首页 > 最新文献

Cognition & Emotion最新文献

英文 中文
Investigating the role of mental imagery use in the assessment of anhedonia. 研究心理想象在失乐症评估中的作用。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-27 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2405008
Julie L Ji, Marcella L Woud, Angela Rölver, Lies Notebaert, Jemma Todd, Patrick J F Clarke, Frances Meeten, Jürgen Margraf, Simon E Blackwell

Anhedonia, or a deficit in the liking, wanting, and seeking of rewards, is typically assessed via self-reported "in-the-moment" emotional and motivational responses to reward stimuli and activities. Given that mental imagery is known to evoke emotion and motivational responses, we conducted two studies to investigate the relationship between mental imagery use and self-reported anhedonia. Using a novel Reward Response Scale (adapted from the Dimensional Anhedonia Rating Scale, DARS; Rizvi et al., 2015) modified to assess deliberate and spontaneous mental imagery use, Study 1 (N = 394) compared uninstructed and instructed mental imagery use, and Study 2 (N = 586) conducted a test of replication of uninstructed mental imagery use. Results showed that greater mental imagery use was associated with higher reward response scores (Study 1 & 2), and this relationship was not moderated by whether imagery use was uninstructed or instructed (Study 1). Importantly, mental imagery use moderated the convergence between reward response and depression scale measures of anhedonia, with lower convergence for those reporting higher mental imagery use (Study 1 & 2). Results suggest that higher spontaneous mental imagery use may increase self-reported reward response and reduce the convergence between reward response scale and depression questionnaire measures of anhedonia. [199 / 200 words].

失乐症,即对奖赏的喜好、渴望和追求不足,通常是通过自我报告的对奖赏刺激和活动的 "当下 "情绪和动机反应来评估的。鉴于心理想象可以唤起情绪和动机反应,我们进行了两项研究来调查心理想象的使用与自我报告的失乐症之间的关系。研究1(N = 394)比较了未经指导和经过指导的心理想象使用情况,研究2(N = 586)对未经指导的心理想象使用情况进行了复制测试。结果表明,更多的心理想象与更高的奖励反应得分相关(研究 1 和研究 2),而且这种关系并不受未指导或指导性心理想象的影响(研究 1)。重要的是,心理意象的使用调节了奖赏反应与抑郁量表中失乐症量表之间的趋同性,那些报告心理意象使用较多的人的趋同性较低(研究 1 和 2)。研究结果表明,使用较多自发心理想象可能会增加自我报告的奖赏反应,并减少奖赏反应量表和抑郁问卷中失乐症测量结果之间的趋同性。[199 / 200字]。
{"title":"Investigating the role of mental imagery use in the assessment of anhedonia.","authors":"Julie L Ji, Marcella L Woud, Angela Rölver, Lies Notebaert, Jemma Todd, Patrick J F Clarke, Frances Meeten, Jürgen Margraf, Simon E Blackwell","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2405008","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2024.2405008","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Anhedonia, or a deficit in the liking, wanting, and seeking of rewards, is typically assessed via self-reported \"in-the-moment\" emotional and motivational responses to reward stimuli and activities. Given that mental imagery is known to evoke emotion and motivational responses, we conducted two studies to investigate the relationship between mental imagery use and self-reported anhedonia. Using a novel Reward Response Scale (adapted from the Dimensional Anhedonia Rating Scale, DARS; Rizvi et al., 2015) modified to assess deliberate and spontaneous mental imagery use, Study 1 (<i>N</i> = 394) compared uninstructed and instructed mental imagery use, and Study 2 (<i>N</i> = 586) conducted a test of replication of uninstructed mental imagery use. Results showed that greater mental imagery use was associated with higher reward response scores (Study 1 & 2), and this relationship was not moderated by whether imagery use was uninstructed or instructed (Study 1). Importantly, mental imagery use moderated the convergence between reward response and depression scale measures of anhedonia, with lower convergence for those reporting higher mental imagery use (Study 1 & 2). Results suggest that higher spontaneous mental imagery use may increase self-reported reward response and reduce the convergence between reward response scale and depression questionnaire measures of anhedonia. [199 / 200 words].</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1-19"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337119","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pride and moral disengagement: associations among comparison-based pride, moral disengagement, and unethical decision-making. 自豪感与道德疏离:基于比较的自豪感、道德疏离和不道德决策之间的关联。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-27 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2407041
Manuel Rengifo, Simon M Laham

