首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Child Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Children weigh internet inaccuracy when trusting in online information 儿童在信任网络信息时会权衡网络的不准确性
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-17 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106105
Yu Tong , Judith H. Danovitch , Fuxing Wang , Weijun Wang
This study examined whether an internet source’s history of inaccuracy influences children’s epistemic trust in online information. Chinese children aged 4 to 8 years (N = 84; 41 girls and 43 boys) accessed information on their own from an image-based website, heard information from the internet that was relayed by an adult, or viewed a person in a video providing information without referring to the internet (in a baseline condition). After the internet source provided three obviously inaccurate statements, children significantly reduced their epistemic trust in the internet source regardless of whether they obtained the information through a direct interaction with the internet or it was relayed by an adult. Moreover, the extent of the reduction in trust was comparable to the baseline video condition. Taken together, these findings demonstrate that 4- to 8-year-old children take into account a history of inaccuracy and revise their beliefs in statements from the internet, just as they do when evaluating human informants.
本研究探讨了网络信息来源的不准确历史是否会影响儿童对网络信息的认识信任。4至8岁的中国儿童(人数=84;女孩41人,男孩43人)自己从一个基于图像的网站上获取信息,或从互联网上听到由成人转述的信息,或观看一个人在视频中提供的信息,但没有提及互联网(基线条件)。在互联网信息源提供了三次明显不准确的陈述后,儿童对互联网信息源的认识信任度明显降低,无论他们是通过与互联网的直接互动获得信息,还是通过成人转述获得信息。此外,信任度降低的程度与基线视频条件相当。综上所述,这些研究结果表明,4 到 8 岁的儿童在评估人类信息提供者时,也会考虑到不准确的历史记录,并修正他们对来自互联网的陈述的信念。
{"title":"Children weigh internet inaccuracy when trusting in online information","authors":"Yu Tong ,&nbsp;Judith H. Danovitch ,&nbsp;Fuxing Wang ,&nbsp;Weijun Wang","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106105","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106105","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>This study examined whether an internet source’s history of inaccuracy influences children’s epistemic trust in online information. Chinese children aged 4 to 8 years (<em>N</em> = 84; 41 girls and 43 boys) accessed information on their own from an image-based website, heard information from the internet that was relayed by an adult, or viewed a person in a video providing information without referring to the internet (in a baseline condition). After the internet source provided three obviously inaccurate statements, children significantly reduced their epistemic trust in the internet source regardless of whether they obtained the information through a direct interaction with the internet or it was relayed by an adult. Moreover, the extent of the reduction in trust was comparable to the baseline video condition. Taken together, these findings demonstrate that 4- to 8-year-old children take into account a history of inaccuracy and revise their beliefs in statements from the internet, just as they do when evaluating human informants.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":"249 ","pages":"Article 106105"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142444977","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effects of prosody and referent characteristics on novel noun learning in children 拟声词和指代特征对儿童学习新名词的影响
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-15 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106104
Julie M. Hupp , Melissa K. Jungers , Samantha A. McDonald , Yujin Song
Prosody is how words are spoken, often affecting the messages we convey. When prosody is relevant to the meaning, word learning is enhanced; however, it is unknown whether children attend to prosody that is not relevant to a word learning task (e.g., fearful for no reason). Previous research with adult noun learning showed that some emotional prosodies (e.g., warning) decreased word learning in comparison with a neutral Name prosody, demonstrating adults’ inability to ignore this irrelevant information. Given preschool children’s developing abilities to use prosody, the current research examined the effect of emotional prosody on children’s novel noun learning. In this study, preschool children (N = 67) were trained on novel labels paired to novel referents across five prosodic categories. Results suggest that referent complexity, animacy status, and prosody affect children’s noun learning, but in a different way than they affect adults. Prosody affected learning labels only for simple inanimate referents, with Happy and Fear prosodies being more accurate than the neutral Name. Whereas adults in previous research have been seemingly distracted by emotional prosody in learning nouns, children’s novel noun learning for simple objects was enhanced by Happy and Fear prosodies. This demonstrates the potential benefits that emotional prosody may have on children’s word learning.
