首页 > 最新文献

Cognition & Emotion最新文献

英文 中文
Targeting schema change in social anxiety via autobiographical memory reconstruction. 基于自传体记忆重建的社会焦虑图式改变研究。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-02 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2433516
Signy Sheldon, Luke Atack, Nguyet Ngo, Morris Moscovitch, David A Moscovitch

Negative self-schemas are fundamental to social anxiety disorder and contribute to its persistence, thus understanding how to change schemas is of critical importance. Memory-based interventions and associated theories propose that reconstructing autobiographical memories tethered to schemas with conceptual details that challenge the associated expectations will lead to schema change. Here, we test this proposal in a between-subjects behavioural experiment with undergraduate participants with social anxiety. All participants were asked to recall aversive social memories, evaluated these memories on a series of scales, including estimates of reoccurrence, and provided ratings of negative and positive schema beliefs. Next, half the participants reconstructed (rescripted) these aversive memories with conceptual details that challenged the active schema (conceptual condition) and the other half reconstructed the memories with additional experiential details (perceptual condition). All participants provided again evaluations of the original memory and their schema beliefs. Our analysis revealed that the conceptual condition led to significant reductions in negative self-schemas, increases in positive self-schemas, and decreases in estimates of future negative event reoccurrence. Thus, effective schema-change, both a weakening of negative schemas and a strengthening of more positive, adaptive schemas, is dependent on altering the underlying meaning of associated autobiographical memories.

消极自我图式是社交焦虑障碍的基础,也是其持续存在的原因之一,因此了解如何改变消极自我图式至关重要。基于记忆的干预和相关理论提出,重建与图式相关的自传式记忆,这些记忆具有挑战相关期望的概念细节,将导致图式改变。在这里,我们在一个被试之间的行为实验中测试了这一建议,实验对象是患有社交焦虑的大学生。所有的参与者都被要求回忆令人厌恶的社会记忆,用一系列的量表评估这些记忆,包括对重现的估计,并提供消极和积极图式信念的评级。接下来,一半的参与者用挑战主动图式的概念性细节重构(重写)这些厌恶记忆(概念条件),另一半用额外的经验细节重构记忆(知觉条件)。所有参与者再次对原始记忆和他们的图式信念进行评估。我们的分析表明,概念性条件导致消极自我图式的显著减少,积极自我图式的增加,以及对未来消极事件再次发生的估计的降低。因此,有效的图式改变,无论是消极图式的弱化还是积极适应性图式的强化,都依赖于相关自传体记忆的潜在意义的改变。
{"title":"Targeting schema change in social anxiety via autobiographical memory reconstruction.","authors":"Signy Sheldon, Luke Atack, Nguyet Ngo, Morris Moscovitch, David A Moscovitch","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2433516","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2024.2433516","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Negative self-schemas are fundamental to social anxiety disorder and contribute to its persistence, thus understanding how to change schemas is of critical importance. Memory-based interventions and associated theories propose that reconstructing autobiographical memories tethered to schemas with conceptual details that challenge the associated expectations will lead to schema change. Here, we test this proposal in a between-subjects behavioural experiment with undergraduate participants with social anxiety. All participants were asked to recall aversive social memories, evaluated these memories on a series of scales, including estimates of reoccurrence, and provided ratings of negative and positive schema beliefs. Next, half the participants reconstructed (rescripted) these aversive memories with conceptual details that challenged the active schema (conceptual condition) and the other half reconstructed the memories with additional experiential details (perceptual condition). All participants provided again evaluations of the original memory and their schema beliefs. Our analysis revealed that the conceptual condition led to significant reductions in negative self-schemas, increases in positive self-schemas, and decreases in estimates of future negative event reoccurrence. Thus, effective schema-change, both a weakening of negative schemas and a strengthening of more positive, adaptive schemas, is dependent on altering the underlying meaning of associated autobiographical memories.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1-9"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-12-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142773802","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The interplay of social rank perceptions of Trump and Biden and emotions following the U.S. presidential election 2020. 对特朗普和拜登的社会等级认知与 2020 年美国总统大选后情绪的相互作用。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-26 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2356713
Lea Boecker, Hannes M Petrowsky, David D Loschelder, Jens Lange

