首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Communication Disorders最新文献

英文 中文
Automatic metaphor processing in developmental dyslexia 发育性阅读障碍中的自动隐喻处理。
IF 1.8 3区 医学 Q2 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-26 DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106448
Rita Cersosimo , Filippo Domaneschi , Hamad Al-Azary

Introduction

Previous research found metaphor impairments with dyslexia; however, it is unclear if difficulties are due to initial activation of the metaphorical meaning or to subsequent discourse integration processes. The study examines the presence of early automatic processing of metaphors in adults with developmental dyslexia, considering the role of executive functions and metaphor familiarity.

Methods

Using a sentence recall task and a semantic judgment task from the Metaphor Interference Effect (MIE) paradigm, we evaluated two early stages of metaphor comprehension, namely the generation of the figurative meaning and the suppression of the literal meaning. High and low familiar metaphors, and their scrambled counterparts, were aurally presented to participants, who were asked to judge whether sentences were literally true or literally false. Afterwards, they were provided ten minutes to recall the sentences they heard to verify the depth of processing for each type of stimulus. A total of 26 participants with dyslexia were included in the experimental group, and 31 in the control group.

Results

Individuals with dyslexia showed a MIE and an accuracy rate that are similar to participants without dyslexia. Inhibition correlated with the MIE size only for high familiar metaphors, and working memory seemed to play no role in the process. In the recall task, both groups demonstrated a better encoding of the metaphorical sentences compared to scrambled metaphors, but participants with dyslexia recalled less metaphors than did the control group, showing that metaphors are no exception to the limitations in sentence retrieval typically found in dyslexia.

Conclusions

Our findings suggest that individuals with dyslexia are comparable to participants without dyslexia in their ability to automatically compute metaphorical meanings. Thus, difficulties in metaphor comprehension in people with dyslexia that have been detected in previous studies might depend on meaning construction in context rather than online semantic processing.

导言:以前的研究发现,隐喻障碍与阅读障碍有关;然而,目前还不清楚困难是由于隐喻意义的最初激活还是由于随后的话语整合过程造成的。本研究考虑了执行功能和隐喻熟悉程度的作用,探讨了发育性阅读障碍成人是否存在隐喻的早期自动加工:方法:我们使用隐喻干扰效应(MIE)范式中的句子回忆任务和语义判断任务,评估了隐喻理解的两个早期阶段,即形象意义的生成和字面意义的抑制。我们用声音向受试者呈现了高熟悉度隐喻和低熟悉度隐喻,以及它们的混淆对应物,要求受试者判断句子字面上的真假。之后,他们有十分钟的时间来回忆听到的句子,以验证对每种刺激的处理深度。实验组共有 26 名阅读障碍患者,对照组有 31 名:结果:有阅读障碍者的 MIE 和准确率与无阅读障碍者相似。抑制与 MIE 大小相关的只有高度熟悉的隐喻,而工作记忆似乎在这一过程中没有发挥作用。在回忆任务中,与乱码隐喻相比,两组受试者对隐喻句子的编码效果都更好,但阅读障碍受试者回忆起的隐喻少于对照组受试者,这表明隐喻也不例外,阅读障碍患者在句子检索方面通常会受到限制:我们的研究结果表明,阅读障碍患者在自动计算隐喻含义方面的能力与无阅读障碍患者相当。因此,以往研究中发现的阅读障碍患者在隐喻理解方面的困难可能取决于语境中的意义建构,而不是在线语义处理。
{"title":"Automatic metaphor processing in developmental dyslexia","authors":"Rita Cersosimo ,&nbsp;Filippo Domaneschi ,&nbsp;Hamad Al-Azary","doi":"10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106448","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106448","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Introduction</h3><p>Previous research found metaphor impairments with dyslexia; however, it is unclear if difficulties are due to initial activation of the metaphorical meaning or to subsequent discourse integration processes. The study examines the presence of early automatic processing of metaphors in adults with developmental dyslexia, considering the role of executive functions and metaphor familiarity.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>Using a sentence recall task and a semantic judgment task from the Metaphor Interference Effect (MIE) paradigm, we evaluated two early stages of metaphor comprehension, namely the generation of the figurative meaning and the suppression of the literal meaning. High and low familiar metaphors, and their scrambled counterparts, were aurally presented to participants, who were asked to judge whether sentences were literally true or literally false. Afterwards, they were provided ten minutes to recall the sentences they heard to verify the depth of processing for each type of stimulus. A total of 26 participants with dyslexia were included in the experimental group, and 31 in the control group.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Individuals with dyslexia showed a MIE and an accuracy rate that are similar to participants without dyslexia. Inhibition correlated with the MIE size only for high familiar metaphors, and working memory seemed to play no role in the process. In the recall task, both groups demonstrated a better encoding of the metaphorical sentences compared to scrambled metaphors, but participants with dyslexia recalled less metaphors than did the control group, showing that metaphors are no exception to the limitations in sentence retrieval typically found in dyslexia.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>Our findings suggest that individuals with dyslexia are comparable to participants without dyslexia in their ability to automatically compute metaphorical meanings. Thus, difficulties in metaphor comprehension in people with dyslexia that have been detected in previous studies might depend on meaning construction in context rather than online semantic processing.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":49175,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Communication Disorders","volume":"111 ","pages":"Article 106448"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141545415","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Lexical development in Kuwaiti Arabic in typically developing children and late talkers 发育典型儿童和说话较晚儿童的科威特阿拉伯语词汇发展
IF 1.7 3区 医学 Q2 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-22 DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106432
Fauzia Abdalla , Abdessattar Mahfoudhi , Saleh Shaalan , Reem Almarri , Maryam Aldousari , Hessa Alseedeqi

Introduction

This study explored vocabulary development and lexical composition in young typically developing (TD) Kuwaiti children and late talkers (LT) using the Kuwaiti Arabic Communicative Development Inventory-Words and Sentences (KACDI-WS) Abdalla et al., 2016). The sample included 161 children aged 20 to 37 months: 127 TD and 34 children who were late talkers (LT group). The late talkers were first identified based on a background questionnaire answered by the parents. All the caregivers completed a 698-item web-based KACDI expressive vocabulary inventory by selecting non-imitative words that their children produced.

