首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Child Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Do children match described probabilities? The sampling hypothesis applied to repeated risky choice 孩子是否符合描述的概率?抽样假设适用于重复的风险选择。
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-03 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106126
Anna I. Thoma , Christin Schulze
One way in which children can learn about probabilities of different outcomes before making a decision is from description, for instance, by observing graphical representations of frequency distributions. But how do repeated risky choices develop in early childhood when outcome probabilities are learned from description? Integrating previous findings from children’s sampling processes in causal learning and adults’ repeated choice behavior, we investigated repeated choices from 201 children aged 3 to 7 years and 100 adults in a child-friendly risky choice task. We expected young children to probability match and predicted that the perceived dependency between choices would shape the underlying choice process. However, the assumed cognitive processes derived from the causal learning and risky choice literature did not generalize to children’s or adults’ repeated risky choices when outcome probabilities were learned from graphical representations prior to making a decision. Moreover, choice behavior did not differ as a function of the perceived dependency between guesses. Instead, children broadly diversified choices, and switching between options dominated older children’s choice behavior. Our results contribute to increasing evidence of childhood as a phase for heightened exploration and highlight the importance of considering the learning format when studying repeated choice across development.
孩子们在做出决定之前了解不同结果的概率的一种方法是通过描述,例如,通过观察频率分布的图形表示。但是,当结果的可能性是通过描述来学习的时候,在儿童早期,重复的冒险选择是如何发展的呢?结合前人关于儿童因果学习抽样过程和成人重复选择行为的研究结果,我们对201名3 - 7岁儿童和100名成人在儿童友好型风险选择任务中的重复选择进行了研究。我们期望幼儿概率匹配,并预测选择之间的感知依赖性会影响潜在的选择过程。然而,从因果学习和风险选择文献中得出的假设认知过程并不能推广到儿童或成人在做出决定之前从图形表示中了解结果概率时的重复风险选择。此外,选择行为并没有因为猜测之间的感知依赖性而有所不同。相反,孩子们的选择范围很广,在选项之间切换主导着大孩子的选择行为。我们的研究结果提供了更多的证据,证明童年是一个高度探索的阶段,并强调了在研究跨越发展的重复选择时考虑学习形式的重要性。
{"title":"Do children match described probabilities? The sampling hypothesis applied to repeated risky choice","authors":"Anna I. Thoma ,&nbsp;Christin Schulze","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106126","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106126","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>One way in which children can learn about probabilities of different outcomes before making a decision is from <em>description,</em> for instance, by observing graphical representations of frequency distributions. But how do repeated risky choices develop in early childhood when outcome probabilities are learned from description? Integrating previous findings from children’s sampling processes in causal learning and adults’ repeated choice behavior, we investigated repeated choices from 201 children aged 3 to 7 years and 100 adults in a child-friendly risky choice task. We expected young children to probability match and predicted that the perceived dependency between choices would shape the underlying choice process. However, the assumed cognitive processes derived from the causal learning and risky choice literature did not generalize to children’s or adults’ repeated risky choices when outcome probabilities were learned from graphical representations prior to making a decision. Moreover, choice behavior did not differ as a function of the perceived dependency between guesses. Instead, children broadly diversified choices, and switching between options dominated older children’s choice behavior. Our results contribute to increasing evidence of childhood as a phase for heightened exploration and highlight the importance of considering the learning format when studying repeated choice across development.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":"251 ","pages":"Article 106126"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142781489","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The relation of verbal and nonverbal skills to basic numerical processing of preterm versus term-born preschoolers 早产儿和足月出生学龄前儿童语言和非语言技能与基本数字处理的关系。
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106128
Isabelle Farmer , Paige M. Nelson , Tilbe Göksun , Ö. Ece Demir-Lira
Verbal and nonverbal skills significantly contribute to individual differences in children’s numerical development at the group level. However, less is known about whether the nature of the relations between verbal and nonverbal systems and numerical cognition varies depending on the unique characteristics children bring into numerical learning. To better delineate these associations, we examined the association between verbal and nonverbal skills and symbolic numerical development in preterm-born (PTB; n = 93; <37 weeks of gestation) children and term-born children (n = 104). We showed that PTB preschoolers, as a group, were at a higher risk of falling behind on certain numerical tasks (cardinality) but not on others (counting). There was, however, significant individual variability within the groups. Verbal and nonverbal skills contributed to the variability of children’s numerical performance but did so differentially across the full spectrum of gestational age. Specifically, verbal skills moderated the association between gestational age and symbolic number performance (cardinality). The relation between verbal and cardinality skills was stronger at higher gestational ages compared with lower gestational ages. In addition, at higher gestational ages, children more frequently used retrieval strategy and less often relied solely on finger counting for the cardinality task. Shifting the focus from group differences to understanding individuals and their unique developmental pathways may enhance our insight into the risk and protective factors underlying the variability observed in all children.
