首页 > 最新文献

Cognition & Emotion最新文献

英文 中文
Experimentally manipulated anger activates implicit cognitions about social hierarchy. 实验操纵的愤怒会激活对社会等级的内隐认知。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-21 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2331811
Harrison M Miller, Connor R Hasty, Jon K Maner

A correlational pilot study (N = 143) and an integrative data analysis of two experiments (total N = 377) provide evidence linking anger to the psychology of social hierarchy. The experiments demonstrate that the experience of anger increases the psychological accessibility of implicit cognitions related to social hierarchy: compared to participants in a control condition, participants in an anger-priming condition completed word stems with significantly more hierarchy-related words. We found little support for sex differences in the effect of anger on implicit hierarchy-related cognition; effects were equivalent across male and female participants. Findings fit with functionalist evolutionary views of anger suggesting that anger may motivate the use of dominance to strive for high social rank in group hierarchies. Implications for downstream behaviour, including aggression and negotiation, are discussed.

一项相关性试验研究(N = 143)和两项实验(总 N = 377)的综合数据分析提供了将愤怒与社会等级心理联系起来的证据。实验证明,愤怒的体验会增加与社会等级相关的内隐认知的心理可及性:与对照条件下的参与者相比,愤怒刺激条件下的参与者完成的词干中与等级相关的词明显较多。我们发现,愤怒对内隐等级认知的影响几乎不存在性别差异;男性和女性参与者的效果相当。研究结果符合愤怒的功能主义进化观点,即愤怒可能会促使人们利用支配地位来争取在群体等级中的较高社会地位。研究还讨论了下游行为(包括攻击和谈判)的影响。
{"title":"Experimentally manipulated anger activates implicit cognitions about social hierarchy.","authors":"Harrison M Miller, Connor R Hasty, Jon K Maner","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2331811","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2331811","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A correlational pilot study (<i>N</i> = 143) and an integrative data analysis of two experiments (total <i>N</i> = 377) provide evidence linking anger to the psychology of social hierarchy. The experiments demonstrate that the experience of anger increases the psychological accessibility of implicit cognitions related to social hierarchy: compared to participants in a control condition, participants in an anger-priming condition completed word stems with significantly more hierarchy-related words. We found little support for sex differences in the effect of anger on implicit hierarchy-related cognition; effects were equivalent across male and female participants. Findings fit with functionalist evolutionary views of anger suggesting that anger may motivate the use of dominance to strive for high social rank in group hierarchies. Implications for downstream behaviour, including aggression and negotiation, are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"872-883"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140177245","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of category valence in prototype preference. 类别价值在原型偏好中的作用。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-30 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2335536
Moritz Ingendahl, Nadja Propheter, Tobias Vogel

People prefer prototypical stimuli over atypical stimuli. The dominant explanation for this prototype preference effect is that prototypical stimuli are processed more fluently. However, a more recent account proposes that prototypes are more strongly associated with their category's valence, leading to a reversed prototype preference effect for negative categories. One critical but untested assumption of this category-valence account is that no prototype preference should emerge for entirely neutral categories. We tested this prediction by conditioning categories of dot patterns positively, negatively, or neutrally. In line with previous findings on the category-valence account, prototype preference reversed for negatively conditioned categories. However, prototype preference was similarly strong for positive and neutral categories. These findings imply that prototype preferences do not only reflect a transfer of category valence to exemplars. Instead, the results suggest that prototype preference is a multi-process phenomenon arising from the activated category valence and a fluency-based process. We discuss further implications for theories on fluency and prototype preference.

