首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Child Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Early skills that predict English reading ability: A longitudinal study of bilingual children from 5 to 10 years 预测英语阅读能力的早期技能:对 5 至 10 岁双语儿童的纵向研究。
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-29 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105993
David Giguere , Michelle K. Tulloch , Cynthia Core , Erika Hoff

Despite substantial research, the contribution of oral language skills acquired in Spanish to Spanish–English bilingual children’s acquisition of English reading skill is unclear. The current study addressed this question with data on the oral language and pre-literacy skills of 101 Spanish–English bilingual learners at 5 years of age and their English word reading (i.e., decoding) and reading comprehension skills at 6, 7, 8, 9, and 10 years. Separate multilevel models using English language, Spanish language, and pre-literacy skills as predictors of these outcomes identified English phonological awareness, Spanish phonological awareness, and concepts of print knowledge as positive predictors of word reading. A final model including all these significant predictors found only Spanish phonological awareness and concept of print to be significant predictors. Significant predictors of reading comprehension in separate models were English vocabulary, Spanish phonological awareness, and concepts about print. In the final model, only English vocabulary and Spanish phonological awareness predicted English reading comprehension. These findings provide evidence that phonological awareness is a language-general skill that supports reading across languages, consistent with the common underlying proficiency model of bilingual reading development. The finding that only English vocabulary predicts English reading comprehension suggests that vocabulary knowledge is not part of a common underlying proficiency but is language specific in its value to reading ability.

尽管有大量的研究,但用西班牙语学习的口语技能对西班牙语-英语双语儿童掌握英语阅读技能的贡献尚不清楚。本研究利用 101 名西班牙语-英语双语学习者 5 岁时的口语和识字前技能数据,以及他们 6、7、8、9 和 10 岁时的英语单词阅读(即解码)和阅读理解技能数据,探讨了这一问题。使用英语语言、西班牙语语言和识字前技能作为这些结果的预测因素的独立多层次模型发现,英语语音意识、西班牙语语音意识和印刷知识概念对单词阅读有积极的预测作用。包括所有这些重要预测因素的最终模型发现,只有西班牙语语音意识和印刷概念是重要的预测因素。在不同的模型中,对阅读理解有显著预测作用的是英语词汇、西班牙语语音意识和印刷概念。在最终模型中,只有英语词汇和西班牙语语音意识能预测英语阅读理解能力。这些研究结果证明,语音意识是一种支持跨语言阅读的语言通用技能,这与双语阅读发展的共同基础能力模型是一致的。只有英语词汇能预测英语阅读理解能力的研究结果表明,词汇知识并不是共同基础能力的一部分,而是对阅读能力有特定价值的语言。
{"title":"Early skills that predict English reading ability: A longitudinal study of bilingual children from 5 to 10 years","authors":"David Giguere ,&nbsp;Michelle K. Tulloch ,&nbsp;Cynthia Core ,&nbsp;Erika Hoff","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105993","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105993","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Despite substantial research, the contribution of oral language skills acquired in Spanish to Spanish–English bilingual children’s acquisition of English reading skill is unclear. The current study addressed this question with data on the oral language and pre-literacy skills of 101 Spanish–English bilingual learners at 5 years of age and their English word reading (i.e., decoding) and reading comprehension skills at 6, 7, 8, 9, and 10 years. Separate multilevel models using English language, Spanish language, and pre-literacy skills as predictors of these outcomes identified English phonological awareness, Spanish phonological awareness, and concepts of print knowledge as positive predictors of word reading. A final model including all these significant predictors found only Spanish phonological awareness and concept of print to be significant predictors. Significant predictors of reading comprehension in separate models were English vocabulary, Spanish phonological awareness, and concepts about print. In the final model, only English vocabulary and Spanish phonological awareness predicted English reading comprehension. These findings provide evidence that phonological awareness is a language-general skill that supports reading across languages, consistent with the common underlying proficiency model of bilingual reading development. The finding that only English vocabulary predicts English reading comprehension suggests that vocabulary knowledge is not part of a common underlying proficiency but is language specific in its value to reading ability.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022096524001334/pdfft?md5=88e2f51d698ec08400e299e47af44799&pid=1-s2.0-S0022096524001334-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141471689","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The costs and benefits of kindness for kids 善待孩子的代价和益处
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-24 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105987
Oliver Scott Curry , Chloe San Miguel , Mehmet Necip Tunç

What do children think makes an act kind? Which kind acts are children likely to perform? Previous research with adults suggests that the kindness of acts depends largely on the benefit provided and to a lesser extent on the cost incurred, and that adults are more likely to perform low-cost, high-benefit kind acts. In the current study, children (9–12 years, n = 945) and teens (13–17 years, n = 939) rated the benefit, cost, kindness, and likelihood of performing 173 acts of kindness, and adults (18+ years, n = 891) rated how beneficial, costly, kind, and likely the acts would be for young people to perform. Among children and teens, benefit but not cost predicted the kindness of acts, and benefit positively predicted, but cost negatively predicted, performance (for “kindness quotients” of 61% and 65%, respectively). Among adults, benefit and cost predicted the kindness of acts, and cost, but not benefit, negatively predicted performance (for a kindness quotient of 59%). The results for children and teens are similar to those from previous research with adults; however, adults are more sensitive to cost when rating kindness, are less sensitive to benefit when rating performance by young people, and are less likely to think young people will perform acts of kindness overall. In practical terms, the results suggest that recommending cost-effective acts may be the best way to encourage children to be kinder.