Pride has rarely been explored in the context of moral disengagement and unethical decision-making. Although some research has examined the associations between "authentic" and "hubristic" pride and unethical behaviour, little attention has been paid to potential mechanisms. Across two correlational studies (N = 379), we explore the associations between two facets of pride rooted on comparisons - social comparison-based pride, and self-based pride, moral disengagement, and unethical decision-making. Results show that social comparison-based pride consistently (positively) relates to moral disengagement, and that moral disengagement accounts for the association between social comparison-based pride and unethical decision-making. In sum, our findings contribute in novel ways to the understanding of how pride based in different comparison frames may lead to antisocial decision-making.

人们很少从道德脱离和不道德决策的角度来探讨自豪感。尽管有些研究探讨了 "真实的 "和 "自大的 "自豪感与不道德行为之间的关联,但很少关注潜在的机制。通过两项相关研究(N = 379),我们探讨了基于比较的自豪感的两个方面--基于社会比较的自豪感和基于自我的自豪感--与道德脱离和不道德决策之间的关联。结果表明,基于社会比较的自豪感与道德脱离有一致(正向)的关系,而道德脱离则解释了基于社会比较的自豪感与不道德决策之间的关联。总之,我们的研究结果有助于人们以新颖的方式理解基于不同比较框架的自豪感是如何导致反社会决策的。
{"title":"Pride and moral disengagement: associations among comparison-based pride, moral disengagement, and unethical decision-making.","authors":"Manuel Rengifo, Simon M Laham","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2407041","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2024.2407041","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Pride has rarely been explored in the context of moral disengagement and unethical decision-making. Although some research has examined the associations between \"authentic\" and \"hubristic\" pride and unethical behaviour, little attention has been paid to potential mechanisms. Across two correlational studies (<i>N</i> = 379), we explore the associations between two facets of pride rooted on comparisons - social comparison-based pride, and self-based pride, moral disengagement, and unethical decision-making. Results show that social comparison-based pride consistently (positively) relates to moral disengagement, and that moral disengagement accounts for the association between social comparison-based pride and unethical decision-making. In sum, our findings contribute in novel ways to the understanding of how pride based in different comparison frames may lead to antisocial decision-making.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1-15"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142337120","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction. 更正。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2406631
{"title":"Correction.","authors":"","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2406631","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2024.2406631","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298823","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Working memory capacity relates to reduced negative emotion in daily life. 工作记忆能力与减少日常生活中的负面情绪有关。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-16 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2402939
Justin N Wahlers, Katie E Garrison, Brandon J Schmeichel

Working memory capacity (WMC) refers to the ability to maintain information in short-term memory while attending to the immediate environment, and has been associated with emotional states. Yet, research on the link between WMC and emotion in naturalistic settings is growing and inconsistencies have been observed. In the current study (N = 109), we directly replicated the procedures of a prior experience sampling study (Garrison & Schmeichel, 2022), which found that higher WMC attenuates the relationship between stressful events in daily life and negative affect. We measured WMC in the laboratory and then measured the occurrence of stressful events, momentary emotional states, and coping responses to stress several times a day for six days. Higher WMC was associated with reduced momentary negative emotion, but this relationship did not depend on the occurrence of a stressful event. Exploratory analyses found that higher WMC was associated with a greater likelihood of planning as a coping response to stress and greater number of coping strategies reported per stressful event. However, coping did not mediate the link between WMC and momentary negative emotion. Our results contribute to the robustness and ecological validity of the link between WMC and reduced negative emotion in daily life.

