Given that the vast majority of brand extensions fail, it is important to understand how extension failure influences consumer judgments of the parent brand that launched the extension. In the brand extension literature, there is a paucity of research on the role of consumer characteristics in influencing response to such failures. To fill this gap, the present research examines the impact of consumers' implicit theory orientation—their perspective on whether personality traits are malleable versus fixed—on the severity of negative feedback effects following extension failure. Seven studies show that entity theorists, who believe in the fixedness of personality traits, penalize parent brands more than incremental theorists, who endorse trait malleability. This brand penalty effect arises because as compared to incremental theorists, entity theorists are motivated to view brands as a cohesive group and therefore equate extension failure with the diminishment of the overarching parent brand. This effect is more likely when brand cohesiveness is low or ambiguous, but it is less likely when brand cohesiveness is high. Furthermore, while entity theorists are more likely to reduce brand evaluations after extension failure, the two groups do not differ in parent brand evaluations after extension success.
{"title":"Brand extension failure and parent brand penalty: The role of implicit theories","authors":"Shailendra Pratap Jain, Pragya Mathur, Mathew S. Isaac, Huifang Mao, Durairaj Maheswaran","doi":"10.1002/jcpy.1343","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jcpy.1343","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Given that the vast majority of brand extensions fail, it is important to understand how extension failure influences consumer judgments of the parent brand that launched the extension. In the brand extension literature, there is a paucity of research on the role of consumer characteristics in influencing response to such failures. To fill this gap, the present research examines the impact of consumers' implicit theory orientation—their perspective on whether personality traits are malleable versus fixed—on the severity of negative feedback effects following extension failure. Seven studies show that entity theorists, who believe in the fixedness of personality traits, penalize parent brands more than incremental theorists, who endorse trait malleability. This brand penalty effect arises because as compared to incremental theorists, entity theorists are motivated to view brands as a cohesive group and therefore equate extension failure with the diminishment of the overarching parent brand. This effect is more likely when brand cohesiveness is low or ambiguous, but it is less likely when brand cohesiveness is high. Furthermore, while entity theorists are more likely to reduce brand evaluations after extension failure, the two groups do not differ in parent brand evaluations after extension success.</p>","PeriodicalId":48365,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Consumer Psychology","volume":"34 1","pages":"49-65"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2023-01-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48940319","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"管理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Product owners often find it challenging to part with their possessions. As a result, owners may pursue various behavioral strategies to facilitate disposal, even when considering products that are no longer needed or personally useful. According to ethnographic research, one such strategy is to move products into purgatories, or temporary liminal spaces. Using an experimental approach, we aim to causally assess whether product purgatories do in fact induce disposal. Findings from three studies indicate that moving items into purgatories allows owners to psychologically brace for subsequent product disposal. However, unlike prior research that has focused on reduced product attachment as a primary driver of disposal preparedness, the present research documents a novel mechanism that does not necessarily entail attachment reduction. Specifically, purgatories are shown to trigger mental simulation of the product disposal process, thereby helping product owners brace for the looming loss of their product.