儿童认为什么行为是善良的?儿童有可能做出哪些善举?以往对成年人的研究表明,行为是否善良主要取决于所提供的利益,其次才是所付出的代价,而且成年人更有可能做出低代价、高利益的善良行为。在本研究中,儿童(9-12 岁,945 人)和青少年(13-17 岁,939 人)对 173 种善举的益处、成本、善良程度和实施的可能性进行了评分,成年人(18 岁以上,891 人)对年轻人实施这些善举的益处、成本、善良程度和可能性进行了评分。在儿童和青少年中,收益而非成本可以预测行为的善意程度,收益可以预测行为的善意程度,而成本可以预测行为的善意程度("善意商数 "分别为 61% 和 65%)。在成人中,利益和成本都能预测行为的善意程度,而成本(而非利益)对表现有负面预测作用(善意商为 59%)。针对儿童和青少年的研究结果与之前针对成年人的研究结果相似;但是,成年人在评价善举时对成本更为敏感,在评价年轻人的表现时对收益不太敏感,而且总体上不太可能认为年轻人会做出善举。实际上,研究结果表明,推荐具有成本效益的行为可能是鼓励孩子变得更善良的最佳方式。
{"title":"The costs and benefits of kindness for kids","authors":"Oliver Scott Curry ,&nbsp;Chloe San Miguel ,&nbsp;Mehmet Necip Tunç","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105987","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105987","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>What do children think makes an act kind? Which kind acts are children likely to perform? Previous research with adults suggests that the kindness of acts depends largely on the benefit provided and to a lesser extent on the cost incurred, and that adults are more likely to perform low-cost, high-benefit kind acts. In the current study, children (9–12 years, <em>n</em> = 945) and teens (13–17 years, <em>n</em> = 939) rated the benefit, cost, kindness, and likelihood of performing 173 acts of kindness, and adults (18+ years, <em>n</em> = 891) rated how beneficial, costly, kind, and likely the acts would be for young people to perform. Among children and teens, benefit but <em>not</em> cost predicted the kindness of acts, and benefit positively predicted, but cost negatively predicted, performance (for “kindness quotients” of 61% and 65%, respectively). Among adults, benefit <em>and</em> cost predicted the kindness of acts, and cost, but <em>not</em> benefit, negatively predicted performance (for a kindness quotient of 59%). The results for children and teens are similar to those from previous research with adults; however, adults are more sensitive to cost when rating kindness, are less sensitive to benefit when rating performance by young people, and are less likely to think young people will perform acts of kindness overall. In practical terms, the results suggest that recommending cost-effective acts may be the best way to encourage children to be kinder.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141451927","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of family socioeconomic status on parental involvement and student engagement during COVID-19 in promoting academic achievement: A longitudinal study in Chinese children COVID-19期间家庭社会经济地位对家长参与和学生参与促进学业成绩的影响:中国儿童纵向研究。
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-24 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105992
Xin Chen , Yinghe Chen , Xiao Yu , Jun Wei , Xiujie Yang

With a three-wave longitudinal design, the current study examined the impact of family socioeconomic status (SES) on parental involvement and student engagement in promoting children’s academic achievement during the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic. We recruited data from 246 mother–primary school student dyads, and the mean age of children at Wave 1 was 10.57 ± 0.97 years (range = 9–13 years). The academic achievement of these children was measured both before and after school closures. Family SES, parental involvement, and student engagement were assessed during the school closures. The results indicated that family SES could predict children’s later academic achievement after accounting for their prior academic achievement and other demographics (i.e., the significant total effect in the model). Moreover, parental involvement and student engagement played chain-mediating roles in the effect of family SES on children’s later academic achievement. Neither parent involvement nor student involvement alone mediated the relationships between family SES and subsequent academic achievement. Suggestions are provided to minimize the negative impact of low family SES on children’s academic achievement during pandemics.