The outcome of the 2020 U.S. election between Trump and Biden evoked strong emotions. In U.S. American (Study 1; N = 405) and German (Study 2; N = 123) samples, we investigated how observers' group membership (i.e. political orientation) and the social rank attainment of both candidates (i.e. dominance vs. prestige) predicted emotional reactions. Trump was generally perceived as more dominant, and Biden as more prestigious. However, perceptions of social rank attainment differed depending on the observers' political orientation, either matching or not matching with the leaders (i.e. Republicans and Democrats, respectively). The candidate who did not share the participants' political orientation was perceived as less prestigious and more dominant and elicited stronger contrastive emotions (i.e. schadenfreude, malicious envy) and weaker assimilative emotions (i.e. happy-for-ness, sympathy, anger), and vice versa. Crucially, dominance and prestige perceptions explained variance in the emotional reactions of more conservative and more liberal participants. Prestige positively predicted assimilative emotions and dominance contrastive emotions. Our work advances theorising by providing evidence that dominance and prestige perceptions contribute to the elicitation of various emotions. Furthermore, it suggests that prestige and dominance are not fixed characteristics of liberal and conservative leaders but depend on the observers' group membership.

特朗普和拜登之间的 2020 年美国大选结果引发了强烈的情绪波动。在美国(研究 1;样本数 = 405)和德国(研究 2;样本数 = 123)样本中,我们调查了观察者的群体成员身份(即政治倾向)和两位候选人的社会地位(即优势与声望)如何预测情绪反应。人们普遍认为特朗普更有优势,拜登更有声望。然而,观察者的政治倾向不同,对社会地位的看法也不同,有的与领导者(即共和党人和民主党人)一致,有的与领导者不一致。与受试者政治倾向不一致的候选人被认为声望较低、支配地位较高,会引起较强的对比情绪(如幸灾乐祸、恶意嫉妒)和较弱的同化情绪(如高兴、同情、愤怒),反之亦然。最重要的是,主导地位和声望感解释了更保守和更自由的参与者在情绪反应上的差异。威望对同化情绪和优势对比情绪有积极的预测作用。我们的研究提供了证据,证明支配地位和威望认知有助于激发各种情绪,从而推进了理论研究。此外,我们的研究还表明,威望和支配地位并不是自由派和保守派领导人的固定特征,而是取决于观察者的群体成员身份。
{"title":"The interplay of social rank perceptions of Trump and Biden and emotions following the U.S. presidential election 2020.","authors":"Lea Boecker, Hannes M Petrowsky, David D Loschelder, Jens Lange","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2356713","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2356713","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The outcome of the 2020 U.S. election between Trump and Biden evoked strong emotions. In U.S. American (Study 1; <i>N</i> = 405) and German (Study 2; <i>N</i> = 123) samples, we investigated how observers' group membership (i.e. political orientation) and the social rank attainment of both candidates (i.e. dominance vs. prestige) predicted emotional reactions. Trump was generally perceived as more dominant, and Biden as more prestigious. However, perceptions of social rank attainment differed depending on the observers' political orientation, either matching or not matching with the leaders (i.e. Republicans and Democrats, respectively). The candidate who did not share the participants' political orientation was perceived as less prestigious and more dominant and elicited stronger contrastive emotions (i.e. schadenfreude, malicious envy) and weaker assimilative emotions (i.e. happy-for-ness, sympathy, anger), and vice versa. Crucially, dominance and prestige perceptions explained variance in the emotional reactions of more conservative and more liberal participants. Prestige positively predicted assimilative emotions and dominance contrastive emotions. Our work advances theorising by providing evidence that dominance and prestige perceptions contribute to the elicitation of various emotions. Furthermore, it suggests that prestige and dominance are not fixed characteristics of liberal and conservative leaders but depend on the observers' group membership.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1210-1228"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141451929","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The perceived controllability of negatively-valenced episodic future thinking modulates delay discounting. 受到负面影响的偶发未来思维的可控性会调节延迟贴现。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-02 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2370463
Cong Fan, Xiwen Chen, Jiayi Sun, Wenbo Luo

Intertemporal decision-making is important for both economy and physical health. Nevertheless, in daily life, individuals tend to prefer immediate and smaller rewards to delayed and larger rewards, which is known as delay discounting (DD). Episodic future thinking (EFT) has been proven to influence DD. However, there is still no inconsistent conclusion on the effect of negative EFT on DD. Considering the perceived controllability of negative EFT may address the issue (Controllability refers to the extent to which progress and result of an event could be controlled by ourselves). In the current study, we manipulated EFT conditions (baseline, neutral EFT, negative-controllable EFT and negative-uncontrollable EFT), delayed time (i.e. 1 week, 1 month, 3 months, 6 months, 1 year and 3 years) and reward magnitude (small, large). We mainly found that when experiencing negative-uncontrollable EFT compared to negative-controllable EFT in the delayed time of 6 months with large rewards, individuals chose more delayed rewards, suggesting that negative-uncontrollable EFT effectively reduced DD under conditions of both large-magnitude reward and longer delayed time. The current study provides new insight for healthy groups on optimising EFT. In that case, individuals are able to gain long-term benefits in financial management and healthcare.