{"title":"Bracing for the sting of disposal: Product purgatories encourage mental simulation of the disposal process","authors":"Mathew S. Isaac, Poornima Vinoo","doi":"10.1002/jcpy.1342","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jcpy.1342","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Product owners often find it challenging to part with their possessions. As a result, owners may pursue various behavioral strategies to facilitate disposal, even when considering products that are no longer needed or personally useful. According to ethnographic research, one such strategy is to move products into purgatories, or temporary liminal spaces. Using an experimental approach, we aim to causally assess whether product purgatories do in fact induce disposal. Findings from three studies indicate that moving items into purgatories allows owners to psychologically brace for subsequent product disposal. However, unlike prior research that has focused on reduced product attachment as a primary driver of disposal preparedness, the present research documents a novel mechanism that does not necessarily entail attachment reduction. Specifically, purgatories are shown to trigger <i>mental simulation</i> of the product disposal process, thereby helping product owners brace for the looming loss of their product.</p>","PeriodicalId":48365,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Consumer Psychology","volume":"33 3","pages":"575-582"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2023-01-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47128273","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"管理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Alexandra Aguirre-Rodriguez, David Luna, Cecilia M. O. Alvarez, Detra Montoya
A great deal of research has investigated how various aspects of ethnic identity influence consumer behavior, yet this literature is fragmented. The objective of this article was to present an integrative theoretical model of how individuals are motivated to think and act in a manner consistent with their salient ethnic identities. The model emerges from a review of social science and consumer research about US Hispanics, but researchers could apply it in its general form and/or adapt it to other populations. Our model extends Oyserman's (Journal of Consumer Psychology, 19, 250) identity-based motivation (IBM) model by differentiating between two types of antecedents of ethnic identity salience: longitudinal cultural processes and situational activation by contextual cues, each with different implications for the availability and accessibility of ethnic cultural knowledge. We provide new insights by introducing three ethnic identity motives that are unique to ethnic (nonmajority) cultural groups: belonging, distinctiveness, and defense. These three motives are in constant tension with one another and guide longitudinal processes like acculturation, and ultimately influence consumers' procedural readiness and action readiness. Our integrative framework organizes and offers insights into the current body of Hispanic consumer research, and highlights gaps in the literature that present opportunities for future research.
大量的研究调查了种族认同的各个方面如何影响消费者行为,但这些文献是碎片化的。本文的目的是提出一个综合理论模型,说明个人是如何被激励以与其突出的种族身份相一致的方式思考和行动的。该模型来自对美国西班牙裔美国人的社会科学和消费者研究的回顾,但研究人员可以将其应用于一般形式和/或将其应用于其他人群。我们的模型扩展了Oyserman (Journal of Consumer Psychology, 199,250)基于身份的动机(IBM)模型,通过区分两种类型的种族认同显著性的前因:纵向文化过程和情境激活的语境线索,每一种都对种族文化知识的可用性和可及性有不同的影响。我们通过介绍种族(非多数)文化群体独有的三种种族认同动机:归属、独特性和防御,提供了新的见解。这三种动机之间存在着持续的紧张关系,并引导着文化适应等纵向过程,最终影响着消费者的程序准备和行动准备。我们的综合框架组织并提供了对当前西班牙裔消费者研究的见解,并突出了文献中的空白,为未来的研究提供了机会。
{"title":"Ethnic identity-based motivation: A model emergent from US Hispanic consumers","authors":"Alexandra Aguirre-Rodriguez, David Luna, Cecilia M. O. Alvarez, Detra Montoya","doi":"10.1002/jcpy.1340","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jcpy.1340","url":null,"abstract":"<p>A great deal of research has investigated how various aspects of ethnic identity influence consumer behavior, yet this literature is fragmented. The objective of this article was to present an integrative theoretical model of how individuals are motivated to think and act in a manner consistent with their salient ethnic identities. The model emerges from a review of social science and consumer research about US Hispanics, but researchers could apply it in its general form and/or adapt it to other populations. Our model extends Oyserman's (<i>Journal of Consumer Psychology</i>, <b>19</b>, 250) identity-based motivation (IBM) model by differentiating between two types of antecedents of ethnic identity salience: longitudinal cultural processes and situational activation by contextual cues, each with different implications for the availability and accessibility of ethnic cultural knowledge. We provide new insights by introducing three ethnic identity motives that are unique to ethnic (nonmajority) cultural groups: belonging, distinctiveness, and defense. These three motives are in constant tension with one another and guide longitudinal processes like acculturation, and ultimately influence consumers' procedural readiness and action readiness. Our integrative framework organizes and offers insights into the current body of Hispanic consumer research, and highlights gaps in the literature that present opportunities for future research.</p>","PeriodicalId":48365,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Consumer Psychology","volume":"33 2","pages":"303-327"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2022-12-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43948230","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"管理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Tiered discounts offer larger discounts as consumers meet higher spending thresholds (e.g., spend $100+, receive 10% off; spend $200+, receive 20% off). This research investigates how consumers treat these multiple dollar thresholds as reference points for spending. We find that tiered discounts with smaller increments between thresholds encourage higher spending compared to those with larger increments. This effect occurs because consumers treat thresholds as motivational spending goals when the distance to higher thresholds is smaller (vs. larger). Consistent with this reasoning, signaling goal progress (i.e., displaying cart amount while shopping) attenuates the spending difference smaller versus larger increment sizes yield. Additionally, the effect of tier increment size on spending is more prominent for maximizers. From a theoretical perspective, this work contributes to our understanding of how individuals process multiple reference points within a single promotion and identifies that spending thresholds in price promotions may be treated as spending goals. From a managerial perspective, this work investigates the relationship between tiered discount design and consumer spending.