Results

Lexical size and composition (nouns, predicates, and closed-class words) were analyzed. Across the TD age groups (20–26, 27–31, 32–37 months), a significant age effect for vocabulary size and composition was found in favor of the older groups. Nouns were more prevalent than predicates or closed-class words in within-group comparisons. The vocabulary size of the TD (M= 263.8) was significantly larger than that of the LT group (M= 69.2). The development of their lexical composition followed a similar pattern.

Conclusions

The results suggested that the KACDI parent report instrument has the potential for measuring vocabulary development in TD children and could serve as an initial screening tool to identify late talkers.

导言:本研究使用科威特阿拉伯语交际发展清单--单词和句子(KACDI-WS,Abdalla et al.样本包括 161 名 20 至 37 个月大的儿童:127名TD儿童和34名晚期说话儿童(LT组)。首先根据家长回答的背景问卷确定了晚说话儿童。所有照顾者通过选择其孩子所产生的非定量词汇,完成了一份包含 698 个词条的 KACDI 表达性词汇网络清单。结果分析了词汇量和构成(名词、谓词和闭类词)。在训练和发展年龄组(20-26 个月、27-31 个月、32-37 个月)中,年龄越大,词汇量和词汇构成的年龄效应越明显。在组内比较中,名词比谓词或封闭类词更普遍。TD组的词汇量(M= 263.8)明显大于LT组(M= 69.2)。结果表明,KACDI 家长报告工具具有测量 TD 儿童词汇发展的潜力,可作为识别晚说话者的初步筛选工具。
{"title":"Lexical development in Kuwaiti Arabic in typically developing children and late talkers","authors":"Fauzia Abdalla ,&nbsp;Abdessattar Mahfoudhi ,&nbsp;Saleh Shaalan ,&nbsp;Reem Almarri ,&nbsp;Maryam Aldousari ,&nbsp;Hessa Alseedeqi","doi":"10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106432","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106432","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Introduction</h3><p>This study explored vocabulary development and lexical composition in young typically developing (TD) Kuwaiti children and late talkers (LT) using the <em>Kuwaiti Arabic Communicative Development Inventory-Words and Sentences</em> (<em>KACDI-WS) Abdalla</em> et al.<em>, 2016</em>). The sample included 161 children aged 20 to 37 months: 127 TD and 34 children who were late talkers (LT group). The late talkers were first identified based on a background questionnaire answered by the parents. All the caregivers completed a 698-item web-based <em>KACDI</em> expressive vocabulary inventory by selecting non-imitative words that their children produced.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Lexical size and composition (nouns, predicates, and closed-class words) were analyzed. Across the TD age groups (20–26, 27–31, 32–37 months), a significant age effect for vocabulary size and composition was found in favor of the older groups. Nouns were more prevalent than predicates or closed-class words in within-group comparisons. The vocabulary size of the TD (<em>M</em>= 263.8) was significantly larger than that of the LT group (<em>M</em>= 69.2). The development of their lexical composition followed a similar pattern.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>The results suggested that the <em>KACDI</em> parent report instrument has the potential for measuring vocabulary development in TD children and could serve as an initial screening tool to identify late talkers<del>.</del></p></div>","PeriodicalId":49175,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Communication Disorders","volume":"110 ","pages":"Article 106432"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141083809","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The perceived guilt and innocence of adults with developmental language disorder and adults with typical language during a mock interrogation 有发育性语言障碍的成年人和有典型语言障碍的成年人在模拟审讯中对有罪和无罪的感知
IF 1.7 3区 医学 Q2 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-15 DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106429
Tammie J. Spaulding , Audra Blewitt

Purpose

This study examined if there were differences in the guilty and not guilty judgments of adults with developmental language disorder (DLD) and those with typical language (TL) functioning.

Method

Twenty-four adults (12 DLD, 12 TL) were assigned to either the guilty or not guilty conditions. Those in the guilty condition engaged in a mock crime while those in the not guilty condition were informed that a crime had been committed. Peer jurors were presented with video interrogations of the DLD (6 guilty, 6 not guilty) and TL (6 guilty, 6 not guilty) participants and were asked to make categorical judgments of guilty and not guilty and to indicate confidence in their judgments.

Results

In general, peer jurors were not accurate in their judgments of the accused, and were more likely to judge individuals with DLD as guilty relative to accused individuals with TL. Peer jurors were particularly poor at judging innocent adults with DLD as not guilty and guilty adults with TL as guilty. Despite this, peer jurors were more confident than not in their guilty and not guilty determinations.

Conclusions

Peer jurors are confident in their judgments of the guilt of the accused when they should not be, particularly in the case of accused adults with DLD. Implications are discussed.

本研究探讨了患有发育性语言障碍(DLD)的成年人和具有典型语言功能(TL)的成年人在有罪和无罪判断方面是否存在差异。有罪条件下的陪审员参与模拟犯罪,而无罪条件下的陪审员则被告知发生了一起犯罪。同行陪审员观看了 DLD(6 人有罪,6 人无罪)和 TL(6 人有罪,6 人无罪)参与者的审讯录像,并被要求做出有罪和无罪的分类判断,以及表示对其判断的信心。结果一般而言,同行陪审员对被告的判断并不准确,相对于 TL 被告而言,他们更有可能将 DLD 个人判定为有罪。同伴陪审员尤其不善于判断患有 DLD 的无辜成年人无罪,而判断患有 TL 的有罪成年人有罪。尽管如此,同侪陪审员在判定有罪和无罪时比不判定有罪时更有信心。结论同侪陪审员在判定被告有罪时很有信心,而在他们不应该有信心时却很有信心,特别是在被控患有 DLD 的成年人的案件中。本文讨论了其影响。
{"title":"The perceived guilt and innocence of adults with developmental language disorder and adults with typical language during a mock interrogation","authors":"Tammie J. Spaulding ,&nbsp;Audra Blewitt","doi":"10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106429","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106429","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Purpose</h3><p>This study examined if there were differences in the guilty and not guilty judgments of adults with developmental language disorder (DLD) and those with typical language (TL) functioning.</p></div><div><h3>Method</h3><p>Twenty-four adults (12 DLD, 12 TL) were assigned to either the guilty or not guilty conditions. Those in the guilty condition engaged in a mock crime while those in the not guilty condition were informed that a crime had been committed. Peer jurors were presented with video interrogations of the DLD (6 guilty, 6 not guilty) and TL (6 guilty, 6 not guilty) participants and were asked to make categorical judgments of guilty and not guilty and to indicate confidence in their judgments.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>In general, peer jurors were not accurate in their judgments of the accused, and were more likely to judge individuals with DLD as guilty relative to accused individuals with TL. Peer jurors were particularly poor at judging innocent adults with DLD as not guilty and guilty adults with TL as guilty. Despite this, peer jurors were more confident than not in their guilty and not guilty determinations.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>Peer jurors are confident in their judgments of the guilt of the accused when they should not be, particularly in the case of accused adults with DLD. Implications are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":49175,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Communication Disorders","volume":"110 ","pages":"Article 106429"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140948124","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A meta-analysis of expressive prosody in cochlear implant users 人工耳蜗使用者的表达式拟声词荟萃分析
IF 1.7 3区 医学 Q2 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-14 DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106431
Kara Hawthorne