在群体水平上,语言和非语言技能对儿童数字发展的个体差异有显著影响。然而,关于语言和非语言系统与数字认知之间关系的性质是否取决于儿童带入数字学习的独特特征,人们知之甚少。为了更好地描述这些关联,我们研究了言语和非言语技能与早产儿(PTB;n = 93;
{"title":"The relation of verbal and nonverbal skills to basic numerical processing of preterm versus term-born preschoolers","authors":"Isabelle Farmer ,&nbsp;Paige M. Nelson ,&nbsp;Tilbe Göksun ,&nbsp;Ö. Ece Demir-Lira","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106128","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106128","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Verbal and nonverbal skills significantly contribute to individual differences in children’s numerical development at the group level. However, less is known about whether the nature of the relations between verbal and nonverbal systems and numerical cognition varies depending on the unique characteristics children bring into numerical learning. To better delineate these associations, we examined the association between verbal and nonverbal skills and symbolic numerical development in preterm-born (PTB; <em>n</em> = 93; &lt;37 weeks of gestation) children and term-born children (<em>n</em> = 104). We showed that PTB preschoolers, as a group, were at a higher risk of falling behind on certain numerical tasks (cardinality) but not on others (counting). There was, however, significant individual variability within the groups. Verbal and nonverbal skills contributed to the variability of children’s numerical performance but did so differentially across the full spectrum of gestational age. Specifically, verbal skills moderated the association between gestational age and symbolic number performance (cardinality). The relation between verbal and cardinality skills was stronger at higher gestational ages compared with lower gestational ages. In addition, at higher gestational ages, children more frequently used retrieval strategy and less often relied solely on finger counting for the cardinality task. Shifting the focus from group differences to understanding individuals and their unique developmental pathways may enhance our insight into the risk and protective factors underlying the variability observed in all children.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":"251 ","pages":"Article 106128"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-12-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142773681","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Examining children’s verb learning in the United States and Japan: Do comparisons help? 考察美国和日本儿童的动词学习:比较有帮助吗?
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-30 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106129
Jane B. Childers , Mutsumi Imai , Masato Ohba , Faith Perry , Leah Marsh
Learning verbs is an important part of learning one’s native language. Prior studies have shown that children younger than 5 years can have difficulty in learning and extending new verbs. The current study extended these studies by showing children multiple events that can be compared during learning, including Japanese- and English-speaking children. In the study, 2-, 3-, and 4-year-olds saw two similar events and then one varied (progressive alignment) or three varied (low alignable) events in a learning phase before test, and this was repeated for four sets. Children were asked to extend these novel verbs in easy (non-cross-mapping) and difficult (cross-mapping) test trials. A repeated-measures analysis of variance showed a significant Age by Condition interaction. In contrast to prior results, the 4-year-olds in both languages did well in both conditions and across test trial types. The 3-year-olds, especially in Japanese, performed best in the progressive alignment condition, showing that experience in seeing similar events was useful for verb learning. The 2-year-olds mostly struggled in this task, showing success only in the low-alignment condition, non-cross-mapping (easy) test trial. These are new findings given that no previous study has examined the role of different levels of variability during learning in a cross-language sample, and no prior study has examined the impact of objects at test in this way. This study shows that an important mechanism for verb learning—the comparison of events—could be useful across languages.
