首页 > 最新文献

Cognition & Emotion最新文献

英文 中文
The empathic measure of true emotion (EMOTE): a novel set of stimuli for measuring emotional responding. 真实情感的移情测量法(EMOTE):一套用于测量情感反应的新型刺激。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-13 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2427889
Sarah A Grainger, Alana J Topsfield, Julie D Henry, Sarah P Coundouris

Empathy plays a fundamental role in successful social interactions. However, most tasks currently available for measuring empathy have limited ecological validity and therefore may not elicit true emotional responses in observers. To address this gap, we developed the Empathic Measure of True Emotion (EMOTE), the first emotion stimuli set to include footage of genuine positive and negative emotions unfolding in naturalistic contexts. We validated the EMOTE in a sample of 216 participants. The EMOTE demonstrated acceptable internal consistency, construct validity, and alternate forms reliability for both cognitive and affective empathy. We also found that, relative to conventional empathy measures, the EMOTE elicited stronger affective empathy ratings in observers, and the stimuli were rated higher in both genuineness and emotional intensity. Together, these findings demonstrate that the EMOTE is a reliable and valid measure of cognitive and affective empathy with enhanced ecological validity, providing a valuable new tool for measuring empathy in both clinical and research settings.

移情在成功的社会交往中起着根本性的作用。然而,目前大多数用于测量移情的任务的生态有效性有限,因此可能无法引起观察者的真实情绪反应。为了弥补这一不足,我们开发了共情真实情绪测量(EMOTE),这是第一套包含在自然情境中展开的真实积极和消极情绪片段的情绪刺激集。我们在 216 名参与者的样本中验证了 EMOTE。EMOTE 在认知移情和情感移情方面都表现出了可接受的内部一致性、建构有效性和替代形式可靠性。我们还发现,与传统的移情测量法相比,EMOTE 能引起观察者更强烈的情感移情评价,而且刺激物的真实性和情感强度都得到了更高的评价。这些发现共同表明,EMOTE 是一种可靠有效的认知共情和情感共情测量方法,具有更高的生态效度,为临床和研究环境中的共情测量提供了一种有价值的新工具。
{"title":"The empathic measure of true emotion (EMOTE): a novel set of stimuli for measuring emotional responding.","authors":"Sarah A Grainger, Alana J Topsfield, Julie D Henry, Sarah P Coundouris","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2427889","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2024.2427889","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Empathy plays a fundamental role in successful social interactions. However, most tasks currently available for measuring empathy have limited ecological validity and therefore may not elicit true emotional responses in observers. To address this gap, we developed the Empathic Measure of True Emotion (EMOTE), the first emotion stimuli set to include footage of genuine positive and negative emotions unfolding in naturalistic contexts. We validated the EMOTE in a sample of 216 participants. The EMOTE demonstrated acceptable internal consistency, construct validity, and alternate forms reliability for both cognitive and affective empathy. We also found that, relative to conventional empathy measures, the EMOTE elicited stronger affective empathy ratings in observers, and the stimuli were rated higher in both genuineness and emotional intensity. Together, these findings demonstrate that the EMOTE is a reliable and valid measure of cognitive and affective empathy with enhanced ecological validity, providing a valuable new tool for measuring empathy in both clinical and research settings.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1-10"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142630715","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Not feeling it: lack of robust emotion effects on breadth of attention. 没有感觉:情绪对注意力广度缺乏强有力的影响。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-12 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2427329
Martin Kolnes, Andero Uusberg

Emotional states are believed to broaden or to narrow the focus of attention. However, numerous inconsistent findings call for renewed efforts to understand the conditions under which such effects occur. We conducted a pair of high-powered web experiments. Emotional states were manipulated across valence categories and appraisal dimensions using autobiographical recall (Experiment 1) and emotional images (Experiment 2). Breadth of attention was assessed using the Navon task coupled with induction sensitivity and mouse tracking analyses. We did not find robust evidence for emotional effects on breadth of attention. Negative images led surprisingly to slightly broader attention in Experiment 2, but this may reflect the slow release of cognitive resources from preceding negative stimuli amplifying the global precedence effect. Breadth of attention also had very small positive relationships with goal-congruence appraisal in the first and control appraisal in the second experiment. We also found no evidence for moderation by mood or personality. Taken together, our findings add to the growing list of failures to observe emotional modulation of breadth of attention and call for continued efforts to chart the boundary conditions of these effects.