工作记忆能力(WMC)是指在关注周围环境的同时将信息保存在短时记忆中的能力,它与情绪状态有关。然而,在自然环境中,关于工作记忆容量与情绪之间联系的研究越来越多,而且观察到的结果也不一致。在目前的研究中(N = 109),我们直接复制了之前一项经验取样研究(Garrison & Schmeichel,2022 年)的程序,该研究发现,较高的 WMC 会减弱日常生活中压力事件与负面情绪之间的关系。我们在实验室测量了 WMC,然后测量了压力事件的发生、瞬间情绪状态以及对压力的应对反应,每天测量数次,持续六天。WMC越高,瞬间负面情绪越少,但这种关系并不取决于压力事件的发生。探索性分析发现,WMC 越高,越有可能将计划作为应对压力的一种方法,而且每次压力事件中报告的应对策略数量也越多。然而,应对策略并不能调节 WMC 与瞬间负面情绪之间的联系。我们的研究结果有助于证明 WMC 与减少日常生活中的负面情绪之间的联系的稳健性和生态有效性。
{"title":"Working memory capacity relates to reduced negative emotion in daily life.","authors":"Justin N Wahlers, Katie E Garrison, Brandon J Schmeichel","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2402939","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2024.2402939","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Working memory capacity (WMC) refers to the ability to maintain information in short-term memory while attending to the immediate environment, and has been associated with emotional states. Yet, research on the link between WMC and emotion in naturalistic settings is growing and inconsistencies have been observed. In the current study (<i>N</i> = 109), we directly replicated the procedures of a prior experience sampling study (Garrison & Schmeichel, 2022), which found that higher WMC attenuates the relationship between stressful events in daily life and negative affect. We measured WMC in the laboratory and then measured the occurrence of stressful events, momentary emotional states, and coping responses to stress several times a day for six days. Higher WMC was associated with reduced momentary negative emotion, but this relationship did not depend on the occurrence of a stressful event. Exploratory analyses found that higher WMC was associated with a greater likelihood of planning as a coping response to stress and greater number of coping strategies reported per stressful event. However, coping did not mediate the link between WMC and momentary negative emotion. Our results contribute to the robustness and ecological validity of the link between WMC and reduced negative emotion in daily life.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1-12"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298825","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotional false memories: the impact of response bias under speeded retrieval conditions. 情绪性错误记忆:加速检索条件下反应偏差的影响。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-12 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2401611
Lauren M Cooper, Datin Shah

Emotional false memory findings using the DRM paradigm have been marked by higher false alarms to negatively arousing compared to neutral critical lure items. Explanations for these findings have mainly focused on false memory-based accounts. However, here we address the question of whether a response bias for emotional stimuli can, at least in part, explain this phenomenon. Participants viewed both neutral and negative arousing DRM lists and completed a recognition test in speeded or self-paced conditions. Speeded test reduces the opportunity to adjust response bias. Analysis showed no significant difference in false recognition across critical lure types for the speeded condition, but false recognition was higher for negative compared to neutral critical lures in the self-paced condition. We argue that when retrieval does not allow for shifts in response criteria, false alarms to negative emotional critical lures appear more similar to neutral equivalents. The discussion explores memory-based and criterion-shift explanations for the enhanced emotional false memory finding.

使用 DRM 范式进行的情绪假记忆研究发现,与中性临界引诱项目相比,负面唤醒项目的假警报更高。对这些发现的解释主要集中在基于错误记忆的说法上。然而,在这里我们要探讨的问题是,对情绪刺激的反应偏差是否至少可以部分解释这种现象。受试者观看了中性和负面的唤醒 DRM 列表,并在加速或自定步调的条件下完成了识别测试。加速测试减少了调整反应偏差的机会。分析表明,在加速条件下,不同类型关键诱饵的错误识别率没有明显差异,但在自定步调条件下,负面关键诱饵的错误识别率高于中性关键诱饵。我们认为,当检索不允许改变反应标准时,对负面情绪性临界引诱物的错误警报与中性同等引诱物更为相似。讨论探讨了基于记忆和标准转换的情绪错误记忆增强的解释。
{"title":"Emotional false memories: the impact of response bias under speeded retrieval conditions.","authors":"Lauren M Cooper, Datin Shah","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2401611","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2024.2401611","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Emotional false memory findings using the DRM paradigm have been marked by higher false alarms to negatively arousing compared to neutral critical lure items. Explanations for these findings have mainly focused on false memory-based accounts. However, here we address the question of whether a response bias for emotional stimuli can, at least in part, explain this phenomenon. Participants viewed both neutral and negative arousing DRM lists and completed a recognition test in speeded or self-paced conditions. Speeded test reduces the opportunity to adjust response bias. Analysis showed no significant difference in false recognition across critical lure types for the speeded condition, but false recognition was higher for negative compared to neutral critical lures in the self-paced condition. We argue that when retrieval does not allow for shifts in response criteria, false alarms to negative emotional critical lures appear more similar to neutral equivalents. The discussion explores memory-based and criterion-shift explanations for the enhanced emotional false memory finding.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1-8"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298824","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The influence of repetitive thoughts of CS-US pairing on expectancy learning and evaluative conditioning: a fundamental study. 重复思考 CS-US 配对对期望学习和评价性条件反射的影响:一项基础研究。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-02 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2398615
Thierry Kosinski, Vincent Leleu