学习动词是学习母语的重要组成部分。先前的研究表明,5岁以下的儿童在学习和扩展新动词方面可能有困难。目前的研究扩展了这些研究,向儿童展示了在学习过程中可以比较的多个事件,包括日语和英语儿童。在这项研究中,2岁、3岁和4岁的孩子在测试前的学习阶段看到了两个类似的事件,然后一个变化(渐进对齐)或三个变化(低对齐)事件,这一过程重复了四组。孩子们被要求在简单(非交叉映射)和困难(交叉映射)的测试中扩展这些新动词。重复测量方差分析显示显著的年龄-条件相互作用。与之前的结果相比,使用两种语言的4岁儿童在两种情况下和测试类型中都表现良好。3岁的孩子,尤其是日语孩子,在渐进式对齐条件下表现最好,这表明看到类似事件的经历对动词学习很有用。2岁儿童在此任务中大多表现困难,仅在低对齐条件下,非交叉映射(简单)测试中表现成功。这些都是新的发现,因为之前没有研究考察过跨语言样本学习过程中不同水平的可变性的作用,也没有研究以这种方式考察过测试对象的影响。这项研究表明,动词学习的一个重要机制——事件比较——可能对不同语言都有用。
{"title":"Examining children’s verb learning in the United States and Japan: Do comparisons help?","authors":"Jane B. Childers ,&nbsp;Mutsumi Imai ,&nbsp;Masato Ohba ,&nbsp;Faith Perry ,&nbsp;Leah Marsh","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106129","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106129","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Learning verbs is an important part of learning one’s native language. Prior studies have shown that children younger than 5 years can have difficulty in learning and extending new verbs. The current study extended these studies by showing children multiple events that can be compared during learning, including Japanese- and English-speaking children. In the study, 2-, 3-, and 4-year-olds saw two similar events and then one varied (progressive alignment) or three varied (low alignable) events in a learning phase before test, and this was repeated for four sets. Children were asked to extend these novel verbs in easy (non-cross-mapping) and difficult (cross-mapping) test trials. A repeated-measures analysis of variance showed a significant Age by Condition interaction. In contrast to prior results, the 4-year-olds in both languages did well in both conditions and across test trial types. The 3-year-olds, especially in Japanese, performed best in the progressive alignment condition, showing that experience in seeing similar events was useful for verb learning. The 2-year-olds mostly struggled in this task, showing success only in the low-alignment condition, non-cross-mapping (easy) test trial. These are new findings given that no previous study has examined the role of different levels of variability during learning in a cross-language sample, and no prior study has examined the impact of objects at test in this way. This study shows that an important mechanism for verb learning—the comparison of events—could be useful across languages.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":"251 ","pages":"Article 106129"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142745536","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A structural equation model of emotion knowledge and verbal intelligence in peer acceptance in a sample of Portuguese preschoolers 葡萄牙学龄前儿童同伴接纳中情绪知识与言语智力的结构方程模型
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-30 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106131
Mariana Sousa , Sara Cruz , Anabela Silva-Fernandes , Diana Alves
Using a mediation model, this study examined the role of verbal intelligence and emotion knowledge in peer acceptance in a sample of Portuguese preschool children. Participants were 444 5-year-olds (214 girls and 230 boys). Emotion knowledge was assessed with the Portuguese version of the Assessment of Children’s Emotion Skills. Verbal intelligence was measured with the Serial Rapid Naming and Semantic Verbal Fluency tests of the Battery of Phonological Assessment. The Social Status Interview was used to assess peer acceptance. We observed a significant indirect effect of verbal intelligence on peer acceptance mediated by emotion knowledge. These results show that cognitive skills, namely the verbal ones, closely and dynamically interact with emotion knowledge in explaining children’s acceptance by their peers. The importance of promoting emotion knowledge and verbal intelligence is discussed, given their important contribution to social adjustment at a young age.
