首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Consumer Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
An integrative theory of resource discrepancies 资源差异综合理论
IF 4.8 2区 管理学 Q2 BUSINESS Pub Date : 2024-01-07 DOI: 10.1002/jcpy.1406
Christopher Cannon, Kelly Goldsmith, Caroline Roux
A great deal of work in consumer psychology has been devoted to understanding how individuals manage resource discrepancies. This includes tangible resources – such as money, food, and products – as well as intangible resources – such as time, skills, and social relationships. Resource discrepancies can either be positive – as in the case of having substantial wealth – or negative – as in the case of poverty. Several constructs across the behavioral sciences have been introduced to describe how consumers perceive their various resource discrepancies including, but not limited to, power, social status, scarcity, inequality, and social class. However, little guidance is provided to understand when and why these resource-based constructs can produce both overlapping and opposing consequences. This conceptual article provides a resolution to this issue by introducing an integrative theory that situates these constructs within the same unifying framework based on two fundamental dimensions: high (vs. low) personal control and self- (vs. other-) dependence. Based on this framework, we offer eight testable propositions and develop a research agenda for academics interested in studying resource discrepancies.
消费者心理学的大量研究致力于了解个人如何管理资源差异。这包括有形资源(如金钱、食物和产品)和无形资源(如时间、技能和社会关系)。资源差异既可以是积极的--如拥有大量财富,也可以是消极的--如贫困。行为科学中引入了多个概念来描述消费者如何看待他们的各种资源差异,包括但不限于权力、社会地位、稀缺性、不平等和社会阶层。然而,在理解这些基于资源的概念何时以及为何会产生重叠和对立的后果方面,几乎没有提供任何指导。这篇概念性文章通过引入一种综合理论来解决这一问题,该理论将这些概念置于同一个统一的框架中,并以两个基本维度为基础:高(与低)个人控制力和自我(与他人)依赖性。基于这一框架,我们提出了八个可检验的命题,并为有兴趣研究资源差异的学者制定了研究议程。
{"title":"An integrative theory of resource discrepancies","authors":"Christopher Cannon, Kelly Goldsmith, Caroline Roux","doi":"10.1002/jcpy.1406","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/jcpy.1406","url":null,"abstract":"A great deal of work in consumer psychology has been devoted to understanding how individuals manage resource discrepancies. This includes tangible resources – such as money, food, and products – as well as intangible resources – such as time, skills, and social relationships. Resource discrepancies can either be positive – as in the case of having substantial wealth – or negative – as in the case of poverty. Several constructs across the behavioral sciences have been introduced to describe how consumers perceive their various resource discrepancies including, but not limited to, power, social status, scarcity, inequality, and social class. However, little guidance is provided to understand when and why these resource-based constructs can produce both overlapping and opposing consequences. This conceptual article provides a resolution to this issue by introducing an integrative theory that situates these constructs within the same unifying framework based on two fundamental dimensions: high (vs. low) personal control and self- (vs. other-) dependence. Based on this framework, we offer eight testable propositions and develop a research agenda for academics interested in studying resource discrepancies.","PeriodicalId":48365,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Consumer Psychology","volume":"155 3","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-01-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139413403","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"管理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The over-diversification effect: Enhancement in perceived heterogeneity of multiple (vs. single)-others' preferences 过度多样化效应:增强多重(与单一)他人偏好的感知异质性
IF 4.8 2区 管理学 Q2 BUSINESS Pub Date : 2024-01-07 DOI: 10.1002/jcpy.1403
Ritesh Saini, Dian Wang, Haipeng (Allan) Chen
Consumers often make decisions for multiple-others when they do not know each one's true preferences. Under such circumstances, we demonstrate perceptions of enhanced preference heterogeneity for multiple-others, relative to the perceived preference of single-others. As a result, choices for multiple-others are more diversified than those for single-others. We attribute this effect to the perceived uniqueness of others in the multiple-other setting. Notably, when others are perceived as particularly unique, the inclination toward over-diversification intensifies. However, this diversification often leads to choices that do not align with the true preferences of the individuals, causing potential mismatches in demand and supply. We conclude with a discussion on the implications of these findings for managers and suggest avenues for future research.
