首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Psychophysiology最新文献

英文 中文
Physiological Responses to Narrative Anger Recall and Correlates to Anger, Forgiveness, and Rumination 叙事愤怒回忆的生理反应及其与愤怒、宽恕和沉思的相关性
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-02-24 DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000297
Emily Dalrymple Whinery, Aya Musleh, E. Brown, Zachary Alford, Joe C. Anigbogu, Lauren Ellingwood, Marc-Anthony Espinoza, Greg Hawkins, Kaisa Kammer, Kevin J. Krause, Lisa E. Olson
Abstract. Narrative recall, or describing an event from one’s past, is a common method to study anger in the laboratory. However, most research using this method has not included a neutral speaking control, and therefore the physiological response attributable to emotion versus the act of speech is unknown. We evaluated heart rate, blood pressure, skin conductance level, heart rate variability, and salivary alpha-amylase during silent baseline, neutral speaking, anger recall, and recovery periods, and correlated these measures with trait anger, forgiveness, and rumination ( n = 104). Only systolic blood pressure and skin conductance levels were elevated in the anger recall period above the values in the neutral speaking period, showing the need for this important control. Alpha-amylase was inversely correlated to forgiveness, particularly in females. A neutral speaking control is critical for anger recall protocols because the physiological responses are mostly due to speaking, not anger. Salivary alpha-amylase may be a promising autonomic marker in studies of forgiveness and anger.
摘要叙述性回忆,或描述一个人过去的事件,是在实验室研究愤怒的一种常见方法。然而,大多数使用这种方法的研究都没有包括中性的言语控制,因此情绪与言语行为的生理反应尚不清楚。我们评估了无声基线、中性说话、愤怒回忆和恢复期的心率、血压、皮肤电导水平、心率变异性和唾液α-淀粉酶,并将这些指标与愤怒、宽恕和沉思特征相关联(n=104)。只有收缩压和皮肤电导水平在愤怒回忆期高于中性语言期,这表明需要进行这一重要控制。α-淀粉酶与宽恕呈负相关,尤其是在女性中。中性的说话控制对愤怒回忆协议至关重要,因为生理反应主要是由于说话,而不是愤怒。唾液α-淀粉酶在宽恕和愤怒的研究中可能是一种很有前途的自主标记物。
{"title":"Physiological Responses to Narrative Anger Recall and Correlates to Anger, Forgiveness, and Rumination","authors":"Emily Dalrymple Whinery, Aya Musleh, E. Brown, Zachary Alford, Joe C. Anigbogu, Lauren Ellingwood, Marc-Anthony Espinoza, Greg Hawkins, Kaisa Kammer, Kevin J. Krause, Lisa E. Olson","doi":"10.1027/0269-8803/a000297","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000297","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. Narrative recall, or describing an event from one’s past, is a common method to study anger in the laboratory. However, most research using this method has not included a neutral speaking control, and therefore the physiological response attributable to emotion versus the act of speech is unknown. We evaluated heart rate, blood pressure, skin conductance level, heart rate variability, and salivary alpha-amylase during silent baseline, neutral speaking, anger recall, and recovery periods, and correlated these measures with trait anger, forgiveness, and rumination ( n = 104). Only systolic blood pressure and skin conductance levels were elevated in the anger recall period above the values in the neutral speaking period, showing the need for this important control. Alpha-amylase was inversely correlated to forgiveness, particularly in females. A neutral speaking control is critical for anger recall protocols because the physiological responses are mostly due to speaking, not anger. Salivary alpha-amylase may be a promising autonomic marker in studies of forgiveness and anger.","PeriodicalId":50075,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Psychophysiology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-02-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48847752","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
N400 Peculiarities During Intentional Inferences Production in Subjects With Schizotypal Traits 具有分裂型特征的受试者在故意推理过程中的N400特征
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-02-24 DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000292
Anne-Lise Bohec, C. de Loye, M. Baltazar, A. Blanchet, R. Rey, M. Kostova
Abstract. A deficit in context processing may underlie theory of mind (ToM) difficulties in schizophrenia, although few studies to date have explored the impact of contextual processing on ToM performances within the same task. We used electroencephalography to investigate the production of intentional inferences from highly versus weakly structured sentences in healthy participants with schizotypal traits. Thirty-four participants were divided into two groups according to their scores on the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ). The participants listened to stories that required a degree of understanding in order to produce an inference about the main character’s intention. Each story was followed by a target word that could either be highly predictable (HP), weakly predictable (WP), or incompatible with the context. The results indicate that the N400 component for WP targets was stronger in the high-SPQ group. This increase correlated with the negative dimension of schizotypy. This may reflect difficulties with generating intentional inferences when the context is insufficiently structured for high schizotypy participants.