本研究采用三波纵向设计,考察了家庭社会经济地位(SES)对2019年冠状病毒病(COVID-19)大流行期间家长参与和学生参与促进儿童学业成绩的影响。我们从 246 个母亲-小学生二元组中收集了数据,第一波时儿童的平均年龄为 10.57 ± 0.97 岁(范围 = 9-13 岁)。这些儿童的学业成绩在学校关闭前和关闭后都进行了测量。在学校关闭期间,还对家庭经济状况、家长参与度和学生参与度进行了评估。结果表明,在考虑了儿童之前的学业成绩和其他人口统计学因素后(即模型中显著的总效应),家庭社会经济地位可以预测儿童日后的学业成绩。此外,在家庭 SES 对儿童日后学业成绩的影响中,家长参与和学生参与起到了连锁中介作用。无论是家长参与还是学生参与,都不能单独调解家庭 SES 与日后学业成绩之间的关系。本文提出了一些建议,以尽量减少大流行病期间低家庭社会经济地位对儿童学业成绩的负面影响。
{"title":"The impact of family socioeconomic status on parental involvement and student engagement during COVID-19 in promoting academic achievement: A longitudinal study in Chinese children","authors":"Xin Chen ,&nbsp;Yinghe Chen ,&nbsp;Xiao Yu ,&nbsp;Jun Wei ,&nbsp;Xiujie Yang","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105992","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105992","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>With a three-wave longitudinal design, the current study examined the impact of family socioeconomic status (SES) on parental involvement and student engagement in promoting children’s academic achievement during the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic. We recruited data from 246 mother–primary school student dyads, and the mean age of children at Wave 1 was 10.57 ± 0.97 years (range = 9–13 years). The academic achievement of these children was measured both before and after school closures. Family SES, parental involvement, and student engagement were assessed during the school closures. The results indicated that family SES could predict children’s later academic achievement after accounting for their prior academic achievement and other demographics (i.e., the significant total effect in the model). Moreover, parental involvement and student engagement played chain-mediating roles in the effect of family SES on children’s later academic achievement. Neither parent involvement nor student involvement alone mediated the relationships between family SES and subsequent academic achievement. Suggestions are provided to minimize the negative impact of low family SES on children’s academic achievement during pandemics.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141451928","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring relative strengths in people with Down syndrome: Spatial thinking and its role in mathematics 探索唐氏综合症患者的相对优势:空间思维及其在数学中的作用。
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-24 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105986
Su Morris , Emily K. Farran , Katie Gilligan-Lee

There is convincing evidence that training spatial abilities leads to improved mathematics performance in typically developing (TD) children. However, a lack of information on mathematical development and spatial–mathematical associations in people with Down syndrome (DS) hinders the translation of these interventions. Here, we established developmental trajectories of mathematics and explored whether spatial ability predicts attainment on different mathematics measures in individuals with DS. Participants with DS (n = 36; ages 9–35 years) and TD children (n = 132; ages 4–11 years) completed three groups of tasks: spatial tasks assessing different subdomains of spatial thinking; mathematics tasks assessing early mathematics skills, mathematical reasoning, arithmetic, and geometry; and IQ tasks. The developmental trajectories of mathematics performance against mental age revealed similar starting points of the trajectories and similar rates of development for DS and TD groups. Furthermore, after controlling for verbal skills, spatial skills explained 5.8% to 18.1% of the variation in mathematical performance across different mathematics tasks, and the pattern of spatial–mathematical relations was similar for DS and mental age-matched TD groups. This shows that mathematical development in DS groups appears to mirror that in TD children, indicative of delay only. Strong spatial–mathematical relations were observed for individuals with DS, like those seen for TD participants. This is the vital preliminary knowledge needed to support the design and use of spatial intervention for improving mathematics in individuals with DS.