{"title":"Tiered discounts as multiple reference points for spending","authors":"Andong Cheng, Gretchen R. Ross","doi":"10.1002/jcpy.1339","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jcpy.1339","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Tiered discounts offer larger discounts as consumers meet higher spending thresholds (e.g., spend $100+, receive 10% off; spend $200+, receive 20% off). This research investigates how consumers treat these multiple dollar thresholds as reference points for spending. We find that tiered discounts with smaller increments between thresholds encourage higher spending compared to those with larger increments. This effect occurs because consumers treat thresholds as motivational spending goals when the distance to higher thresholds is smaller (vs. larger). Consistent with this reasoning, signaling goal progress (i.e., displaying cart amount while shopping) attenuates the spending difference smaller versus larger increment sizes yield. Additionally, the effect of tier increment size on spending is more prominent for maximizers. From a theoretical perspective, this work contributes to our understanding of how individuals process multiple reference points within a single promotion and identifies that spending thresholds in price promotions may be treated as spending goals. From a managerial perspective, this work investigates the relationship between tiered discount design and consumer spending.</p>","PeriodicalId":48365,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Consumer Psychology","volume":"33 2","pages":"424-431"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2022-12-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44518836","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"管理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Hurricanes, wildfires, pandemics, and other disasters have taken millions of lives in the past few years and caused substantial economic losses. To tackle these extraordinary circumstances, governments, organizations, and companies seek assistance from both humans and high-technology machines such as robots. This research report documents how highlighting robots' (vs. humans') helping behaviors in disaster response can affect consumers' prosociality, explores driving mechanisms, and tests solutions. Study 1 found that consumers donated fewer items of clothing after watching news highlighting robots' (vs. humans') assistance in a mudslide disaster. Featuring the COVID-19 pandemic, Study 2 further showed that this decrease in prosociality occurred because reading about robots' assistance felt less encouraging/inspiring to consumers. Studies 3A-3C (and a supplemental study) explored multiple mechanisms and identified a key driver for the backfire effect—a lower perception of courage in disaster response robots. Accordingly, Study 4 tested three theory-driven solutions to raise the perceived courage in robots to increase consumer prosociality.
{"title":"Robots or humans for disaster response? Impact on consumer prosociality and possible explanations","authors":"Fangyuan Chen, Szu-chi Huang","doi":"10.1002/jcpy.1338","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jcpy.1338","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Hurricanes, wildfires, pandemics, and other disasters have taken millions of lives in the past few years and caused substantial economic losses. To tackle these extraordinary circumstances, governments, organizations, and companies seek assistance from both humans and high-technology machines such as robots. This research report documents how highlighting robots' (vs. humans') helping behaviors in disaster response can affect consumers' prosociality, explores driving mechanisms, and tests solutions. Study 1 found that consumers donated fewer items of clothing after watching news highlighting robots' (vs. humans') assistance in a mudslide disaster. Featuring the COVID-19 pandemic, Study 2 further showed that this decrease in prosociality occurred because reading about robots' assistance felt less encouraging/inspiring to consumers. Studies 3A-3C (and a supplemental study) explored multiple mechanisms and identified a key driver for the backfire effect—a lower perception of courage in disaster response robots. Accordingly, Study 4 tested three theory-driven solutions to raise the perceived courage in robots to increase consumer prosociality.</p>","PeriodicalId":48365,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Consumer Psychology","volume":"33 2","pages":"432-440"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2022-12-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45013084","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"管理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Kumar and Epley (2023) review robust evidence for an intriguing hypothesis: That people fail to appreciate the benefits of everyday social behaviors and thus hesitate to connect with others in ways that would increase well-being. In this commentary, we discuss how consumer research can enrich theory and application in this emerging line of inquiry. We suggest (a) that the hedonic implications of undersociality can be integrated with reputational signaling insights to generate new questions about the wisdom and utility of social behavior, and (b) that undersociality has interesting implications for a consumption domain of particular interest to maximizing welfare: charitable giving.