Repetitive thinking is a common phenomenon, also implicated in a variety of mental disorders. The content of repetitive thoughts can take the form of prediction of aversive events (in worry) or evoking negatively valenced information (in rumination), for instance. Investigating the influence of repetitive thinking through the lens of associative learning could help deepen our understanding of the mechanisms involved in its effects.In two experiments, non-clinical participants were exposed to CS-US pairings with the aim of creating non-threatening expectancy learning (Experiment 1) or evaluative conditioning (Experiment 2). After each conditioning trial, participants were instructed to initiate repetitive thoughts about the pairing (i.e. rehearse) or follow control instructions.Experiment 1 (N = 64) showed that such intervention strengthen the association between the mental representations of the CS and the US, leading to a stronger US expectancy in response to the CS. In Experiment 2 (N = 107), an evaluative conditioning effect was observed; however, it was not influenced by instructions.The study demonstrated that simulated repetitive thinking strengthens the CS-US association and leads to greater US expectancy in expectancy learning, despite appearing not to influence the evaluative conditioning effect. The potential implications of these findings on repetitive thinking are discussed.

重复思维是一种常见现象,也与多种精神障碍有关。例如,重复思维的内容可以是预测厌恶事件(在担忧中)或唤起负面信息(在反刍中)。通过联想学习的视角来研究重复思维的影响,有助于加深我们对其影响机制的理解。在两项实验中,非临床参与者暴露于CS-US配对中,目的是建立非威胁性期望学习(实验1)或评价性条件反射(实验2)。实验 1(N = 64)表明,这种干预加强了 CS 和 US 的心理表征之间的联系,从而导致对 CS 更强的 US 期望。实验 2(N = 107)中观察到了评价性条件反射效应;然而,这种效应并不受指令的影响。该研究表明,模拟重复思维加强了 CS 与美国之间的关联,并导致期望学习中更大的美国期望值,尽管它似乎并不影响评价性条件反射效应。本研究讨论了这些发现对重复思维的潜在影响。
{"title":"The influence of repetitive thoughts of CS-US pairing on expectancy learning and evaluative conditioning: a fundamental study.","authors":"Thierry Kosinski, Vincent Leleu","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2398615","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2024.2398615","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Repetitive thinking is a common phenomenon, also implicated in a variety of mental disorders. The content of repetitive thoughts can take the form of prediction of aversive events (in worry) or evoking negatively valenced information (in rumination), for instance. Investigating the influence of repetitive thinking through the lens of associative learning could help deepen our understanding of the mechanisms involved in its effects.In two experiments, non-clinical participants were exposed to CS-US pairings with the aim of creating non-threatening expectancy learning (Experiment 1) or evaluative conditioning (Experiment 2). After each conditioning trial, participants were instructed to initiate repetitive thoughts about the pairing (i.e. rehearse) or follow control instructions.Experiment 1 (N = 64) showed that such intervention strengthen the association between the mental representations of the CS and the US, leading to a stronger US expectancy in response to the CS. In Experiment 2 (N = 107), an evaluative conditioning effect was observed; however, it was not influenced by instructions.The study demonstrated that simulated repetitive thinking strengthens the CS-US association and leads to greater US expectancy in expectancy learning, despite appearing not to influence the evaluative conditioning effect. The potential implications of these findings on repetitive thinking are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1-13"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142113500","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Experimentally manipulated anger activates implicit cognitions about social hierarchy. 实验操纵的愤怒会激活对社会等级的内隐认知。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-21 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2331811
Harrison M Miller, Connor R Hasty, Jon K Maner