情绪状态被认为会扩大或缩小注意力的焦点。然而,许多不一致的研究结果要求我们重新努力了解这种效应发生的条件。我们进行了一对高能网络实验。我们使用自传体回忆(实验 1)和情绪图像(实验 2)对情绪状态进行了跨情绪类别和评价维度的操纵。通过纳文任务以及诱导敏感性和鼠标跟踪分析来评估注意力的广度。我们没有发现情绪对注意力广度产生影响的有力证据。在实验 2 中,负面图像出乎意料地导致了略微广泛的注意,但这可能反映了之前负面刺激的认知资源释放缓慢,放大了整体优先效应。在第一项实验中,注意广度与目标一致性评价有非常小的正相关关系,而在第二项实验中,注意广度与控制评价也有非常小的正相关关系。我们也没有发现情绪或性格对其有调节作用的证据。总之,我们的研究结果为越来越多未能观察到情绪对注意广度调节的实验增添了新的证据,同时也呼吁人们继续努力,找出这些效应的边界条件。
{"title":"Not feeling it: lack of robust emotion effects on breadth of attention.","authors":"Martin Kolnes, Andero Uusberg","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2427329","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2024.2427329","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Emotional states are believed to broaden or to narrow the focus of attention. However, numerous inconsistent findings call for renewed efforts to understand the conditions under which such effects occur. We conducted a pair of high-powered web experiments. Emotional states were manipulated across valence categories and appraisal dimensions using autobiographical recall (Experiment 1) and emotional images (Experiment 2). Breadth of attention was assessed using the Navon task coupled with induction sensitivity and mouse tracking analyses. We did not find robust evidence for emotional effects on breadth of attention. Negative images led surprisingly to slightly broader attention in Experiment 2, but this may reflect the slow release of cognitive resources from preceding negative stimuli amplifying the global precedence effect. Breadth of attention also had very small positive relationships with goal-congruence appraisal in the first and control appraisal in the second experiment. We also found no evidence for moderation by mood or personality. Taken together, our findings add to the growing list of failures to observe emotional modulation of breadth of attention and call for continued efforts to chart the boundary conditions of these effects.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1-23"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142630697","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Peer threat evaluations shape one's own threat perceptions and feelings of distress. 同伴的威胁评价会影响自己的威胁感和痛苦感。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-12 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2417231
Lisa Espinosa, Erik C Nook, Martin Asperholm, Therese Collins, Juliet Y Davidow, Andreas Olsson

We are continuously exposed to what others think and feel about content online. How do others' evaluations shared in this medium influence our own beliefs and emotional responses? In two pre-registered studies, we investigated the social transmission of threat and safety evaluations in a paradigm that mimicked online social media platforms. In Study 1 (N = 103), participants viewed images and indicated how distressed they made them feel. Participants then categorised these images as threatening or safe for others to see, while seeing how "previous participants" ostensibly categorised them (these values were actually manipulated across images). We found that participants incorporated both peers' categorisations of the images and their own distress ratings when categorizing images as threatening or safe. Study 2 (N = 115) replicated these findings and further demonstrated that peers' categorisations shifted how distressed these images made them feel. Taken together, our results indicate that people integrate their own and others' experiences when exposed to emotional content and that social information can influence both our perceptions of things as threatening or safe, as well as our own emotional responses to them. Our findings provide replicable experimental evidence that social information is a powerful conduit for the transmission of affective evaluations and experiences.

我们不断接触到他人对网络内容的看法和感受。在这种媒介中分享的他人评价如何影响我们自己的信念和情绪反应?在两项预先注册的研究中,我们在模仿网络社交媒体平台的范例中调查了威胁和安全评价的社会传播。在研究 1(N = 103)中,参与者观看图片,并指出图片给他们带来的困扰。然后,参与者将这些图片归类为具有威胁性或安全的图片,供其他人观看,同时查看 "先前参与者 "表面上是如何归类这些图片的(这些数值实际上是在不同图片之间进行操作的)。我们发现,参与者在对图像进行威胁性或安全性分类时,既考虑了同伴对图像的分类,也考虑了自己的痛苦评级。研究 2(N = 115)重复了这些发现,并进一步证明同伴的分类会改变这些图像给他们带来的困扰程度。总之,我们的研究结果表明,当人们接触到情绪内容时,他们会将自己和他人的体验结合起来,社会信息既能影响我们对威胁性或安全性事物的感知,也能影响我们自己对这些事物的情绪反应。我们的研究结果提供了可复制的实验证据,证明社会信息是传递情感评价和体验的有力渠道。
{"title":"Peer threat evaluations shape one's own threat perceptions and feelings of distress.","authors":"Lisa Espinosa, Erik C Nook, Martin Asperholm, Therese Collins, Juliet Y Davidow, Andreas Olsson","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2417231","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2024.2417231","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We are continuously exposed to what others think and feel about content online. How do others' evaluations shared in this medium influence our own beliefs and emotional responses? In two pre-registered studies, we investigated the social transmission of threat and safety evaluations in a paradigm that mimicked online social media platforms. In Study 1 (N = 103), participants viewed images and indicated how distressed they made them feel. Participants then categorised these images as threatening or safe for others to see, while seeing how \"previous participants\" ostensibly categorised them (these values were actually manipulated across images). We found that participants incorporated both peers' categorisations of the images and their own distress ratings when categorizing images as threatening or safe. Study 2 (N = 115) replicated these findings and further demonstrated that peers' categorisations shifted how distressed these images made them feel. Taken together, our results indicate that people integrate their own and others' experiences when exposed to emotional content and that social information can influence both our perceptions of things as threatening or safe, as well as our own emotional responses to them. Our findings provide replicable experimental evidence that social information is a powerful conduit for the transmission of affective evaluations and experiences.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1-14"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142630701","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Humour in trolley problems and other sacrificial dilemmas: killing is not funny at all. 手推车问题和其他牺牲困境中的幽默:杀人一点也不好笑。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-12 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2426674
Robin Carron, Nathalie Blanc, Emmanuelle Brigaud