相比非典型刺激,人们更喜欢原型刺激。对这种原型偏好效应的主要解释是,原型刺激的处理更为流畅。然而,最近的一种解释认为,原型与其类别的价值关联性更强,从而导致了负面类别的反向原型偏好效应。这种类别-价值解释的一个关键但未经验证的假设是,完全中性的类别不应该出现原型偏好。我们通过对点图案类别进行积极、消极或中性的调节来检验这一预测。与之前关于类别-价值账户的研究结果一致,原型偏好对于负向条件化类别而言发生了逆转。然而,原型偏好对于正面和中性类别同样强烈。这些研究结果表明,原型偏好不仅反映了类别价值向范例的转移。相反,这些结果表明,原型偏好是一种多过程现象,由激活的类别价值和基于流畅性的过程产生。我们将进一步讨论流畅性和原型偏好理论的影响。
{"title":"The role of category valence in prototype preference.","authors":"Moritz Ingendahl, Nadja Propheter, Tobias Vogel","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2335536","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2335536","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People prefer prototypical stimuli over atypical stimuli. The dominant explanation for this prototype preference effect is that prototypical stimuli are processed more fluently. However, a more recent account proposes that prototypes are more strongly associated with their category's valence, leading to a reversed prototype preference effect for negative categories. One critical but untested assumption of this category-valence account is that no prototype preference should emerge for entirely neutral categories. We tested this prediction by conditioning categories of dot patterns positively, negatively, or neutrally. In line with previous findings on the category-valence account, prototype preference reversed for negatively conditioned categories. However, prototype preference was similarly strong for positive and neutral categories. These findings imply that prototype preferences do not only reflect a transfer of category valence to exemplars. Instead, the results suggest that prototype preference is a multi-process phenomenon arising from the activated category valence and a fluency-based process. We discuss further implications for theories on fluency and prototype preference.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"963-969"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140330231","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Within- and between-person associations between social interactions and loneliness: students' experiences during the COVID-19 pandemic. 社交互动与孤独感之间的人内和人际关联:COVID-19 大流行期间学生的经历。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-25 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2333920
Alyssa K Truong, Gizem Keskin, Jessica P Lougheed

The COVID-19 pandemic introduced many restrictions to in-person interactions, and remote social interactions may be especially important for managing loneliness when such restrictions are in place. However, it is unclear how social interactions are related to loneliness when in-person interactions are limited. Data were collected between February 2021 and March 2022 from a sample of 581 university students. Participants reported their loneliness and participation in positive in-person or remote social interactions each day for 14 days. Results from dynamic structural equation models showed that participants felt less lonely than they usually felt on the days they engaged in positive remote interactions at the within-person level. Moreover, participants generally felt less lonely when engaging more frequently in remote interactions, but only when in-person interactions were restricted (between-person level). Some of these results varied by changing COVID-19 restrictions. Finally, for participants who felt lonelier in general, the effect of positive in-person and remote interactions on loneliness was less strong. These findings suggest that social interactions may buffer loneliness but are not as impactful for those who experience greater loneliness.

COVID-19 大流行给人与人之间的交往带来了许多限制,而远程社交互动对于在这种限制下控制孤独感可能尤为重要。然而,目前还不清楚当人际交往受到限制时,社交互动与孤独感之间的关系。我们在 2021 年 2 月至 2022 年 3 月期间收集了 581 名大学生的样本数据。参与者在 14 天内每天报告自己的孤独感和参与积极的面对面或远程社交互动的情况。动态结构方程模型的结果表明,在参与积极的远程人际交往的日子里,参与者的孤独感低于他们通常的孤独感。此外,当参与者更频繁地参与远程互动时,他们的孤独感普遍较低,但只有在人际互动受到限制时(人际层面)才会如此。其中一些结果因 COVID-19 限制的改变而不同。最后,对于总体上感觉更孤独的参与者来说,积极的人际交往和远程交往对孤独感的影响较小。这些研究结果表明,社交互动可以缓冲孤独感,但对于那些孤独感较强的人来说,社交互动的影响并不明显。
{"title":"Within- and between-person associations between social interactions and loneliness: students' experiences during the COVID-19 pandemic.","authors":"Alyssa K Truong, Gizem Keskin, Jessica P Lougheed","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2333920","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2333920","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The COVID-19 pandemic introduced many restrictions to in-person interactions, and remote social interactions may be especially important for managing loneliness when such restrictions are in place. However, it is unclear how social interactions are related to loneliness when in-person interactions are limited. Data were collected between February 2021 and March 2022 from a sample of 581 university students. Participants reported their loneliness and participation in positive in-person or remote social interactions each day for 14 days. Results from dynamic structural equation models showed that participants felt less lonely than they usually felt on the days they engaged in positive remote interactions at the within-person level. Moreover, participants generally felt less lonely when engaging more frequently in remote interactions, but only when in-person interactions were restricted (between-person level). Some of these results varied by changing COVID-19 restrictions. Finally, for participants who felt lonelier in general, the effect of positive in-person and remote interactions on loneliness was less strong. These findings suggest that social interactions may buffer loneliness but are not as impactful for those who experience greater loneliness.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"938-946"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140207977","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Threat directionality modulates defensive reactions in humans: cardiac and electrodermal responses. 威胁的方向性调节人类的防御反应:心脏和皮肤电反应。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-30 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2335535
Mariana Xavier, Eliane Volchan, Arthur V Machado, Isabel A David, Letícia Oliveira, Liana C L Portugal, Gabriela G L Souza, Fátima S Erthal, Rita de Cássia S Alves, Izabela Mocaiber, Mirtes G Pereira