A correlational pilot study (N = 143) and an integrative data analysis of two experiments (total N = 377) provide evidence linking anger to the psychology of social hierarchy. The experiments demonstrate that the experience of anger increases the psychological accessibility of implicit cognitions related to social hierarchy: compared to participants in a control condition, participants in an anger-priming condition completed word stems with significantly more hierarchy-related words. We found little support for sex differences in the effect of anger on implicit hierarchy-related cognition; effects were equivalent across male and female participants. Findings fit with functionalist evolutionary views of anger suggesting that anger may motivate the use of dominance to strive for high social rank in group hierarchies. Implications for downstream behaviour, including aggression and negotiation, are discussed.

一项相关性试验研究(N = 143)和两项实验(总 N = 377)的综合数据分析提供了将愤怒与社会等级心理联系起来的证据。实验证明,愤怒的体验会增加与社会等级相关的内隐认知的心理可及性:与对照条件下的参与者相比,愤怒刺激条件下的参与者完成的词干中与等级相关的词明显较多。我们发现,愤怒对内隐等级认知的影响几乎不存在性别差异;男性和女性参与者的效果相当。研究结果符合愤怒的功能主义进化观点,即愤怒可能会促使人们利用支配地位来争取在群体等级中的较高社会地位。研究还讨论了下游行为(包括攻击和谈判)的影响。
{"title":"Experimentally manipulated anger activates implicit cognitions about social hierarchy.","authors":"Harrison M Miller, Connor R Hasty, Jon K Maner","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2331811","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2331811","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A correlational pilot study (<i>N</i> = 143) and an integrative data analysis of two experiments (total <i>N</i> = 377) provide evidence linking anger to the psychology of social hierarchy. The experiments demonstrate that the experience of anger increases the psychological accessibility of implicit cognitions related to social hierarchy: compared to participants in a control condition, participants in an anger-priming condition completed word stems with significantly more hierarchy-related words. We found little support for sex differences in the effect of anger on implicit hierarchy-related cognition; effects were equivalent across male and female participants. Findings fit with functionalist evolutionary views of anger suggesting that anger may motivate the use of dominance to strive for high social rank in group hierarchies. Implications for downstream behaviour, including aggression and negotiation, are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"872-883"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140177245","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of category valence in prototype preference. 类别价值在原型偏好中的作用。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-30 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2335536
Moritz Ingendahl, Nadja Propheter, Tobias Vogel

People prefer prototypical stimuli over atypical stimuli. The dominant explanation for this prototype preference effect is that prototypical stimuli are processed more fluently. However, a more recent account proposes that prototypes are more strongly associated with their category's valence, leading to a reversed prototype preference effect for negative categories. One critical but untested assumption of this category-valence account is that no prototype preference should emerge for entirely neutral categories. We tested this prediction by conditioning categories of dot patterns positively, negatively, or neutrally. In line with previous findings on the category-valence account, prototype preference reversed for negatively conditioned categories. However, prototype preference was similarly strong for positive and neutral categories. These findings imply that prototype preferences do not only reflect a transfer of category valence to exemplars. Instead, the results suggest that prototype preference is a multi-process phenomenon arising from the activated category valence and a fluency-based process. We discuss further implications for theories on fluency and prototype preference.