Three studies were designed to explore a major criticism of sacrificial dilemmas, namely that their potential humorous aspects may distort moral decision-making. We collected moral responses (i.e. moral judgment and choice of action) but also asked participants to rate the funniness of moral dilemmas, in order to combine humour assessment and moral responses. In addition, the emotional responses to moral dilemmas were recorded for both men and women (including emotions related to humour), and the potential effect of individuals' need for humour was also considered. Overall, three main results were reported. Firstly, the dilemmas used in our studies were not rated as funny at all. Secondly, reading moral dilemmas increased negative emotions (i.e. sadness, disgust, guilt) and decreased positive emotions associated with humour (i.e. joy, amusement, and mirth), with gender effects since women experienced stronger negative emotions than men. Thirdly, funniness ratings of sacrificial dilemmas did not vary according to gender and need for humour. This series of studies does not report empirical evidence to support the humorous aspects of trolley-type dilemmas, but invites a more systematic examination of how sacrificial dilemmas are perceived by participants who have to produce moral responses.

我们设计了三项研究来探讨对牺牲型两难处境的一个主要批评,即其潜在的幽默方面可能会扭曲道德决策。我们在收集道德反应(即道德判断和行动选择)的同时,还要求参与者对道德困境的趣味性进行评分,以便将幽默评估和道德反应结合起来。此外,我们还记录了男性和女性对道德困境的情绪反应(包括与幽默有关的情绪),并考虑了个人对幽默需求的潜在影响。总体而言,报告了三项主要结果。首先,我们研究中使用的两难困境根本没有被评为有趣。其次,阅读道德窘境会增加负面情绪(即悲伤、厌恶、内疚),减少与幽默相关的正面情绪(即快乐、愉悦和欢笑)。第三,对牺牲困境的趣味性评价并不因性别和对幽默的需求而异。这一系列的研究报告并没有提供实证证据来支持电车式两难处境的幽默性,但却需要对必须做出道德反应的参与者如何看待牺牲性两难处境进行更系统的研究。
{"title":"Humour in trolley problems and other sacrificial dilemmas: killing is not funny at all.","authors":"Robin Carron, Nathalie Blanc, Emmanuelle Brigaud","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2426674","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2024.2426674","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Three studies were designed to explore a major criticism of sacrificial dilemmas, namely that their potential humorous aspects may distort moral decision-making. We collected moral responses (i.e. moral judgment and choice of action) but also asked participants to rate the funniness of moral dilemmas, in order to combine humour assessment and moral responses. In addition, the emotional responses to moral dilemmas were recorded for both men and women (including emotions related to humour), and the potential effect of individuals' need for humour was also considered. Overall, three main results were reported. Firstly, the dilemmas used in our studies were not rated as funny at all. Secondly, reading moral dilemmas increased negative emotions (i.e. sadness, disgust, guilt) and decreased positive emotions associated with humour (i.e. joy, amusement, and mirth), with gender effects since women experienced stronger negative emotions than men. Thirdly, funniness ratings of sacrificial dilemmas did not vary according to gender and need for humour. This series of studies does not report empirical evidence to support the humorous aspects of trolley-type dilemmas, but invites a more systematic examination of how sacrificial dilemmas are perceived by participants who have to produce moral responses.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1-15"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142630692","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A behavioural test of depression-related probability bias. 抑郁症相关概率偏差的行为测试。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-12 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2425695
Robert W Booth, Selen Gönül, B Deniz Sözügür, Khadija Khalid