拟声词是说话的方式,往往会影响我们传达的信息。当拟声词与词义相关时,单词学习会得到加强;然而,儿童是否会注意与单词学习任务无关的拟声词(如无缘无故的恐惧),目前尚不清楚。之前对成人名词学习的研究表明,与中性的 "名称 "拟声词相比,一些情绪性拟声词(如警告)会降低单词学习效果,这表明成人无法忽略这些不相关的信息。鉴于学龄前儿童使用拟声词的能力正在发展,本研究考察了情绪拟声词对儿童新名词学习的影响。在这项研究中,学龄前儿童(N = 67)接受了与五种拟声类别中的新名词配对的新标签训练。结果表明,指代复杂性、动画状态和拟声词会影响儿童的名词学习,但影响方式与成人不同。拟声词只对简单的无生命指代的学习标签有影响,其中 "快乐 "和 "恐惧 "的拟声词比中性的 "名称 "更准确。在以往的研究中,成人在学习名词时似乎会受到情绪拟声词的干扰,而儿童在学习简单物体的新名词时,快乐和恐惧拟声词会增强他们的学习效果。这说明情绪拟声对儿童的词语学习可能有潜在的好处。
{"title":"The effects of prosody and referent characteristics on novel noun learning in children","authors":"Julie M. Hupp ,&nbsp;Melissa K. Jungers ,&nbsp;Samantha A. McDonald ,&nbsp;Yujin Song","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106104","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106104","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Prosody is <em>how</em> words are spoken, often affecting the messages we convey. When prosody is relevant to the meaning, word learning is enhanced; however, it is unknown whether children attend to prosody that is not relevant to a word learning task (e.g., fearful for no reason). Previous research with adult noun learning showed that some emotional prosodies (e.g., warning) decreased word learning in comparison with a neutral Name prosody, demonstrating adults’ inability to ignore this irrelevant information. Given preschool children’s developing abilities to use prosody, the current research examined the effect of emotional prosody on children’s novel noun learning. In this study, preschool children (<em>N</em> = 67) were trained on novel labels paired to novel referents across five prosodic categories. Results suggest that referent complexity, animacy status, and prosody affect children’s noun learning, but in a different way than they affect adults. Prosody affected learning labels only for simple inanimate referents, with Happy and Fear prosodies being more accurate than the neutral Name. Whereas adults in previous research have been seemingly distracted by emotional prosody in learning nouns, children’s novel noun learning for simple objects was enhanced by Happy and Fear prosodies. This demonstrates the potential benefits that emotional prosody may have on children’s word learning.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":"249 ","pages":"Article 106104"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142441281","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Are students’ math and verbal motivational beliefs malleable? The role of praise in dimensional comparisons 学生的数学和语言动机信念是可塑的吗?表扬在维度比较中的作用。
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-10 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106100
Kexin Ren , Amanda Grenell , Elizabeth A. Gunderson
To determine their academic strengths and weaknesses, students compare their own performance across domains (e.g., math vs. English), a process referred to as dimensional comparisons. For example, individuals’ higher-scoring English performance may negatively affect their math motivational beliefs (competence self-concepts and intrinsic values), resulting in favoritism toward English. Students’ motivation can also be affected by praise from adults. However, praise in one domain (e.g., English) may have unexpected negative effects on motivation in the contrasting domain (e.g., math) through dimensional comparisons. We experimentally investigated the impact of receiving praise in only one domain on students’ domain-specific motivational beliefs. We hypothesized that students would have higher motivational beliefs in the praised domain and lower motivational beliefs in the non-praised domain compared with students who received no praise. Seventh- to ninth-graders (10- to 15-year-olds; N = 108; 46 girls; 92 living in the United States; 84.8% White, 2.9% Asian or Asian American, 2.9% Black or African American, 9.5% multiple races; parents’ education range: 13–18 years) showed heightened verbal competence self-concepts after receiving praise on either verbal or math performance. College students (first to fifth year; N = 109; 89 women; 105 living in the United States; 58.9% White, 21.5% Asian or Asian American, 10.3% Black or African American, 5.6% multiple races, 3.7% other races) showed higher verbal intrinsic values after receiving praise on verbal performance. Results supported positive effects of praise in the verbal domain only and were inconsistent with the predicted negative effects on the non-praised domain. We suggest that students’ verbal motivational beliefs are more malleable than math beliefs when receiving disproportionate praise.