{"title":"On the wisdom and utility of (under)sociality: A consumer psychology perspective","authors":"Ike Silver, Deborah A. Small","doi":"10.1002/jcpy.1333","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jcpy.1333","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Kumar and Epley (2023) review robust evidence for an intriguing hypothesis: That people fail to appreciate the benefits of everyday social behaviors and thus hesitate to connect with others in ways that would increase well-being. In this commentary, we discuss how consumer research can enrich theory and application in this emerging line of inquiry. We suggest (a) that the hedonic implications of undersociality can be integrated with reputational signaling insights to generate new questions about the wisdom and utility of social behavior, and (b) that undersociality has interesting implications for a consumption domain of particular interest to maximizing welfare: charitable giving.</p>","PeriodicalId":48365,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Consumer Psychology","volume":"33 1","pages":"217-220"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2022-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jcpy.1333","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42628308","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"管理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Encouraging people to donate their organs is not an easy endeavor. Globally, the percentage of organ donors is low. Prior research shows that one of the major impediments to organ donation is the perception that donating an organ is equivalent to giving up part of the self. Such a perception may be mitigated by physiological self-tracking though. Findings from three studies reveal that physiological self-tracking facilitates greater acceptance of and encourages organ donation. This happens because a focus on one's physiological data, as part of the self-tracking process, leads consumers to view their body as distinct from their self (i.e., self–body dualism). Consequently, people are less likely to equate donating organs with losing a part of themselves. The findings suggest that encouraging greater use of self-trackers may help people overcome their organ donation inhibitions.
{"title":"The facilitating effect of physiological self-tracking on organ donation","authors":"Chi Hoang, Sharon Ng","doi":"10.1002/jcpy.1337","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jcpy.1337","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Encouraging people to donate their organs is not an easy endeavor. Globally, the percentage of organ donors is low. Prior research shows that one of the major impediments to organ donation is the perception that donating an organ is equivalent to giving up part of the self. Such a perception may be mitigated by physiological self-tracking though. Findings from three studies reveal that physiological self-tracking facilitates greater acceptance of and encourages organ donation. This happens because a focus on one's physiological data, as part of the self-tracking process, leads consumers to view their body as distinct from their self (i.e., self–body dualism). Consequently, people are less likely to equate donating organs with losing a part of themselves. The findings suggest that encouraging greater use of self-trackers may help people overcome their organ donation inhibitions.</p>","PeriodicalId":48365,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Consumer Psychology","volume":"33 2","pages":"394-402"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2022-11-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45231158","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"管理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