Introduction

Prosody is used to express indexical (identifying the talker), linguistic (e.g., question intonation, lexical stress), pragmatic (e.g., contrastive stress, sarcasm), and emotional/affective functions. It is manifested through changes in fundamental frequency (f0), intensity, and duration. F0 and intensity are degraded when perceived through a cochlear implant (CI). The purpose of this meta-analysis is to compare expressive prosody in speech produced by CI users versus normal hearing peers.

Methods

A systematic search of the literature found 25 articles that met all inclusion criteria. These articles were assessed for quality, and data pertaining to the expression of f0, intensity, and duration, as well as classification accuracy and appropriateness ratings from normal hearing listeners, were extracted and meta-analyzed using random effects models.

Results

The articles included in the meta-analysis were generally of acceptable or high quality. Meta-analyses revealed significant differences between individuals with CIs vs. normal hearing on all measures except mean f0, mean intensity, and rhythm. Effect sizes were generally medium to large. There was significant heterogeneity across studies, but little evidence of publication bias.

Conclusions

CI users speak with less variable f0, smaller f0 contours, more variable intensity, a slower speech rate, and reduced final lengthening at syntactic boundaries. These acoustic differences are reflected in significantly poorer ratings of speech produced by CI users compared to their normal hearing peers, as assessed by groups of normal hearing listeners. Because atypical expressive prosody is associated with negative outcomes, clinicians should consider targeting prosody when working with individuals who use CIs.

导言前音用于表达索引(识别谈话者)、语言(如疑问语气、词汇重音)、语用(如对比重音、讽刺)和情感/情绪功能。它表现为基频(f0)、强度和持续时间的变化。通过人工耳蜗(CI)感知时,F0 和强度会降低。本荟萃分析的目的是比较 CI 使用者与听力正常的同龄人在语音中的表现性前奏。对这些文章进行了质量评估,并提取了与 f0、强度和持续时间的表达以及正常听力听者的分类准确性和适当性评分有关的数据,然后使用随机效应模型进行了元分析。荟萃分析表明,除平均 f0、平均强度和节奏外,CI 患者与听力正常者在所有指标上都存在显著差异。效应大小一般为中等到较大。结论CI 使用者说话时 f0 变化较少,f0 等高线较小,强度变化较大,语速较慢,句法界限处的末尾延长减少。这些声学差异反映在由听力正常的听者群体对 CI 使用者的语音进行的评估中,与听力正常的同龄人相比,CI 使用者的语音评分明显较低。由于不典型的表达性前语与负面结果相关,临床医生在与使用 CI 的人合作时应考虑针对前语进行治疗。
{"title":"A meta-analysis of expressive prosody in cochlear implant users","authors":"Kara Hawthorne","doi":"10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106431","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106431","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Introduction</h3><p>Prosody is used to express indexical (identifying the talker), linguistic (e.g., question intonation, lexical stress), pragmatic (e.g., contrastive stress, sarcasm), and emotional/affective functions. It is manifested through changes in fundamental frequency (<em>f</em><sub>0</sub>), intensity, and duration. <em>F</em><sub>0</sub> and intensity are degraded when perceived through a cochlear implant (CI). The purpose of this meta-analysis is to compare expressive prosody in speech produced by CI users versus normal hearing peers.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>A systematic search of the literature found 25 articles that met all inclusion criteria. These articles were assessed for quality, and data pertaining to the expression of <em>f</em><sub>0</sub>, intensity, and duration, as well as classification accuracy and appropriateness ratings from normal hearing listeners, were extracted and meta-analyzed using random effects models.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>The articles included in the meta-analysis were generally of acceptable or high quality. Meta-analyses revealed significant differences between individuals with CIs vs. normal hearing on all measures except mean <em>f</em><sub>0</sub>, mean intensity, and rhythm. Effect sizes were generally medium to large. There was significant heterogeneity across studies, but little evidence of publication bias.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>CI users speak with less variable <em>f</em><sub>0</sub>, smaller <em>f</em><sub>0</sub> contours, more variable intensity, a slower speech rate, and reduced final lengthening at syntactic boundaries. These acoustic differences are reflected in significantly poorer ratings of speech produced by CI users compared to their normal hearing peers, as assessed by groups of normal hearing listeners. Because atypical expressive prosody is associated with negative outcomes, clinicians should consider targeting prosody when working with individuals who use CIs.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":49175,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Communication Disorders","volume":"110 ","pages":"Article 106431"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141029509","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Challenges with computing scalar and ad-hoc implicatures in Mandarin-speaking 4–8-year-old autistic children 计算 4-8 岁讲普通话的自闭症儿童的标量蕴涵和临时蕴涵所面临的挑战
IF 1.7 3区 医学 Q2 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106427
Yi Esther Su, Yuhan Jiang

Introduction

Mixed findings have been reported about the computation of scalar or/and ad-hoc implicatures in primarily school-age autistic verbal children and adolescents: while some studies reported their struggles with both implicatures, others observed their strengths in computing scalar implicatures. This study extends the previous investigation by testing the derivation of scalar (including both number and quantifier) and ad-hoc implicatures of a younger group of Mandarin-speaking autistic 4–8-year-olds; moreover, we assess the biological, linguistic, and cognitive factors affecting children's implicature acquisition.

Methods

The participants included 22 4–8-year-old autistic verbal children (mean age = 67.64 months) and 19 typically developing (TD) children who did not significantly differ in age, receptive vocabulary, and non-verbal IQ. Both groups completed a computer-based Truth Value Judgment task, assessing their knowledge of scalar (involving the number ‘three’ and the quantifier ‘some’) and ad-hoc implicatures. We also examined whether their implicature computation was linked to age, receptive vocabulary, non-verbal IQ, and Theory of Mind (ToM).