为了确定自己的学业优势和劣势,学生会比较自己在不同领域(如数学和英语)的表现,这一过程被称为维度比较。例如,个人较高的英语成绩可能会对其数学动机信念(能力自我概念和内在价值)产生负面影响,从而导致偏爱英语。成人的表扬也会影响学生的学习动机。然而,通过维度比较,一个领域(如英语)的表扬可能会对相反领域(如数学)的学习动机产生意想不到的负面影响。我们通过实验研究了只在一个领域接受表扬对学生特定领域动机信念的影响。我们假设,与未受到表扬的学生相比,受到表扬的学生在受表扬领域的动机信念会更高,而未受到表扬的学生在受表扬领域的动机信念会更低。七至九年级学生(10 至 15 岁;人数=108;46 名女生;92 名居住在美国;84.8% 为白人,2.9% 为亚裔或亚裔美国人,2.9% 为黑人或非裔美国人,9.5% 为多种族;父母受教育年限在 13 至 18 年之间)表现出更高的口头表达能力:13-18岁)在口头表达或数学成绩方面受到表扬后,表现出更强的口头表达能力自我概念。大学生(一年级至五年级;人数=109;89 名女性;105 名居住在美国;58.9% 白人,21.5% 亚洲人或亚裔美国人,10.3% 黑人或非洲裔美国人,5.6% 多种族,3.7% 其他种族)在口头表达表现方面受到表扬后,表现出更高的口头表达内在价值。研究结果表明,表扬只在言语领域产生积极影响,而在非表扬领域则与预测的消极影响不一致。我们认为,在受到不相称的表扬时,学生的言语动机信念比数学信念更具可塑性。
{"title":"Are students’ math and verbal motivational beliefs malleable? The role of praise in dimensional comparisons","authors":"Kexin Ren ,&nbsp;Amanda Grenell ,&nbsp;Elizabeth A. Gunderson","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106100","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106100","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>To determine their academic strengths and weaknesses, students compare their own performance across domains (e.g., math vs. English), a process referred to as <em>dimensional comparisons.</em> For example, individuals’ higher-scoring English performance may negatively affect their math motivational beliefs (competence self-concepts and intrinsic values), resulting in favoritism toward English. Students’ motivation can also be affected by praise from adults. However, praise in one domain (e.g., English) may have unexpected negative effects on motivation in the contrasting domain (e.g., math) through dimensional comparisons. We experimentally investigated the impact of receiving praise in only one domain on students’ domain-specific motivational beliefs. We hypothesized that students would have higher motivational beliefs in the praised domain and lower motivational beliefs in the non-praised domain compared with students who received no praise. Seventh- to ninth-graders (10- to 15-year-olds; <em>N</em> = 108; 46 girls; 92 living in the United States; 84.8% White, 2.9% Asian or Asian American, 2.9% Black or African American, 9.5% multiple races; parents’ education range: 13–18 years) showed heightened verbal competence self-concepts after receiving praise on either verbal or math performance. College students (first to fifth year; <em>N</em> = 109; 89 women; 105 living in the United States; 58.9% White, 21.5% Asian or Asian American, 10.3% Black or African American, 5.6% multiple races, 3.7% other races) showed higher verbal intrinsic values after receiving praise on verbal performance. Results supported positive effects of praise in the verbal domain only and were inconsistent with the predicted negative effects on the non-praised domain. We suggest that students’ verbal motivational beliefs are more malleable than math beliefs when receiving disproportionate praise.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":"249 ","pages":"Article 106100"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142401636","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Three- and four-year-old children represent mutually exclusive possible identities 三岁和四岁的孩子代表着相互排斥的可能身份。
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-07 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106078
Esra Nur Turan-Küçük , Melissa M. Kibbe
How do children think about and plan for possible outcomes of events that could happen in the future? Previous work that has investigated children’s ability to think about mutually exclusive possibilities has largely focused on children’s reasoning about one type of possibility—the possible locations of an object. Here, we investigated children’s reasoning about another type of possibility—mutually exclusive possible identities. In two experiments (N = 201 U.S. 3- and 4-year-olds), children were told that two animal characters (e.g., a bunny and a monkey) were going to take turns sliding down a playground slide. Children were told that the animals wanted to eat their favorite foods (e.g., carrots and bananas, respectively) as soon as they got to the bottom of the slide. In an Unambiguous Identity condition, we told children the identity of the animal that would slide down. In an Ambiguous Identity condition, we told children that which animal would slide down first was unknown. To examine children’s representations of possible identities, we asked children to “get snack ready.” We found that children in the Unambiguous Identity condition selected only one of the snacks (i.e., the favorite snack of the animal they were told would slide down), whereas children in the Ambiguous Identity condition selected both snacks, suggesting that they were accounting for both possible identities. These results extend the literature on the development of modal reasoning to include reasoning about possible identities and suggest that this ability may be available to children as young as 3 years.