消费者在不了解每个人的真实偏好时,往往会为多个他人做出决策。在这种情况下,相对于单个他人的感知偏好,我们会发现对多个他人的感知偏好异质性更强。因此,与单个他人相比,多个他人的选择更加多样化。我们将这种效应归因于在多人环境中感知到的他人的独特性。值得注意的是,当他人被认为特别独特时,过度多样化的倾向就会加强。然而,这种多样化往往会导致与个人真实偏好不一致的选择,从而造成潜在的供需错配。最后,我们讨论了这些发现对管理者的影响,并提出了未来的研究方向。
{"title":"The over-diversification effect: Enhancement in perceived heterogeneity of multiple (vs. single)-others' preferences","authors":"Ritesh Saini, Dian Wang, Haipeng (Allan) Chen","doi":"10.1002/jcpy.1403","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/jcpy.1403","url":null,"abstract":"Consumers often make decisions for multiple-others when they do not know each one's true preferences. Under such circumstances, we demonstrate perceptions of enhanced preference heterogeneity for multiple-others, relative to the perceived preference of single-others. As a result, choices for multiple-others are more diversified than those for single-others. We attribute this effect to the perceived uniqueness of others in the multiple-other setting. Notably, when others are perceived as particularly unique, the inclination toward over-diversification intensifies. However, this diversification often leads to choices that do not align with the true preferences of the individuals, causing potential mismatches in demand and supply. We conclude with a discussion on the implications of these findings for managers and suggest avenues for future research.","PeriodicalId":48365,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Consumer Psychology","volume":"88 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-01-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139413462","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"管理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
JCP: The next mile JCP:下一英里
IF 4.8 2区 管理学 Q2 BUSINESS Pub Date : 2024-01-04 DOI: 10.1002/jcpy.1402
David B. Wooten, Rajesh Bagchi, Aparna Labroo
<p>We are honored to serve as Editors of the <i>Journal of Consumer Psychology</i> (JCP), accepting the baton from Lauren Block, Jennifer Argo, and Tom Kramer and continuing down the path of excellence that they and their predecessors have forged for the journal. Prior editors sought to define the journal's scope (e.g., Block et al., <span>2020</span>; Wyer & Shavitt, <span>2002</span>), improve its operational efficiency (e.g., Maheswaran, <span>2006</span>; Wyer & Shavitt, <span>2003</span>), and introduce impact initiatives (e.g., Maheswaran, <span>2006</span>), including alternative formats that support diverse research contributions (Mukhopadhyay et al., <span>2018</span>). Their efforts, as well as those of prior editors, associate editors, reviewers, and authors, have helped make JCP the journal that it is today – a premier outlet for research that advances knowledge of consumer psychology.</p><p>In our initial editorial, we share our thoughts about where the journal is now and the direction we plan to follow, acknowledging the need for us to address other important issues in future editorials. Because this initial editorial builds on the ideas and efforts of the incredible scholars who preceded us as editors, or describes distinctive features of the journal that we plan to continue, we borrow words and phrases from past editorials or the journal website, believing there are limited ways to describe similar perspectives and practices.</p><p>We embrace the notion articulated most recently by our immediate predecessors that consumer psychology involves an understanding of the science underlying consumer behavior (Block et al., <span>2020</span>). It encapsulates understanding consumers' thoughts, feelings, or behaviors as they interact with products, services, or ideas in the marketplace and beyond (Hoyer & MacInnis, <span>2007</span>; Peter & Olson, <span>2017</span>; Schiffman & Kanuk, <span>2000</span>). We share previous editors' inclusive perspective of consumer psychology and echo their sentiments that while consumer psychological research needs to provide insights about consumers and consumption, it does not have to be constrained to the marketplace alone or be limited in terms of its relevance to a narrow set of stakeholders. While consumers make important decisions in retail settings, not all consumer decisions or consumption activities take place in stores. For example, how consumers process information could be of importance even if it does not directly translate into an observable outcome. In other contexts, the decision may occur at home, but could have downstream marketplace consequences. Ultimately, as long as the research provides insights about the psychology of consumption, whether or not these insights inform managerial practice, consumer welfare, or public policy, we believe it may be appropriate for JCP.</p><p>Consistent with the journal's focus on consumers and the psychology of consumption, issues t
第 2 阶段是指研究的 "实践",研究人员在这一阶段评估实证调查如何使研究人员做出所寻求的推论。最后,在第 3 阶段,有效性指的是 "根据每个研究领域的要素、要素之间的关系和背景,努力建立实证研究结果的可信度/可推广性/稳健性"。然而,研究的重要性只能结合其目标来评估--目标是建立理论还是理解现象,使用的方法是演绎法还是非演绎法?我们赞同 Lynch 等人(2012 年)的观点,即信念可以在多个领域得到更新,其中包括"(a)建构与建构之间的联系;(b)建构与可观察之间的联系;(c)可观察与可观察之间的联系;以及(d)认为世界上'存在'某种现象的信念"。虽然用假设-演绎的方法记录或解释建构与建构之间的联系是一项重要贡献,但这并不是改变信念的唯一途径。信念也可以通过其他方式改变。因此,使用相同的标准来评价具有不同目标和方法的研究,并不能很好地帮助我们保持高标准的卓越性。最后,引用一句话来说明好的伙伴是如何提升旅程的,我们很幸运能与接受我们邀请加入我们团队的优秀学者们相伴,共同接过 JCP 下一英里旅程的接力棒。除了副主编和编审委员会成员欣然同意与我们并肩作战外,我们还有幸邀请到了 JCP 的执行主编 Sandy Osaki 继续担任期刊的主编。最终,《JCP》能否继续取得成功,将取决于更广泛的学者群体能否继续提交有趣而严谨的文章,推动消费心理学科学的发展。