Individuals high in depressive symptom severity show probability bias: they believe negative events are relatively probable, and positive events relatively improbable, compared to those with less severe symptoms. However, this has only ever been demonstrated using self-report measures, in which participants explicitly estimate events' probabilities: this leaves open the risk that "probability bias" is merely an artefact of response bias. We tested the veracity of probability bias using an indirect behavioural measure, based on a sentence-reading task. Study 1 tested 112 Turkish students; Study 2 tested 117 international users of online groups for people with depressive and anxiety disorders. As predicted, participants with higher depressive symptom scores responded relatively quickly to sentences stating negative events might occur, and relatively slowly to sentences stating positive events might occur, compared to those with lower scores. This effect was only marginal in Study 1, but reached significance in Study 2. However, contrary to predictions, this effect was not moderated by the probability level stated in the sentence. This makes our findings difficult to interpret, and we must present these studies as a failure to convincingly demonstrate depression-related probability bias. We hope this stimulates more work on the nature and veracity of probability bias.

抑郁症状严重程度高的人表现出概率偏差:与症状较轻的人相比,他们认为消极事件的发生概率相对较高,而积极事件的发生概率相对较低。然而,这种情况只在自我报告测量中得到过证实,在自我报告测量中,参与者会明确估计事件发生的概率:这使得 "概率偏差 "有可能只是反应偏差的一种假象。我们使用一种间接的行为测量方法来测试概率偏差的真实性,该方法基于一项句子阅读任务。研究 1 测试了 112 名土耳其学生;研究 2 测试了 117 名抑郁症和焦虑症患者在线群组的国际用户。正如预测的那样,与得分较低的参与者相比,抑郁症状得分较高的参与者对说明可能会发生消极事件的句子反应相对较快,而对说明可能会发生积极事件的句子反应相对较慢。在研究 1 中,这种效应只是微不足道,但在研究 2 中却达到了显著性。然而,与预测相反的是,这种效应并不受句子中所述概率水平的影响。这使得我们的研究结果难以解释,我们必须将这些研究作为未能令人信服地证明与抑郁有关的概率偏差来看待。我们希望这能激发更多关于概率偏差的性质和真实性的研究。
{"title":"A behavioural test of depression-related probability bias.","authors":"Robert W Booth, Selen Gönül, B Deniz Sözügür, Khadija Khalid","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2425695","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2024.2425695","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Individuals high in depressive symptom severity show probability bias: they believe negative events are relatively probable, and positive events relatively improbable, compared to those with less severe symptoms. However, this has only ever been demonstrated using self-report measures, in which participants explicitly estimate events' probabilities: this leaves open the risk that \"probability bias\" is merely an artefact of response bias. We tested the veracity of probability bias using an indirect behavioural measure, based on a sentence-reading task. Study 1 tested 112 Turkish students; Study 2 tested 117 international users of online groups for people with depressive and anxiety disorders. As predicted, participants with higher depressive symptom scores responded relatively quickly to sentences stating negative events might occur, and relatively slowly to sentences stating positive events might occur, compared to those with lower scores. This effect was only marginal in Study 1, but reached significance in Study 2. However, contrary to predictions, this effect was not moderated by the probability level stated in the sentence. This makes our findings difficult to interpret, and we must present these studies as a failure to convincingly demonstrate depression-related probability bias. We hope this stimulates more work on the nature and veracity of probability bias.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1-15"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142630687","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The dual-feature approach-avoidance task: validity, training efficacy, and the role of contingency awareness in changing food preference. 双特征接近-回避任务:有效性、训练效果以及应急意识在改变食物偏好中的作用。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-06 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2418445
Sercan Kahveci, Hannah van Alebeek, Jens Blechert

The approach-avoidance task (AAT) probes tendencies contributing to unwanted behaviours, like excessive snacking, by measuring RT differences between approach and avoidance responses to different stimuli. It retrains such tendencies using repeated avoidance of appetitive stimuli and approach of healthy alternatives. The most common paradigm, the irrelevant-feature AAT, conceals these stimulus-response contingencies by requiring approach or avoidance based on features irrelevant to the tendencies (e.g. frame colour). Unfortunately, it is an unreliable measure and not always successful as a training, likely because the stimuli can be ignored. In the novel dual-feature AAT, the combination of stimulus and irrelevant feature determines responses: participants approach foods and avoid objects surrounded by frame A, and vice versa given frame B. We trained 219 online participants to approach fruit and avoid chocolate using active (7:1 stimulus-to-frame contingency) and sham (1:1 stimulus-to-frame contingency) versions of these two trainings. Compared to sham, active irrelevant-feature training was associated with more selection and desire to eat fruit, and active dual-feature training increased approach bias for fruit. Participants' perceived rate of approaching fruit versus chocolate correlated with many outcome measures, suggesting contingency awareness plays a major role in AAT training effectiveness, challenging implicit accounts. While the dual-feature paradigm shows potential, its high error rates, RTs, and difficulty mandate improvement.