有令人信服的证据表明,训练空间能力可以提高发育正常(TD)儿童的数学成绩。然而,缺乏有关唐氏综合症(DS)患者数学发展和空间数学关联的信息,阻碍了这些干预措施的转化。在此,我们建立了数学发展轨迹,并探讨了空间能力是否能预测唐氏综合症患者在不同数学测量中的成绩。DS 儿童(36 人,9-35 岁)和 TD 儿童(132 人,4-11 岁)完成了三组任务:评估空间思维不同子域的空间任务;评估早期数学技能、数学推理、算术和几何的数学任务;以及智商任务。数学成绩的发展轨迹与心理年龄的对比显示,DS 组和 TD 组的数学成绩发展轨迹的起点相似,发展速度也相似。此外,在控制言语技能后,空间技能解释了不同数学任务中数学成绩差异的5.8%至18.1%,而且DS组和智力年龄匹配的TD组的空间-数学关系模式相似。这表明,DS 组儿童的数学发展似乎与 TD 组儿童的数学发展相仿,仅有延迟的迹象。在 DS 群体中观察到了强烈的空间-数学关系,就像在 TD 参与者中观察到的一样。这是支持设计和使用空间干预来提高 DS 患者数学水平所需的重要初步知识。
{"title":"Exploring relative strengths in people with Down syndrome: Spatial thinking and its role in mathematics","authors":"Su Morris ,&nbsp;Emily K. Farran ,&nbsp;Katie Gilligan-Lee","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105986","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105986","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>There is convincing evidence that training spatial abilities leads to improved mathematics performance in typically developing (TD) children. However, a lack of information on mathematical development and spatial–mathematical associations in people with Down syndrome (DS) hinders the translation of these interventions. Here, we established developmental trajectories of mathematics and explored whether spatial ability predicts attainment on different mathematics measures in individuals with DS. Participants with DS (<em>n</em> = 36; ages 9–35 years) and TD children (<em>n</em> = 132; ages 4–11 years) completed three groups of tasks: spatial tasks assessing different subdomains of spatial thinking; mathematics tasks assessing early mathematics skills, mathematical reasoning, arithmetic, and geometry; and IQ tasks. The developmental trajectories of mathematics performance against mental age revealed similar starting points of the trajectories and similar rates of development for DS and TD groups. Furthermore, after controlling for verbal skills, spatial skills explained 5.8% to 18.1% of the variation in mathematical performance across different mathematics tasks, and the pattern of spatial–mathematical relations was similar for DS and mental age-matched TD groups. This shows that mathematical development in DS groups appears to mirror that in TD children, indicative of delay only. Strong spatial–mathematical relations were observed for individuals with DS, like those seen for TD participants. This is the vital preliminary knowledge needed to support the design and use of spatial intervention for improving mathematics in individuals with DS.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022096524001267/pdfft?md5=444a98aaab321930340c74278ed53c7d&pid=1-s2.0-S0022096524001267-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141451926","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Impacts of number lines and circle visual displays on caregivers’ fraction understanding 数线和圆圈视觉展示对保育员理解分数的影响。
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-22 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105983
Daniel A. Scheibe , Lauren Wyatt , Charles J. Fitzsimmons , Marta K. Mielicki , Lauren K. Schiller , Clarissa A. Thompson

Playful fraction picture books, together with math instructional content called “back matter,” may promote fraction learning, which is crucial because fractions are difficult and often disliked content. However, open questions remain regarding how different types of back matter may affect caregivers’ ability to use fraction picture books as a teaching tool. The current study offers a novel investigation into how back matter affects caregivers’ (N = 160) fraction understanding (i.e., equivalence and arithmetic) and subjective beliefs about math using a pretest/posttest design. We contrasted existing back matter text with research-informed back matter text crossed with either circle area or number line visual displays. Caregivers’ performance improved from pretest to posttest in the Researcher-Generated + Circles condition (fraction equivalence) and in the Existing + Circles, Researcher-Generated + Circles, and Researcher-Generated + Number Lines conditions (fraction arithmetic). In addition, caregivers were aware of their learning; they predicted improvements in their fraction arithmetic performance over time. These findings suggest that brief interventions, such as back matter in children’s picture books, may improve adults’ fraction understanding.

寓教于乐的分数图画书,加上被称为 "背景材料 "的数学教学内容,可以促进分数的学习,这一点至关重要,因为分数是很难学习的内容,而且往往不喜欢。然而,关于不同类型的背景材料会如何影响照顾者使用分数图画书作为教学工具的能力,仍然存在未决问题。本研究采用前测/后测设计,对背景材料如何影响保育员(160 人)对分数的理解(即等价和算术)以及对数学的主观信念进行了新颖的调查。我们将现有的背景材料文字与经过研究的背景材料文字以及圆圈区域或数字线视觉显示进行了对比。在 "研究者生成+圆圈 "条件下(分数等量关系),以及在 "现有+圆圈"、"研究者生成+圆圈 "和 "研究者生成+数字线 "条件下(分数运算),照顾者的成绩在前测和后测之间都有所提高。此外,保育员也意识到了自己的学习情况;他们预测自己的分数运算成绩会随着时间的推移而提高。这些研究结果表明,简短的干预措施(如儿童图画书的背面内容)可以提高成人对分数的理解。
{"title":"Impacts of number lines and circle visual displays on caregivers’ fraction understanding","authors":"Daniel A. Scheibe ,&nbsp;Lauren Wyatt ,&nbsp;Charles J. Fitzsimmons ,&nbsp;Marta K. Mielicki ,&nbsp;Lauren K. Schiller ,&nbsp;Clarissa A. Thompson","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105983","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105983","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Playful fraction picture books, together with math instructional content called “back matter,” may promote fraction learning, which is crucial because fractions are difficult and often disliked content. However, open questions remain regarding how different types of back matter may affect caregivers’ ability to use fraction picture books as a teaching tool. The current study offers a novel investigation into how back matter affects caregivers’ (<em>N</em> = 160) fraction understanding (i.e., equivalence and arithmetic) and subjective beliefs about math using a pretest/posttest design. We contrasted existing back matter text with research-informed back matter text crossed with either circle area or number line visual displays. Caregivers’ performance improved from pretest to posttest in the Researcher-Generated + Circles condition (fraction equivalence) and in the Existing + Circles, Researcher-Generated + Circles, and Researcher-Generated + Number Lines conditions (fraction arithmetic). In addition, caregivers were aware of their learning; they predicted improvements in their fraction arithmetic performance over time. These findings suggest that brief interventions, such as back matter in children’s picture books, may improve adults’ fraction understanding.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141443573","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Episodic foresight, episodic memory, and executive functions in children engaged with Child Protective Services: The role of cumulative risk 接受儿童保护服务的儿童的外显预见、外显记忆和执行功能:累积风险的作用
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-22 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105985
Sanmya Salomão, Ana Catarina Canário, Orlanda Cruz