<p>When people decide whether to perform a behavior, they typically base their decision on how the behavior will make them feel the extent to which a behavior will maximize their own utility. In addition, in instances in which the behavior may involve an interpersonal interaction, people will also base their decisions on how their behavior will be perceived by others. Unfortunately, people often make systematic errors in forecasting their own affect (Wilson & Gilbert, <span>2003</span>) and forecasting how others will think and feel (Epley & Eyal, <span>2019</span>; Epley & Waytz, <span>2010</span>).</p><p>In the target article for this Research Dialogue, Kumar and Epley (<span>2023</span>) explicate some unfortunate consequences of these misperceptions, in particular, how miscalibrations in perceptions of the affective outcomes of prosocial acts may result in missed opportunities to connect with others, a phenomenon they refer to as <i>undersociality</i>. In their review of their emerging program of research on undersociality, they document across numerous studies that people often have chances to engage positively with others through simple behaviors such as expressing gratitude and appreciation, giving compliments, and engaging in kind acts but are reluctant to do so because they systematically underestimate the positive effects these simple behaviors will have on recipients. Undersociality is unfortunate because, as Kumar and Epley also show in their research, engaging in even small prosocial acts makes both the giver and the recipient feel better, and perhaps even more unfortunate given that feelings of loneliness and lack of social connection have been steadily increasing in recent years and are at all-time highs (Shrum et al., <span>2023</span>).</p><p>In the first commentary on Kumar and Epley's (<span>2023</span>) target article, Ratner et al. (<span>2023</span>) approach the issue of the benefits (vs. costs) of prosociality from a different direction. While acknowledging the apparent benefits of small, low-cost acts of kindness and social connection with others for both givers and receivers, they raise the question of just how much prosociality is optimal for givers' wellbeing and what the appropriate (and optimal) mix of other-oriented and self-oriented behaviors might be, particularly when the other-oriented prosocial behaviors may have nontrivial costs (e.g., giving up much-needed “alone time” to spend time with others). In doing so, they discuss possible factors that may influence the extent to which a prosocial, other-oriented consumption behavior will enhance or diminish consumer wellbeing, and relatedly, factors that influence consumers' decisions regarding whether to embrace or forego a prosocial, other-oriented opportunity at the expense of a self-oriented one.</p><p>In the second commentary, Silver and Small (<span>2023</span>) discuss how consumer research can potentially enrich both theory and application of Kuma
当人们决定是否执行一项行为时,他们的决定通常基于该行为将如何使他们感受到该行为将在多大程度上最大化他们自己的效用。此外,在行为可能涉及人际互动的情况下,人们也会根据他人如何看待他们的行为来做出决定。不幸的是,人们经常在预测自己的影响时犯系统性错误(威尔逊&;Gilbert, 2003),并预测其他人的想法和感受(Epley &Eyal, 2019;爱普雷,Waytz, 2010)。在本研究对话的目标文章中,Kumar和Epley(2023)解释了这些误解的一些不幸后果,特别是对亲社会行为的情感结果的误解如何导致错失与他人联系的机会,他们将这种现象称为社会性不足。在他们对新兴的社会欠发达研究项目的回顾中,他们记录了大量的研究,表明人们经常有机会通过简单的行为与他人积极互动,比如表达感激和欣赏,给予赞美,以及参与善意的行为,但他们不愿意这样做,因为他们系统地低估了这些简单行为对接受者的积极影响。不合群是不幸的,因为,正如库马尔和埃普利在他们的研究中也表明的那样,即使是很小的亲社会行为,也会让给予者和接受者感觉更好,也许更不幸的是,近年来,孤独感和缺乏社会联系的感觉一直在稳步增加,达到历史最高水平(Shrum et al., 2023)。在对Kumar和Epley(2023)的目标文章的第一篇评论中,Ratner等人(2023)从不同的方向探讨了亲社会性的收益(相对于成本)问题。虽然承认小的、低成本的善意行为和与他人的社会联系对给予者和接受者都有明显的好处,但他们提出了一个问题,即多少亲社会行为对给予者的幸福是最优的,以及以他人为导向和以自我为导向的行为的适当(和最优)组合可能是什么,特别是当以他人为导向的亲社会行为可能有非微不足道的成本(例如,放弃迫切需要的“独处时间”来与他人共度时间)。在此过程中,他们讨论了可能影响亲社会、他者导向消费行为增加或减少消费者福祉程度的因素,以及相关的影响消费者决定是否接受或放弃亲社会、他者导向机会而牺牲自我导向机会的因素。在第二篇评论中,Silver和Small(2023)讨论了消费者研究如何能够潜在地丰富Kumar和Epley(2023)的下层社会性研究计划的理论和应用。在他们评论的第一部分中,他们深入探讨了为什么人们会放弃一个小的、低成本的亲社会机会,重点关注给予者的潜在成本。他们承认,库马尔和埃普利的研究清楚地表明,放弃一个容易的亲社会机会的决定,至少在一定程度上是由于给予者低估了亲社会行为对接受者的积极影响。然而,他们也指出,可能还有其他因素会抑制亲社会行为,特别是自我表现和声誉方面的考虑(例如,对某人的赞美可能会被其他人认为是不真诚和自私的;请求帮忙可能会显示你缺乏能力)。在他们评论的第二部分中,Silver和Small讨论了欠社会性对慈善捐赠消费领域的影响,并阐述了几个研究问题,即如何利用对导致预测错误的误解的理解来增加慈善捐赠的影响。最后,本研究对话以Kumar和Epley(2023)对两篇评论的回应结束,他们在评论中提到的观察和建议引发了额外的研究问题。他们确定了三个重叠的领域,这将是富有成效的研究途径:(1)在决定是否参与亲社会行为时给予者的意图,(2)可能导致社会性不足的预期印象,以及(3)人们对亲社会效应的预期与实际经验之间的错误校准的可能调节因素。总的来说,目标文章、评论和回应提供了一个丰富的研究领域,对消费者研究具有重要意义。