本研究采用中介模型,考察了语言智力和情感知识在葡萄牙学龄前儿童同伴接纳中的作用。参与者是444名5岁儿童(214名女孩和230名男孩)。用葡萄牙语版《儿童情绪技能评估》对情绪知识进行评估。言语智力采用语音评估系列快速命名和语义性言语流畅性测试来测量。社会地位访谈被用来评估同伴的接受程度。我们观察到言语智力在情绪知识介导下对同伴接受有显著的间接影响。这些结果表明,认知技能,即语言技能,在解释儿童被同伴接受的过程中与情感知识密切而动态地相互作用。鉴于情绪知识和语言智力对幼儿社会适应的重要贡献,本文讨论了提高情绪知识和语言智力的重要性。
{"title":"A structural equation model of emotion knowledge and verbal intelligence in peer acceptance in a sample of Portuguese preschoolers","authors":"Mariana Sousa ,&nbsp;Sara Cruz ,&nbsp;Anabela Silva-Fernandes ,&nbsp;Diana Alves","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106131","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106131","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Using a mediation model, this study examined the role of verbal intelligence and emotion knowledge in peer acceptance in a sample of Portuguese preschool children. Participants were 444 5-year-olds (214 girls and 230 boys). Emotion knowledge was assessed with the Portuguese version of the Assessment of Children’s Emotion Skills. Verbal intelligence was measured with the Serial Rapid Naming and Semantic Verbal Fluency tests of the Battery of Phonological Assessment. The Social Status Interview <em>was</em> used to assess peer acceptance. We observed a significant indirect effect of verbal intelligence on peer acceptance mediated by emotion knowledge. These results show that cognitive skills, namely the verbal ones, closely and dynamically interact with emotion knowledge in explaining children’s acceptance by their peers. The importance of promoting emotion knowledge and verbal intelligence is discussed, given their important contribution to social adjustment at a young age.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":"251 ","pages":"Article 106131"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142745528","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Moral gradients based on social boundaries: Children prioritize themselves and their ingroup when resources are limited 基于社会边界的道德梯度:当资源有限时,儿童优先考虑自己和他们的内部群体
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-29 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106125
Mioko Sudo, Mitsuhiko Ishikawa
In an ideal world, there would be sufficient resources to be fairly allocated to everyone. The reality, however, is that resources are often limited. How do children navigate resource distribution decisions in the face of scarcity and sufficiency? Our study consisted of two experiments with 4- to 12-year-olds (N = 96), where children were required to distribute resources among themselves, a gender ingroup member, and a gender outgroup member when there was a limited number of resources (Experiment 1) and when there were sufficient resources for an equitable distribution (Experiment 2). When resources were limited, children demonstrated an overall tendency to allocate more resources to themselves and the gender ingroup member at a disadvantage of the gender outgroup member. However, children were not indifferent to the welfare of the gender outgroup member, as evidenced by their tendency to minimize the disadvantage that the gender outgroup member experienced. Furthermore, when the number of resources allowed for an equitable distribution, children showed a robust tendency to fairly allocate the same number of resources to each individual. Thus, our findings suggest that children were acting on a moral gradient, whereby they included both the gender ingroup member and the gender outgroup member within their moral considerations, albeit demonstrating slight favoritism toward the former.
在一个理想的世界里,应该有足够的资源公平地分配给每个人。然而,现实情况是,资源往往是有限的。面对稀缺和充足的资源,孩子们如何进行资源分配决策?我们的研究包括两个实验,对象为4- 12岁的儿童(N = 96),在实验1中,儿童被要求在资源有限的情况下(实验1),在资源足够公平分配的情况下(实验2),在他们自己、一个性别内群体成员和一个性别外群体成员之间分配资源。儿童总体上倾向于将更多的资源分配给自己和性别群体内的成员,而不是性别群体外的成员。然而,儿童并非对性别外群体成员的福利漠不关心,这可以从他们倾向于将性别外群体成员所经历的不利因素最小化的趋势中得到证明。此外,当资源数量允许公平分配时,孩子们表现出一种强烈的倾向,即公平地将相同数量的资源分配给每个人。因此,我们的研究结果表明,儿童的行为是基于道德梯度的,即他们在道德考虑中同时考虑了群体内性别成员和群体外性别成员,尽管表现出对前者的轻微偏爱。
{"title":"Moral gradients based on social boundaries: Children prioritize themselves and their ingroup when resources are limited","authors":"Mioko Sudo,&nbsp;Mitsuhiko Ishikawa","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106125","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106125","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>In an ideal world, there would be sufficient resources to be fairly allocated to everyone. The reality, however, is that resources are often limited. How do children navigate resource distribution decisions in the face of scarcity and sufficiency? Our study consisted of two experiments with 4- to 12-year-olds (<em>N</em> = 96), where children were required to distribute resources among themselves, a gender ingroup member, and a gender outgroup member when there was a limited number of resources (Experiment 1) and when there were sufficient resources for an equitable distribution (Experiment 2). When resources were limited, children demonstrated an overall tendency to allocate more resources to themselves and the gender ingroup member at a disadvantage of the gender outgroup member. However, children were not indifferent to the welfare of the gender outgroup member, as evidenced by their tendency to minimize the disadvantage that the gender outgroup member experienced. Furthermore, when the number of resources allowed for an equitable distribution, children showed a robust tendency to fairly allocate the same number of resources to each individual. Thus, our findings suggest that children were acting on a moral gradient, whereby they included both the gender ingroup member and the gender outgroup member within their moral considerations, albeit demonstrating slight favoritism toward the former.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":"251 ","pages":"Article 106125"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142745525","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Could the impact of emotional states on learning in children vary with task difficulty? 情绪状态对儿童学习的影响会因任务难度而异吗?