Previous studies have determined that exposure to risk and adversities may impair children’s cognitive abilities. In particular, children engaged with Child Protective Services (CPS) seem to be at greater risk for enhanced detrimental effects resulting from the cumulative risk factors to which they are exposed. However, little is known about children’s future thinking when they face adverse circumstances, and it is not clear how the associations among episodic foresight abilities, episodic memory, and executive functions work with children under such circumstances. The current study describes the episodic foresight abilities of CPS-involved school-aged children, its association with other cognitive abilities, and how this association is affected by the exposure to cumulative risk and adversity factors. Episodic foresight, episodic memory, executive functions, and a composite of cumulative risk factors were analyzed in a sample of 95 school-aged children engaged with CPS in Portugal. Results suggest the detrimental effect of cumulative risk on the episodic foresight abilities of CPS-involved children. Episodic memory and cognitive flexibility were significant predictors of episodic foresight abilities, and cumulative risk exposure moderated the relation between episodic memory and episodic foresight. The current study provides a better understanding of the influences of multiple adversities on CPS-involved children’s episodic foresight abilities and related cognitive outcomes.

以往的研究表明,暴露在风险和逆境中可能会损害儿童的认知能力。特别是,接受儿童保护服务(CPS)的儿童似乎面临着更大的风险,他们所面临的累积风险因素会对他们造成更大的不利影响。然而,人们对儿童在面临不利环境时的未来思维知之甚少,也不清楚在这种情况下,儿童的外显预见能力、外显记忆和执行功能之间的关联是如何发挥作用的。本研究描述了涉及 CPS 的学龄儿童的外显预见能力、外显预见能力与其他认知能力之间的关联,以及这种关联如何受到累积风险和逆境因素的影响。我们对葡萄牙 95 名参与 CPS 的学龄儿童进行了外显预见、外显记忆、执行功能和累积风险因素综合分析。结果表明,累积性风险对参与 CPS 的儿童的外显预见能力有不利影响。外显记忆和认知灵活性是外显预见能力的重要预测因素,累积性风险暴露调节了外显记忆和外显预见能力之间的关系。本研究有助于更好地了解多重逆境对涉及 CPS 的儿童的外显预见能力及相关认知结果的影响。
{"title":"Episodic foresight, episodic memory, and executive functions in children engaged with Child Protective Services: The role of cumulative risk","authors":"Sanmya Salomão,&nbsp;Ana Catarina Canário,&nbsp;Orlanda Cruz","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105985","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105985","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous studies have determined that exposure to risk and adversities may impair children’s cognitive abilities. In particular, children engaged with Child Protective Services (CPS) seem to be at greater risk for enhanced detrimental effects resulting from the cumulative risk factors to which they are exposed. However, little is known about children’s future thinking when they face adverse circumstances, and it is not clear how the associations among episodic foresight abilities, episodic memory, and executive functions work with children under such circumstances. The current study describes the episodic foresight abilities of CPS-involved school-aged children, its association with other cognitive abilities, and how this association is affected by the exposure to cumulative risk and adversity factors. Episodic foresight, episodic memory, executive functions, and a composite of cumulative risk factors were analyzed in a sample of 95 school-aged children engaged with CPS in Portugal. Results suggest the detrimental effect of cumulative risk on the episodic foresight abilities of CPS-involved children. Episodic memory and cognitive flexibility were significant predictors of episodic foresight abilities, and cumulative risk exposure moderated the relation between episodic memory and episodic foresight. The current study provides a better understanding of the influences of multiple adversities on CPS-involved children’s episodic foresight abilities and related cognitive outcomes.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022096524001255/pdfft?md5=912713a6c55761f5ab605b72d4c354fa&pid=1-s2.0-S0022096524001255-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141443572","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Malleability of the sense of bodily self in early childhood: 5- and 6-year-old children children show the enfacement illusion 幼儿期身体自我感觉的可塑性:5-6 岁儿童的 "贴面错觉"。
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-22 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105990
Konstantin Steinmassl, Markus Paulus