Features of threatening cues and the associated context influence the perceived imminence of threat and the defensive responses evoked. To provide additional knowledge about how the directionality of a threat (i.e. directed-towards or away from the viewer) might impact defensive responses in humans, participants were shown pictures of a man carrying a gun (threat) or nonlethal object (neutral) directed-away from or towards the participant. Cardiac and electrodermal responses were collected. Compared to neutral images, threatening images depicting a gun directed-towards the participant induced sustained bradycardia and an increased electrodermal response, interpreted as immobility under attack. This defensive immobility reaction is evoked by high perceived threat and inescapable situations and indicates intense action preparation. Pictures of guns directed-away from the participant induced shorter bradycardia and no significant modulation of the electrodermal response compared to neutral pictures, which might be consistent with the perception of a less threatening situation. The results show that the directionality of threat stimuli is a key factor that prompts different patterns of defensive responses.

威胁线索的特征和相关情境会影响对威胁迫近性的感知和引起的防御反应。为了进一步了解威胁的方向性(即朝向或远离观看者)会如何影响人类的防御反应,研究人员向参与者展示了一名男子携带枪支(威胁)或非致命物品(中性)的图片,图片的方向是朝向或远离参与者。研究人员收集了心电和皮电反应。与中性图像相比,描绘枪口朝向被试的威胁性图像会诱发持续的心动过缓,并增加电皮反应,这被解释为被攻击时的不动反应。这种防卫性的不动反应是在感知到高度威胁和无法逃脱的情况下产生的,并表明了紧张的行动准备。与中性图片相比,枪支朝向远离被试者的图片引起的心动过缓时间较短,而对皮电反应的调节不明显,这可能与被试感受到的威胁性较低的情况相一致。结果表明,威胁刺激的方向性是引发不同防御反应模式的关键因素。
{"title":"Threat directionality modulates defensive reactions in humans: cardiac and electrodermal responses.","authors":"Mariana Xavier, Eliane Volchan, Arthur V Machado, Isabel A David, Letícia Oliveira, Liana C L Portugal, Gabriela G L Souza, Fátima S Erthal, Rita de Cássia S Alves, Izabela Mocaiber, Mirtes G Pereira","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2335535","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2335535","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Features of threatening cues and the associated context influence the perceived imminence of threat and the defensive responses evoked. To provide additional knowledge about how the directionality of a threat (i.e. directed-towards or away from the viewer) might impact defensive responses in humans, participants were shown pictures of a man carrying a gun (threat) or nonlethal object (neutral) directed-away from or towards the participant. Cardiac and electrodermal responses were collected. Compared to neutral images, threatening images depicting a gun directed-towards the participant induced sustained bradycardia and an increased electrodermal response, interpreted as immobility under attack. This defensive immobility reaction is evoked by high perceived threat and inescapable situations and indicates intense action preparation. Pictures of guns directed-away from the participant induced shorter bradycardia and no significant modulation of the electrodermal response compared to neutral pictures, which might be consistent with the perception of a less threatening situation. The results show that the directionality of threat stimuli is a key factor that prompts different patterns of defensive responses.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"954-962"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140330232","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Linking a latent variable trait-state-occasion model of emotion regulation to cognitive control. 将情绪调节的潜变量特质-状态-事件模型与认知控制联系起来。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-25 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2332594
Bunmi O Olatunji, Kelly A Knowles, Alexandra M Adamis, David A Cole