{"title":"Causes and consequences of missed opportunities for prosociality: Introduction to Research Dialogue","authors":"L. J. Shrum","doi":"10.1002/jcpy.1334","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jcpy.1334","url":null,"abstract":"<p>When people decide whether to perform a behavior, they typically base their decision on how the behavior will make them feel the extent to which a behavior will maximize their own utility. In addition, in instances in which the behavior may involve an interpersonal interaction, people will also base their decisions on how their behavior will be perceived by others. Unfortunately, people often make systematic errors in forecasting their own affect (Wilson & Gilbert, <span>2003</span>) and forecasting how others will think and feel (Epley & Eyal, <span>2019</span>; Epley & Waytz, <span>2010</span>).</p><p>In the target article for this Research Dialogue, Kumar and Epley (<span>2023</span>) explicate some unfortunate consequences of these misperceptions, in particular, how miscalibrations in perceptions of the affective outcomes of prosocial acts may result in missed opportunities to connect with others, a phenomenon they refer to as <i>undersociality</i>. In their review of their emerging program of research on undersociality, they document across numerous studies that people often have chances to engage positively with others through simple behaviors such as expressing gratitude and appreciation, giving compliments, and engaging in kind acts but are reluctant to do so because they systematically underestimate the positive effects these simple behaviors will have on recipients. Undersociality is unfortunate because, as Kumar and Epley also show in their research, engaging in even small prosocial acts makes both the giver and the recipient feel better, and perhaps even more unfortunate given that feelings of loneliness and lack of social connection have been steadily increasing in recent years and are at all-time highs (Shrum et al., <span>2023</span>).</p><p>In the first commentary on Kumar and Epley's (<span>2023</span>) target article, Ratner et al. (<span>2023</span>) approach the issue of the benefits (vs. costs) of prosociality from a different direction. While acknowledging the apparent benefits of small, low-cost acts of kindness and social connection with others for both givers and receivers, they raise the question of just how much prosociality is optimal for givers' wellbeing and what the appropriate (and optimal) mix of other-oriented and self-oriented behaviors might be, particularly when the other-oriented prosocial behaviors may have nontrivial costs (e.g., giving up much-needed “alone time” to spend time with others). In doing so, they discuss possible factors that may influence the extent to which a prosocial, other-oriented consumption behavior will enhance or diminish consumer wellbeing, and relatedly, factors that influence consumers' decisions regarding whether to embrace or forego a prosocial, other-oriented opportunity at the expense of a self-oriented one.</p><p>In the second commentary, Silver and Small (<span>2023</span>) discuss how consumer research can potentially enrich both theory and application of Kuma","PeriodicalId":48365,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Consumer Psychology","volume":"33 1","pages":"197-198"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2022-11-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jcpy.1334","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42448040","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"管理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Ratner et al. (2023) and Silver and Small (2023) raise deeply interesting questions about the broad consequences of increasing sociality, about people's ability to anticipate the outcomes of sociality, and about broader issues people may be considering when thinking of connecting with another person. We focus this response on the potential role of intentions and anticipated impressions in affecting undersociality, as well as the possibility of interactions that could moderate the gap between people's expectations of social engagement and their actual experiences. Many unanswered and important questions remain in need of critical empirical attention. We encourage future research that provides a better understanding of undersociality by focusing on intentions, impressions, and interactions.