Results

Compared with the TD controls, autistic children derived significantly fewer scalar and ad-hoc implicatures. Specifically, TD children successfully computed number and ad-hoc implicatures, contrasting to the bimodal distribution of their pragmatic vs. logical responses to quantifier implicatures. Though autistic children performed better with number implicatures slightly above the chance level, they had difficulties in computing quantifier and ad-hoc implicatures. Further, autistic children's knowledge of the number and ad-hoc implicatures was linked to their ToM skills.

Conclusions

These findings underscore the overall delayed implicature knowledge of young autistic children, and their low sensitivity to the implicatures is related to the core ToM deficits. Furthermore, our data confirm the coherent pattern of the earlier acquisition of number over quantifier implicatures and illuminate the distinct mechanisms underlying the computation of scalar vs. ad-hoc implicatures.

引言关于学龄自闭症言语儿童和青少年计算标量或/和临时蕴涵的研究结果不一:一些研究报告称他们在这两种蕴涵上都很吃力,而另一些研究则观察到他们在计算标量蕴涵上的优势。本研究对之前的研究进行了扩展,测试了一组年龄较小的 4-8 岁讲普通话的自闭症儿童的标量(包括数字和数量词)隐含式和临时隐含式的推导能力;此外,我们还评估了影响儿童隐含式习得的生物、语言和认知因素。两组儿童都完成了一项基于计算机的真值判断任务,评估他们对标量(涉及数字 "三 "和量词 "一些")和临时暗示的知识。我们还研究了他们的暗示计算是否与年龄、接受词汇量、非言语智商和心智理论(ToM)有关。特别是,TD 儿童成功地计算了数字和临时蕴涵,这与他们对量词蕴涵的语用与逻辑反应的双峰分布形成了鲜明对比。虽然自闭症儿童在数字蕴涵方面的表现略高于偶然水平,但他们在计算量词和临时蕴涵方面却有困难。此外,自闭症儿童对数字和专指蕴涵的知识与他们的 ToM 技能有关。结论这些发现强调了年幼自闭症儿童对蕴涵知识的整体延迟,他们对蕴涵的低敏感性与核心 ToM 缺陷有关。此外,我们的数据证实了数字蕴涵比量词蕴涵更早获得的一致模式,并阐明了标量蕴涵与临时蕴涵计算的不同机制。
{"title":"Challenges with computing scalar and ad-hoc implicatures in Mandarin-speaking 4–8-year-old autistic children","authors":"Yi Esther Su,&nbsp;Yuhan Jiang","doi":"10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106427","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106427","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Introduction</h3><p>Mixed findings have been reported about the computation of scalar or/and ad-hoc implicatures in primarily school-age autistic verbal children and adolescents: while some studies reported their struggles with both implicatures, others observed their strengths in computing scalar implicatures. This study extends the previous investigation by testing the derivation of scalar (including both number and quantifier) and ad-hoc implicatures of a younger group of Mandarin-speaking autistic 4–8-year-olds; moreover, we assess the biological, linguistic, and cognitive factors affecting children's implicature acquisition.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>The participants included 22 4–8-year-old autistic verbal children (mean age = 67.64 months) and 19 typically developing (TD) children who did not significantly differ in age, receptive vocabulary, and non-verbal IQ. Both groups completed a computer-based Truth Value Judgment task, assessing their knowledge of scalar (involving the number ‘three’ and the quantifier ‘some’) and ad-hoc implicatures. We also examined whether their implicature computation was linked to age, receptive vocabulary, non-verbal IQ, and Theory of Mind (ToM).</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Compared with the TD controls, autistic children derived significantly fewer scalar and ad-hoc implicatures. Specifically, TD children successfully computed number and ad-hoc implicatures, contrasting to the bimodal distribution of their pragmatic vs. logical responses to quantifier implicatures. Though autistic children performed better with number implicatures slightly above the chance level, they had difficulties in computing quantifier and ad-hoc implicatures. Further, autistic children's knowledge of the number and ad-hoc implicatures was linked to their ToM skills.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>These findings underscore the overall delayed implicature knowledge of young autistic children, and their low sensitivity to the implicatures is related to the core ToM deficits. Furthermore, our data confirm the coherent pattern of the earlier acquisition of number over quantifier implicatures and illuminate the distinct mechanisms underlying the computation of scalar vs. ad-hoc implicatures.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":49175,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Communication Disorders","volume":"110 ","pages":"Article 106427"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140902007","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Normative values for tests of central auditory processing disorder in children aged from 6 to 12 years old 6 至 12 岁儿童中枢听觉处理障碍测试的标准值
IF 1.7 3区 医学 Q2 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106426
Piotr H. Skarzynski , Natalia Czajka , Rita Zdanowicz , Aleksandra Kolodziejak , Ewelina Bukato , Małgorzata Talarek , Zuzanna Pankowska , Henryk Skarzynski

Introduction

Central auditory processing disorders (CAPD) can significantly affect the daily functioning of a child, and the first step in determining whether rehabilitation procedures are required is a proper diagnosis. Different guidelines for making diagnoses have been published in the literature, and in various centers normative values for psychoacoustic tests of CAPD have been used internally. The material presented in this paper is based on more than 1000 children and is the largest collection so far published. The aim of this study is to present normative values for tests assessing CAPD in children aged 6 to 12 years, divided by age at last birthday.

Method

We tested 1037 children aged 6 to 12 years who were attending primary schools and kindergartens. The criteria for inclusion were a normal audiogram, intellectually normal, no developmental problems, and no difficulties in auditory processing. To evaluate auditory processing all children were given three tests on the Senses Examination Platform: the Frequency Pattern Test (FPT), Duration Pattern Test (DPT), and Dichotic Digit Test (DDT).

Results

The results from 1,037 children allowed us to determine normative values for FPT, DPT, and DDT in seven different age groups (6 through to 12 years). We developed a newapproach, based on quantile-based norms, to determine normative values in each group. Three categories – average, below-average, and above-average – allow for a broader but more realistic interpretation than those used previously. We compare our results with published standards.

Conclusions

Our study is the largest normative database published to date for CAPD testing, setting a standard for each child by age in years. We used the Senses Examination Platform, a universal tool, to unify standards for the classification of CAPD. Our study can serve as a basis for the development of a Polish model for the diagnosis of CAPD.