Emotion dysregulation (ED) is a vulnerability factor for affective disorders that may originate from deficits in cognitive control (CC). Although measures of ED are often designed to assess trait-like tendencies, the extent to which such measures capture a time-varying (TV) or state-like construct versus a time-invariant (TI) or trait-like personality characteristic is unclear. The link between the TV and TI components of ED and CC is also unclear. In a 6-wave, 5-month longitudinal study, community participants (n = 1281) completed the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS-16), a commonly used measure of ED and measures of CC. A latent variable (trait-state-occasion) model showed that the proportion of TI factor variance (.80) was greater than the TV factor variance (.19). Although TV factor stability was significant, the coefficients were small in magnitude. Furthermore, regression weights for the ED TI factor (average β = -.62) were significant and larger than those for the TV factor (average β = -.10) in predicting latent CC at each of the six-time points. These findings suggest that ED, as assessed by the DERS-16, is largely TI and this TI component is more strongly linked to CC than the TV component.

情绪失调(ED)是情感障碍的一个易感因素,可能源于认知控制(CC)的缺陷。尽管情绪失调的测量通常是为了评估特质样倾向而设计的,但此类测量在多大程度上捕捉到了时变(TV)或状态样结构,而不是时不变(TI)或特质样人格特征,目前尚不清楚。ED的TV和TI成分与CC之间的联系也不清楚。在一项为期 6 波、5 个月的纵向研究中,社区参与者(n = 1281)完成了情绪调节困难量表(DERS-16),这是一种常用的 ED 和 CC 测量方法。潜变量(特质-状态-事件)模型显示,TI因子方差(0.80)大于TV因子方差(0.19)。虽然 TV 因子的稳定性显著,但系数的幅度较小。此外,在预测六个时间点中每个时间点的潜在 CC 时,ED TI 因子(平均 β = -.62)的回归权重显著且大于 TV 因子(平均 β = -.10)的回归权重。这些研究结果表明,DERS-16 评估的 ED 主要是 TI,而 TI 因素比 TV 因素与 CC 的关联性更强。
{"title":"Linking a latent variable trait-state-occasion model of emotion regulation to cognitive control.","authors":"Bunmi O Olatunji, Kelly A Knowles, Alexandra M Adamis, David A Cole","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2332594","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2332594","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Emotion dysregulation (ED) is a vulnerability factor for affective disorders that may originate from deficits in cognitive control (CC). Although measures of ED are often designed to assess trait-like tendencies, the extent to which such measures capture a time-varying (TV) or state-like construct versus a time-invariant (TI) or trait-like personality characteristic is unclear. The link between the TV and TI components of ED and CC is also unclear. In a 6-wave, 5-month longitudinal study, community participants (<i>n</i> = 1281) completed the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS-16), a commonly used measure of ED and measures of CC. A latent variable (trait-state-occasion) model showed that the proportion of TI factor variance (.80) was greater than the TV factor variance (.19). Although TV factor stability was significant, the coefficients were small in magnitude. Furthermore, regression weights for the ED TI factor (average <math><mtext>β</mtext></math> = -.62) were significant and larger than those for the TV factor (average <math><mtext>β</mtext></math> = -.10) in predicting latent CC at each of the six-time points. These findings suggest that ED, as assessed by the DERS-16, is largely TI and this TI component is more strongly linked to CC than the TV component.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"898-912"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140207976","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Unpacking affect maintenance and its association with depressive symptoms: integrating positive and negative affects. 解读情感维持及其与抑郁症状的关联:整合积极情感与消极情感。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-02 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2334843
Noa Vardi, Eva Gilboa-Schechtman, Shimrit Daches

ABSTRACTDepression is associated with increased maintenance of negative affect (NA) and reduced - blunted and short-lived - maintenance of positive affect (PA). Studies have focused on factors associated with the maintenance of NA, specifically, the emotion regulation strategy of brooding and the capacity to hold negative affective experiences in working memory (WM). Despite its theoretical importance, less attention has been given to factors associated with the maintenance of PA in depression. This study aims to synthesise factors playing a role in the maintenance of both NA and PA. Specifically, we used self-reported assessment of PA and NA regulation and performance-based measures of NA and PA processing in WM to predict depressive symptoms severity. Participants (N = 219) completed the Affective Maintenance Task (AMT, Mikels et al., 2008), which provided performance-based measures of PA and NA maintenance, and filled out questionnaires assessing brooding, positive rumination and depressive severity. Brooding, positive rumination and AMT-based measures of positive (but not negative) affective information processing were independently associated with depressive symptoms. We highlight the unique contributions of PA processing, as well as of self-reported emotion regulation strategies in understanding depression maintenance.