The mechanisms underlying the developing sense of bodily self are debated. Whereas some scholars stress the role of sensory factors, others propose the importance of contextual factors. By manipulating multisensory stimulation and social familiarity with the other person, we explored two factors that are proposed to relate to young children’s developing sense of bodily self. Including an adult sample allowed us to investigate age-related differences of the malleability of the bodily self. To this end, the study implemented an enfacement illusion with children (N = 64) and adults (N = 33). Participants were exposed to one trial with synchronous interpersonal multisensory stimulation and one trial with asynchronous interpersonal multisensory stimulation—either with a stranger or with the mother as the other person. A self-recognition task using morph videos of self and other and an enfacement questionnaire were implemented as dependent measures. Results revealed evidence for the presence of the enfacement effect in children in both measures. The identity of the other person had a significant effect on the self-recognition task. Contrary to our hypothesis, the effect was significantly smaller in the caregiver condition. No significant differences between children and adults emerged. Our results demonstrate the role of both multisensory stimulation and contextual—here social familiarity—factors for the construction and development of a bodily self. The study provides developmental science with a novel approach to the bodily self by showing the validity of the self-recognition task in a child sample. Overall, the study supports proposals that the sense of bodily self is malleable early in development.

关于身体自我意识发展的内在机制存在争议。一些学者强调感官因素的作用,而另一些学者则提出了环境因素的重要性。通过操纵多感官刺激和与他人的社会熟悉度,我们探索了与幼儿身体自我意识发展相关的两个因素。成人样本的加入使我们能够研究身体自我可塑性的年龄差异。为此,本研究对儿童(64 人)和成人(33 人)实施了 "置入幻觉"。受试者分别接受了一次同步人际多感官刺激试验和一次异步人际多感官刺激试验--要么是陌生人,要么是母亲作为对方。使用自我和他人的变形视频进行的自我认知任务以及安置问卷作为因果测量。结果显示,在这两项测量中,都有证据表明儿童存在 "依附效应"。他人的身份对自我认知任务有显著影响。与我们的假设相反,这种效应在照顾者条件下明显较小。儿童和成人之间没有出现明显差异。我们的研究结果表明,多感官刺激和情境--这里是社会熟悉度--这两个因素对身体自我的构建和发展都有作用。这项研究通过在儿童样本中展示自我认知任务的有效性,为发展科学提供了一种研究身体自我的新方法。总之,该研究支持了身体自我意识在发育早期具有可塑性的观点。
{"title":"Malleability of the sense of bodily self in early childhood: 5- and 6-year-old children children show the enfacement illusion","authors":"Konstantin Steinmassl,&nbsp;Markus Paulus","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105990","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105990","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The mechanisms underlying the developing sense of bodily self are debated. Whereas some scholars stress the role of sensory factors, others propose the importance of contextual factors. By manipulating multisensory stimulation and social familiarity with the other person, we explored two factors that are proposed to relate to young children’s developing sense of bodily self. Including an adult sample allowed us to investigate age-related differences of the malleability of the bodily self. To this end, the study implemented an enfacement illusion with children (<em>N</em> = 64) and adults (<em>N</em> = 33). Participants were exposed to one trial with synchronous interpersonal multisensory stimulation and one trial with asynchronous interpersonal multisensory stimulation—either with a stranger or with the mother as the other person. A self-recognition task using morph videos of self and other and an enfacement questionnaire were implemented as dependent measures. Results revealed evidence for the presence of the enfacement effect in children in both measures. The identity of the other person had a significant effect on the self-recognition task. Contrary to our hypothesis, the effect was significantly smaller in the caregiver condition. No significant differences between children and adults emerged. Our results demonstrate the role of both multisensory stimulation and contextual—here social familiarity—factors for the construction and development of a bodily self. The study provides developmental science with a novel approach to the bodily self by showing the validity of the self-recognition task in a child sample. Overall, the study supports proposals that the sense of bodily self is malleable early in development.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022096524001309/pdfft?md5=540e4ef9b0d83d17e00b9dd0f9b00e91&pid=1-s2.0-S0022096524001309-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141443574","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The influence of language on the formation of number concepts: Evidence from preschool children who are bilingual in English and Arabic 语言对数字概念形成的影响:来自学龄前英语和阿拉伯语双语儿童的证据
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-19 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105988
Rima Alhaider, Merle Mahon, Chris Donlan

We asked whether grammatical number marking has specific influence on the formation of early number concepts. In particular, does comprehension of dual case marking support young children’s understanding of cardinality? We assessed number knowledge in 77 3-year-old Arabic–English bilingual children using the Give-a-Number task in both languages. Given recent concerns around the administration and scoring of the Give-a-Number task, we used two complementary approaches: one based on conceptual levels and the other based on overall test scores. We also tested comprehension of dual case marking in Arabic and number sequence knowledge in both languages. Regression analyses showed that dual case comprehension exerts a strong influence on cardinality tested in Arabic independent of age, general language skills, and number sequence knowledge. No such influence was found for cardinality tested in English, indicating a language-specific effect. Further analyses tested for transfer of cardinality knowledge between languages. These revealed, in addition to the findings outlined above, a powerful cross-linguistic transfer effect. Our findings are consistent with a model in which the direct effect of dual case marking is language specific, but concepts, once acquired, may be represented abstractly and transferred between languages.