摘要语境处理的缺陷可能是精神分裂症心理理论困难的基础,尽管迄今为止很少有研究探讨语境处理对同一任务中心理理论表现的影响。我们使用脑电图来研究具有分裂型特征的健康参与者从高结构句子和弱结构句子中产生的有意推断。34名参与者根据他们在分裂型人格问卷(SPQ)上的得分被分为两组。参与者听的故事需要一定程度的理解,以便对主角的意图进行推断。每个故事后面都有一个目标词,该词可能是高度可预测的(HP),也可能是弱预测的(WP),或者与上下文不兼容。结果表明,在高SPQ组中,WP靶标的N400成分更强。这种增加与分裂型的负面维度相关。这可能反映出,当情境对高分裂型参与者来说不够结构化时,产生有意推断的困难。
{"title":"N400 Peculiarities During Intentional Inferences Production in Subjects With Schizotypal Traits","authors":"Anne-Lise Bohec, C. de Loye, M. Baltazar, A. Blanchet, R. Rey, M. Kostova","doi":"10.1027/0269-8803/a000292","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000292","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. A deficit in context processing may underlie theory of mind (ToM) difficulties in schizophrenia, although few studies to date have explored the impact of contextual processing on ToM performances within the same task. We used electroencephalography to investigate the production of intentional inferences from highly versus weakly structured sentences in healthy participants with schizotypal traits. Thirty-four participants were divided into two groups according to their scores on the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ). The participants listened to stories that required a degree of understanding in order to produce an inference about the main character’s intention. Each story was followed by a target word that could either be highly predictable (HP), weakly predictable (WP), or incompatible with the context. The results indicate that the N400 component for WP targets was stronger in the high-SPQ group. This increase correlated with the negative dimension of schizotypy. This may reflect difficulties with generating intentional inferences when the context is insufficiently structured for high schizotypy participants.","PeriodicalId":50075,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Psychophysiology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-02-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44827207","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Increasing Love Feelings, Marital Satisfaction, and Motivated Attention to the Spouse 增加爱的感觉,婚姻满意度和对配偶的动机关注
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-02-03 DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000294
Sandra J. E. Langeslag, Kruti Surti
Abstract. Love typically decreases over time, sometimes leading to divorces. We tested whether positively reappraising the spouse and/or up-regulating positive emotions unrelated to the spouse increases infatuation with and attachment to the spouse, marital satisfaction, and motivated attention to the spouse as measured by the late positive potential (LPP). Married individuals completed a regulation task in which they viewed spouse, pleasant, and neutral pictures without regulation prompt as well as spouse and pleasant pictures that were preceded by regulation prompts. Event-related potentials were recorded, and self-reported infatuation, attachment, and marital satisfaction were assessed. Viewing spouse pictures increased infatuation, attachment, and marital satisfaction compared to viewing pleasant or neutral pictures in the no regulation condition. Thinking about positive aspects of the spouse and increasing positive emotions unrelated to the spouse did not increase infatuation, attachment, and marital satisfaction any further. Motivated attention, measured by the LPP amplitude, was greatest to spouse pictures, intermediate to pleasant pictures, and minimal to neutral pictures. Although the typical up-regulation effect on the LPP amplitude was observed for pleasant pictures, positively reappraising the spouse did not increase the LPP amplitude and hence motivated attention to the spouse any further. This study indicates that looking at spouse pictures increases love and marital satisfaction, which is not due to increased positive emotions unrelated to the spouse. Looking at spouse pictures is an easy strategy that could be used to stabilize marriages in which the main problem is the decline of love feelings over time.