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-29 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106122
Gisella Decarli, Simone Zasso, Laura Franchin
Emotions are known to influence memory, in particular retention and recall, with positive emotions enhancing performances and negative emotions showing mixed effects. Although the influence of emotions on memory is well-established, their precise impact on the learning process remains a matter of debate and investigation. We implemented two experiments with children aged 6 to 8 years to examine how different emotional states affect training with tasks of varying difficulty. In Experiment 1, children were assessed in a letter recognition task and were assigned to positive, negative, or neutral emotional training conditions. Results showed significant performance improvements across all emotional conditions, indicating that emotional states did not differentially affect this task. In Experiment 2, using a more difficult non-word dictation task, significant improvements were found only in the positive and neutral conditions but not in the negative condition, suggesting that task difficulty modulates the impact of emotional states. These findings highlight the importance of considering both emotional states and task difficulty in educational settings. Positive and neutral emotions may facilitate cognitive processes under challenging conditions, whereas negative emotions might hinder them.
众所周知,情绪会影响记忆,尤其是保留和回忆,积极情绪会提高表现,而消极情绪则表现出混合效果。尽管情绪对记忆的影响已经得到证实,但它们对学习过程的确切影响仍是一个有争议和调查的问题。我们对6至8岁的儿童进行了两个实验,以研究不同的情绪状态如何影响不同难度任务的训练。在实验1中,儿童在字母识别任务中被评估,并被分配到积极、消极和中性的情绪训练条件下。结果显示,所有情绪状态下的表现都有显著提高,表明情绪状态对这项任务的影响没有差异。在实验2中,使用难度更高的非单词听写任务,只有在积极和中性条件下才有显著的改善,而在消极条件下没有显著的改善,这表明任务难度调节了情绪状态的影响。这些发现强调了在教育环境中同时考虑情绪状态和任务难度的重要性。积极和中性情绪可能促进挑战性条件下的认知过程,而消极情绪可能阻碍认知过程。
{"title":"Could the impact of emotional states on learning in children vary with task difficulty?","authors":"Gisella Decarli,&nbsp;Simone Zasso,&nbsp;Laura Franchin","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106122","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106122","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Emotions are known to influence memory, in particular retention and recall, with positive emotions enhancing performances and negative emotions showing mixed effects. Although the influence of emotions on memory is well-established, their precise impact on the learning process remains a matter of debate and investigation. We implemented two experiments with children aged 6 to 8 years to examine how different emotional states affect training with tasks of varying difficulty. In Experiment 1, children were assessed in a letter recognition task and were assigned to positive, negative, or neutral emotional training conditions. Results showed significant performance improvements across all emotional conditions, indicating that emotional states did not differentially affect this task. In Experiment 2, using a more difficult non-word dictation task, significant improvements were found only in the positive and neutral conditions but not in the negative condition, suggesting that task difficulty modulates the impact of emotional states. These findings highlight the importance of considering both emotional states and task difficulty in educational settings. Positive and neutral emotions may facilitate cognitive processes under challenging conditions, whereas negative emotions might hinder them.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":"251 ","pages":"Article 106122"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142745526","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does self-regulation mediate the relation between parent–child relationships and peer acceptance during early childhood? 自我调节是否在幼儿期亲子关系与同伴接纳之间起中介作用?
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-29 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106134
Jessica N. Lucas-Nihei, Alycia M. Hund, Matthew S. Hesson-McInnis
Peer acceptance is an important facet of child development. Much of the previous research focused on peer acceptance during the elementary school years, so more work is needed to understand peer acceptance and its antecedents during the preschool years. The goal of this study was to test the extent to which children’s self-regulation mediates the association between parent–child relationships and peer acceptance during the preschool years in our sample of 134 4- and 5-year-old children. Two mediation models were tested via a structural equation modeling approach using path analysis. Contrary to predictions, results were not consistent with mediation but rather indicated that parent–child closeness and children’s self-regulation are significant direct predictors of children’s peer acceptance and that age significantly predicts children’s self-regulation. These results contribute to the literature regarding factors that predict peer acceptance during early childhood and have implications for children, families, and professionals who support young children’s development.