接近-回避任务(AAT)通过测量对不同刺激的接近反应和回避反应之间的RT差异,来探测导致过量吃零食等不良行为的倾向。它通过反复回避食欲性刺激和接近健康的替代品来重新训练这种倾向。最常见的范式是无关特征 AAT,它要求根据与倾向无关的特征(如框架颜色)来接近或回避,从而掩盖了这些刺激-反应或然性。遗憾的是,这种测量方法并不可靠,而且作为一种训练方法也并不总是成功的,这很可能是因为刺激可以被忽略。在新颖的双特征 AAT 中,刺激和无关特征的组合决定了反应:参与者接近食物并避开被框架 A 包围的物体,反之亦然。我们使用这两种训练的主动版本(刺激与框架的或然率为 7:1)和假版本(刺激与框架的或然率为 1:1),训练 219 名在线参与者接近水果并避开巧克力。与假训练相比,积极的无关特征训练与更多选择和想吃水果有关,而积极的双特征训练增加了接近水果的偏差。参与者接近水果和巧克力的感知率与许多结果测量相关,这表明或然意识在AAT训练效果中起着重要作用,对隐含的说法提出了挑战。虽然双特征范式显示出了潜力,但其高错误率、高反应时间和高难度仍有待改进。
{"title":"The dual-feature approach-avoidance task: validity, training efficacy, and the role of contingency awareness in changing food preference.","authors":"Sercan Kahveci, Hannah van Alebeek, Jens Blechert","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2418445","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2024.2418445","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The approach-avoidance task (AAT) probes tendencies contributing to unwanted behaviours, like excessive snacking, by measuring RT differences between approach and avoidance responses to different stimuli. It retrains such tendencies using repeated avoidance of appetitive stimuli and approach of healthy alternatives. The most common paradigm, the irrelevant-feature AAT, conceals these stimulus-response contingencies by requiring approach or avoidance based on features irrelevant to the tendencies (e.g. frame colour). Unfortunately, it is an unreliable measure and not always successful as a training, likely because the stimuli can be ignored. In the novel dual-feature AAT, the <i>combination</i> of stimulus and irrelevant feature determines responses: participants approach foods and avoid objects surrounded by frame A, and vice versa given frame B. We trained 219 online participants to approach fruit and avoid chocolate using active (7:1 stimulus-to-frame contingency) and sham (1:1 stimulus-to-frame contingency) versions of these two trainings. Compared to sham, active irrelevant-feature training was associated with more selection and desire to eat fruit, and active dual-feature training increased approach bias for fruit. Participants' <i>perceived</i> rate of approaching fruit versus chocolate correlated with many outcome measures, suggesting contingency awareness plays a major role in AAT training effectiveness, challenging implicit accounts. While the dual-feature paradigm shows potential, its high error rates, RTs, and difficulty mandate improvement.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1-23"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142592145","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotional stimuli boost incidental learning through predictive processing. 情绪刺激通过预测处理促进附带学习。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-05 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2418444
Meital Friedman-Oskar, Tomer Sahar, Tal Makovski, Hadas Okon-Singer

Extracting regularities and probabilities from the environment is a fundamental and critical ability in an ever-changing surrounding. Previous findings showed that people are highly efficient in learning these regularities and that emotional stimuli are better learned than neutral ones. Yet, the generality and the underlying mechanism of this benefit are poorly understood. Here, participants viewed a stream of images with negative and neutral valence. Unbeknownst, the items recurred in regularity as triplets. Then, to assess learning, a surprised familiarity test was conducted. The results of Experiment 1, using two sets of stimuli, found better statistical learning for negative triplets than for neutral triplets. Experiment 2 revealed similar benefits even when only a single negative item was in the triplet at the second or third position, suggesting the advantage is not cumulative. We speculated that the predictability of the negative items is driving the effect. Consequently, Experiment 3 confirmed that the memory for neutral items preceding negative items was better than for neutral items preceding neutral items. Together, these findings provide novel insights into the mechanism of how the learning of incidental temporal associations is influenced by negative stimuli and the role of predictability in the negative valence benefit.