{"title":"JCP: The next mile","authors":"David B. Wooten,&nbsp;Rajesh Bagchi,&nbsp;Aparna Labroo","doi":"10.1002/jcpy.1402","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/jcpy.1402","url":null,"abstract":"&lt;p&gt;We are honored to serve as Editors of the &lt;i&gt;Journal of Consumer Psychology&lt;/i&gt; (JCP), accepting the baton from Lauren Block, Jennifer Argo, and Tom Kramer and continuing down the path of excellence that they and their predecessors have forged for the journal. Prior editors sought to define the journal's scope (e.g., Block et al., &lt;span&gt;2020&lt;/span&gt;; Wyer &amp; Shavitt, &lt;span&gt;2002&lt;/span&gt;), improve its operational efficiency (e.g., Maheswaran, &lt;span&gt;2006&lt;/span&gt;; Wyer &amp; Shavitt, &lt;span&gt;2003&lt;/span&gt;), and introduce impact initiatives (e.g., Maheswaran, &lt;span&gt;2006&lt;/span&gt;), including alternative formats that support diverse research contributions (Mukhopadhyay et al., &lt;span&gt;2018&lt;/span&gt;). Their efforts, as well as those of prior editors, associate editors, reviewers, and authors, have helped make JCP the journal that it is today – a premier outlet for research that advances knowledge of consumer psychology.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;In our initial editorial, we share our thoughts about where the journal is now and the direction we plan to follow, acknowledging the need for us to address other important issues in future editorials. Because this initial editorial builds on the ideas and efforts of the incredible scholars who preceded us as editors, or describes distinctive features of the journal that we plan to continue, we borrow words and phrases from past editorials or the journal website, believing there are limited ways to describe similar perspectives and practices.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;We embrace the notion articulated most recently by our immediate predecessors that consumer psychology involves an understanding of the science underlying consumer behavior (Block et al., &lt;span&gt;2020&lt;/span&gt;). It encapsulates understanding consumers' thoughts, feelings, or behaviors as they interact with products, services, or ideas in the marketplace and beyond (Hoyer &amp; MacInnis, &lt;span&gt;2007&lt;/span&gt;; Peter &amp; Olson, &lt;span&gt;2017&lt;/span&gt;; Schiffman &amp; Kanuk, &lt;span&gt;2000&lt;/span&gt;). We share previous editors' inclusive perspective of consumer psychology and echo their sentiments that while consumer psychological research needs to provide insights about consumers and consumption, it does not have to be constrained to the marketplace alone or be limited in terms of its relevance to a narrow set of stakeholders. While consumers make important decisions in retail settings, not all consumer decisions or consumption activities take place in stores. For example, how consumers process information could be of importance even if it does not directly translate into an observable outcome. In other contexts, the decision may occur at home, but could have downstream marketplace consequences. Ultimately, as long as the research provides insights about the psychology of consumption, whether or not these insights inform managerial practice, consumer welfare, or public policy, we believe it may be appropriate for JCP.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;Consistent with the journal's focus on consumers and the psychology of consumption, issues t","PeriodicalId":48365,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Consumer Psychology","volume":"34 1","pages":"3-5"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-01-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jcpy.1402","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139109953","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"管理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How rejected recommendations shape recommenders’ future product intentions 被拒绝的推荐如何影响推荐人未来的产品意向
IF 4.8 2区 管理学 Q2 BUSINESS Pub Date : 2023-12-02 DOI: 10.1002/jcpy.1401
Matthew J. Hall, Jamie D. Hyodo, Kirk Kristofferson
When a consumer (a recommender) recommends a product to another consumer (a recommendee), it is not uncommon to learn whether the recommendee chose the recommended option (i.e., accepted the recommendation) or a different option (i.e., rejected the recommendation). Our research examines how rejected recommendations affect recommenders’ subsequent intentions toward the originally recommended product. We find that upon learning one's recommendation was rejected, recommenders are less likely to repurchase or choose the product in the future. This negative effect emerges because recommenders question their knowledge about the recommended product (i.e., self-perceived expertise is reduced). Such questioning is more likely to occur when the recommendee is a close other and less likely to occur when the recommended product is perceived to primarily differ from alternatives due to subjective preferences (i.e., horizontal differentiation is salient). Importantly, this rejected recommendation effect is shown to be distinct from a social proof account. The current research contributes to WOM theory by identifying a novel outcome of recommendation interactions—rejected recommendations—and by demonstrating that this outcome can cause consumers to shift away from a product despite having felt positively enough about the product to recommend it to others.