我们的问题是,语法数字标记是否对早期数字概念的形成有特定影响。特别是,对双例标记的理解是否有助于幼儿对万有引力的理解?我们用两种语言的 "给一个数"(Give-a-Number)任务评估了 77 名 3 岁阿拉伯语和英语双语儿童的数字知识。鉴于最近对 "给一个数 "任务的管理和评分的关注,我们采用了两种互补的方法:一种是基于概念水平,另一种是基于测试总分。我们还测试了阿拉伯语的双例标记理解能力和两种语言的数列知识。回归分析表明,阿拉伯语的双例理解能力对心算测试有很大的影响,与年龄、一般语言技能和数列知识无关。而用英语进行的心算测试则没有发现这种影响,这表明存在语言特异性效应。进一步的分析检验了心算知识在不同语言之间的迁移。除了上述发现外,这些分析还揭示了一种强大的跨语言迁移效应。我们的研究结果符合这样一个模型,即双例标记的直接效应是特定于语言的,但概念一旦获得,就可以抽象地表示出来,并在不同语言之间迁移。
{"title":"The influence of language on the formation of number concepts: Evidence from preschool children who are bilingual in English and Arabic","authors":"Rima Alhaider,&nbsp;Merle Mahon,&nbsp;Chris Donlan","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105988","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105988","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We asked whether grammatical number marking has specific influence on the formation of early number concepts. In particular, does comprehension of dual case marking support young children’s understanding of cardinality? We assessed number knowledge in 77 3-year-old Arabic–English bilingual children using the Give-a-Number task in both languages. Given recent concerns around the administration and scoring of the Give-a-Number task, we used two complementary approaches: one based on conceptual levels and the other based on overall test scores. We also tested comprehension of dual case marking in Arabic and number sequence knowledge in both languages. Regression analyses showed that dual case comprehension exerts a strong influence on cardinality tested in Arabic independent of age, general language skills, and number sequence knowledge. No such influence was found for cardinality tested in English, indicating a language-specific effect. Further analyses tested for transfer of cardinality knowledge between languages. These revealed, in addition to the findings outlined above, a powerful cross-linguistic transfer effect. Our findings are consistent with a model in which the direct effect of dual case marking is language specific, but concepts, once acquired, may be represented abstractly and transferred between languages.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141424529","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Contribution of orthography to vocabulary acquisition in a second language: Evidence of an early word-learning advantage in elementary-school children 正字法对第二语言词汇学习的贡献:小学生早期词汇学习优势的证据。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-17 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105978
Florian Salomé , Eva Commissaire , Séverine Casalis

Recent studies have shown that children benefit from orthography when learning new words. This orthographic facilitation can be explained by the fact that written language acts as an anchor device due to the transient nature of spoken language. There is also a close and reciprocal relationship between spoken and written language. Second-language word learning poses specific challenges in terms of orthography–phonology mappings that do not fully overlap with first-language mappings. The current study aimed to investigate whether orthographic information facilitates second-language word learning in developing readers, namely third and fifth graders. In a first experiment French children learned 16 German words, and in a second experiment they learned 24 German words. Word learning was assessed by picture designation, spoken word recognition, and orthographic choice. In both experiments, orthographic facilitation was found in both less and more advanced readers. The theoretical and practical implications of these findings are discussed.

最近的研究表明,儿童在学习新单词时受益于正字法。这种正字法的促进作用可以解释为,由于口语的短暂性,书面语言起到了锚定工具的作用。口语和书面语之间也存在密切的互惠关系。第二语言单词学习在正字法-语音映射方面面临特殊挑战,因为正字法-语音映射与第一语言映射并不完全重合。本研究旨在探讨正字法信息是否有助于发展中读者(即三年级和五年级学生)的第二语言单词学习。在第一项实验中,法国儿童学习了 16 个德语单词;在第二项实验中,他们学习了 24 个德语单词。单词学习通过图片指定、口语单词识别和正字法选择进行评估。在这两项实验中,发现正字法对低级和高级阅读者都有促进作用。本文讨论了这些发现的理论和实践意义。
{"title":"Contribution of orthography to vocabulary acquisition in a second language: Evidence of an early word-learning advantage in elementary-school children","authors":"Florian Salomé ,&nbsp;Eva Commissaire ,&nbsp;Séverine Casalis","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105978","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105978","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Recent studies have shown that children benefit from orthography when learning new words. This orthographic facilitation can be explained by the fact that written language acts as an anchor device due to the transient nature of spoken language. There is also a close and reciprocal relationship between spoken and written language. Second-language word learning poses specific challenges in terms of orthography–phonology mappings that do not fully overlap with first-language mappings. The current study aimed to investigate whether orthographic information facilitates second-language word learning in developing readers, namely third and fifth graders. In a first experiment French children learned 16 German words, and in a second experiment they learned 24 German words. Word learning was assessed by picture designation, spoken word recognition, and orthographic choice. In both experiments, orthographic facilitation was found in both less and more advanced readers. The theoretical and practical implications of these findings are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022096524001188/pdfft?md5=a87d31f83ebb9960bd742b387f130cea&pid=1-s2.0-S0022096524001188-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141421474","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dragging but not tapping promotes preschoolers’ numerical estimating with touchscreens 拖动而不是点击能促进学龄前儿童使用触摸屏进行数字估算。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-17 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105989
Yunyi Wu , Xinyun Cao , Mark Nielsen , Yichen Mao , Fuxing Wang