儿童如何思考和计划未来可能发生的事件的可能结果?以往研究儿童思考相互排斥可能性的能力的工作主要集中在儿童对一种可能性--物体的可能位置--的推理上。在这里,我们研究了儿童对另一种可能性--相互排斥的可能身份--的推理。在两个实验中(N = 201 名美国 3-4 岁儿童),儿童被告知两个动物角色(如兔子和猴子)将轮流从游乐场的滑梯上滑下。孩子们被告知,一旦滑到滑梯底部,动物们就想吃自己最喜欢的食物(如胡萝卜和香蕉)。在 "身份不明确 "条件下,我们告诉儿童将滑下来的动物的身份。在 "身份不明确 "条件下,我们告诉儿童不知道哪种动物会先滑下来。为了考察儿童对可能身份的表征,我们要求儿童 "准备好零食"。我们发现,在 "身份不明确 "条件下,儿童只选择了其中一种零食(即他们被告知会滑下来的动物最喜欢的零食),而在 "身份模糊 "条件下,儿童同时选择了两种零食,这表明他们同时考虑了两种可能的身份。这些结果扩展了模态推理发展的文献,将可能身份的推理也包括在内,并表明3岁的儿童就可能具备这种能力。
{"title":"Three- and four-year-old children represent mutually exclusive possible identities","authors":"Esra Nur Turan-Küçük ,&nbsp;Melissa M. Kibbe","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106078","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106078","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>How do children think about and plan for possible outcomes of events that <em>could</em> happen in the future? Previous work that has investigated children’s ability to think about mutually exclusive possibilities has largely focused on children’s reasoning about one type of possibility—the possible locations of an object. Here, we investigated children’s reasoning about another type of possibility—mutually exclusive possible identities. In two experiments (<em>N =</em> 201 U.S. 3- and 4-year-olds), children were told that two animal characters (e.g., a bunny and a monkey) were going to take turns sliding down a playground slide. Children were told that the animals wanted to eat their favorite foods (e.g., carrots and bananas, respectively) as soon as they got to the bottom of the slide. In an Unambiguous Identity condition, we told children the identity of the animal that would slide down. In an Ambiguous Identity condition, we told children that which animal would slide down first was unknown. To examine children’s representations of possible identities, we asked children to “get snack ready.” We found that children in the Unambiguous Identity condition selected only one of the snacks (i.e., the favorite snack of the animal they were told would slide down), whereas children in the Ambiguous Identity condition selected <em>both</em> snacks, suggesting that they were accounting for <em>both</em> possible identities. These results extend the literature on the development of modal reasoning to include reasoning about possible identities and suggest that this ability may be available to children as young as 3 years.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":"249 ","pages":"Article 106078"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142394261","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Embodying anticipated affect enhances proactive behavior in 5-year-old children 体现预期情感可增强 5 岁儿童的主动行为。
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-04 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106099
Felix Schreiber , Silvia Schneider , Albert Newen , Babett Voigt
Imagining anticipated affects can foster future-oriented behavior in adults. However, children often still have difficulties in vividly imagining how they will feel in a specific episode (affective episodic future thinking [EFT]). We investigated whether enacting anticipated affects helps children to imagine how they will feel and whether this enhances proactive behavior in turn. A total of 90 5-year-old children were randomly assigned to one of three groups. In the embodiment group, children were instructed to imagine and physically enact how positive and negative they would feel in an upcoming performance test. Children in the EFT-only group underwent a similar procedure but did not enact their future affect. In the control group, children were reminded of the upcoming test only without receiving a prompt to imagine the upcoming test. After the manipulation, children had the opportunity to play one of three games. One game was relevant for the test. Children’s choice to play the relevant game in advance of the test served as an indicator for proactive behavior. Mechanisms (e.g., detailedness of the envisioned event) and moderators (theory of mind and neuroticism) of the link between embodied EFT and proactive behavior were explored. Children in the embodiment group chose the relevant game above chance level, but they did not choose the relevant game more often than children in the EFT-only group and the control group. Those results were independent of the assumed mediators and moderators.
想象预期的情感可以培养成人面向未来的行为。然而,儿童往往仍然难以生动地想象自己在某一特定事件中的感受(情感发作性未来思维 [EFT])。我们研究了实施预期情感是否有助于儿童想象自己的感受,以及这是否会反过来增强儿童的主动行为。共有 90 名 5 岁儿童被随机分配到三组中的一组。在 "体现 "组,儿童被要求想象并用身体表现出他们在即将到来的成绩测试中的积极和消极感受。纯 EFT 组的儿童也接受了类似的程序,但并不表现他们未来的情绪。在对照组中,只提醒儿童即将进行测试,而不提示他们想象即将进行的测试。操作结束后,孩子们有机会玩三个游戏中的一个。其中一个游戏与测试相关。孩子们在测试前选择玩相关游戏作为主动行为的指标。我们探讨了具身 EFT 与积极主动行为之间联系的机制(如设想事件的详细程度)和调节因素(心智理论和神经质)。体现组的儿童选择相关游戏的次数高于偶然水平,但他们选择相关游戏的次数并不比纯 EFT 组和对照组的儿童多。这些结果与假定的中介和调节因素无关。
{"title":"Embodying anticipated affect enhances proactive behavior in 5-year-old children","authors":"Felix Schreiber ,&nbsp;Silvia Schneider ,&nbsp;Albert Newen ,&nbsp;Babett Voigt","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106099","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106099","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Imagining anticipated affects can foster future-oriented behavior in adults. However, children often still have difficulties in vividly imagining how they will feel in a specific episode (affective episodic future thinking [EFT]). We investigated whether enacting anticipated affects helps children to imagine how they will feel and whether this enhances proactive behavior in turn. A total of 90 5-year-old children were randomly assigned to one of three groups. In the embodiment group, children were instructed to imagine and physically enact how positive and negative they would feel in an upcoming performance test. Children in the EFT-only group underwent a similar procedure but did not enact their future affect. In the control group, children were reminded of the upcoming test only without receiving a prompt to imagine the upcoming test. After the manipulation, children had the opportunity to play one of three games. One game was relevant for the test. Children’s choice to play the relevant game in advance of the test served as an indicator for proactive behavior. Mechanisms (e.g., detailedness of the envisioned event) and moderators (theory of mind and neuroticism) of the link between embodied EFT and proactive behavior were explored. Children in the embodiment group chose the relevant game above chance level, but they did not choose the relevant game more often than children in the EFT-only group and the control group. Those results were independent of the assumed mediators and moderators.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":"249 ","pages":"Article 106099"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142378374","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The near and far transfer effects of computerized working memory training in typically developing preschool children: Evidence from event-related potentials 计算机化工作记忆训练对发育正常的学龄前儿童的远近迁移效应:事件相关电位的证据