Ratner et al.(2023)和Silver and Small(2023)提出了一些非常有趣的问题,这些问题涉及社会性增加的广泛后果,人们预测社会性结果的能力,以及人们在考虑与他人联系时可能考虑的更广泛的问题。我们将这一反应集中在意图和预期印象在影响欠社会性方面的潜在作用,以及可以缓和人们对社会参与的期望与实际经验之间差距的相互作用的可能性。许多悬而未决的重要问题仍然需要批判性的经验关注。我们鼓励未来的研究通过关注意图、印象和互动来更好地理解社会性不足。
{"title":"Understanding undersociality: Intentions, impressions, and interactions","authors":"Amit Kumar, Nicholas Epley","doi":"10.1002/jcpy.1332","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jcpy.1332","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Ratner et al. (2023) and Silver and Small (2023) raise deeply interesting questions about the broad consequences of increasing sociality, about people's ability to anticipate the outcomes of sociality, and about broader issues people may be considering when thinking of connecting with another person. We focus this response on the potential role of intentions and anticipated impressions in affecting undersociality, as well as the possibility of interactions that could moderate the gap between people's expectations of social engagement and their actual experiences. Many unanswered and important questions remain in need of critical empirical attention. We encourage future research that provides a better understanding of undersociality by focusing on intentions, impressions, and interactions.</p>","PeriodicalId":48365,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Consumer Psychology","volume":"33 1","pages":"221-225"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2022-11-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jcpy.1332","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45630328","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"管理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Rebecca K. Ratner, Nicole You Jeung Kim, Yuechen Wu
Kumar and Epley (2023) argue that people underinvest in spending time, effort, and money on other people, and that consumers' own well-being would improve from increased “sociality.” We pose two questions to enhance understanding of the relationship between sociality and efforts to benefit one's own well-being: (1) when will other-oriented consumption promote versus hinder consumers' own well-being, and (2) what leads consumers to embrace versus forego efforts to improve their well-being (i.e., self-care) that does not involve sociality? We propose that the degree to which the consumer is concerned about incorporating others' preferences, the magnitude of resources involved, and the temporal dynamics of consumption will be relevant factors in addressing these two questions. Future research to explore the proposed three factors and other factors will be important for consumers who seek to improve their well-being as well as marketers who seek to promote it.
{"title":"When is sociality congruent with self-care?","authors":"Rebecca K. Ratner, Nicole You Jeung Kim, Yuechen Wu","doi":"10.1002/jcpy.1335","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jcpy.1335","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Kumar and Epley (2023) argue that people underinvest in spending time, effort, and money on other people, and that consumers' own well-being would improve from increased “sociality.” We pose two questions to enhance understanding of the relationship between sociality and efforts to benefit one's own well-being: (1) when will other-oriented consumption promote versus hinder consumers' own well-being, and (2) what leads consumers to embrace versus forego efforts to improve their well-being (i.e., self-care) that does not involve sociality? We propose that the degree to which the consumer is concerned about incorporating others' preferences, the magnitude of resources involved, and the temporal dynamics of consumption will be relevant factors in addressing these two questions. Future research to explore the proposed three factors and other factors will be important for consumers who seek to improve their well-being as well as marketers who seek to promote it.</p>","PeriodicalId":48365,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Consumer Psychology","volume":"33 1","pages":"213-216"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2022-11-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jcpy.1335","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41369496","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"管理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}