摘要爱情通常会随着时间的推移而减少,有时会导致离婚。我们测试了积极地重新评价配偶和/或上调与配偶无关的积极情绪是否会增加对配偶的迷恋和依恋、婚姻满意度和对配偶的动机关注,这是由晚期积极电位(LPP)衡量的。已婚个体完成了一个调节任务,在这个任务中,他们观看了没有调节提示的配偶、愉快的和中性的照片,以及有调节提示的配偶和愉快的照片。记录事件相关电位,评估自我报告的迷恋、依恋和婚姻满意度。与在无调节条件下观看愉快或中性的照片相比,观看配偶照片增加了迷恋、依恋和婚姻满意度。考虑配偶的积极方面,增加与配偶无关的积极情绪,并没有进一步增加迷恋、依恋和婚姻满意度。通过LPP振幅测量,动机注意对配偶图片的影响最大,对愉快图片的影响居中,对中性图片的影响最小。虽然在愉快的图片中观察到典型的LPP振幅上调效应,但积极地重新评价配偶并没有增加LPP振幅,因此没有进一步激发对配偶的注意。这项研究表明,看配偶照片会增加爱情和婚姻满意度,这不是由于与配偶无关的积极情绪的增加。看配偶的照片是一种简单的策略,可以用来稳定主要问题是爱情随着时间的推移而下降的婚姻。
{"title":"Increasing Love Feelings, Marital Satisfaction, and Motivated Attention to the Spouse","authors":"Sandra J. E. Langeslag, Kruti Surti","doi":"10.1027/0269-8803/a000294","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000294","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. Love typically decreases over time, sometimes leading to divorces. We tested whether positively reappraising the spouse and/or up-regulating positive emotions unrelated to the spouse increases infatuation with and attachment to the spouse, marital satisfaction, and motivated attention to the spouse as measured by the late positive potential (LPP). Married individuals completed a regulation task in which they viewed spouse, pleasant, and neutral pictures without regulation prompt as well as spouse and pleasant pictures that were preceded by regulation prompts. Event-related potentials were recorded, and self-reported infatuation, attachment, and marital satisfaction were assessed. Viewing spouse pictures increased infatuation, attachment, and marital satisfaction compared to viewing pleasant or neutral pictures in the no regulation condition. Thinking about positive aspects of the spouse and increasing positive emotions unrelated to the spouse did not increase infatuation, attachment, and marital satisfaction any further. Motivated attention, measured by the LPP amplitude, was greatest to spouse pictures, intermediate to pleasant pictures, and minimal to neutral pictures. Although the typical up-regulation effect on the LPP amplitude was observed for pleasant pictures, positively reappraising the spouse did not increase the LPP amplitude and hence motivated attention to the spouse any further. This study indicates that looking at spouse pictures increases love and marital satisfaction, which is not due to increased positive emotions unrelated to the spouse. Looking at spouse pictures is an easy strategy that could be used to stabilize marriages in which the main problem is the decline of love feelings over time.","PeriodicalId":50075,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Psychophysiology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-02-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43787490","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Under Which Circumstances Does Academic Workload Lead to Stress? 在什么情况下工作量会导致压力?
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-02-03 DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000293
Susanne Koudela-Hamila, P. Santangelo, U. Ebner-Priemer, W. Schlotz
Abstract. Academic stress is associated with a wide range of adverse outcomes, including detrimental effects on mental health, achievement, and well-being. Numerous studies have shown an association between the cortisol awakening response (CAR) and various health and risk factors. Some studies revealed a protective function of the CAR as a stress buffer preventing the stress system from overshooting. We investigated the moderating effect of the CAR on the within-subject association between academic workload and academic stress in participants’ daily lives using ambulatory assessment. We assessed 77 undergraduate university participants for 2 days at the beginning of the semester and approximately 3 months later, individually starting one week before an exam. Participants provided academic stress and academic workload ratings hourly during their waking time using smartphone-based e-diaries and salivary cortisol samples at awakening as well as 30, 45, and 60 min later on two consecutive days during each of the assessment waves. Average within-subject associations between academic workload and academic stress were analyzed using multilevel models. Interactions with indicators of the cortisol awakening response (CAR) were included to test moderating effects of the CAR on the workload-stress associations. There was a significant positive within-subject association between academic workload and academic stress. Significant cross-level interactions showed a moderating effect of the CAR on this association. The results point out the importance of the CAR in the regulation of the workload-stress associations in academic life and underscore the relevance of investigating the influence of specific stressor-dependent reciprocal effects of the CAR on learning and experienced stress.