导言中枢性听觉处理障碍(CAPD)会严重影响儿童的日常功能,而确定是否需要进行康复治疗的第一步就是进行正确的诊断。文献中发表了不同的诊断指南,各中心内部也使用了中枢听觉处理障碍心理声学测试的标准值。本文所介绍的材料基于 1000 多名儿童,是迄今为止所发表的最大规模的资料集。本研究的目的是按最后一个生日时的年龄划分,提出评估 6 至 12 岁儿童听力障碍测试的标准值。方法我们对就读于小学和幼儿园的 1037 名 6 至 12 岁儿童进行了测试。纳入标准为听力正常、智力正常、无发育问题、听觉处理无困难。为了评估听觉处理能力,我们在感官检查平台上对所有儿童进行了三项测试:频率模式测试(FPT)、持续时间模式测试(DPT)和二分法数字测试(DDT)。结果我们从 1037 名儿童的测试结果中确定了七个不同年龄组(6 至 12 岁)的频率模式测试、持续时间模式测试和二分法数字测试的标准值。我们开发了一种基于量化标准的新方法来确定每个组别的标准值。平均值、低于平均值和高于平均值这三个类别允许比以前使用的标准更广泛但更现实的解释。我们将我们的结果与已公布的标准进行了比较。结论我们的研究是迄今为止已公布的最大的 CAPD 测试常模数据库,按年龄为每个儿童设定了一个标准。我们使用感官检查平台这一通用工具来统一 CAPD 的分类标准。我们的研究可作为波兰CAPD诊断模型发展的基础。
{"title":"Normative values for tests of central auditory processing disorder in children aged from 6 to 12 years old","authors":"Piotr H. Skarzynski ,&nbsp;Natalia Czajka ,&nbsp;Rita Zdanowicz ,&nbsp;Aleksandra Kolodziejak ,&nbsp;Ewelina Bukato ,&nbsp;Małgorzata Talarek ,&nbsp;Zuzanna Pankowska ,&nbsp;Henryk Skarzynski","doi":"10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106426","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106426","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Introduction</h3><p>Central auditory processing disorders (CAPD) can significantly affect the daily functioning of a child, and the first step in determining whether rehabilitation procedures are required is a proper diagnosis. Different guidelines for making diagnoses have been published in the literature, and in various centers normative values for psychoacoustic tests of CAPD have been used internally. The material presented in this paper is based on more than 1000 children and is the largest collection so far published. The aim of this study is to present normative values for tests assessing CAPD in children aged 6 to 12 years, divided by age at last birthday.</p></div><div><h3>Method</h3><p>We tested 1037 children aged 6 to 12 years who were attending primary schools and kindergartens. The criteria for inclusion were a normal audiogram, intellectually normal, no developmental problems, and no difficulties in auditory processing. To evaluate auditory processing all children were given three tests on the Senses Examination Platform: the Frequency Pattern Test (FPT), Duration Pattern Test (DPT), and Dichotic Digit Test (DDT).</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>The results from 1,037 children allowed us to determine normative values for FPT, DPT, and DDT in seven different age groups (6 through to 12 years). We developed a newapproach, based on quantile-based norms, to determine normative values in each group. Three categories – average, below-average, and above-average – allow for a broader but more realistic interpretation than those used previously. We compare our results with published standards.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>Our study is the largest normative database published to date for CAPD testing, setting a standard for each child by age in years. We used the Senses Examination Platform, a universal tool, to unify standards for the classification of CAPD. Our study can serve as a basis for the development of a Polish model for the diagnosis of CAPD.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":49175,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Communication Disorders","volume":"109 ","pages":"Article 106426"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0021992424000224/pdfft?md5=2cc819a0f7d06610fa39d1ee31e1108a&pid=1-s2.0-S0021992424000224-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140779754","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Differential spectral characteristics of the Spanish fricative /s/ in the articulation of individuals with dysarthria and apraxia of speech 西班牙摩擦音 /s/ 在构音障碍和语言障碍患者发音中的不同频谱特征
IF 1.7 3区 医学 Q2 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106428
Natalia Melle , Carlos Gallego , José María Lahoz-Bengoechea , Silvia Nieva

Purpose

This study examines whether there are differences in the speech of speakers with dysarthria, speakers with apraxia and healthy speakers in spectral acoustic measures during production of the central-peninsular Spanish alveolar sibilant fricative /s/.

Method

To this end, production of the sibilant was analyzed in 20 subjects with dysarthria, 8 with apraxia of speech and 28 healthy speakers. Participants produced 12 sV(C) words. The variables compared across groups were the fricative's spectral amplitude difference (AmpD) and spectral moments in the temporal midpoint of fricative execution.

Results

The results indicate that individuals with dysarthria can be distinguished from healthy speakers in terms of the spectral characteristics AmpD, standard deviation (SD), center of gravity (CoG) and skewness, the last two in context with unrounded vowel, while no differences in kurtosis were detected. Participants with AoS group differ significantly from healthy speaker group in AmpD, SD and CoG and Kurtosis, the first one followed unrounded vowel and the latter two followed by rounded vowels. In addition, speakers with apraxia of speech group returned significant differences with respect to speakers with dysarthria group in AmpD, CoG and skewness.

Conclusions

The differences found between the groups in the measures studied as a function of the type of vowel context could provide insights into the distinctive manifestations of motor speech disorders, contributing to the differential diagnosis between apraxia and dysarthria in motor control processes.