跨时空决策对经济和身体健康都很重要。然而,在日常生活中,人们往往倾向于选择即时和较小的回报,而不是延迟和较大的回报,这就是所谓的延迟贴现(DD)。外显未来思维(EFT)已被证明会影响延迟贴现。然而,关于消极未来思维对延迟贴现的影响,目前还没有一致的结论。考虑消极 EFT 的可控性(可控性是指事件的进展和结果在多大程度上可以由我们自己控制)可能会解决这个问题。在本次研究中,我们操纵了EFT条件(基线、中性EFT、消极可控EFT和消极不可控EFT)、延迟时间(即1周、1个月、3个月、6个月、1年和3年)和奖励幅度(小、大)。我们主要发现,在6个月的延迟时间内,与负向不可控EFT相比,在大奖励的延迟时间内,个体选择了更多的延迟奖励,这表明负向不可控EFT在大奖励和较长的延迟时间条件下有效地减少了DD。本研究为健康群体优化 EFT 提供了新的启示。在这种情况下,个人能够在财务管理和医疗保健方面获得长期收益。
{"title":"The perceived controllability of negatively-valenced episodic future thinking modulates delay discounting.","authors":"Cong Fan, Xiwen Chen, Jiayi Sun, Wenbo Luo","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2370463","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2370463","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Intertemporal decision-making is important for both economy and physical health. Nevertheless, in daily life, individuals tend to prefer immediate and smaller rewards to delayed and larger rewards, which is known as delay discounting (DD). Episodic future thinking (EFT) has been proven to influence DD. However, there is still no inconsistent conclusion on the effect of negative EFT on DD. Considering the perceived controllability of negative EFT may address the issue (Controllability refers to the extent to which progress and result of an event could be controlled by ourselves). In the current study, we manipulated EFT conditions (baseline, neutral EFT, negative-controllable EFT and negative-uncontrollable EFT), delayed time (i.e. 1 week, 1 month, 3 months, 6 months, 1 year and 3 years) and reward magnitude (small, large). We mainly found that when experiencing negative-uncontrollable EFT compared to negative-controllable EFT in the delayed time of 6 months with large rewards, individuals chose more delayed rewards, suggesting that negative-uncontrollable EFT effectively reduced DD under conditions of both large-magnitude reward and longer delayed time. The current study provides new insight for healthy groups on optimising EFT. In that case, individuals are able to gain long-term benefits in financial management and healthcare.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1393-1403"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141477676","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
"Love looks not with the eyes": supranormal processing of emotional speech in individuals with late-blindness versus preserved processing in individuals with congenital-blindness. "爱不是用眼睛看的":晚期盲人对情感语言的超常处理与先天性盲人对情感语言的保留处理。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-24 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2357656
Boaz M Ben-David, Daniel-Robert Chebat, Michal Icht

Processing of emotional speech in the absence of visual information relies on two auditory channels: semantics and prosody. No study to date has investigated how blindness impacts this process. Two theories, Perceptual Deficit, and Sensory Compensation, yiled different expectations about the role of visual experience (or its lack thereof) in processing emotional speech. To test the effect of vision and early visual experience on processing of emotional speech, we compared individuals with congenital blindness (CB, n = 17), individuals with late blindness (LB, n = 15), and sighted controls (SC, n = 21) on identification and selective-attention of semantic and prosodic spoken-emotions. Results showed that individuals with blindness performed at least as well as SC, supporting Sensory Compensation and the role of cortical reorganisation. Individuals with LB outperformed individuals with CB, in accordance with Perceptual Deficit, supporting the role of early visual experience. The LB advantage was moderated by executive functions (working-memory). Namely, the advantage was erased for individuals with CB who showed higher levels of executive functions. Results suggest that vision is not necessary for processing of emotional speech, but early visual experience could improve it. The findings support a combination of the two aforementioned theories and reject a dichotomous view of deficiencies/enhancements of blindness.