摘要学业压力与一系列不良后果有关,包括对心理健康、成就和幸福感的不利影响。大量研究表明,皮质醇觉醒反应(CAR)与各种健康和风险因素之间存在关联。一些研究揭示了CAR作为应力缓冲器的保护功能,防止应力系统超调。我们使用动态评估研究了CAR对参与者日常生活中学业工作量和学业压力之间的学科内关联的调节作用。我们在学期初和大约3个月后对77名本科生进行了为期2天的评估,分别从考试前一周开始。参与者在清醒时使用基于智能手机的电子日记和清醒时的唾液皮质醇样本,以及30、45、,并且60分钟后在每个评估波期间的连续两天。使用多水平模型分析了学业负荷和学业压力之间的平均学科内关联。包括与皮质醇觉醒反应(CAR)指标的相互作用,以测试CAR对工作量-压力相关性的调节作用。学业负荷与学业压力之间存在显著的正相关。显著的跨水平交互作用显示CAR对这种关联的调节作用。研究结果指出了CAR在调节学术生活中的工作量-压力关联中的重要性,并强调了研究CAR对学习和经历压力的特定压力源依赖性互惠效应的影响的相关性。
{"title":"Under Which Circumstances Does Academic Workload Lead to Stress?","authors":"Susanne Koudela-Hamila, P. Santangelo, U. Ebner-Priemer, W. Schlotz","doi":"10.1027/0269-8803/a000293","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000293","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. Academic stress is associated with a wide range of adverse outcomes, including detrimental effects on mental health, achievement, and well-being. Numerous studies have shown an association between the cortisol awakening response (CAR) and various health and risk factors. Some studies revealed a protective function of the CAR as a stress buffer preventing the stress system from overshooting. We investigated the moderating effect of the CAR on the within-subject association between academic workload and academic stress in participants’ daily lives using ambulatory assessment. We assessed 77 undergraduate university participants for 2 days at the beginning of the semester and approximately 3 months later, individually starting one week before an exam. Participants provided academic stress and academic workload ratings hourly during their waking time using smartphone-based e-diaries and salivary cortisol samples at awakening as well as 30, 45, and 60 min later on two consecutive days during each of the assessment waves. Average within-subject associations between academic workload and academic stress were analyzed using multilevel models. Interactions with indicators of the cortisol awakening response (CAR) were included to test moderating effects of the CAR on the workload-stress associations. There was a significant positive within-subject association between academic workload and academic stress. Significant cross-level interactions showed a moderating effect of the CAR on this association. The results point out the importance of the CAR in the regulation of the workload-stress associations in academic life and underscore the relevance of investigating the influence of specific stressor-dependent reciprocal effects of the CAR on learning and experienced stress.","PeriodicalId":50075,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Psychophysiology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-02-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44405046","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Enhancing the Journal of Psychophysiology 加强《心理生理学杂志》
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000296
M. Falkenstein, J. Yordanova
{"title":"Enhancing the Journal of Psychophysiology","authors":"M. Falkenstein, J. Yordanova","doi":"10.1027/0269-8803/a000296","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000296","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":50075,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Psychophysiology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44677542","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Influence of Neuroticism on the Muscle Response in the Trapezius and Frontalis Muscles to Anticipatory Stress 神经质对斜方肌和额肌对预期应激反应的影响
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2021-10-14 DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000289
Florestan Wagenblast, R. Seibt, T. Läubli, M. Rieger, B. Steinhilber
Abstract. Objective quantification of mental stress in the workplace would be beneficial for designing work tasks to avoid the negative consequences of mental stress. Methods such as surface electromyography have proven to be sensitive to mental demands. However, there is little knowledge about the muscle response and moderating factors during anticipatory stress paradigms. This study examined whether the personality dimension neuroticism moderates the muscle response to the expectation of an unpredictable electrical shock. Forty-seven subjects underwent three expectation phases, in which they could expect a pleasant audio signal (NoShock) or an electric shock in two conditions (anticipation of the first: Shock1, and second electric shock: Shock2) at an unpredictable moment. The frontalis muscle activity and the upper and upper/middle parts of the trapezius muscle were recorded using surface electromyography. Neuroticism was surveyed using the Big Five Inventory to assign the subjects to a group with lower or higher neuroticism. Shock1 only induced higher trapezius muscle activity in the higher neuroticism group, which vanished during Shock2, while the frontalis muscle showed no significant effects. The results suggest that neuroticism should be considered a moderating factor in assessing anticipatory stress using surface electromyography at the trapezius muscle.