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-03 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106096
Yan Hong , Ting Zhang , Cong Pang , Ling Zou , Ming Li , Renlai Zhou
Working memory (WM) refers to the ability to actively maintain and process information needed to complete complex tasks such as comprehension, learning, and reasoning. Recent studies have examined the efficacy of computerized working memory training (WMT) in improving cognitive functions in general and WM in particular, with mixed results. Thus, to what extent can WMT produce near and far transfer effects to cognitive function is currently unclear. This study investigated the transfer effects of a computerized WMT for preschool children and also examined the possible neural correlates using the event-related potential (ERP) technique. A total of 50 Chinese preschoolers (64.44 ± 7.76 months old; 24 girls) received 4-week training during school hours. Compared with those in the active control group, children in the training group showed better gains in behavioral performance in the WM task and significantly more changes in ERP markers of the WM and inhibitory control tasks (near transfer effect). However, no evidence was found for transfer to fluid intelligence (far transfer effect). These findings suggest that WMT is capable of enhancing cognitive functioning in preschool children, and as such this work has important implications for educational practice and it may help to design and refine cognitive interventions for typically developing children and those with WM problems or other cognitive deficits (e.g., children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder).
工作记忆(WM)是指主动保持和处理完成复杂任务(如理解、学习和推理)所需信息的能力。最近有研究探讨了计算机化工作记忆训练(WMT)在改善认知功能,尤其是工作记忆方面的效果,但结果不一。因此,目前还不清楚计算机工作记忆训练能在多大程度上对认知功能产生远近迁移效应。本研究调查了学龄前儿童电脑WMT的迁移效应,并利用事件相关电位(ERP)技术研究了可能的神经相关因素。共有 50 名中国学龄前儿童(64.44 ± 7.76 个月大;24 名女孩)在上课时间接受了为期 4 周的训练。与积极对照组的儿童相比,训练组的儿童在WM任务中的行为表现有更好的提高,在WM和抑制控制任务中的ERP标记也有显著的变化(近转移效应)。但是,没有证据表明他们的智能迁移到了流体智能上(远迁移效应)。这些研究结果表明,WMT 能够增强学龄前儿童的认知功能,因此,这项研究对教育实践具有重要意义,它可能有助于设计和完善针对发育正常儿童、有 WM 问题或其他认知缺陷儿童(如注意力缺陷/多动障碍儿童)的认知干预措施。
{"title":"The near and far transfer effects of computerized working memory training in typically developing preschool children: Evidence from event-related potentials","authors":"Yan Hong ,&nbsp;Ting Zhang ,&nbsp;Cong Pang ,&nbsp;Ling Zou ,&nbsp;Ming Li ,&nbsp;Renlai Zhou","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106096","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106096","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Working memory (WM) refers to the ability to actively maintain and process information needed to complete complex tasks such as comprehension, learning, and reasoning. Recent studies have examined the efficacy of computerized working memory training (WMT) in improving cognitive functions in general and WM in particular, with mixed results. Thus, to what extent can WMT produce near and far transfer effects to cognitive function is currently unclear. This study investigated the transfer effects of a computerized WMT for preschool children and also examined the possible neural correlates using the event-related potential (ERP) technique. A total of 50 Chinese preschoolers (64.44 ± 7.76 months old; 24 girls) received 4-week training during school hours. Compared with those in the active control group, children in the training group showed better gains in behavioral performance in the WM task and significantly more changes in ERP markers of the WM and inhibitory control tasks (near transfer effect). However, no evidence was found for transfer to fluid intelligence (far transfer effect). These findings suggest that WMT is capable of enhancing cognitive functioning in preschool children, and as such this work has important implications for educational practice and it may help to design and refine cognitive interventions for typically developing children and those with WM problems or other cognitive deficits (e.g., children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder).</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":"249 ","pages":"Article 106096"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142376153","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Finger use mirroring young children’s ways of experiencing numbers 手指的使用反映了幼儿体验数字的方式。
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106076
Camilla Björklund
Children who encounter questions about quantities or numbers are observed to use their fingers in different ways to aid their problem solving. This study aimed to contribute to the area of finger counting research with an inquiry of what children’s finger use tells us about their knowledge of numbers. A basic argument is that it is not sufficient to observe the actual use of fingers; there is a need for interpretations of what the finger use means to the children, taking the children’s perspective as the outset. This was done by analyzing 4- and 5-year-olds’ finger use through the lens of phenomenography and variation theory of learning to describe the qualitatively different ways in which children use fingers as an expression of their ways of experiencing the meanings of numbers. Five categories of finger use that show a variety in the meanings the fingers represent emerged: Fingers represent individual items, quantities, countables, number relations, and number structure. The results show that children’s finger use may give access to their ways of understanding numbers; some ways of using fingers indicate expressions of more or less advanced meanings of numbers. A conclusion from the results is that to develop number knowledge and skills, children are aided by learning to see and use their fingers as representing composed units. Some children need the structural support of fingers to solve number problems, and fingers should be used to explore number structures rather than used as countables.