当一个消费者(推荐人)向另一个消费者(推荐人)推荐一个产品时,了解推荐人是选择了被推荐的选项(即接受推荐)还是选择了不同的选项(即拒绝推荐)是很常见的。我们的研究考察了被拒绝的推荐如何影响推荐人对最初推荐产品的后续意向。我们发现,在得知自己的推荐被拒绝后,推荐人在未来不太可能再次购买或选择该产品。这种负面影响的出现是因为推荐者质疑他们对被推荐产品的了解(即,自我认知的专业知识减少了)。当被推荐的产品与其他产品接近时,这种质疑更有可能发生,而当被推荐的产品由于主观偏好而被认为与替代产品主要不同时(即,横向差异是显著的),这种质疑就不太可能发生。重要的是,这种拒绝推荐效应被证明与社会证明账户不同。目前的研究通过确定推荐互动的一个新结果——拒绝推荐——并通过证明这个结果可能导致消费者离开产品,尽管他们对产品有足够的好感,可以推荐给其他人,从而为口碑理论做出了贡献。
{"title":"How rejected recommendations shape recommenders’ future product intentions","authors":"Matthew J. Hall, Jamie D. Hyodo, Kirk Kristofferson","doi":"10.1002/jcpy.1401","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/jcpy.1401","url":null,"abstract":"When a consumer (a recommender) recommends a product to another consumer (a recommendee), it is not uncommon to learn whether the recommendee chose the recommended option (i.e., accepted the recommendation) or a different option (i.e., rejected the recommendation). Our research examines how rejected recommendations affect recommenders’ subsequent intentions toward the originally recommended product. We find that upon learning one's recommendation was rejected, recommenders are less likely to repurchase or choose the product in the future. This negative effect emerges because recommenders question their knowledge about the recommended product (i.e., self-perceived expertise is reduced). Such questioning is more likely to occur when the recommendee is a close other and less likely to occur when the recommended product is perceived to primarily differ from alternatives due to subjective preferences (i.e., horizontal differentiation is salient). Importantly, this rejected recommendation effect is shown to be distinct from a social proof account. The current research contributes to WOM theory by identifying a novel outcome of recommendation interactions—rejected recommendations—and by demonstrating that this outcome can cause consumers to shift away from a product despite having felt positively enough about the product to recommend it to others.","PeriodicalId":48365,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Consumer Psychology","volume":"47 1-2","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2023-12-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138525516","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"管理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Trying too hard or not hard enough: How effort shapes status 太过努力或不够努力:努力如何塑造地位
IF 4 2区 管理学 Q2 BUSINESS Pub Date : 2023-11-27 DOI: 10.1002/jcpy.1400
Nathan B. Warren, Caleb Warren

Is trying to earn status effective or self-defeating? We show that whether effort increases or decreases admiration and respect (i.e., status) depends on how the person is trying to earn status. Groups evaluate people along multiple status dimensions (e.g., wealth, coolness). Each dimension is associated with a different ideology, or set of beliefs, that ascribe status to behaviors that contribute to the group's goals. Whether behaviors, including effort, increase status, thus, depends on the ideologies that people use to interpret if a behavior contributes to the group. Four experiments demonstrate that people earn more status when they try to become wealthy compared to when they are effortlessly wealthy, but earn less status when they try to become cool compared to when they are effortlessly cool. Effort increases status when directed at wealth but not at coolness because contemporary ideologies suggest that people who gain wealth through effort contribute more to society, whereas people who gain coolness through effort contribute less.

努力争取地位是有效的还是弄巧成拙的?我们表明,努力是增加还是减少钦佩和尊重(即地位)取决于这个人如何努力获得地位。群体根据多个地位维度(例如,财富,酷)来评估一个人。每个维度都与不同的意识形态或信仰相关联,这些意识形态或信仰将地位归因于有助于实现群体目标的行为。因此,包括努力在内的行为是否会提高地位,取决于人们用来解释一种行为是否对群体有贡献的意识形态。四项实验表明,当人们试图变得富有时,他们获得的地位比他们毫不费力地富有时要高,但当他们试图变得酷时,他们获得的地位比他们毫不费力地酷时要低。在财富方面,努力会增加地位,但在冷静方面却不会,因为当代的意识形态表明,通过努力获得财富的人对社会的贡献更多,而通过努力获得冷静的人对社会的贡献更少。
{"title":"Trying too hard or not hard enough: How effort shapes status","authors":"Nathan B. Warren,&nbsp;Caleb Warren","doi":"10.1002/jcpy.1400","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jcpy.1400","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Is trying to earn status effective or self-defeating? We show that whether effort increases or decreases admiration and respect (i.e., status) depends on how the person is trying to earn status. Groups evaluate people along multiple status dimensions (e.g., wealth, coolness). Each dimension is associated with a different ideology, or set of beliefs, that ascribe status to behaviors that contribute to the group's goals. Whether behaviors, including effort, increase status, thus, depends on the ideologies that people use to interpret if a behavior contributes to the group. Four experiments demonstrate that people earn more status when they try to become wealthy compared to when they are effortlessly wealthy, but earn less status when they try to become cool compared to when they are effortlessly cool. Effort increases status when directed at wealth but not at coolness because contemporary ideologies suggest that people who gain wealth through effort contribute more to society, whereas people who gain coolness through effort contribute less.</p>","PeriodicalId":48365,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Consumer Psychology","volume":"34 4","pages":"660-669"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jcpy.1400","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138513170","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"管理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reconsidering the path for neural and physiological methods in consumer psychology 重新思考消费心理学中神经与生理方法的路径
IF 4.8 2区 管理学 Q2 BUSINESS Pub Date : 2023-11-21 DOI: 10.1002/jcpy.1397
John A. Clithero, Uma R. Karmarkar, Gideon Nave, Hilke Plassmann

Recent decades have witnessed a burst of neuroscience research investigating mental and physiological processes central to consumer behavior, including sensory perception, memory, and decision making. Nonetheless, few publications that include neural and physiological measures, or develop conceptual frameworks around neuroscience principles, have been published in consumer psychology. It is clear that “consumer neuroscience” has thus far not lived up to its promises in the marketing literature. We suggest three main reasons for this. First, neural and other biological markers are often mistaken to be identical to the overlaying psychological constructs in traditional consumer psychology work. Second, somewhat surprisingly, there has been an overly narrow utilization of neural data. Most previous work focused on linking existing behavioral phenomena or psychological constructs central to consumer research to neural correlates using brain imaging techniques while ignoring other methods. We argue that much can be gained from improved integration of physiological measures and through them, different levels of analysis. Third, there remain significant structural hurdles to the broad adoption of neural and physiological measures for consumer researchers. We outline how addressing these three components can translate to a more holistic understanding of the consumer via both broader and deeper consumer insights.