在没有视觉信息的情况下,情感语言的处理依赖于两个听觉通道:语义和韵律。迄今为止,还没有研究调查过失明如何影响这一过程。知觉缺陷和感觉补偿这两种理论对视觉经验(或缺乏视觉经验)在情绪语言处理中的作用有着不同的预期。为了检验视觉和早期视觉经验对情绪语言处理的影响,我们比较了先天性失明患者(CB,n = 17)、晚期失明患者(LB,n = 15)和视力正常对照组(SC,n = 21)对语义和拟声口语情绪的识别和选择性注意。结果显示,失明患者的表现至少与健视对照组相同,这支持了感觉补偿和大脑皮层重组的作用。根据知觉缺陷(Perceptual Deficit),盲人的表现优于盲人,这支持了早期视觉经验的作用。枸杞多糖的优势受到执行功能(工作记忆)的调节。也就是说,执行功能水平较高的 CB 患者的优势会被削弱。研究结果表明,视觉并不是情绪化言语处理的必要条件,但早期视觉经验可以改善情绪化言语处理。研究结果支持上述两种理论的结合,并否定了将盲人的缺陷/增强一分为二的观点。
{"title":"\"Love looks not with the eyes\": supranormal processing of emotional speech in individuals with late-blindness versus preserved processing in individuals with congenital-blindness.","authors":"Boaz M Ben-David, Daniel-Robert Chebat, Michal Icht","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2357656","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2357656","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Processing of emotional speech in the absence of visual information relies on two auditory channels: semantics and prosody. No study to date has investigated how blindness impacts this process. Two theories, Perceptual Deficit, and Sensory Compensation, yiled different expectations about the role of visual experience (or its lack thereof) in processing emotional speech. To test the effect of vision and early visual experience on processing of emotional speech, we compared individuals with congenital blindness (CB, <i>n</i> = 17), individuals with late blindness (LB, <i>n</i> = 15), and sighted controls (SC, <i>n</i> = 21) on identification and selective-attention of semantic and prosodic spoken-emotions. Results showed that individuals with blindness performed at least as well as SC, supporting Sensory Compensation and the role of cortical reorganisation. Individuals with LB outperformed individuals with CB, in accordance with Perceptual Deficit, supporting the role of early visual experience. The LB advantage was moderated by executive functions (working-memory). Namely, the advantage was erased for individuals with CB who showed higher levels of executive functions. Results suggest that vision is not necessary for processing of emotional speech, but early visual experience could improve it. The findings support a combination of the two aforementioned theories and reject a dichotomous view of deficiencies/enhancements of blindness.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1354-1367"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141089021","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
More than meets the gut: a prototype analysis of the lay conceptions of intuition and analysis. 超越直觉:外行人对直觉和分析概念的原型分析。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-29 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2359740
Filipe Loureiro, Teresa Garcia-Marques, Duane T Wegener

Using a prototype approach, we assessed people's lay conceptions of intuition and analysis. Open-ended descriptions of intuition and analysis were generated by participants (Study 1) and resulting exemplars were sorted into features subsequently rated in centrality by independent participants (Study 2). Feature centrality was validated by showing that participants were quicker and more accurate in classifying central (as compared to peripheral) features (Study 3). Centrality ratings suggested a single-factor structure describing analysis but revealed that participants held lay conceptions of intuition as involving two different types of processes: (1) as an automatic, affective, and non-logical processing, and (2) as a holistic processing that can assist in problem-solving. Additional analyses showed that the centrality ratings of intuition's facets were predicted by participants' self-reported intuitive style, suggesting intuition is differently perceived by intuitive and non-intuitive people. We discuss the implications of these results for the study of intuition and analysis.

我们采用原型方法评估了人们对直觉和分析的非专业概念。由参与者对直觉和分析进行开放式描述(研究 1),然后由独立参与者对所产生的示例进行分类,并评定其中心性(研究 2)。研究结果表明,参与者对中心特征(与边缘特征相比)的分类更快、更准确(研究 3),从而验证了特征中心性。中心度评级表明,分析采用的是单因素结构,但也揭示了参与者对直觉的非专业概念,即直觉涉及两种不同类型的过程:(1) 自动、情感和非逻辑处理;(2) 有助于解决问题的整体处理。其他分析表明,直觉各方面的中心性评级会受到参与者自我报告的直觉风格的影响,这表明直觉型和非直觉型的人对直觉的认识是不同的。我们将讨论这些结果对直觉和分析研究的影响。
{"title":"More than meets the gut: a prototype analysis of the lay conceptions of intuition and analysis.","authors":"Filipe Loureiro, Teresa Garcia-Marques, Duane T Wegener","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2359740","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2359740","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Using a prototype approach, we assessed people's lay conceptions of intuition and analysis. Open-ended descriptions of intuition and analysis were generated by participants (Study 1) and resulting exemplars were sorted into features subsequently rated in centrality by independent participants (Study 2). Feature centrality was validated by showing that participants were quicker and more accurate in classifying central (as compared to peripheral) features (Study 3). Centrality ratings suggested a single-factor structure describing analysis but revealed that participants held lay conceptions of intuition as involving two different types of processes: (1) as an automatic, affective, and non-logical processing, and (2) as a holistic processing that can assist in problem-solving. Additional analyses showed that the centrality ratings of intuition's facets were predicted by participants' self-reported intuitive style, suggesting intuition is differently perceived by intuitive and non-intuitive people. We discuss the implications of these results for the study of intuition and analysis.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1229-1245"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141176690","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reining in regret: emotion regulation modulates regret in decision making. 控制后悔:情绪调节调节决策中的后悔。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-04 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2357847
Crystal Reeck, Kevin S LaBar