据观察,儿童在遇到有关数量或数字的问题时,会以不同的方式使用手指来帮助解决问题。本研究旨在通过探究儿童使用手指的情况来了解他们对数字的认识,从而为手指计数研究领域做出贡献。一个基本论点是,仅仅观察手指的实际使用情况是不够的;还需要从儿童的角度出发,解释手指的使用对儿童意味着什么。为此,我们从现象学和学习变异理论的角度分析了 4 岁和 5 岁儿童使用手指的情况,描述了儿童使用手指的不同方式,以此表达他们体验数字含义的方式。结果发现,手指的使用有五种类型,分别代表不同的意义:手指分别代表单个物品、数量、可数名词、数字关系和数字结构。结果表明,儿童使用手指的方式可以让人了解他们理解数字的方式;使用手指的某些方 式表明他们表达了或多或少的数字高级含义。研究结果得出的结论是,要发展数字知识和技能,儿童应学会把手指看作是组成单位并加以使用。有些幼儿需要手指的结构支持来解决数字问题,手指应被用来探索数字结构,而不是用作计数器。
{"title":"Finger use mirroring young children’s ways of experiencing numbers","authors":"Camilla Björklund","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106076","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106076","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Children who encounter questions about quantities or numbers are observed to use their fingers in different ways to aid their problem solving. This study aimed to contribute to the area of finger counting research with an inquiry of what children’s finger use tells us about their knowledge of numbers. A basic argument is that it is not sufficient to observe the actual use of fingers; there is a need for interpretations of what the finger use means to the children, taking the children’s perspective as the outset. This was done by analyzing 4- and 5-year-olds’ finger use through the lens of phenomenography and variation theory of learning to describe the qualitatively different ways in which children use fingers as an expression of their ways of experiencing the meanings of numbers. Five categories of finger use that show a variety in the meanings the fingers represent emerged: Fingers represent individual items, quantities, countables, number relations, and number structure. The results show that children’s finger use may give access to their ways of understanding numbers; some ways of using fingers indicate expressions of more or less advanced meanings of numbers. A conclusion from the results is that to develop number knowledge and skills, children are aided by learning to see and use their fingers as representing composed units. Some children need the structural support of fingers to solve number problems, and fingers should be used to explore number structures rather than used as countables.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":"249 ","pages":"Article 106076"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142366966","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What motivates early lies? Deception in 2½- to 5-year-olds 早期谎言的动机是什么?2岁半至5岁儿童的欺骗行为。
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106079
Cynthia Xinran Guo, Philippe Rochat
What motivates young children to produce early lies? A total of 217 2½- to 5-year-old children (Mage = 44.5 months, SD = 8.45; 54% girls; 61.7% White) from the southeastern United States were tested using a modified third-party transgression paradigm to examine the motivation behind their deception. Children were assigned to one of three conditions—baseline, self-motivated, or other-motivated condition—and their propensity to lie was captured through both verbal and nonverbal measures. Results show that children’s early lies are primarily driven by a self-serving motivation. However, the motivation to lie diversifies by 4 years of age, when children begin to lie for both self-serving and other-serving motivations.
是什么促使幼儿产生早期谎言?美国东南部共 217 名 2 岁半至 5 岁的儿童(年龄 = 44.5 个月,SD = 8.45;54% 为女孩;61.7% 为白人)接受了改良的第三方越轨范式测试,以研究他们说谎背后的动机。儿童被分配到三种条件之一--基准条件、自我激励条件或其他激励条件--并通过言语和非言语测量来捕捉他们说谎的倾向。结果表明,儿童早期说谎的主要动机是为自己服务。然而,到了 4 岁,儿童说谎的动机开始多样化,既有出于为自己服务的动机,也有出于为他人服务的动机。
{"title":"What motivates early lies? Deception in 2½- to 5-year-olds","authors":"Cynthia Xinran Guo,&nbsp;Philippe Rochat","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106079","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106079","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>What motivates young children to produce early lies? A total of 217 2½- to 5-year-old children (<em>M</em><sub>age</sub> = 44.5 months, <em>SD</em> = 8.45; 54% girls; 61.7% White) from the southeastern United States were tested using a modified third-party transgression paradigm to examine the motivation behind their deception. Children were assigned to one of three conditions—baseline, self-motivated, or other-motivated condition—and their propensity to lie was captured through both verbal and nonverbal measures. Results show that children’s early lies are primarily driven by a self-serving motivation. However, the motivation to lie diversifies by 4 years of age, when children begin to lie for both self-serving and other-serving motivations.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":"249 ","pages":"Article 106079"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142366967","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Transposed-character effects during learning to read: When does letter and non-letter strings processing become different? 学习阅读过程中的转换字符效应:字母和非字母字符串的处理何时变得不同?