同伴接纳是儿童发展的一个重要方面。以前的研究大多集中在小学阶段的同伴接受上,因此需要更多的工作来了解学前阶段的同伴接受及其前因。本研究以134名4- 5岁儿童为样本,旨在检验幼儿自我调节在亲子关系和同伴接纳之间的中介作用程度。两个中介模型通过结构方程建模方法使用路径分析进行检验。与预测相反,结果与中介不一致,而是表明亲子亲密和儿童自我调节是儿童同伴接受的显著直接预测因子,年龄显著预测儿童自我调节。这些结果促进了有关预测幼儿期同伴接纳的因素的文献,并对儿童、家庭和支持幼儿发展的专业人员具有启示意义。
{"title":"Does self-regulation mediate the relation between parent–child relationships and peer acceptance during early childhood?","authors":"Jessica N. Lucas-Nihei,&nbsp;Alycia M. Hund,&nbsp;Matthew S. Hesson-McInnis","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106134","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106134","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Peer acceptance is an important facet of child development. Much of the previous research focused on peer acceptance during the elementary school years, so more work is needed to understand peer acceptance and its antecedents during the preschool years. The goal of this study was to test the extent to which children’s self-regulation mediates the association between parent–child relationships and peer acceptance during the preschool years in our sample of 134 4- and 5-year-old children. Two mediation models were tested via a structural equation modeling approach using path analysis. Contrary to predictions, results were not consistent with mediation but rather indicated that parent–child closeness and children’s self-regulation are significant direct predictors of children’s peer acceptance and that age significantly predicts children’s self-regulation. These results contribute to the literature regarding factors that predict peer acceptance during early childhood and have implications for children, families, and professionals who support young children’s development.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":"251 ","pages":"Article 106134"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142745527","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A happy face advantage for pareidolic faces in children and adults 儿童和成人的 "快乐面孔 "优势
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-27 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106127
Krisztina V. Jakobsen , Cate M. Hickman , Elizabeth A. Simpson
Pareidolic faces—illusory faces in objects—offer a unique context for studying biases in the development of facial processing because they are visually diverse (e.g., color, shape) while lacking key elements of real faces (e.g., race, species). In an online study, 7- and 8-year-old children (n = 32) and adults (n = 32) categorized happy and angry expressions in human and pareidolic face images. We found that children have a robust, adult-like happy face advantage for human and pareidolic faces, reflected in speed and accuracy. These results suggest that the happy face advantage is not unique to human faces, supporting the hypothesis that humans employ comparable face templates for processing pareidolic and human faces. Our findings add to a growing list of other processing similarities between human and pareidolic faces and suggest that children may likewise show these similarities.