Whereas the influence of regret on decision making is well-established, it remains unclear whether emotion regulation may modulate both the affective experience of regret and its influence on decisions. To examine this question, participants made decisions about options involving uncertainty using two different, instructed emotion regulation strategies. In one case, they were instructed to treat each choice individually, while in the other they were encouraged to treat a series of decisions as a portfolio. The present experiment demonstrates that approaching a series of decisions as a portfolio led to less extreme affective reactions to outcomes and lowered physiological arousal levels compared to focusing on each decision in isolation. However, the different emotion regulation strategies did not alter the influence of anticipatory regret on choices. The results indicate that these different emotion regulation strategies can be used to alter the experience of regret. These findings support a role for cognitive strategies in mitigating the affective experience of regret and suggest a means to encourage consumer welfare.

虽然遗憾对决策的影响已得到证实,但情绪调节是否会同时调节遗憾的情感体验及其对决策的影响仍不清楚。为了研究这个问题,受试者使用两种不同的指令性情绪调节策略对涉及不确定性的选项进行决策。在一种情况下,他们被要求单独对待每一个选择,而在另一种情况下,他们被鼓励将一系列决定作为一个组合来对待。本实验表明,与孤立地关注每个决定相比,将一系列决定作为一个组合处理会导致对结果的极端情绪反应减少,生理唤醒水平降低。然而,不同的情绪调节策略并没有改变预期后悔对选择的影响。结果表明,这些不同的情绪调节策略可以用来改变后悔的体验。这些研究结果支持认知策略在减轻后悔情绪体验中的作用,并提出了一种鼓励消费者福利的方法。
{"title":"Reining in regret: emotion regulation modulates regret in decision making.","authors":"Crystal Reeck, Kevin S LaBar","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2357847","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2357847","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Whereas the influence of regret on decision making is well-established, it remains unclear whether emotion regulation may modulate both the affective experience of regret and its influence on decisions. To examine this question, participants made decisions about options involving uncertainty using two different, instructed emotion regulation strategies. In one case, they were instructed to treat each choice individually, while in the other they were encouraged to treat a series of decisions as a portfolio. The present experiment demonstrates that approaching a series of decisions as a portfolio led to less extreme affective reactions to outcomes and lowered physiological arousal levels compared to focusing on each decision in isolation. However, the different emotion regulation strategies did not alter the influence of anticipatory regret on choices. The results indicate that these different emotion regulation strategies can be used to alter the experience of regret. These findings support a role for cognitive strategies in mitigating the affective experience of regret and suggest a means to encourage consumer welfare.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1368-1375"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141237982","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reducing discrepancies between actual and ideal affect across adulthood: the roles of activity flow conduciveness, pleasantness, and familiarity. 减少成年期实际情感与理想情感之间的差异:活动流的诱导性、愉悦感和熟悉感的作用。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-17 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2367782
Da Jiang, Dwight C K Tse, Xianmin Gong, Vivian H L Tsang, Helene H Fung, Ajit S Mann, Jeanne Nakamura, Jeanne L Tsai

Previous findings demonstrate that people often do not feel how they want to feel, supporting the distinction between "actual affect" and "ideal affect." But are there certain activities that reduce the discrepancy between actual and ideal affect? Based on flow theory and socioemotional selectivity theory, we examined whether the discrepancy between people's actual and ideal positive affect would be smaller during activities that were more conducive to flow (a state of intense absorption and concentration), pleasant, and familiar. In Study 1, U.S. participants aged 17-79 (N = 393) reported their ideal affect and how they felt during activities with varying degrees of challenges and skills. For both low-arousal positive affect (LAP) and high-arousal positive affect (HAP), participants reported smaller actual-ideal affect discrepancies during flow-conducive activities (when skills matched challenges). Study 2 was a 14-day experience sampling study, in which Hong Kong participants aged 18-83 (Nindividual = 109) reported their momentary actual and ideal affect, and how pleasant and familiar their activities were (Nexperience = 3,815). Greater activity familiarity was associated with smaller discrepancies in actual-ideal LAP, while greater activity pleasantness was associated with smaller discrepancies in actual-ideal HAP. These findings provide insights on the activities that help people achieve their ideal affect more easily.