最近几十年见证了神经科学研究的爆发,研究消费者行为的核心心理和生理过程,包括感官知觉、记忆和决策。然而,在消费者心理学中,很少有包括神经和生理测量,或围绕神经科学原理开发概念框架的出版物发表。很明显,到目前为止,“消费者神经科学”并没有兑现其在营销文献中的承诺。我们提出了三个主要原因。首先,在传统的消费者心理学工作中,神经和其他生物标记常常被误认为与重叠的心理构念相同。其次,有些令人惊讶的是,神经数据的使用过于狭隘。大多数先前的工作都集中在使用脑成像技术将消费者研究的核心行为现象或心理结构与神经相关联系起来,而忽略了其他方法。我们认为,许多可以从改进的生理措施的整合,并通过他们,不同层次的分析。第三,对于消费者研究人员来说,广泛采用神经和生理测量仍然存在重大的结构性障碍。我们概述了如何通过更广泛和更深入的消费者洞察来解决这三个组成部分,从而更全面地了解消费者。
{"title":"Reconsidering the path for neural and physiological methods in consumer psychology","authors":"John A. Clithero,&nbsp;Uma R. Karmarkar,&nbsp;Gideon Nave,&nbsp;Hilke Plassmann","doi":"10.1002/jcpy.1397","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jcpy.1397","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Recent decades have witnessed a burst of neuroscience research investigating mental and physiological processes central to consumer behavior, including sensory perception, memory, and decision making. Nonetheless, few publications that include neural and physiological measures, or develop conceptual frameworks around neuroscience principles, have been published in consumer psychology. It is clear that “consumer neuroscience” has thus far not lived up to its promises in the marketing literature. We suggest three main reasons for this. First, neural and other biological markers are often mistaken to be identical to the overlaying psychological constructs in traditional consumer psychology work. Second, somewhat surprisingly, there has been an overly narrow utilization of neural data. Most previous work focused on linking existing behavioral phenomena or psychological constructs central to consumer research to neural correlates using brain imaging techniques while ignoring other methods. We argue that much can be gained from improved integration of physiological measures and through them, different levels of analysis. Third, there remain significant structural hurdles to the broad adoption of neural and physiological measures for consumer researchers. We outline how addressing these three components can translate to a more holistic understanding of the consumer via both broader and deeper consumer insights.</p>","PeriodicalId":48365,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Consumer Psychology","volume":"34 1","pages":"196-213"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2023-11-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jcpy.1397","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138513196","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"管理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The moral superiority of temporal (vs. social) comparisons 时间(与社会)比较的道德优越性
IF 4 2区 管理学 Q2 BUSINESS Pub Date : 2023-11-13 DOI: 10.1002/jcpy.1399
Sokiente W. Dagogo-Jack

Brands often encourage consumers to compare themselves to two types of standards: other people (i.e., social comparisons) and their own past (i.e., temporal comparisons). Although research has drawn many parallels between these two self-comparisons, relatively little work has examined how they diverge. Moreover, existing research on their differences focuses on individuals engaging in—rather than brands encouraging—different self-comparisons. The present research identifies moral perceptions as one critical dimension on which brand-elicited temporal and social comparisons differ. Four studies find that evoking downward social (vs. temporal) comparisons undermines brand morality perceptions and, consequently, brand evaluations and choice. Providing preliminary insight into the mechanism, when brands evoke downward social (vs. temporal) comparisons, consumers perceive them as promoting status-seeking behavior, which mediates morality judgments. Furthermore, the effects of comparison type are eliminated among consumers with stronger status motives—those who are less prone to condemn status-seeking behavior. Altogether, these findings reveal a lay belief in the moral superiority of downward temporal (vs. social) comparisons and the downstream consequences for brands that elicit such comparisons.

品牌经常鼓励消费者将自己与两类标准进行比较:他人(即社会比较)和自己的过去(即时间比较)。尽管研究已经得出了这两种自我比较之间的许多相似之处,但相对而言,很少有研究对它们之间的差异进行探讨。此外,关于这两种自我比较的差异的现有研究主要集中在个人参与--而不是品牌鼓励--不同的自我比较。目前的研究发现,道德观念是品牌引发的时间比较和社会比较产生差异的一个关键维度。四项研究发现,唤起向下的社会(与时间)比较会削弱品牌的道德认知,进而影响对品牌的评价和选择。对这一机制的初步洞察表明,当品牌唤起向下的社会(与时间)比较时,消费者认为它们会促进追求地位的行为,而这种行为会对道德判断起到中介作用。此外,在地位动机较强的消费者中,比较类型的影响被消除了--这些消费者不容易谴责追求地位的行为。总之,这些发现揭示了一种非专业的信念,即向下的时间(与社会)比较在道德上具有优越性,以及引起这种比较的品牌的下游后果。
{"title":"The moral superiority of temporal (vs. social) comparisons","authors":"Sokiente W. Dagogo-Jack","doi":"10.1002/jcpy.1399","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jcpy.1399","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Brands often encourage consumers to compare themselves to two types of standards: other people (i.e., social comparisons) and their own past (i.e., temporal comparisons). Although research has drawn many parallels between these two self-comparisons, relatively little work has examined how they diverge. Moreover, existing research on their differences focuses on individuals engaging in—rather than brands encouraging—different self-comparisons. The present research identifies moral perceptions as one critical dimension on which brand-elicited temporal and social comparisons differ. Four studies find that evoking downward social (vs. temporal) comparisons undermines brand morality perceptions and, consequently, brand evaluations and choice. Providing preliminary insight into the mechanism, when brands evoke downward social (vs. temporal) comparisons, consumers perceive them as promoting status-seeking behavior, which mediates morality judgments. Furthermore, the effects of comparison type are eliminated among consumers with stronger status motives—those who are less prone to condemn status-seeking behavior. Altogether, these findings reveal a lay belief in the moral superiority of downward temporal (vs. social) comparisons and the downstream consequences for brands that elicit such comparisons.