence 机遇与障碍:利用特征分析研究青少年早期阅读自我效能感的来源
IF 3.8 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EDUCATIONAL Pub Date : 2024-10-14 DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102573
Jaeyun Han , Pilvi Peura , Calah J. Ford , Amanda R. Butz , Ellen L. Usher
When investigating the sources of self-efficacy, researchers have historically focused on learners' perceptions of positive (e.g., direct & vicarious successes, encouragement & praise) and negative (e.g., stress, anxiety) experiences. These approaches may not capture other types of efficacy-relevant experiences. The purpose of this study was to investigate how early adolescent readers perceive and integrate diverse types of efficacy-relevant information. Students (N = 1804) responded to items related to four hypothesized sources of self-efficacy that reflected opportunities and obstacles in reading. Latent profile analysis indicated four profiles related to students' efficacy-relevant reading experiences. These profiles were examined in relation to students' gender, socioeconomic status, grade level, and race/ethnicity. Students exposed to mostly positive sources of information showed higher levels of reading self-efficacy and reading achievement than those reporting more exposure to negative sources of information. Results highlight the importance of providing opportunities and minimizing obstacles to support adolescents' reading self-efficacy.
在研究自我效能感的来源时,研究人员历来侧重于学习者对积极(如直接&;替代成功、鼓励&;表扬)和消极(如压力、焦虑)体验的感知。这些方法可能无法捕捉到其他类型的与效率相关的体验。本研究旨在调查青少年早期读者如何感知和整合不同类型的效率相关信息。学生(人数 = 1804)回答了与四种自我效能感假设来源相关的项目,这些假设来源反映了阅读中的机会和障碍。潜特征分析表明,有四种特征与学生与效率相关的阅读经验有关。这些特征与学生的性别、社会经济地位、年级和种族/民族有关。与较多接触负面信息源的学生相比,接触正面信息源的学生显示出更高的阅读自我效能感和阅读成就。研究结果凸显了提供机会和减少障碍对提高青少年阅读自我效能的重要性。
{"title":"Opportunities and obstacles: Using profile analysis to examine sources of reading self-efficacy in early adolescence","authors":"Jaeyun Han ,&nbsp;Pilvi Peura ,&nbsp;Calah J. Ford ,&nbsp;Amanda R. Butz ,&nbsp;Ellen L. Usher","doi":"10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102573","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.lindif.2024.102573","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>When investigating the sources of self-efficacy, researchers have historically focused on learners' perceptions of positive (e.g., direct &amp; vicarious successes, encouragement &amp; praise) and negative (e.g., stress, anxiety) experiences. These approaches may not capture other types of efficacy-relevant experiences. The purpose of this study was to investigate how early adolescent readers perceive and integrate diverse types of efficacy-relevant information. Students (<em>N</em> = 1804) responded to items related to four hypothesized sources of self-efficacy that reflected opportunities and obstacles in reading. Latent profile analysis indicated four profiles related to students' efficacy-relevant reading experiences. These profiles were examined in relation to students' gender, socioeconomic status, grade level, and race/ethnicity. Students exposed to mostly positive sources of information showed higher levels of reading self-efficacy and reading achievement than those reporting more exposure to negative sources of information. Results highlight the importance of providing opportunities and minimizing obstacles to support adolescents' reading self-efficacy.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48336,"journal":{"name":"Learning and Individual Differences","volume":"116 ","pages":"Article 102573"},"PeriodicalIF":3.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142433907","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Learning and Individual Differences
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1