以往的研究结果表明,人们往往无法感受到自己想要的感觉,这支持了 "实际情感 "和 "理想情感 "之间的区别。但是,是否有某些活动可以减少实际情感与理想情感之间的差异呢?基于流动理论和社会情感选择性理论,我们研究了在更有利于流动(一种高度集中和专注的状态)、令人愉悦和熟悉的活动中,人们的实际积极情感和理想积极情感之间的差异是否会更小。在研究 1 中,年龄在 17-79 岁之间的美国参与者(393 人)报告了他们的理想情感以及他们在具有不同程度挑战和技能的活动中的感受。对于低唤醒积极情感(LAP)和高唤醒积极情感(HAP),参与者报告说,在流动诱导活动中(当技能与挑战相匹配时),实际情感与理想情感的差异较小。研究 2 是一项为期 14 天的经验取样研究,18-83 岁的香港参与者(Nindividual = 109)报告了他们的瞬间实际情绪和理想情绪,以及活动的愉快程度和熟悉程度(Nexperience = 3,815)。活动熟悉程度越高,实际与理想情绪之间的差异越小;活动愉快程度越高,实际与理想情绪之间的差异越小。这些发现为我们提供了有关活动的见解,这些活动有助于人们更轻松地实现理想情感。
{"title":"Reducing discrepancies between actual and ideal affect across adulthood: the roles of activity flow conduciveness, pleasantness, and familiarity.","authors":"Da Jiang, Dwight C K Tse, Xianmin Gong, Vivian H L Tsang, Helene H Fung, Ajit S Mann, Jeanne Nakamura, Jeanne L Tsai","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2367782","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2367782","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous findings demonstrate that people often do not feel how they want to feel, supporting the distinction between \"actual affect\" and \"ideal affect.\" But are there certain activities that reduce the discrepancy between actual and ideal affect? Based on flow theory and socioemotional selectivity theory, we examined whether the discrepancy between people's actual and ideal positive affect would be smaller during activities that were more conducive to flow (a state of intense absorption and concentration), pleasant, and familiar. In Study 1, U.S. participants aged 17-79 (<i>N </i>= 393) reported their ideal affect and how they felt during activities with varying degrees of challenges and skills. For both low-arousal positive affect (LAP) and high-arousal positive affect (HAP), participants reported smaller actual-ideal affect discrepancies during flow-conducive activities (when skills matched challenges). Study 2 was a 14-day experience sampling study, in which Hong Kong participants aged 18-83 (<i>N</i><sub>individual</sub> = 109) reported their momentary actual and ideal affect, and how pleasant and familiar their activities were (<i>N</i><sub>experience </sub>= 3,815). Greater activity familiarity was associated with smaller discrepancies in actual-ideal LAP, while greater activity pleasantness was associated with smaller discrepancies in actual-ideal HAP. These findings provide insights on the activities that help people achieve their ideal affect more easily.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1303-1317"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11663131/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141635032","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Who expresses their pride when? The regulation of pride expressions as a function of self-monitoring and social context. 谁会在什么时候表达自豪感?自我监控和社会环境对自豪感表达的调节作用。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-26 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2333070
Chau Tran, Bengisu Sezer, Yvette van Osch

Pride expressions draw attention to one's achievement, and therefore can enhance one's status. However, such attention has been linked to negative interpersonal consequences (i.e. envy). Fortunately, people have been found to regulate their pride expressions accordingly. Specifically, pride expressions are lower when the domain of the achievement is of high relevance to observers. We set out to replicate this effect in a non-Western sample. Additionally, we extended the current finding by investigating the moderating role of self-monitoring, an individual's ability and willingness to adjust their behaviours under different social contexts to cultivate status. This allows us to explore the previously assumed underlying status motive in regulating pride expressions. Data from two preregistered studies (N1 = 913; N2 = 1081) replicated the effect that pride expressions are inhibited when the achievement domain is relevant. A significant main effect of self-monitoring was found, such that high self-monitors express more pride than low self-monitors, consistent with the conceptualisation of self-monitoring as rooted within a status-enhancement motive. The assumed interaction effect between domain relevance and self-monitoring was not significant. Our findings suggest that the effect of domain relevance on pride expression is robust and status driven.