</p>","PeriodicalId":48365,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Consumer Psychology","volume":"34 4","pages":"650-659"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jcpy.1399","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136351860","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"管理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Snyre for your nasal congestion: Using phonesthemes to imbue non-word brand names with meaning 治疗鼻塞的 Snyre使用音素赋予非文字品牌名称意义
IF 4 2区 管理学 Q2 BUSINESS Pub Date : 2023-11-10 DOI: 10.1002/jcpy.1396
Stacey Brennan, Jasmina Ilicic, Shai Danziger

A brand name is a fundamental component of a brand's identity. This research introduces a novel linguistic tool for brand name creation: phonesthemes—sound and spelling letter clusters that are associated with one dominant meaning. For instance, sn, one of over 140 phonesthemes in English, consistently appears in words related to the nose or breathing (sneeze, sniff, snort). Six experiments reveal positive effects of phonesthemic non-word brand names (e.g., Glif; gl-; e.g., glow, glimmer; meaning “light”) on consumer preference, attitude, purchase intent, and choice when the dominant meaning activated by the phonestheme is semantically congruent with the product category or product attribute (e.g., luminant car wax), due to enhanced processing fluency. Phonological (sound) and orthographic (spelling) priming are eliminated as alternative explanations for the phenomenon. This research advances psycholinguistic research in marketing and the emerging area of brand linguistics by broadening the focus beyond brand name phonology.

品牌名称是品牌识别的基本组成部分。这项研究为品牌名称的创建引入了一种新颖的语言工具:音素--与一个主要含义相关联的音和拼写字母群。例如,"sn "是英语中 140 多个音素之一,经常出现在与鼻子或呼吸有关的单词中(sneeze、sniff、snort)。六项实验显示,当音素激活的主导含义与产品类别或产品属性(如发光汽车蜡)在语义上一致时,音素非词品牌名称(如 Glif;gl-;如 glow、glimmer;意为 "光")会对消费者的偏好、态度、购买意向和选择产生积极影响,这是由于加工流畅性增强所致。语音(声音)和正字法(拼写)引物被排除在对这一现象的其他解释之外。这项研究将重点从品牌名称语音学扩展到了市场营销中的心理语言学研究和新兴的品牌语言学领域。
{"title":"Snyre for your nasal congestion: Using phonesthemes to imbue non-word brand names with meaning","authors":"Stacey Brennan,&nbsp;Jasmina Ilicic,&nbsp;Shai Danziger","doi":"10.1002/jcpy.1396","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jcpy.1396","url":null,"abstract":"<p>A brand name is a fundamental component of a brand's identity. This research introduces a novel linguistic tool for brand name creation: phonesthemes—sound and spelling letter clusters that are associated with one dominant meaning. For instance, <i>sn</i>, one of over 140 phonesthemes in English, consistently appears in words related to the nose or breathing (<i>sneeze</i>, <i>sniff</i>, <i>snort</i>). Six experiments reveal positive effects of phonesthemic non-word brand names (e.g., <i>Gl</i>if; <i>gl</i>-; e.g., <i>glow</i>, <i>glimmer</i>; meaning “light”) on consumer preference, attitude, purchase intent, and choice when the dominant meaning activated by the phonestheme is semantically congruent with the product category or product attribute (e.g., luminant car wax), due to enhanced processing fluency. Phonological (sound) and orthographic (spelling) priming are eliminated as alternative explanations for the phenomenon. This research advances psycholinguistic research in marketing and the emerging area of brand linguistics by broadening the focus beyond brand name phonology.</p>","PeriodicalId":48365,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Consumer Psychology","volume":"34 4","pages":"601-619"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/jcpy.1396","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135186212","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"管理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Commentaries on “Reconsidering the path for neural and physiological methods in consumer psychology” 关于 "重新考虑神经和生理方法在消费者心理学中的应用路径 "的评论
IF 4.8 2区 管理学 Q2 BUSINESS Pub Date : 2023-11-09 DOI: 10.1002/jcpy.1398
J. Wesley Hutchinson, Martin Reimann, Brian Knutson, Joel Huber