目的 本研究探讨了构音障碍者、语言障碍者和健康者在发出中枢-下颌西班牙语齿龈咝音摩擦音/s/时,在频谱声学测量方面是否存在差异。方法 为此,研究人员对 20 名构音障碍者、8 名语言障碍者和 28 名健康者发出的咝音进行了分析。受试者发出了 12 个 sV(C) 词。结果表明,构音障碍者与健康说话者在频谱特征 AmpD、标准差 (SD)、重心 (CoG) 和偏度(后两者与非圆元音有关)方面存在差异,而在峰度方面没有发现差异。在 AmpD、SD、CoG 和峰度方面,AoS 组的参与者与健康说话者组的参与者有显著差异,前者是在非圆润元音的情况下,而后两者是在圆润元音的情况下。此外,语言障碍组说话者与构音障碍组说话者在 AmpD、CoG 和偏度方面也有显著差异。结论在所研究的测量指标中发现的组间差异是元音上下文类型的函数,这有助于深入了解运动性言语障碍的独特表现,有助于在运动控制过程中对语言障碍和构音障碍进行鉴别诊断。
{"title":"Differential spectral characteristics of the Spanish fricative /s/ in the articulation of individuals with dysarthria and apraxia of speech","authors":"Natalia Melle ,&nbsp;Carlos Gallego ,&nbsp;José María Lahoz-Bengoechea ,&nbsp;Silvia Nieva","doi":"10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106428","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106428","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Purpose</h3><p>This study examines whether there are differences in the speech of speakers with dysarthria, speakers with apraxia and healthy speakers in spectral acoustic measures during production of the central-peninsular Spanish alveolar sibilant fricative /s/.</p></div><div><h3>Method</h3><p>To this end, production of the sibilant was analyzed in 20 subjects with dysarthria, 8 with apraxia of speech and 28 healthy speakers. Participants produced 12 sV(C) words. The variables compared across groups were the fricative's spectral amplitude difference (AmpD) and spectral moments in the temporal midpoint of fricative execution.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>The results indicate that individuals with dysarthria can be distinguished from healthy speakers in terms of the spectral characteristics AmpD, standard deviation (SD), center of gravity (CoG) and skewness, the last two in context with unrounded vowel, while no differences in kurtosis were detected. Participants with AoS group differ significantly from healthy speaker group in AmpD, SD and CoG and Kurtosis, the first one followed unrounded vowel and the latter two followed by rounded vowels. In addition, speakers with apraxia of speech group returned significant differences with respect to speakers with dysarthria group in AmpD, CoG and skewness.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>The differences found between the groups in the measures studied as a function of the type of vowel context could provide insights into the distinctive manifestations of motor speech disorders, contributing to the differential diagnosis between apraxia and dysarthria in motor control processes.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":49175,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Communication Disorders","volume":"109 ","pages":"Article 106428"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140917846","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Changes in speech prosody perception during Parkinson's disease: A comprehensive analysis 帕金森病期间语音前奏感的变化:综合分析
IF 1.7 3区 医学 Q2 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106430
Nasrin Shahouzaei , Zahra Ghayoumi-Anaraki , Toktam Maleki Shahmahmood , Narges Torke Ladani , Ali Shoeibi

Introduction

Parkinson's disease (PD) is a progressive neurodegenerative disorder that affects approximately 1%–2% of individuals aged 60 and above. Communication disorders in PD can significantly impact the overall quality of life. As prosody plays a vital role in verbal communication, the present study examines Persian prosody perception in PD, focusing on linguistic and emotional aspects of prosody.

Methods

This cross-sectional study aimed to compare the perception of linguistic and emotional prosody in three groups: middle-aged adults (n = 22; mean age = 50.40 years), healthy older adults (n = 22; mean age = 68.31 years), and individuals with Parkinson's disease (n = 22; mean age = 65years). All individuals with PD were classified in stages 1; 1.5; 2; 2.5, and 3 of the disease using the Hoehn and Yahr scale. All participants had an MMSE score of 24 or above. The Florida Affect Battery (FAB) was used to evaluate prosody perception. This Battery was validated in the Persian language and its reliability and validity were reported as 94 % and 100 % respectively.

Results

Participants with PD presented significantly lower scores than the older adults in all subtests of the FAB (p < 0.05), while healthy older adults were significantly different only in linguistic discrimination (β = -2.14; -3.68 to -0.61), and linguistic naming of prosody (β = 1.25; 0.17 to 2.33) compared to middle-aged adults.

Conclusions

The present study sheds light on the influence of PD on Persian prosody perception. Given the crucial role of prosody in verbal communication, these findings enhance our understanding of communication disorders in PD and could bring attention to consider prosody perception, among other aspects, when assessing individuals affected by PD.

导言帕金森病(PD)是一种进行性神经退行性疾病,约有 1%-2%的 60 岁及以上的人患有此病。帕金森病患者的交流障碍会严重影响其整体生活质量。这项横断面研究旨在比较三组人对语言和情感拟声词的感知:中年人(22 人;平均年龄 50.40 岁)、健康老年人(22 人;平均年龄 68.31 岁)和帕金森病患者(22 人;平均年龄 65 岁)。所有帕金森病患者均根据霍恩和雅尔量表被分为 1 期、1.5 期、2 期、2.5 期和 3 期。所有参与者的 MMSE 均为 24 分或以上。佛罗里达情感测验(Florida Affect Battery,FAB)用于评估前奏感知。该测验已在波斯语中得到验证,其信度和效度分别为 94 % 和 100 %。结果帕金森病患者在 FAB 的所有分测验中的得分均明显低于老年人(P < 0.05),而健康的老年人仅在语言辨别(β = -2.14; -3.68 to -0.61)和拟声词的语言命名(β = 1.25; 0.17 to 2.33)方面与中年人相比有明显差异。鉴于拟声词在言语交际中的关键作用,这些研究结果加深了我们对帕金森病患者交际障碍的理解,并可引起我们在评估帕金森病患者时对拟声词感知等方面的关注。
{"title":"Changes in speech prosody perception during Parkinson's disease: A comprehensive analysis","authors":"Nasrin Shahouzaei ,&nbsp;Zahra Ghayoumi-Anaraki ,&nbsp;Toktam Maleki Shahmahmood ,&nbsp;Narges Torke Ladani ,&nbsp;Ali Shoeibi","doi":"10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106430","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106430","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Introduction</h3><p>Parkinson's disease (PD) is a progressive neurodegenerative disorder that affects approximately 1%–2% of individuals aged 60 and above. Communication disorders in PD can significantly impact the overall quality of life. As prosody plays a vital role in verbal communication, the present study examines Persian prosody perception in PD, focusing on linguistic and emotional aspects of prosody.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>This cross-sectional study aimed to compare the perception of linguistic and emotional prosody in three groups: middle-aged adults (<em>n</em> = 22; mean age = 50.40 years), healthy older adults (<em>n</em> = 22; mean age = 68.31 years), and individuals with Parkinson's disease (<em>n</em> = 22; mean age = 65years). All individuals with PD were classified in stages 1; 1.5; 2; 2.5, and 3 of the disease using the Hoehn and Yahr scale. All participants had an MMSE score of 24 or above. The Florida Affect Battery (FAB) was used to evaluate prosody perception. This Battery was validated in the Persian language and its reliability and validity were reported as 94 % and 100 % respectively.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Participants with PD presented significantly lower scores than the older adults in all subtests of the FAB (<em>p</em> &lt; 0.05), while healthy older adults were significantly different only in linguistic discrimination (β = -2.14; -3.68 to -0.61), and linguistic naming of prosody (β = 1.25; 0.17 to 2.33) compared to middle-aged adults.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>The present study sheds light on the influence of PD on Persian prosody perception. Given the crucial role of prosody in verbal communication, these findings enhance our understanding of communication disorders in PD and could bring attention to consider prosody perception, among other aspects, when assessing individuals affected by PD.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":49175,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Communication Disorders","volume":"110 ","pages":"Article 106430"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140948730","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Associations between social anxiety, physiological reactivity, and speech disfluencies in autistic young adults and controls 自闭症青少年与对照组在社交焦虑、生理反应和言语不流畅之间的关系
IF 1.7 3区 医学 Q2 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-04-02 DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106425
Veera Pirinen , Kurt Eggers , Katja Dindar , Terhi Helminen , Aija Kotila , Sanna Kuusikko-Gauffin , Leena Mäkinen , Hanna Ebeling , Tuula Hurtig , Mirjami Mäntymaa , Soile Loukusa