自豪感的表达会让人注意到自己的成就,从而提高自己的地位。然而,这种关注也与负面的人际后果(即嫉妒)有关。幸运的是,研究发现人们会相应地调节自豪感的表达。具体来说,当成就的领域与观察者高度相关时,自豪感的表达就会降低。我们试图在非西方样本中复制这种效应。此外,我们还研究了自我监控的调节作用,即个体在不同社会背景下调整行为以提高地位的能力和意愿,从而扩展了当前的研究结果。这使我们能够探索之前假设的调节自豪感表达的潜在地位动机。来自两项预先登记的研究(N1 = 913;N2 = 1081)的数据重复了当成就领域相关时,自豪感表达受到抑制的效果。研究发现,自我监控具有明显的主效应,因此高自我监控者比低自我监控者表达更多的自豪感,这与自我监控根植于地位提升动机的概念是一致的。假设的领域相关性与自我监控之间的交互效应并不显著。我们的研究结果表明,领域相关性对自豪感表达的影响是稳健的,并且是由地位驱动的。
{"title":"Who expresses their pride when? The regulation of pride expressions as a function of self-monitoring and social context.","authors":"Chau Tran, Bengisu Sezer, Yvette van Osch","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2333070","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2333070","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Pride expressions draw attention to one's achievement, and therefore can enhance one's status. However, such attention has been linked to negative interpersonal consequences (i.e. envy). Fortunately, people have been found to regulate their pride expressions accordingly. Specifically, pride expressions are lower when the domain of the achievement is of high relevance to observers. We set out to replicate this effect in a non-Western sample. Additionally, we extended the current finding by investigating the moderating role of self-monitoring, an individual's ability and willingness to adjust their behaviours under different social contexts to cultivate status. This allows us to explore the previously assumed underlying status motive in regulating pride expressions. Data from two preregistered studies (<i>N<sub>1</sub></i> = 913; <i>N<sub>2</sub></i> = 1081) replicated the effect that pride expressions are inhibited when the achievement domain is relevant. A significant main effect of self-monitoring was found, such that high self-monitors express more pride than low self-monitors, consistent with the conceptualisation of self-monitoring as rooted within a status-enhancement motive. The assumed interaction effect between domain relevance and self-monitoring was not significant. Our findings suggest that the effect of domain relevance on pride expression is robust and status driven.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1343-1353"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140294893","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effects of nonverbal pride and skill on judgements of victory and social influence: a boxing study. 非语言自豪感和技能对胜利判断和社会影响的影响:一项拳击研究。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-24 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2358381
Jason P Martens, Lucy Doytchinova

Displaying nonverbal pride after a boxing match leads to judgements of success. However, it is not clear the extent to which this effect generalises nor whether it can override competing information. An experimental design had 214 participants watch two boxing clips that were manipulated so that one was evenly matched and the other had a fighter with an advantage (i.e. demonstrating more skill). Nonverbal behaviour at the completion of the fight varied between fighters (pride versus neutral). When the fight was evenly matched, the fighters displaying nonverbal pride were judged as winning the fight, but the fighter did not garner increased social influence. In contrast, when fighters demonstrated superior skill, the more skilled fighters who displayed neutral postures rather than the less-skilled ones displaying pride were judged as winning the fight, and the skilled fighters garnered increased social influence. These results suggest that in a boxing context, a pride bias works in evenly matched scenarios, but when differences in skill are more clearly present, a skill bias is more pronounced and leads to more social influence. Furthermore, perceptions of skill were associated with judgments of victory across stimuli, suggesting the importance of skill perceptions in such judgments.

拳击比赛后表现出的非语言自豪感会导致对成功的判断。然而,目前还不清楚这种效应的普遍程度,也不清楚这种效应是否能超越竞争信息。一项实验设计让 214 名参与者观看了两个拳击比赛片段,这两个片段经过处理,一个是势均力敌的比赛,另一个是拳击手占优势的比赛(即表现出更高的技巧)。比赛结束时,不同拳手的非语言行为各不相同(骄傲与中立)。当比赛双方势均力敌时,表现出自豪感的一方被认为赢得了比赛,但其社会影响力并没有增加。相反,当拳击手表现出高超的技术时,表现出中立姿态的技术高超的拳击手而不是表现出骄傲姿态的技术较低的拳击手被判定为赢得了比赛,技术高超的拳击手获得了更大的社会影响力。这些结果表明,在拳击比赛中,自豪感偏差在势均力敌的情况下会起作用,但当技术差异更加明显时,技术偏差就会更加明显,并导致更多的社会影响。此外,对技能的感知与不同刺激下的胜利判断相关,这表明技能感知在此类判断中的重要性。
{"title":"The effects of nonverbal pride and skill on judgements of victory and social influence: a boxing study.","authors":"Jason P Martens, Lucy Doytchinova","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2358381","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2358381","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Displaying nonverbal pride after a boxing match leads to judgements of success. However, it is not clear the extent to which this effect generalises nor whether it can override competing information. An experimental design had 214 participants watch two boxing clips that were manipulated so that one was evenly matched and the other had a fighter with an advantage (i.e. demonstrating more skill). Nonverbal behaviour at the completion of the fight varied between fighters (pride versus neutral). When the fight was evenly matched, the fighters displaying nonverbal pride were judged as winning the fight, but the fighter did not garner increased social influence. In contrast, when fighters demonstrated superior skill, the more skilled fighters who displayed neutral postures rather than the less-skilled ones displaying pride were judged as winning the fight, and the skilled fighters garnered increased social influence. These results suggest that in a boxing context, a pride bias works in evenly matched scenarios, but when differences in skill are more clearly present, a skill bias is more pronounced and leads to more social influence. Furthermore, perceptions of skill were associated with judgments of victory across stimuli, suggesting the importance of skill perceptions in such judgments.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1376-1382"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141089023","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Today's positive affect predicts tomorrow's experience of meaningful coincidences: a cross-lagged multilevel analysis. 今天的积极情绪能预测明天对有意义巧合的体验:跨滞后多层次分析。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-09 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2349280
Christian Rominger, Andreas Fink, Corinna M Perchtold-Stefan, Andreas R Schwerdtfeger