拟人化人脸--物体中的虚幻人脸--为研究人脸处理过程中的偏差提供了一个独特的背景,因为它们在视觉上具有多样性(如颜色、形状),同时又缺乏真实人脸的关键要素(如种族、物种)。在一项在线研究中,7、8 岁的儿童(n = 32)和成人(n = 32)对人脸和狰狞人脸图像中的喜怒表情进行了分类。我们发现,儿童在对人脸和狰狞脸进行分类时具有类似成人的强大的快乐表情优势,这体现在速度和准确性上。这些结果表明,喜脸优势并不是人脸所独有的,这支持了人类在处理栅栏脸和人脸时使用相似人脸模板的假设。我们的研究结果补充了越来越多的人类和楹联人脸在处理过程中的相似之处,并表明儿童也可能同样表现出这些相似之处。
{"title":"A happy face advantage for pareidolic faces in children and adults","authors":"Krisztina V. Jakobsen ,&nbsp;Cate M. Hickman ,&nbsp;Elizabeth A. Simpson","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106127","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106127","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Pareidolic faces—illusory faces in objects—offer a unique context for studying biases in the development of facial processing because they are visually diverse (e.g., color, shape) while lacking key elements of real faces (e.g., race, species). In an online study, 7- and 8-year-old children (<em>n</em> = 32) and adults (<em>n</em> = 32) categorized happy and angry expressions in human and pareidolic face images. We found that children have a robust, adult-like happy face advantage for human and pareidolic faces, reflected in speed and accuracy. These results suggest that the happy face advantage is not unique to human faces, supporting the hypothesis that humans employ comparable face templates for processing pareidolic and human faces. Our findings add to a growing list of other processing similarities between human and pareidolic faces and suggest that children may likewise show these similarities.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":"251 ","pages":"Article 106127"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142723210","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children plan manual actions similarly in structured tasks and in free play 在有组织的任务和自由游戏中,儿童计划手动操作的方式类似。
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-22 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106124
Sara Golmakani , Brianna E Kaplan , Karen E Adolph , Ori Ossmy
Visually guided planning is fundamental for manual actions on objects. Multi-step planning—when only the requirements for the initial action are directly visible in the scene—necessitates initial visual guidance to optimize the subsequent actions. We found that 3- to 5-year-old children (n = 23) who exhibited visually guided, multi-step planning in a structured tool-use task (hammering down a peg) also demonstrated visually guided planning during unstructured free play while interlocking Duplo bricks and Squigz pieces. Children who exhibited visually guided planning in the hammering task also spent more time looking at the to-be-grasped free-play object and at their construction during reach and transport compared with children who did not demonstrate multi-step planning in the hammering task. Moreover, visually guided planning in the Duplo and Squigz tasks was positively correlated, indicating that planning generalizes across contexts. Findings show that visually guided planning in young children generalizes across different manual actions on objects, including structured tool use and unstructured free play.
视觉引导规划是对物体进行手动操作的基础。多步骤规划--当场景中只有初始动作的要求是直接可见的--需要初始视觉引导来优化后续动作。我们发现,3 到 5 岁的儿童(n=23)在结构化的工具使用任务(敲下木桩)中表现出视觉引导下的多步骤规划时,他们在非结构化的自由游戏中将 Duplo 砖块和 Squigz 块联锁时也表现出视觉引导下的规划。与在锤击任务中未表现出多步骤规划的儿童相比,在锤击任务中表现出视觉引导规划的儿童在拿取和搬运过程中也会花更多的时间去观察即将被抓取的自由游戏物体及其构造。此外,在 Duplo 和 Squigz 任务中,视觉引导下的规划呈正相关,这表明规划可在不同情境下通用。研究结果表明,幼儿在视觉引导下对物体进行的规划可泛化到不同的手动操作中,包括有组织的工具使用和无组织的自由游戏。
{"title":"Children plan manual actions similarly in structured tasks and in free play","authors":"Sara Golmakani ,&nbsp;Brianna E Kaplan ,&nbsp;Karen E Adolph ,&nbsp;Ori Ossmy","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106124","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106124","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Visually guided planning is fundamental for manual actions on objects. Multi-step planning—when only the requirements for the initial action are directly visible in the scene—necessitates initial visual guidance to optimize the subsequent actions. We found that 3- to 5-year-old children (<em>n</em> = 23) who exhibited visually guided, multi-step planning in a structured tool-use task (hammering down a peg) also demonstrated visually guided planning during unstructured free play while interlocking Duplo bricks and Squigz pieces. Children who exhibited visually guided planning in the hammering task also spent more time looking at the to-be-grasped free-play object and at their construction during reach and transport compared with children who did not demonstrate multi-step planning in the hammering task. Moreover, visually guided planning in the Duplo and Squigz tasks was positively correlated, indicating that planning generalizes across contexts. Findings show that visually guided planning in young children generalizes across different manual actions on objects, including structured tool use and unstructured free play.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":"250 ","pages":"Article 106124"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142695876","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Concurrent and predictive associations between executive function and numerical skills in early childhood 幼儿期执行功能与计算技能之间的并发和预测关联。
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-20 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106113
Jasmine R. Ernst , Michèle M.M. Mazzocco, Stephanie M. Carlson
The importance of early numerical and executive function (EF) skills is well-established, with each skill set positively and specifically predicting later mathematics achievement, income, postsecondary education, and more. Less is known, however, about the relations between EF and numerical skills. Therefore, we examined the concurrent and predictive relations between EF and numerical skills in preschoolers to third graders (N = 205; 4.67–8.75 years of age; 43.9% female; 51.2% White non-Hispanic, 18% multiracial, 6.3% Hispanic, 12.2% Black, 2% American Indian/Alaska Native, 4.9% Asian, 1% not otherwise listed). We found positive concurrent relations between EF and all six numerical skills examined: nonsymbolic magnitude comparison, verbal counting, numerical literacy, count on, non-rote counting, and numerical problem solving. There were unidirectional predictive relations between EF and four of the six numerical skills after controlling for covariates and prior performance on the skill of interest. Bidirectional relations were found only for EF and nonsymbolic magnitude comparison. We also found that the concurrent relation between EF and count on was higher for children with typical versus persistently low mathematics achievement. All other concurrent and predictive relations were similar for children with typical and persistently low mathematics achievement. Overall, these findings show that the relations between EF and numerical skills are both pervasive and nuanced, such that they vary by timing of assessments (i.e., concurrent or predictive) and numerical skill. These results can inform future theoretical models on the role of EF in numerical development and have practical implications for designing interventions targeting these skill sets in early childhood.