在瞬息万变的环境中,从环境中提取规律性和概率是一项基本而关键的能力。以前的研究结果表明,人们学习这些规律性的效率很高,而且情绪性刺激比中性刺激学习效果更好。然而,人们对这种益处的普遍性和内在机制还知之甚少。在这里,参与者观看的是具有负面和中性情绪的图像流。不知不觉中,这些项目以三胞胎的形式有规律地重复出现。然后,为了评估学习效果,进行了惊讶熟悉度测试。实验 1 使用了两组刺激物,结果发现负面三连拍的统计学习效果比中性三连拍更好。实验 2 发现,即使三连音中只有一个负面项目位于第二或第三位,也能获得类似的优势,这表明这种优势不是累积性的。我们推测是负面项目的可预测性推动了这种效应。因此,实验 3 证实,对负面项目之前的中性项目的记忆优于对中性项目之前的中性项目的记忆。总之,这些发现为我们提供了新的视角,让我们了解负面刺激如何影响偶然时间联想的学习机制,以及可预测性在负价益处中的作用。
{"title":"Emotional stimuli boost incidental learning through predictive processing.","authors":"Meital Friedman-Oskar, Tomer Sahar, Tal Makovski, Hadas Okon-Singer","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2418444","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2024.2418444","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Extracting regularities and probabilities from the environment is a fundamental and critical ability in an ever-changing surrounding. Previous findings showed that people are highly efficient in learning these regularities and that emotional stimuli are better learned than neutral ones. Yet, the generality and the underlying mechanism of this benefit are poorly understood. Here, participants viewed a stream of images with negative and neutral valence. Unbeknownst, the items recurred in regularity as triplets. Then, to assess learning, a surprised familiarity test was conducted. The results of Experiment 1, using two sets of stimuli, found better statistical learning for negative triplets than for neutral triplets. Experiment 2 revealed similar benefits even when only a single negative item was in the triplet at the second or third position, suggesting the advantage is not cumulative. We speculated that the predictability of the negative items is driving the effect. Consequently, Experiment 3 confirmed that the memory for neutral items preceding negative items was better than for neutral items preceding neutral items. Together, these findings provide novel insights into the mechanism of how the learning of incidental temporal associations is influenced by negative stimuli and the role of predictability in the negative valence benefit.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1-18"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142584419","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bouncing back from emotional ups and downs: insights in emotional recovery using survival analyses of burst ESM data. 从情绪起伏中反弹:利用突发 ESM 数据生存分析法了解情绪恢复情况。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-05 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2423706
Marieke J Schreuder, Sigert Ariens, Ginette Lafit, Eva Ceulemans

Many experience sampling (ESM) studies suggested that high resilience is reflected by quickly recovering one's emotional baseline. However, former studies relied on coarse data and did not look into differential recovery from emotional ups and downs. This preregistered proof-of-concept study therefore used high-resolution data collected in 2022 to compute emotional recovery after high levels of positive versus negative emotions. Adults (N = 68) participated in a three-week ESM study with eight assessments per day, complemented by short-spaced burst assessments. Resilience was assessed at baseline (trait-level; TR) and daily (day-level; DR). Multilevel survival analyses showed that high DR predicted faster returns from negative emotions, but also delayed returns following positive emotions (exp(β) = 1.32, p = 0.006). Instead, TR did not relate to emotional recovery (exp(β) = 0.85, p = 0.067). These findings were generally robust across different sensitivity analyses. This illustrates how innovative ESM designs combined with time-to-event analyses may further our insight in emotional recovery and the timescale at which it unfolds.