The perception of meaningful patterns in random arrangements and unrelated events takes place in our everyday lives, coined apophenia, synchronicity, or the experience of meaningful coincidences. However, we do not know yet what predicts this phenomenon. To investigate this, we re-analyzed a combined data set of two daily diary studies with a total of N = 169 participants (mean age 29.95 years; 54 men). We investigated if positive or negative affect (PA, NA) predicts the number of meaningful coincidences on the following day (or vice versa). By means of a cross-lagged multilevel modelling approach (Bayesian estimation) we evaluated with which of two theoretical assumptions the data are more in line. First, if meaningful coincidences are facilitated by a broader and more flexible thinking style, PA should positively predict meaningful coincidences at the following day. However, if the experience of meaningful coincidences signifies a strategy to cope with negative feeling states, NA should predict the experience of meaningful coincidences during the following day. In favour of a more flexible thinking style, we found that PA predicted the number of perceived coincidences the following day. We did not find any effect for NA, and therefore, no evidence arguing for the coping mechanism hypothesis of meaningful coincidences.

在我们的日常生活中,我们会在随机的安排和不相关的事件中感知到有意义的模式,这种现象被称为 "apophenia"、"synchronicity "或 "有意义的巧合体验"。然而,我们还不知道是什么预测了这种现象。为了研究这个问题,我们重新分析了两项每日日记研究的合并数据集,共有 169 名参与者(平均年龄 29.95 岁;54 名男性)参与了研究。我们调查了积极或消极情绪(PA、NA)是否会预测第二天有意义的巧合的数量(反之亦然)。通过交叉滞后多层次建模方法(贝叶斯估计法),我们评估了数据与两个理论假设中哪一个更为吻合。首先,如果有意义的巧合是由更宽广、更灵活的思维方式促成的,那么 PA 应该可以正向预测第二天有意义的巧合。然而,如果有意义巧合的经历是一种应对消极情绪状态的策略,那么 NA 就应该预测第二天有意义巧合的经历。为了支持更灵活的思维方式,我们发现 PA 预测了第二天感知到的巧合的数量。我们没有发现 NA 有任何影响,因此没有证据证明有意义巧合的应对机制假设。
{"title":"Today's positive affect predicts tomorrow's experience of meaningful coincidences: a cross-lagged multilevel analysis.","authors":"Christian Rominger, Andreas Fink, Corinna M Perchtold-Stefan, Andreas R Schwerdtfeger","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2349280","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2349280","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The perception of meaningful patterns in random arrangements and unrelated events takes place in our everyday lives, coined apophenia, synchronicity, or the experience of meaningful coincidences. However, we do not know yet what predicts this phenomenon. To investigate this, we re-analyzed a combined data set of two daily diary studies with a total of <i>N</i> = 169 participants (mean age 29.95 years; 54 men). We investigated if positive or negative affect (PA, NA) predicts the number of meaningful coincidences on the following day (or vice versa). By means of a cross-lagged multilevel modelling approach (Bayesian estimation) we evaluated with which of two theoretical assumptions the data are more in line. First, if meaningful coincidences are facilitated by a broader and more flexible thinking style, PA should positively predict meaningful coincidences at the following day. However, if the experience of meaningful coincidences signifies a strategy to cope with negative feeling states, NA should predict the experience of meaningful coincidences during the following day. In favour of a more flexible thinking style, we found that PA predicted the number of perceived coincidences the following day. We did not find any effect for NA, and therefore, no evidence arguing for the coping mechanism hypothesis of meaningful coincidences.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1152-1159"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140899645","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognition & Emotion
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1