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-28 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106081
Stéphanie Massol , Joana Acha , Lisa Rondot , Marta Vergara-Martinez , Emilie Favre , Bernard Lété
Efficient reading requires the association of different letter identities with their positions in the written word. This leads to the development of a specialized mechanism for encoding flexible location-invariant letter positions through learning to read. In this study, we investigated not only the emergence and development of this position coding mechanism but also whether this mechanism is a consequence of the orthographic code (i.e., letter specific) or inherent to generic visual object recognition. To do so, the same–different matching task was used with children from Grade 1 to Grade 5 (Experiment 1) and with adults (Experiment 2). In both experiments, reference and target stimuli were composed of four-character strings (consonants, digits, and geometrical forms) and could be identical or different by transposing or substituting two internal characters. Analyses of response times, error rates, and discriminability indices revealed a transposed-character effect regardless of the type of characters in Grades 1 and 2, whereas transposed-character effects were greater for letter strings than for familiar non-letter strings in Grade 3, lasting up to Grade 5 as well as in adults. These results provided evidence in favor of a flexible position coding mechanism that is specific to letter strings, which emerges with reading experience as a consequence of parallel processing of letters within words.
高效阅读需要将不同字母的特性与它们在书面文字中的位置联系起来。这就导致在学习阅读的过程中形成了一种专门的机制,用于编码灵活的位置不变的字母位置。在这项研究中,我们不仅研究了这种位置编码机制的出现和发展,而且还研究了这种机制是正字法编码的结果(即特定字母)还是一般视觉对象识别的固有机制。为此,我们对一至五年级的儿童(实验 1)和成人(实验 2)进行了同异匹配任务。在这两项实验中,参照物和目标物都由四个字符串(辅音、数字和几何图形)组成,可以通过调换或替换两个内部字符来实现相同或不同。对反应时间、错误率和可辨别性指数的分析表明,在一年级和二年级,无论字符的类型如何,转置字符效应都存在;而在三年级,字母字符串的转置字符效应要大于熟悉的非字母字符串,这种效应一直持续到五年级以及成人。这些结果提供了支持一种灵活的位置编码机制的证据,这种机制是字母串特有的,随着阅读经验的积累而出现,是单词中字母并行处理的结果。
{"title":"Transposed-character effects during learning to read: When does letter and non-letter strings processing become different?","authors":"Stéphanie Massol ,&nbsp;Joana Acha ,&nbsp;Lisa Rondot ,&nbsp;Marta Vergara-Martinez ,&nbsp;Emilie Favre ,&nbsp;Bernard Lété","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106081","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106081","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Efficient reading requires the association of different letter identities with their positions in the written word. This leads to the development of a specialized mechanism for encoding flexible location-invariant letter positions through learning to read. In this study, we investigated not only the emergence and development of this position coding mechanism but also whether this mechanism is a consequence of the orthographic code (i.e., letter specific) or inherent to generic visual object recognition. To do so, the same–different matching task was used with children from Grade 1 to Grade 5 (Experiment 1) and with adults (Experiment 2). In both experiments, reference and target stimuli were composed of four-character strings (consonants, digits, and geometrical forms) and could be identical or different by transposing or substituting two internal characters. Analyses of response times, error rates, and discriminability indices revealed a transposed-character effect regardless of the type of characters in Grades 1 and 2, whereas transposed-character effects were greater for letter strings than for familiar non-letter strings in Grade 3, lasting up to Grade 5 as well as in adults. These results provided evidence in favor of a flexible position coding mechanism that is specific to letter strings, which emerges with reading experience as a consequence of parallel processing of letters within words.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":"249 ","pages":"Article 106081"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337169","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Birth order moderates the association between adverse childhood experiences and externalizing behavior symptoms in adolescence 出生顺序调节童年不良经历与青春期外化行为症状之间的关系
IF 1.8 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-26 DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106077
Marcela Soto , Lauren Micalizzi , Dayna Price , Michelle L. Rogers , Kristina M. Jackson
Adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) are associated with externalizing behaviors. Whereas some ACEs affect individual children (i.e., child-specific; e.g., failing a grade), others affect the family unit (i.e., family-wide; e.g., parent losing a job); effects of ACEs on externalizing behavior may manifest differently across groupings of ACEs. Moreover, birth order may modify the association between child-specific and family-wide ACEs and externalizing behavior due to differences in the experience of being a younger versus older sibling. This study examined the externalizing behavior of siblings in relation to their experiences of child-specific and family-wide ACEs to test the hypothesis that younger siblings are at greater risk for developing externalizing symptoms following familial ACE exposure. Participants were 61 sibling pairs (younger sibling Mage = 11.37 years, 44.1% male; older sibling Mage = 13.1 years, 52.5% male) recruited from six schools in the northeastern United States. Parents rated each child’s externalizing behaviors (e.g., bullying, meanness) and retrospectively reported on each child’s experience of 34 ACEs; two raters categorized ACEs as child-specific (n = 10) or family-wide (n = 24). Multilevel modeling revealed that both child-specific and family-wide ACEs were associated with increased externalizing behaviors. Birth order moderated the effect of family-wide (but not child-specific) ACEs on externalizing behaviors, independent of sex and age. Externalizing behavior was higher for younger siblings as compared with older siblings, particularly when a high number of ACEs (6+) were reported. This research should prompt future exploration of mechanistic theories of the impact of family-wide and child-specific ACEs and the role of birth order.