摘要抑郁症与消极情绪(NA)的维持能力增强和积极情绪(PA)的维持能力减弱有关,消极情绪的维持能力减弱且持续时间较短。研究主要集中在与维持消极情绪相关的因素上,特别是情绪调节策略--"沉思"(brooding)和在工作记忆(WM)中保持消极情绪体验的能力。尽管 PA 在理论上具有重要意义,但人们对抑郁症患者 PA 维持的相关因素关注较少。本研究旨在总结在维持 NA 和 PA 方面发挥作用的因素。具体来说,我们使用自我报告的 PA 和 NA 调节评估以及基于表现的 NA 和 PA 在 WM 中的处理测量来预测抑郁症状的严重程度。参与者(N = 219)完成了 "情感维持任务"(AMT,Mikels 等人,2008 年),该任务提供了基于表现的 PA 和 NA 维持测量,参与者还填写了评估忧郁、积极反刍和抑郁严重程度的问卷。耿耿于怀、积极反刍和基于 AMT 的积极(而非消极)情感信息处理测量均与抑郁症状独立相关。我们强调了 PA 处理以及自我报告的情绪调节策略在理解抑郁维持方面的独特贡献。
{"title":"Unpacking affect maintenance and its association with depressive symptoms: integrating positive and negative affects.","authors":"Noa Vardi, Eva Gilboa-Schechtman, Shimrit Daches","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2334843","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2334843","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p><b>ABSTRACT</b>Depression is associated with increased maintenance of negative affect (NA) and reduced - blunted and short-lived - maintenance of positive affect (PA). Studies have focused on factors associated with the maintenance of NA, specifically, the emotion regulation strategy of brooding and the capacity to hold negative affective experiences in working memory (WM). Despite its theoretical importance, less attention has been given to factors associated with the maintenance of PA in depression. This study aims to synthesise factors playing a role in the maintenance of both NA and PA. Specifically, we used self-reported assessment of PA and NA regulation and performance-based measures of NA and PA processing in WM to predict depressive symptoms severity. Participants (<i>N</i> = 219) completed the Affective Maintenance Task (AMT, Mikels et al., 2008), which provided performance-based measures of PA and NA maintenance, and filled out questionnaires assessing brooding, positive rumination and depressive severity. Brooding, positive rumination and AMT-based measures of positive (but not negative) affective information processing were independently associated with depressive symptoms. We highlight the unique contributions of PA processing, as well as of self-reported emotion regulation strategies in understanding depression maintenance.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"947-953"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140337313","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotion malleability beliefs matter in emotion regulation: a comprehensive review and meta-analysis. 情绪可塑性信念在情绪调节中的重要性:全面回顾与荟萃分析。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-28 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2334833
Yunsu Kim, Sooyeon Kim, Sunkyung Yoon

Individuals' beliefs about the malleability of emotions have been theorised to play a role in their psychological distress by influencing emotion regulation processes, such as the use of emotion regulation strategies. We conducted a meta-analysis to test this idea across studies with a focus on the relationships between emotion malleability beliefs and five distinct emotion regulation strategies: cognitive reappraisal, suppression, avoidance, rumination, and acceptance. Further, using two-stage meta-analytic structural equation modelling (TSSEM), we examined whether the emotion regulation strategies mediate the cross-sectional relationship between emotion malleability beliefs and psychological distress across studies. Thirty-seven studies were included in the meta-analyses and 55 cross-sectional studies were included in the TSSEM. Results demonstrated that, across studies, emotion malleability beliefs were significantly associated with greater use of putatively helpful strategies (particularly with cognitive reappraisal) and less use of putatively unhelpful strategies (particularly with avoidance). The use of cognitive reappraisal and avoidance partially mediated the relationship between emotion malleability beliefs and psychological distress. These results highlight the importance of considering beliefs about the malleability of emotions in the context of emotion regulation. These findings suggest the potential role of emotion malleability beliefs in interventions for individuals with emotion regulation-related difficulties and psychological distress.