<p>The initial version of the article by Clithero, Karmarkar, Nave, and Plassmann (Journal of Consumer Psychology, 2024) was critiqued by open comments from a small group of scholars. Their suggestions encouraged the authors to clarify challenging relationships between brain processes and emotions, beliefs, and actions. The revision expanded fMRI and EEG to include measures of vision, facial expression, breathing, heart rhythms, and blood chemistry. The paper provides multiple avenues of joint work between neurological and psychological scholars. The comments below reflect different reactions to the final article. Wes Hutchinson acknowledges that neuroscience insights complement cognitive measures that generate explicit measures of thought, emotion, or preferences, but he warns that repeated measures over time are problematic for both types of measurement, and the inherent complexity of brain–behavior relationships is often underestimated. With both orientations, understanding the functioning of human behavior is akin to making sense of an orchestra, where the interactive blending of different instruments and musicians reflects a complex activity that generates sounds, emotions, and stories. Both consumer neuroscientists and psychologists need to broaden their paradigmatic approaches with bodily measures and advanced psychological procedures to overcome challenges to joint progress. Martin Reiman asserts that despite difficulties with measures that have different levels of abstraction or velocity, research has provided remarkable associations between brain activity and consumer behavior. Effective studies merging brain and behavior can effectively proceed with studies that differ in two dimensions: first, by altering the number of variables, and second, by shifting whether the scientific paradigm is inductive or deductive. In its simple form, the Excavation path explores brain activity when a person is exposed to specific statements or emotions. In its most challenging form, Integrative Studies generate predictions from theories that test the convergent validity of divergent measures and leverage skills from different researchers. Studies reflecting high levels on one dimension but low levels on the other can also provide fruitful research opportunities. Brian Knutson, like Reimann, counters the idea that consumer psychology has not lived up to its promises. He references studies showing that activity from very specific areas of the brain reliably predicts choices better than explicit ratings or choices. Such research generates deductions from increasingly precise neural maps that enable confirmation of theory. That said, he acknowledges that consumer neuroscience is not able to identify a brain button that would alter choice through manipulated neurostimulation. However, since human brains are similar across people, the depth of neural insights that are consistent across a small sample of 40 respondents may generate greater insights than convent
Clithero、Karmarkar、Nave 和 Plassmann 的文章(《消费者心理学杂志》,2024 年)的最初版本受到了一小部分学者的公开评论。他们的建议鼓励作者澄清大脑过程与情感、信念和行动之间的挑战性关系。修订后,fMRI 和 EEG 的范围扩大到了视觉、面部表情、呼吸、心律和血液化学测量。该论文为神经学和心理学学者之间的合作提供了多种途径。以下评论反映了对最终文章的不同反应。韦斯-哈钦森(Wes Hutchinson)承认神经科学的见解是对认知测量的补充,认知测量可以明确测量思想、情感或偏好,但他警告说,长期重复测量对这两种测量都有问题,而且大脑与行为关系的内在复杂性往往被低估。从这两个方向来看,理解人类行为的功能就好比理解一个交响乐团,不同乐器和乐手的互动融合反映了产生声音、情感和故事的复杂活动。消费者神经科学家和心理学家都需要通过身体测量和先进的心理程序来拓宽他们的范式方法,以克服共同进步所面临的挑战。马丁-雷曼(Martin Reiman)断言,尽管抽象程度或速度不同的测量方法存在困难,但研究已经提供了大脑活动与消费者行为之间的显著关联。将大脑和行为结合起来的有效研究可以在两个方面有效地进行:第一,改变变量的数量;第二,改变科学范式是归纳法还是演绎法。就其简单形式而言,"挖掘"(Excavation)路径探索的是人在接触特定语句或情绪时的大脑活动。在最具挑战性的形式中,"整合研究 "从理论中得出预测,测试不同测量方法的趋同有效性,并利用不同研究人员的技能。在一个维度上反映出高水平而在另一个维度上反映出低水平的研究也能提供富有成效的研究机会。布赖恩-克努特森(Brian Knutson)和莱曼一样,也反驳了消费者心理学没有实现其承诺的观点。他提到有研究表明,大脑特定区域的活动比明确的评级或选择更能可靠地预测选择。这些研究从越来越精确的神经图谱中得出推论,从而证实了理论。尽管如此,他承认消费神经科学还无法确定一个大脑按钮,通过操纵神经刺激来改变选择。不过,由于人的大脑在不同人群中是相似的,因此,在 40 个受访者的小样本中,神经洞察的深度是一致的,这可能会比拥有 2000 个受访者的传统营销研究产生更大的洞察力。随着可靠性、有效性和可推广性的提高,神经科学的成本将进一步降低,尤其是在采用身体测量方法的情况下。他承认,理论方面的发展要慢于应用,尤其是得到赞助机构支持的应用,这些赞助机构更满足于局部洞察而非一般模型。
{"title":"Commentaries on “Reconsidering the path for neural and physiological methods in consumer psychology”","authors":"J. Wesley Hutchinson,&nbsp;Martin Reimann,&nbsp;Brian Knutson,&nbsp;Joel Huber","doi":"10.1002/jcpy.1398","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jcpy.1398","url":null,"abstract":"&lt;p&gt;The initial version of the article by Clithero, Karmarkar, Nave, and Plassmann (Journal of Consumer Psychology, 2024) was critiqued by open comments from a small group of scholars. Their suggestions encouraged the authors to clarify challenging relationships between brain processes and emotions, beliefs, and actions. The revision expanded fMRI and EEG to include measures of vision, facial expression, breathing, heart rhythms, and blood chemistry. The paper provides multiple avenues of joint work between neurological and psychological scholars. The comments below reflect different reactions to the final article. Wes Hutchinson acknowledges that neuroscience insights complement cognitive measures that generate explicit measures of thought, emotion, or preferences, but he warns that repeated measures over time are problematic for both types of measurement, and the inherent complexity of brain–behavior relationships is often underestimated. With both orientations, understanding the functioning of human behavior is akin to making sense of an orchestra, where the interactive blending of different instruments and musicians reflects a complex activity that generates sounds, emotions, and stories. Both consumer neuroscientists and psychologists need to broaden their paradigmatic approaches with bodily measures and advanced psychological procedures to overcome challenges to joint progress. Martin Reiman asserts that despite difficulties with measures that have different levels of abstraction or velocity, research has provided remarkable associations between brain activity and consumer behavior. Effective studies merging brain and behavior can effectively proceed with studies that differ in two dimensions: first, by altering the number of variables, and second, by shifting