相比非典型刺激,人们更喜欢原型刺激。对这种原型偏好效应的主要解释是,原型刺激的处理更为流畅。然而,最近的一种解释认为,原型与其类别的价值关联性更强,从而导致了负面类别的反向原型偏好效应。这种类别-价值解释的一个关键但未经验证的假设是,完全中性的类别不应该出现原型偏好。我们通过对点图案类别进行积极、消极或中性的调节来检验这一预测。与之前关于类别-价值账户的研究结果一致,原型偏好对于负向条件化类别而言发生了逆转。然而,原型偏好对于正面和中性类别同样强烈。这些研究结果表明,原型偏好不仅反映了类别价值向范例的转移。相反,这些结果表明,原型偏好是一种多过程现象,由激活的类别价值和基于流畅性的过程产生。我们将进一步讨论流畅性和原型偏好理论的影响。
{"title":"The role of category valence in prototype preference.","authors":"Moritz Ingendahl, Nadja Propheter, Tobias Vogel","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2335536","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2335536","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People prefer prototypical stimuli over atypical stimuli. The dominant explanation for this prototype preference effect is that prototypical stimuli are processed more fluently. However, a more recent account proposes that prototypes are more strongly associated with their category's valence, leading to a reversed prototype preference effect for negative categories. One critical but untested assumption of this category-valence account is that no prototype preference should emerge for entirely neutral categories. We tested this prediction by conditioning categories of dot patterns positively, negatively, or neutrally. In line with previous findings on the category-valence account, prototype preference reversed for negatively conditioned categories. However, prototype preference was similarly strong for positive and neutral categories. These findings imply that prototype preferences do not only reflect a transfer of category valence to exemplars. Instead, the results suggest that prototype preference is a multi-process phenomenon arising from the activated category valence and a fluency-based process. We discuss further implications for theories on fluency and prototype preference.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"963-969"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140330231","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Within- and between-person associations between social interactions and loneliness: students' experiences during the COVID-19 pandemic. 社交互动与孤独感之间的人内和人际关联:COVID-19 大流行期间学生的经历。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-25 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2333920
Alyssa K Truong, Gizem Keskin, Jessica P Lougheed

The COVID-19 pandemic introduced many restrictions to in-person interactions, and remote social interactions may be especially important for managing loneliness when such restrictions are in place. However, it is unclear how social interactions are related to loneliness when in-person interactions are limited. Data were collected between February 2021 and March 2022 from a sample of 581 university students. Participants reported their loneliness and participation in positive in-person or remote social interactions each day for 14 days. Results from dynamic structural equation models showed that participants felt less lonely than they usually felt on the days they engaged in positive remote interactions at the within-person level. Moreover, participants generally felt less lonely when engaging more frequently in remote interactions, but only when in-person interactions were restricted (between-person level). Some of these results varied by changing COVID-19 restrictions. Finally, for participants who felt lonelier in general, the effect of positive in-person and remote interactions on loneliness was less strong. These findings suggest that social interactions may buffer loneliness but are not as impactful for those who experience greater loneliness.

COVID-19 大流行给人与人之间的交往带来了许多限制,而远程社交互动对于在这种限制下控制孤独感可能尤为重要。然而,目前还不清楚当人际交往受到限制时,社交互动与孤独感之间的关系。我们在 2021 年 2 月至 2022 年 3 月期间收集了 581 名大学生的样本数据。参与者在 14 天内每天报告自己的孤独感和参与积极的面对面或远程社交互动的情况。动态结构方程模型的结果表明,在参与积极的远程人际交往的日子里,参与者的孤独感低于他们通常的孤独感。此外,当参与者更频繁地参与远程互动时,他们的孤独感普遍较低,但只有在人际互动受到限制时(人际层面)才会如此。其中一些结果因 COVID-19 限制的改变而不同。最后,对于总体上感觉更孤独的参与者来说,积极的人际交往和远程交往对孤独感的影响较小。这些研究结果表明,社交互动可以缓冲孤独感,但对于那些孤独感较强的人来说,社交互动的影响并不明显。
{"title":"Within- and between-person associations between social interactions and loneliness: students' experiences during the COVID-19 pandemic.","authors":"Alyssa K Truong, Gizem Keskin, Jessica P Lougheed","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2333920","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2333920","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The COVID-19 pandemic introduced many restrictions to in-person interactions, and remote social interactions may be especially important for managing loneliness when such restrictions are in place. However, it is unclear how social interactions are related to loneliness when in-person interactions are limited. Data were collected between February 2021 and March 2022 from a sample of 581 university students. Participants reported their loneliness and participation in positive in-person or remote social interactions each day for 14 days. Results from dynamic structural equation models showed that participants felt less lonely than they usually felt on the days they engaged in positive remote interactions at the within-person level. Moreover, participants generally felt less lonely when engaging more frequently in remote interactions, but only when in-person interactions were restricted (between-person level). Some of these results varied by changing COVID-19 restrictions. Finally, for participants who felt lonelier in general, the effect of positive in-person and remote interactions on loneliness was less strong. These findings suggest that social interactions may buffer loneliness but are not as impactful for those who experience greater loneliness.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"938-946"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140207977","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Threat directionality modulates defensive reactions in humans: cardiac and electrodermal responses. 威胁的方向性调节人类的防御反应:心脏和皮肤电反应。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-30 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2335535
Mariana Xavier, Eliane Volchan, Arthur V Machado, Isabel A David, Letícia Oliveira, Liana C L Portugal, Gabriela G L Souza, Fátima S Erthal, Rita de Cássia S Alves, Izabela Mocaiber, Mirtes G Pereira