不良童年经历(ACEs)与外化行为有关。有些不良童年经历会影响单个儿童(即针对特定儿童,如不及格),有些则会影响整个家庭(即针对整个家庭,如父母失业);不同的不良童年经历对外化行为的影响可能不同。此外,出生顺序可能会改变特定儿童和整个家庭的 ACE 与外化行为之间的关联,这是因为作为弟弟妹妹和哥哥姐姐的经历不同。本研究考察了兄弟姐妹的外化行为与他们所经历的儿童特异性和家庭范围的 ACE 之间的关系,以验证这样一个假设,即年龄较小的兄弟姐妹在受到家庭范围的 ACE 影响后出现外化症状的风险更大。参与者是从美国东北部的六所学校招募的 61 对兄弟姐妹(弟弟妹妹年龄为 11.37 岁,44.1% 为男性;哥哥姐姐年龄为 13.1 岁,52.5% 为男性)。家长们对每个孩子的外化行为(如欺凌、刻薄)进行评分,并回顾性地报告了每个孩子的 34 种 ACE 经历;两名评分员将 ACE 归类为特定儿童(10 人)或整个家庭(24 人)。多层次模型显示,儿童特有的和家庭范围内的 ACE 与外化行为的增加有关。出生顺序调节了家庭范围内(而非针对儿童的)ACE 对外化行为的影响,与性别和年龄无关。与年长的兄弟姐妹相比,年幼的兄弟姐妹的外化行为较高,尤其是在报告了大量 ACE(6 次以上)的情况下。这项研究应促使我们在未来探索有关家庭和儿童特定 ACE 影响的机制理论以及出生顺序的作用。
{"title":"Birth order moderates the association between adverse childhood experiences and externalizing behavior symptoms in adolescence","authors":"Marcela Soto ,&nbsp;Lauren Micalizzi ,&nbsp;Dayna Price ,&nbsp;Michelle L. Rogers ,&nbsp;Kristina M. Jackson","doi":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106077","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jecp.2024.106077","url":null,"abstract":"<div><div>Adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) are associated with externalizing behaviors. Whereas some ACEs affect individual children (i.e., child-specific; e.g., failing a grade), others affect the family unit (i.e., family-wide; e.g., parent losing a job); effects of ACEs on externalizing behavior may manifest differently across groupings of ACEs. Moreover, birth order may modify the association between child-specific and family-wide ACEs and externalizing behavior due to differences in the experience of being a younger versus older sibling. This study examined the externalizing behavior of siblings in relation to their experiences of child-specific and family-wide ACEs to test the hypothesis that younger siblings are at greater risk for developing externalizing symptoms following familial ACE exposure. Participants were 61 sibling pairs (younger sibling M<sub>age</sub> = 11.37 years, 44.1% male; older sibling M<sub>age</sub> = 13.1 years, 52.5% male) recruited from six schools in the northeastern United States. Parents rated each child’s externalizing behaviors (e.g., bullying, meanness) and retrospectively reported on each child’s experience of 34 ACEs; two raters categorized ACEs as child-specific (n = 10) or family-wide (n = 24). Multilevel modeling revealed that both child-specific and family-wide ACEs were associated with increased externalizing behaviors. Birth order moderated the effect of family-wide (but not child-specific) ACEs on externalizing behaviors, independent of sex and age. Externalizing behavior was higher for younger siblings as compared with older siblings, particularly when a high number of ACEs (6+) were reported. This research should prompt future exploration of mechanistic theories of the impact of family-wide and child-specific ACEs and the role of birth order.</div></div>","PeriodicalId":48391,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Child Psychology","volume":"249 ","pages":"Article 106077"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142324147","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1