早期运算和执行功能(EF)技能的重要性已得到公认,每种技能都能积极而具体地预测日后的数学成绩、收入、中学后教育等。然而,人们对 EF 与计算技能之间的关系却知之甚少。因此,我们研究了学龄前儿童至三年级学生(人数=205;年龄4.67-8.75岁;43.9%为女性;51.2%为非西班牙裔白人;18%为多种族;6.3%为西班牙裔;12.2%为黑人;2%为美洲印第安人/阿拉斯加原住民;4.9%为亚裔;1%为未列名者)中的EF与计算技能之间的并发关系和预测关系。我们发现,EF 与所考察的所有六种数字技能之间均存在正并发关系:非符号大小比较、口头计数、数字识字、数数、非罗盘计数和数字问题解决。在控制了协变量和相关技能的先前表现后,EF 与六种数字技能中的四种之间存在单向预测关系。只有在 EF 和非符号大小比较中发现了双向关系。我们还发现,对于数学成绩典型的儿童和数学成绩持续低下的儿童来说,EF 和 "数数 "之间的并发关系更强。所有其他并发关系和预测关系对于数学成绩典型儿童和数学成绩持续低下儿童来说都是相似的。总之,这些研究结果表明,EF 与计算技能之间的关系既普遍又微妙,它们因评估的时间(即并发或预测)和计算技能而异。这些结果可以为未来建立关于幼儿情绪情感在运算能力发展中的作用的理论模型提供依据,并对设计针对幼儿期运算能力的干预措施具有实际意义。
{"title":"Concurrent and predictive associations between executive function and numerical skills in early childhood","authors":"Jasmine R. Ernst ,&nbsp;Michèle M.M. Mazzocco,&nbsp;Stephanie M. Carlson","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106113","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106113","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>The importance of early numerical and executive function (EF) skills is well-established, with each skill set positively and specifically predicting later mathematics achievement, income, postsecondary education, and more. Less is known, however, about the relations <em>between</em> EF and numerical skills. Therefore, we examined the concurrent and predictive relations between EF and numerical skills in preschoolers to third graders (<em>N</em> = 205; 4.67–8.75 years of age; 43.9% female; 51.2% White non-Hispanic, 18% multiracial, 6.3% Hispanic, 12.2% Black, 2% American Indian/Alaska Native, 4.9% Asian, 1% not otherwise listed). We found positive concurrent relations between EF and all six numerical skills examined: nonsymbolic magnitude comparison, verbal counting, numerical literacy, count on, non-rote counting, and numerical problem solving. There were unidirectional predictive relations between EF and four of the six numerical skills after controlling for covariates and prior performance on the skill of interest. Bidirectional relations were found only for EF and nonsymbolic magnitude comparison. We also found that the concurrent relation between EF and count on was higher for children with typical versus persistently low mathematics achievement. All other concurrent and predictive relations were similar for children with typical and persistently low mathematics achievement. Overall, these findings show that the relations between EF and numerical skills are both pervasive and nuanced, such that they vary by timing of assessments (i.e., concurrent or predictive) and numerical skill. These results can inform future theoretical models on the role of EF in numerical development and have practical implications for designing interventions targeting these skill sets in early childhood.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":"250 ","pages":"Article 106113"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-11-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142683081","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1