许多经验取样(ESM)研究表明,快速恢复一个人的情绪基线反映了高复原力。然而,以前的研究依赖于粗略的数据,并没有研究情绪起伏的不同恢复情况。因此,这项预先注册的概念验证研究使用了 2022 年收集的高分辨率数据来计算高水平积极情绪和消极情绪后的情绪恢复情况。成人(68 人)参加了为期三周的 ESM 研究,每天进行八次评估,并辅以短时突发评估。复原力在基线(特质水平;TR)和每日(日水平;DR)进行评估。多层次生存分析表明,高DR预示着消极情绪恢复更快,但也预示着积极情绪恢复延迟(exp(β) = 1.32, p = 0.006)。相反,TR 与情绪恢复无关(exp(β) = 0.85, p = 0.067)。这些结果在不同的敏感性分析中总体上是稳健的。这说明创新性的 ESM 设计与时间到事件分析相结合,可以进一步加深我们对情绪恢复及其发展时间尺度的了解。
{"title":"Bouncing back from emotional ups and downs: insights in emotional recovery using survival analyses of burst ESM data.","authors":"Marieke J Schreuder, Sigert Ariens, Ginette Lafit, Eva Ceulemans","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2423706","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2024.2423706","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many experience sampling (ESM) studies suggested that high resilience is reflected by quickly recovering one's emotional baseline. However, former studies relied on coarse data and did not look into differential recovery from emotional ups and downs. This preregistered proof-of-concept study therefore used high-resolution data collected in 2022 to compute emotional recovery after high levels of positive versus negative emotions. Adults (<i>N</i> = 68) participated in a three-week ESM study with eight assessments per day, complemented by short-spaced burst assessments. Resilience was assessed at baseline (trait-level; TR) and daily (day-level; DR). Multilevel survival analyses showed that high DR predicted faster returns from negative emotions, but also delayed returns following positive emotions (exp(<i>β</i>) = 1.32, <i>p</i> = 0.006). Instead, TR did not relate to emotional recovery (exp(<i>β</i>) = 0.85, <i>p</i> = 0.067). These findings were generally robust across different sensitivity analyses. This illustrates how innovative ESM designs combined with time-to-event analyses may further our insight in emotional recovery and the timescale at which it unfolds.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1-10"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142584374","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Overestimating the intensity of negative feelings in autobiographical memory: evidence from the 9/11 attack and COVID-19 pandemic. 高估自传体记忆中负面情绪的强度:来自 9/11 袭击和 COVID-19 大流行的证据。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-07 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2346757
Juan Castillo, Haoxue Fan, Olivia T Karaman, Jocelyn Shu, Yoann Stussi, M Alexandra Kredlow, Sophia Vranos, Javiera P Oyarzún, Hayley M Dorfman, Deshawn Chatman Sambrano, Robert Meksin, William Hirst, Elizabeth A Phelps

When recalling autobiographical events, people not only retrieve event details but also the feelings they experienced. The current study examined whether people are able to consistently recall the intensity of past feelings associated with two consequential and negatively valenced events, i.e. the 9/11 attack (N = 769) and the COVID-19 pandemic (N = 726). By comparing experienced and recalled intensities of negative feelings, we discovered that people systematically recall a higher intensity of negative feelings than initially reported - overestimating the intensity of past negative emotional experiences. The COVID-19 dataset also revealed that individuals who experienced greater improvement in emotional well-being displayed smaller biases in recalling their feelings. Across both datasets, the intensity of remembered feelings was correlated with initial feelings and current feelings, but the impact of the current feelings was stronger in the COVID-19 dataset than in the 9/11 dataset. Our results demonstrate that when recalling negative autobiographical events, people tend to overestimate the intensity of prior negative emotional experiences with their degree of bias influenced by current feelings and well-being.

在回忆自传体事件时,人们不仅会检索事件细节,还会检索他们所经历的感受。本研究考察了人们是否能够持续回忆起过去与两个后果性和负向情感事件相关的情感强度,即 9/11 袭击(769 人)和 COVID-19 大流行(726 人)。通过比较经历过的和回忆起的负面情绪强度,我们发现人们系统性地回忆起了比最初报告的更高强度的负面情绪--高估了过去负面情绪经历的强度。COVID-19 数据集还显示,情绪健康状况改善较多的人在回忆自己的感受时表现出的偏差较小。在这两个数据集中,记忆中的情感强度与最初的情感和当前的情感相关,但在 COVID-19 数据集中,当前情感的影响比在 9/11 数据集中更大。我们的研究结果表明,在回忆负面自传事件时,人们往往会高估之前负面情绪体验的强度,其偏差程度会受到当前感受和幸福感的影响。
{"title":"Overestimating the intensity of negative feelings in autobiographical memory: evidence from the 9/11 attack and COVID-19 pandemic.","authors":"Juan Castillo, Haoxue Fan, Olivia T Karaman, Jocelyn Shu, Yoann Stussi, M Alexandra Kredlow, Sophia Vranos, Javiera P Oyarzún, Hayley M Dorfman, Deshawn Chatman Sambrano, Robert Meksin, William Hirst, Elizabeth A Phelps","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2346757","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2346757","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When recalling autobiographical events, people not only retrieve event details but also the feelings they experienced. The current study examined whether people are able to consistently recall the intensity of past feelings associated with two consequential and negatively valenced events, i.e. the 9/11 attack (<i>N</i> = 769) and the COVID-19 pandemic (<i>N</i> = 726). By comparing experienced and recalled intensities of negative feelings, we discovered that people systematically recall a higher intensity of negative feelings than initially reported - overestimating the intensity of past negative emotional experiences. The COVID-19 dataset also revealed that individuals who experienced greater improvement in emotional well-being displayed smaller biases in recalling their feelings. Across both datasets, the intensity of remembered feelings was correlated with initial feelings and current feelings, but the impact of the current feelings was stronger in the COVID-19 dataset than in the 9/11 dataset. Our results demonstrate that when recalling negative autobiographical events, people tend to overestimate the intensity of prior negative emotional experiences with their degree of bias influenced by current feelings and well-being.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1048-1063"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11502283/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140867868","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Probability, cost, and interpretation biases' relationships with depressive and anxious symptom severity: differential mediation by worry and repetitive negative thinking. 概率、成本和解释偏差与抑郁和焦虑症状严重程度的关系:担心和重复负面思维的不同中介作用。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-01 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2348031
Robert W Booth, Bundy Mackintosh, Servet Hasşerbetçi