Introduction

The aim of this study was to examine possible associations of social anxiety (SA) and speaking-related physiological reactivity with the frequencies of a) total disfluencies, b) typical disfluencies, and c) stuttering-like disfluencies, as well as d) stuttering-severity in autistic young adults and controls.

Methods

Thirty-two autistic young adults and 35 controls participated in this study. Participants were presented with video clips (viewing condition) and were then asked to talk about the videos (narrating condition). SA was measured by the self-report Social Phobia and Anxiety Inventory (SPAI). Speaking-related physiological reactivity was measured by the electrodermal activity (EDA), an index of emotional arousal. The speech samples from the narrating condition were analyzed for type and frequency of speech disfluencies and used for determining the stuttering severity. SA and speaking-related physiological reactivity were compared between the groups. Correlation between SA, physiological reactivity, disfluency frequencies, and stuttering severity were tested separately for both groups.

Results

No between-group differences were found in the overall SA, yet differences were found in SPAI subscales of social interaction, group interaction, and avoidance, as well as in agoraphobia. Both groups had higher physiological arousal in narrating condition in comparison to the video viewing condition, yet there was no between-group difference in the reactivity. No associations were found between SPAI measures, physiological reactivity, disfluency frequencies, and stuttering severity in the autistic group. In the control group, a negative association was found between physiological reactivity and total and typical disfluency frequencies.

Conclusions

SA or speaking-related physiological reactivity were not associated with disfluency frequencies or stuttering severity in autistic persons. Negative association between physiological reactivity and disfluency frequencies found in the control group may indicate that the physiological arousal may impact the speech production process by reducing the overt disfluencies.

简介:本研究旨在探讨社交焦虑(SA)和与说话相关的生理反应与自闭症青少年和对照组中 a) 总口吃、b) 典型口吃、c) 口吃样口吃以及 d) 口吃严重程度的频率之间可能存在的关联。研究人员向受试者播放视频短片(观看条件),然后要求受试者讲述视频内容(讲述条件)。自闭症通过自我报告社交恐惧症和焦虑量表(SPAI)进行测量。与说话相关的生理反应性通过皮电活动(EDA)进行测量,这是一种情绪唤醒指数。对叙述状态下的言语样本进行分析,以确定言语不流畅的类型和频率,并用于确定口吃的严重程度。对各组之间的口吃症状和与说话相关的生理反应进行了比较。结果 在总体口吃症状方面没有发现组间差异,但在社会交往、群体交往和回避等 SPAI 子量表以及广场恐惧症方面发现了差异。与观看视频的条件相比,两组受试者在叙述条件下的生理唤醒程度都更高,但在反应性方面却没有组间差异。在自闭症组中,SPAI测量值、生理反应性、不流利频率和口吃严重程度之间没有关联。在对照组中,生理反应与总的和典型的口吃频率之间呈负相关。在对照组中发现的生理反应与口吃频率之间的负相关可能表明,生理唤醒可能会通过减少明显的口吃来影响言语生成过程。
{"title":"Associations between social anxiety, physiological reactivity, and speech disfluencies in autistic young adults and controls","authors":"Veera Pirinen ,&nbsp;Kurt Eggers ,&nbsp;Katja Dindar ,&nbsp;Terhi Helminen ,&nbsp;Aija Kotila ,&nbsp;Sanna Kuusikko-Gauffin ,&nbsp;Leena Mäkinen ,&nbsp;Hanna Ebeling ,&nbsp;Tuula Hurtig ,&nbsp;Mirjami Mäntymaa ,&nbsp;Soile Loukusa","doi":"10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106425","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106425","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Introduction</h3><p>The aim of this study was to examine possible associations of social anxiety (SA) and speaking-related physiological reactivity with the frequencies of a) total disfluencies, b) typical disfluencies, and c) stuttering-like disfluencies, as well as d) stuttering-severity in autistic young adults and controls.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>Thirty-two autistic young adults and 35 controls participated in this study. Participants were presented with video clips (viewing condition) and were then asked to talk about the videos (narrating condition). SA was measured by the self-report <em>Social Phobia and Anxiety Inventory</em> (SPAI). Speaking-related physiological reactivity was measured by the electrodermal activity (EDA), an index of emotional arousal. The speech samples from the narrating condition were analyzed for type and frequency of speech disfluencies and used for determining the stuttering severity. SA and speaking-related physiological reactivity were compared between the groups. Correlation between SA, physiological reactivity, disfluency frequencies, and stuttering severity were tested separately for both groups.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>No between-group differences were found in the overall SA, yet differences were found in SPAI subscales of social interaction, group interaction, and avoidance, as well as in agoraphobia. Both groups had higher physiological arousal in narrating condition in comparison to the video viewing condition, yet there was no between-group difference in the reactivity. No associations were found between SPAI measures, physiological reactivity, disfluency frequencies, and stuttering severity in the autistic group. In the control group, a negative association was found between physiological reactivity and total and typical disfluency frequencies.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>SA or speaking-related physiological reactivity were not associated with disfluency frequencies or stuttering severity in autistic persons. Negative association between physiological reactivity and disfluency frequencies found in the control group may indicate that the physiological arousal may impact the speech production process by reducing the overt disfluencies.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":49175,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Communication Disorders","volume":"109 ","pages":"Article 106425"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140536129","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Narrative skills and language comprehension in preschool children with cochlear implants: A comparison with children with Developmental Language Disorder or typical development 人工耳蜗学龄前儿童的叙事能力和语言理解能力:与发育性语言障碍或典型发育儿童的比较
IF 1.7 3区 医学 Q2 AUDIOLOGY & SPEECH-LANGUAGE PATHOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-26 DOI: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106424
Marinella Majorano , Valentina Persici , Michela Santangelo , Rachele Ferrari , Beatrice Bertelli , Elena Florit , Manuela Lavelli , Tamara Bastianello , Letizia Guerzoni , Domenico Cuda