Features of threatening cues and the associated context influence the perceived imminence of threat and the defensive responses evoked. To provide additional knowledge about how the directionality of a threat (i.e. directed-towards or away from the viewer) might impact defensive responses in humans, participants were shown pictures of a man carrying a gun (threat) or nonlethal object (neutral) directed-away from or towards the participant. Cardiac and electrodermal responses were collected. Compared to neutral images, threatening images depicting a gun directed-towards the participant induced sustained bradycardia and an increased electrodermal response, interpreted as immobility under attack. This defensive immobility reaction is evoked by high perceived threat and inescapable situations and indicates intense action preparation. Pictures of guns directed-away from the participant induced shorter bradycardia and no significant modulation of the electrodermal response compared to neutral pictures, which might be consistent with the perception of a less threatening situation. The results show that the directionality of threat stimuli is a key factor that prompts different patterns of defensive responses.

威胁线索的特征和相关情境会影响对威胁迫近性的感知和引起的防御反应。为了进一步了解威胁的方向性(即朝向或远离观看者)会如何影响人类的防御反应,研究人员向参与者展示了一名男子携带枪支(威胁)或非致命物品(中性)的图片,图片的方向是朝向或远离参与者。研究人员收集了心电和皮电反应。与中性图像相比,描绘枪口朝向被试的威胁性图像会诱发持续的心动过缓,并增加电皮反应,这被解释为被攻击时的不动反应。这种防卫性的不动反应是在感知到高度威胁和无法逃脱的情况下产生的,并表明了紧张的行动准备。与中性图片相比,枪支朝向远离被试者的图片引起的心动过缓时间较短,而对皮电反应的调节不明显,这可能与被试感受到的威胁性较低的情况相一致。结果表明,威胁刺激的方向性是引发不同防御反应模式的关键因素。
{"title":"Threat directionality modulates defensive reactions in humans: cardiac and electrodermal responses.","authors":"Mariana Xavier, Eliane Volchan, Arthur V Machado, Isabel A David, Letícia Oliveira, Liana C L Portugal, Gabriela G L Souza, Fátima S Erthal, Rita de Cássia S Alves, Izabela Mocaiber, Mirtes G Pereira","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2335535","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2335535","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Features of threatening cues and the associated context influence the perceived imminence of threat and the defensive responses evoked. To provide additional knowledge about how the directionality of a threat (i.e. directed-towards or away from the viewer) might impact defensive responses in humans, participants were shown pictures of a man carrying a gun (threat) or nonlethal object (neutral) directed-away from or towards the participant. Cardiac and electrodermal responses were collected. Compared to neutral images, threatening images depicting a gun directed-towards the participant induced sustained bradycardia and an increased electrodermal response, interpreted as immobility under attack. This defensive immobility reaction is evoked by high perceived threat and inescapable situations and indicates intense action preparation. Pictures of guns directed-away from the participant induced shorter bradycardia and no significant modulation of the electrodermal response compared to neutral pictures, which might be consistent with the perception of a less threatening situation. The results show that the directionality of threat stimuli is a key factor that prompts different patterns of defensive responses.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"954-962"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140330232","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognition & Emotion
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1