People high in depressive or anxious symptom severity show repetitive negative thinking, including worry and rumination. They also show various cognitive phenomena, including probability, cost, and interpretation biases. Since there is conceptual overlap between these cognitive biases and repetitive negative thinking - all involve thinking about potential threats and misfortunes - we wondered whether repetitive negative thinking could account for (mediate) these cognitive biases' associations with depressive and anxious symptom severity. In three studies, conducted in two languages and cultures, cost bias and (in two studies) interpretation bias only predicted symptom severity via worry and repetitive negative thinking; this suggests these biases are actually associated with repetitive negative thinking, rather than with symptoms. In contrast, probability bias showed direct relationships with depressive (all studies) and anxious (two studies) symptom severity, suggesting its relationships with symptoms are partly independent of repetitive negative thinking. These results show the value of studying relationships among the various cognitive features of psychopathology. Furthermore, new interventions which target cognitive biases in depression or anxiety must show that they can improve upon cognitive behavioural therapy, which is already widely available, targets both repetitive negative thinking and probability bias, and is highly effective.

抑郁或焦虑症状严重程度高的人表现出反复的负面思维,包括担忧和反刍。他们还表现出各种认知现象,包括概率偏差、成本偏差和解释偏差。由于这些认知偏差与重复性消极思维在概念上存在重叠--都涉及对潜在威胁和不幸的思考,我们想知道重复性消极思维是否能解释(调解)这些认知偏差与抑郁和焦虑症状严重程度的关联。在以两种语言和文化进行的三项研究中,成本偏差和(两项研究中的)解释偏差只能通过担忧和重复性消极思维来预测症状严重程度;这表明这些偏差实际上与重复性消极思维有关,而不是与症状有关。相反,概率偏差与抑郁(所有研究)和焦虑(两项研究)症状严重程度有直接关系,这表明概率偏差与症状的关系部分独立于重复性消极思维。这些结果表明了研究精神病理学各种认知特征之间关系的价值。此外,针对抑郁症或焦虑症认知偏差的新干预措施必须证明它们能够改进认知行为疗法,因为认知行为疗法已经广泛使用,既针对重复性消极思维,也针对概率偏差,而且非常有效。
{"title":"Probability, cost, and interpretation biases' relationships with depressive and anxious symptom severity: differential mediation by worry and repetitive negative thinking.","authors":"Robert W Booth, Bundy Mackintosh, Servet Hasşerbetçi","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2348031","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2348031","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People high in depressive or anxious symptom severity show repetitive negative thinking, including worry and rumination. They also show various cognitive phenomena, including probability, cost, and interpretation biases. Since there is conceptual overlap between these cognitive biases and repetitive negative thinking - all involve thinking about potential threats and misfortunes - we wondered whether repetitive negative thinking could account for (mediate) these cognitive biases' associations with depressive and anxious symptom severity. In three studies, conducted in two languages and cultures, cost bias and (in two studies) interpretation bias only predicted symptom severity via worry and repetitive negative thinking; this suggests these biases are actually associated with repetitive negative thinking, rather than with symptoms. In contrast, probability bias showed direct relationships with depressive (all studies) and anxious (two studies) symptom severity, suggesting its relationships with symptoms are partly independent of repetitive negative thinking. These results show the value of studying relationships among the various cognitive features of psychopathology. Furthermore, new interventions which target cognitive biases in depression or anxiety must show that they can improve upon cognitive behavioural therapy, which is already widely available, targets both repetitive negative thinking and probability bias, and is highly effective.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1064-1079"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140874909","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognition & Emotion
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1