Introduction

The narrative skills of children with cochlear implants (CIs) are fragile, but the factors at play and whether these difficulties could be similar to those detected in language impairment are not clear. The present study aims to assess, at the microstructural level, narrative skills, comparing children with CIs with children with Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) or Typical Development (TD). Furthermore, the relationship between verbal (lexical and morphosyntactic) comprehension and narrative skills across groups is investigated.

Methods

The narratives of 19 children with CIs (Mage = 62.42 months, SD = 6.83), 13 children with DLD (Mage = 65.38 months, SD = 4.27), and 18 preschool children with TD (Mage = 63.67 months, SD = 4.31) were assessed in a standardized task. Articles, prepositions, pronouns, gender and number agreement, accuracy in the use of verbs, and number of arguments in each sentence were analysed. Lexical and morphosyntactic comprehension were also assessed. Performance was compared across groups using ANOVAs or Kruskal–Wallis tests. The role of lexical and morphosyntactic comprehension in predicting each morphological and syntactic element in the narrative task was examined using linear regressions.

Results

Data analysis showed that both children with CIs and DLD had fragilities in narration, both in the morphological and syntactic components. Although some differences between children with CIs and those with DLD emerged in descriptive analyses, these were not statistically significant. Regressions showed that morphosyntactic comprehension predicted the number of pronouns produced only in the TD group.

Conclusions

The scarce differences between CI and DLD groups and the absence of an effect of morphosyntactic comprehension on pronoun production may be due to their low production of these elements in the narrative task and/or to a difficulty in managing pronouns in an expressive task regardless of their ability to comprehend them. Potential implications of these results are discussed.

导言:植入人工耳蜗(CI)的儿童的叙事能力很脆弱,但其影响因素以及这些困难是否与语言障碍中发现的困难相似尚不清楚。本研究旨在从微观结构层面评估叙事能力,并将人工耳蜗儿童与发育性语言障碍(DLD)或典型发育(TD)儿童进行比较。方法通过标准化任务对 19 名 CI 儿童(年龄 = 62.42 个月,SD = 6.83)、13 名 DLD 儿童(年龄 = 65.38 个月,SD = 4.27)和 18 名 TD 学龄前儿童(年龄 = 63.67 个月,SD = 4.31)的叙事能力进行评估。对每个句子中的冠词、介词、代词、性别和数量一致、动词使用的准确性以及论据的数量进行了分析。此外,还对词汇和语法理解能力进行了评估。采用方差分析或 Kruskal-Wallis 检验对各组的成绩进行比较。结果数据分析显示,CIs 儿童和 DLD 儿童在叙述方面都有缺陷,包括形态和句法部分。尽管在描述性分析中,CIs 儿童和 DLD 儿童之间出现了一些差异,但这些差异在统计学上并不显著。结论 CI 组和 DLD 组之间的差异很小,而且形态句法理解能力对代词的产生没有影响,这可能是由于他们在叙事任务中很少产生这些元素和/或在表达任务中很难管理代词,无论他们的理解能力如何。本文讨论了这些结果的潜在影响。
{"title":"Narrative skills and language comprehension in preschool children with cochlear implants: A comparison with children with Developmental Language Disorder or typical development","authors":"Marinella Majorano ,&nbsp;Valentina Persici ,&nbsp;Michela Santangelo ,&nbsp;Rachele Ferrari ,&nbsp;Beatrice Bertelli ,&nbsp;Elena Florit ,&nbsp;Manuela Lavelli ,&nbsp;Tamara Bastianello ,&nbsp;Letizia Guerzoni ,&nbsp;Domenico Cuda","doi":"10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106424","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2024.106424","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Introduction</h3><p>The narrative skills of children with cochlear implants (CIs) are fragile, but the factors at play and whether these difficulties could be similar to those detected in language impairment are not clear. The present study aims to assess, at the microstructural level, narrative skills, comparing children with CIs with children with Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) or Typical Development (TD). Furthermore, the relationship between verbal (lexical and morphosyntactic) comprehension and narrative skills across groups is investigated.</p></div><div><h3>Methods</h3><p>The narratives of 19 children with CIs (<em>M<sub>age</sub> =</em> 62.42 months, <em>SD =</em> 6.83), 13 children with DLD (<em>M<sub>age</sub> =</em> 65.38 months, <em>SD =</em> 4.27), and 18 preschool children with TD (<em>M<sub>age</sub> =</em> 63.67 months, <em>SD =</em> 4.31) were assessed in a standardized task. Articles, prepositions, pronouns, gender and number agreement, accuracy in the use of verbs, and number of arguments in each sentence were analysed. Lexical and morphosyntactic comprehension were also assessed. Performance was compared across groups using ANOVAs or Kruskal–Wallis tests. The role of lexical and morphosyntactic comprehension in predicting each morphological and syntactic element in the narrative task was examined using linear regressions.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Data analysis showed that both children with CIs and DLD had fragilities in narration, both in the morphological and syntactic components. Although some differences between children with CIs and those with DLD emerged in descriptive analyses, these were not statistically significant. Regressions showed that morphosyntactic comprehension predicted the number of pronouns produced only in the TD group.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>The scarce differences between CI and DLD groups and the absence of an effect of morphosyntactic comprehension on pronoun production may be due to their low production of these elements in the narrative task and/or to a difficulty in managing pronouns in an expressive task regardless of their ability to comprehend them. Potential implications of these results are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":49175,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Communication Disorders","volume":"109 ","pages":"Article 106424"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0021992424000200/pdfft?md5=64bb0dba8f1b1f7e3fea62c1b1a99d9a&pid=1-s2.0-S0021992424000200-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140348107","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Communication Disorders
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1