whether the scientific paradigm is inductive or deductive. In its simple form, the Excavation path explores brain activity when a person is exposed to specific statements or emotions. In its most challenging form, Integrative Studies generate predictions from theories that test the convergent validity of divergent measures and leverage skills from different researchers. Studies reflecting high levels on one dimension but low levels on the other can also provide fruitful research opportunities. Brian Knutson, like Reimann, counters the idea that consumer psychology has not lived up to its promises. He references studies showing that activity from very specific areas of the brain reliably predicts choices better than explicit ratings or choices. Such research generates deductions from increasingly precise neural maps that enable confirmation of theory. That said, he acknowledges that consumer neuroscience is not able to identify a brain button that would alter choice through manipulated neurostimulation. However, since human brains are similar across people, the depth of neural insights that are consistent across a small sample of 40 respondents may generate greater insights than convent","PeriodicalId":48365,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Consumer Psychology","volume":"34 1","pages":"214-221"},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2023-11-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135291331","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"管理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
So bad it's good: When and why consumers prefer bad options 坏得好:消费者何时以及为何偏爱糟糕的选择
IF 4 2区 管理学 Q2 BUSINESS Pub Date : 2023-11-05 DOI: 10.1002/jcpy.1394
Evan Weingarten, Amit Bhattacharjee, Patti Williams

The assumption that consumers prefer better quality options over worse ones seems almost definitional. However, a variety of marketplace examples suggest that consumers sometimes choose content that is “so bad it's good,” such as Tommy Wiseau's The Room or Rebecca Black's “Friday,” over apparently better alternatives (e.g., those of mediocre quality). In 12 preregistered studies (N = 5393) across several content domains (e.g., jokes, talent show auditions), we provide the first controlled, empirical demonstration of consumer preferences for badness (i.e., choosing options because consumers expect them to be bad). We provide initial evidence that these preferences are rooted in expectations of entertainment value from the worst available option. Preferences for these options emerge more frequently when their deviations from quality standards are perceived as benign (i.e., inconsequential). Accordingly, such preferences are less prevalent when consumption is consequential, and involves utilitarian goals or monetary costs. We conclude by exploring the extent to which dimensions such as humor, absurdity, esthetic quality, and utilitarian value underlie so-bad-it's-good perceptions, and highlight several open questions to spark future research.

消费者宁愿选择质量更好的内容,也不愿选择质量更差的内容,这一假设几乎是一成不变的。然而,各种市场实例表明,消费者有时会选择 "坏到极致 "的内容,如汤米-威索(Tommy Wiseau)的《房间》或丽贝卡-布莱克(Rebecca Black)的《星期五》(Friday),而不是明显更好的替代内容(如质量一般的内容)。在 12 项预先登记的研究(N = 5393)中,我们对多个内容领域(如笑话、选秀节目试镜)进行了研究,首次对消费者的 "坏 "偏好(即由于消费者认为选项不好而选择它们)进行了控制性实证论证。我们提供的初步证据表明,这些偏好源于对最差选项娱乐价值的预期。当这些选项与质量标准的偏差被认为是良性的(即无足轻重)时,对这些选项的偏好就会更频繁地出现。相应地,当消费具有后果性、涉及功利目标或金钱成本时,这种偏好就不那么普遍。最后,我们探讨了幽默、荒诞、审美质量和功利价值等维度在多大程度上影响了人们对 "好得不能再好 "的看法,并强调了几个有待解决的问题,以引发未来的研究。
{"title":"So bad it's good: When and why consumers prefer bad options","authors":"Evan Weingarten,&nbsp;Amit Bhattacharjee,&nbsp;Patti Williams","doi":"10.1002/jcpy.1394","DOIUrl":"10.1002/jcpy.1394","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The assumption that consumers prefer better quality options over worse ones seems almost definitional. However, a variety of marketplace examples suggest that consumers sometimes choose content that is “so bad it's good,” such as Tommy Wiseau's <i>The Room</i> or Rebecca Black's “Friday,” over apparently better alternatives (e.g., those of mediocre quality). In 12 preregistered studies (<i>N</i> = 5393) across several content domains (e.g., jokes, talent show auditions), we provide the first controlled, empirical demonstration of consumer preferences for badness (i.e., choosing options <i>because</i> consumers expect them to be bad). We provide initial evidence that these preferences are rooted in expectations of entertainment value from the worst available option. Preferences for these options emerge more frequently when their deviations from quality standards are perceived as benign (i.e., inconsequential). Accordingly, such preferences are less prevalent when consumption is consequential, and involves utilitarian goals or monetary costs. We conclude by exploring the extent to which dimensions such as humor, absurdity, esthetic quality, and utilitarian value underlie so-bad-it's-good perceptions, and highlight several open questions to spark future research.</p>","PeriodicalId":48365,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Consumer Psychology","volume":"34 4","pages":"632-640"},"PeriodicalIF":4.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135725201","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"管理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Consumer Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1