When solving mathematical problems, young children will perform better when they can use gestures that match mental representations. However, despite their increasing prevalence in educational settings, few studies have explored this effect in touchscreen-based interactions. Thus, we investigated the impact on young children’s performance of dragging (where a continuous gesture is performed that is congruent with the change in number) and tapping (involving a discrete gesture that is incongruent) on a touchscreen device when engaged in a continuous number line estimation task. By examining differences in the set size and position of the number line estimation, we were also able to explore the boundary conditions for the superiority effect of congruent gestures. We used a 2 (Gesture Type: drag or tap) × 2 (Set Size: Set 0–10 or Set 0–20) × 2 (Position: left of midpoint or right of midpoint) mixed design. A total of 70 children aged 5 and 6 years (33 girls) were recruited and randomly assigned to either the Drag or Tap group. We found that the congruent gesture (drag) generally facilitated better performance with the touchscreen but with boundary conditions. When completing difficult estimations (right side in the large set size), the Drag group was more accurate, responded to the stimulus faster, and spent more time manipulating than the Tap group. These findings suggest that when children require explicit scaffolding, congruent touchscreen gestures help to release mental resources for strategic adjustments, decrease the difficulty of numerical estimation, and support constructing mental representations.

在解决数学问题时,如果能使用与心理表征相匹配的手势,幼儿的表现会更好。然而,尽管这种手势在教育环境中越来越普遍,却很少有研究探讨这种手势在触摸屏互动中的效果。因此,我们研究了在进行连续数列估算任务时,在触摸屏设备上拖动(使用与数字变化一致的连续手势)和点击(使用不一致的离散手势)对幼儿表现的影响。通过研究数字线估算的集合大小和位置差异,我们还能够探索一致手势优势效应的边界条件。我们采用了 2(手势类型:拖动或点击)×2(集合大小:集合 0-10 或集合 0-20)×2(位置:中点左侧或中点右侧)混合设计。我们共招募了 70 名 5 至 6 岁的儿童(33 名女孩),并将他们随机分配到拖动组或点击组。我们发现,在使用触摸屏时,一致手势(拖动)通常有助于提高成绩,但有边界条件。在完成难度较大的估算时(大集合中的右侧),拖动组比敲击组更准确,对刺激的反应更快,花费的操作时间更多。这些研究结果表明,当儿童需要明确的支架时,一致的触摸屏手势有助于释放心理资源进行策略调整,降低数字估算的难度,并支持构建心理表征。
{"title":"Dragging but not tapping promotes preschoolers’ numerical estimating with touchscreens","authors":"Yunyi Wu ,&nbsp;Xinyun Cao ,&nbsp;Mark Nielsen ,&nbsp;Yichen Mao ,&nbsp;Fuxing Wang","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105989","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.105989","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>When solving mathematical problems, young children will perform better when they can use gestures that match mental representations. However, despite their increasing prevalence in educational settings, few studies have explored this effect in touchscreen-based interactions. Thus, we investigated the impact on young children’s performance of dragging (where a continuous gesture is performed that is congruent with the change in number) and tapping (involving a discrete gesture that is incongruent) on a touchscreen device when engaged in a continuous number line estimation task. By examining differences in the set size and position of the number line estimation, we were also able to explore the boundary conditions for the superiority effect of congruent gestures. We used a 2 (Gesture Type: drag or tap) × 2 (Set Size: Set 0–10 or Set 0–20) × 2 (Position: left of midpoint or right of midpoint) mixed design. A total of 70 children aged 5 and 6 years (33 girls) were recruited and randomly assigned to either the Drag or Tap group. We found that the congruent gesture (drag) generally facilitated better performance with the touchscreen but with boundary conditions. When completing difficult estimations (right side in the large set size), the Drag group was more accurate, responded to the stimulus faster, and spent more time manipulating than the Tap group. These findings suggest that when children require explicit scaffolding, congruent touchscreen gestures help to release mental resources for strategic adjustments, decrease the difficulty of numerical estimation, and support constructing mental representations.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141421475","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1