有理论认为,个体对情绪可塑性的信念会影响情绪调节过程,如情绪调节策略的使用,从而在心理困扰中发挥作用。我们进行了一项荟萃分析,在各项研究中检验了这一观点,重点研究了情绪可塑性信念与五种不同的情绪调节策略之间的关系:认知再评价、压抑、回避、反刍和接受。此外,我们还利用两阶段元分析结构方程模型(TSSEM),考察了情绪调节策略是否在不同研究中调解了情绪可塑性信念与心理困扰之间的横截面关系。37 项研究被纳入荟萃分析,55 项横断面研究被纳入 TSSEM。结果表明,在各项研究中,情绪可塑性信念与更多地使用可能有帮助的策略(尤其是认知再评价)和较少地使用可能无帮助的策略(尤其是回避)有显著关联。认知重评和回避在一定程度上调节了情绪可塑性信念与心理困扰之间的关系。这些结果凸显了在情绪调节中考虑情绪可塑性信念的重要性。这些研究结果表明,情绪可塑性信念在对有情绪调节相关困难和心理困扰的个体进行干预时具有潜在的作用。
{"title":"Emotion malleability beliefs matter in emotion regulation: a comprehensive review and meta-analysis.","authors":"Yunsu Kim, Sooyeon Kim, Sunkyung Yoon","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2334833","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2334833","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Individuals' beliefs about the malleability of emotions have been theorised to play a role in their psychological distress by influencing emotion regulation processes, such as the use of emotion regulation strategies. We conducted a meta-analysis to test this idea across studies with a focus on the relationships between emotion malleability beliefs and five distinct emotion regulation strategies: cognitive reappraisal, suppression, avoidance, rumination, and acceptance. Further, using two-stage meta-analytic structural equation modelling (TSSEM), we examined whether the emotion regulation strategies mediate the cross-sectional relationship between emotion malleability beliefs and psychological distress across studies. Thirty-seven studies were included in the meta-analyses and 55 cross-sectional studies were included in the TSSEM. Results demonstrated that, across studies, emotion malleability beliefs were significantly associated with greater use of putatively helpful strategies (particularly with cognitive reappraisal) and less use of putatively unhelpful strategies (particularly with avoidance). The use of cognitive reappraisal and avoidance partially mediated the relationship between emotion malleability beliefs and psychological distress. These results highlight the importance of considering beliefs about the malleability of emotions in the context of emotion regulation. These findings suggest the potential role of emotion malleability beliefs in interventions for individuals with emotion regulation-related difficulties and psychological distress.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"841-856"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140307374","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Blinded by wistfulness: on how nostalgia strengthens attitudes. 被怀旧蒙蔽了双眼:论怀旧如何强化态度。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-30 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2336196
LaCount J Togans, Allen R McConnell

Across four studies, we explored how feeling nostalgic about an attitude object impacts the metacognitive characteristics of the attitude toward that object and how those metacognitions predict the evaluation's underlying strength. In each study, participants reflected on and evaluated a song or television show that either did or did not elicit nostalgia. Across these studies, we found support for the hypotheses that nostalgic attitude objects are viewed more positively, appraised with greater attitudinal importance, and exhibited less objective ambivalence. In Study 4, we observed that nostalgic attitudes are associated with greater behavioural intentions and that this relationship was mediated both by attitudinal importance and objective ambivalence. These studies contribute to our understanding of how nostalgia affects attitude formation processes.

在四项研究中,我们探讨了对态度对象的怀旧感如何影响对该对象态度的元认知特征,以及这些元认知如何预测评价的基本强度。在每项研究中,参与者都对一首歌曲或电视节目进行了反思和评价,这些歌曲或电视节目有的引起了怀旧情绪,有的则没有。在这些研究中,我们发现怀旧态度对象被更积极地看待、被赋予更高的态度重要性以及表现出更少的客观矛盾等假设得到了支持。在研究 4 中,我们观察到怀旧态度与更大的行为意向相关,而且这种关系受到态度重要性和客观矛盾性的调节。这些研究有助于我们理解怀旧如何影响态度的形成过程。
{"title":"Blinded by wistfulness: on how nostalgia strengthens attitudes.","authors":"LaCount J Togans, Allen R McConnell","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2336196","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2336196","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Across four studies, we explored how feeling nostalgic about an attitude object impacts the metacognitive characteristics of the attitude toward that object and how those metacognitions predict the evaluation's underlying strength. In each study, participants reflected on and evaluated a song or television show that either did or did not elicit nostalgia. Across these studies, we found support for the hypotheses that nostalgic attitude objects are viewed more positively, appraised with greater attitudinal importance, and exhibited less objective ambivalence. In Study 4, we observed that nostalgic attitudes are associated with greater behavioural intentions and that this relationship was mediated both by attitudinal importance and objective ambivalence. These studies contribute to our understanding of how nostalgia affects attitude formation processes.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"913-927"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140330230","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Small talk in videoconferencing improves conversational experience and fosters relationships. 视频会议中的闲聊可以改善对话体验,促进人际关系。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-28 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2392178
Andrew J Guydish, Jean E Fox Tree

Small talk plays a big role in conversational perception. In the study here, pairs of conversational participants engaged in three iterations of an ecologically valid task-break dialogue where the break was either small talk via videoconferencing or waiting the same amount of time with cameras and mics turned off. Small talk increased conversational participants' enjoyment of conversations, their willingness to engage in future conversations with their addressees, and their actual engagement in unprompted conversations with their addressees. Dyads who were instructed to engage in small talk conversation during breaks were approximately three and a half times more likely to have conversations in the sixty second unprompted conversation period at the end of the study compared to dyads whose cameras and mics were off during the earlier break periods. Reciprocity effects previously observed in audio-only and text-only communication were not observed in this study. The findings presented here demonstrate that not only can the positive influence of small talk be replicated and extended to videoconferencing interactions, but such talk can also lead to an increased desire for continued interactions with conversational partners.

闲聊在会话感知中起着重要作用。在本研究中,成对的会话参与者进行了三次生态有效的任务-休息对话,休息时间要么是通过视频会议进行小交谈,要么是在关闭摄像机和麦克风的情况下等待相同的时间。闲聊增加了对话参与者对对话的乐趣,提高了他们今后与对方进行对话的意愿,也增加了他们与对方进行无提示对话的实际参与度。在研究结束后的 60 秒无提示对话时间里,与在休息时间关闭摄像机和麦克风的对话者相比,在休息时间接受指导进行小对话的对话者进行对话的可能性要高出约三倍半。本研究没有观察到之前在纯音频和纯文本交流中观察到的互惠效应。本文的研究结果表明,小范围交谈的积极影响不仅可以复制并扩展到视频会议互动中,而且这种交谈还能增加与交谈伙伴继续互动的愿望。
{"title":"Small talk in videoconferencing improves conversational experience and fosters relationships.","authors":"Andrew J Guydish, Jean E Fox Tree","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2392178","DOIUrl":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2392178","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Small talk plays a big role in conversational perception. In the study here, pairs of conversational participants engaged in three iterations of an ecologically valid task-break dialogue where the break was either small talk via videoconferencing or waiting the same amount of time with cameras and mics turned off. Small talk increased conversational participants' enjoyment of conversations, their willingness to engage in future conversations with their addressees, and their actual engagement in unprompted conversations with their addressees. Dyads who were instructed to engage in small talk conversation during breaks were approximately three and a half times more likely to have conversations in the sixty second unprompted conversation period at the end of the study compared to dyads whose cameras and mics were off during the earlier break periods. Reciprocity effects previously observed in audio-only and text-only communication were not observed in this study. The findings presented here demonstrate that not only can the positive influence of small talk be replicated and extended to videoconferencing interactions, but such talk can also lead to an increased desire for continued interactions with conversational partners.</p>","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1-13"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-08-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142082207","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction. 更正。
IF 2.6 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-26 DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2024.2396761
{"title":"Correction.","authors":"","doi":"10.1080/02699931.2024.2396761","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2024.2396761","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48412,"journal":{"name":"Cognition & Emotion","volume":" ","pages":"1"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-08-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142056975","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognition & Emotion
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1