摘要对工作场所的心理压力进行客观量化,有利于设计工作任务,避免心理压力带来的负面影响。表面肌电图等方法已被证明对精神需求很敏感。然而,对预期应激模式下的肌肉反应及其调节因素的了解甚少。本研究考察了人格维度神经质是否会调节肌肉对不可预测的电击预期的反应。47名受试者经历了三个预期阶段,在这三个阶段中,他们可以在两种情况下(预期第一种:电击1,第二次电击2)在不可预测的时刻期待愉快的音频信号(无电击)或电击。采用表面肌电图记录额肌活动和斜方肌上部及上/中部。研究人员使用大五人格量表对神经质进行调查,并将受试者分为神经质程度较低或较高的两组。在高神经质组中,Shock1只引起较高的斜方肌活动,而在Shock2中消失,而额肌没有明显的影响。结果表明,神经质应该被认为是使用斜方肌表面肌电图评估预期应激的调节因素。
{"title":"The Influence of Neuroticism on the Muscle Response in the Trapezius and Frontalis Muscles to Anticipatory Stress","authors":"Florestan Wagenblast, R. Seibt, T. Läubli, M. Rieger, B. Steinhilber","doi":"10.1027/0269-8803/a000289","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000289","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. Objective quantification of mental stress in the workplace would be beneficial for designing work tasks to avoid the negative consequences of mental stress. Methods such as surface electromyography have proven to be sensitive to mental demands. However, there is little knowledge about the muscle response and moderating factors during anticipatory stress paradigms. This study examined whether the personality dimension neuroticism moderates the muscle response to the expectation of an unpredictable electrical shock. Forty-seven subjects underwent three expectation phases, in which they could expect a pleasant audio signal (NoShock) or an electric shock in two conditions (anticipation of the first: Shock1, and second electric shock: Shock2) at an unpredictable moment. The frontalis muscle activity and the upper and upper/middle parts of the trapezius muscle were recorded using surface electromyography. Neuroticism was surveyed using the Big Five Inventory to assign the subjects to a group with lower or higher neuroticism. Shock1 only induced higher trapezius muscle activity in the higher neuroticism group, which vanished during Shock2, while the frontalis muscle showed no significant effects. The results suggest that neuroticism should be considered a moderating factor in assessing anticipatory stress using surface electromyography at the trapezius muscle.","PeriodicalId":50075,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Psychophysiology","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-10-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41985990","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
In Identifying the Source of the Incongruent Effect 识别不一致效应的来源
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2021-10-14 DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000290
Tingji Chen, Yanting Sun, Chengzhi Feng, Wenfeng Feng
Abstract. Emotional signals from the face and body are normally perceived as an integrated whole in everyday life. Previous studies have revealed an incongruent effect which refers to distinctive behavioral and neural responses to emotionally congruent versus incongruent face-body compounds. However, it remains unknown which kind of the face-body compounds caused the incongruence effect. In the present study, we added neutral face and neutral body stimuli to form new face-body compounds. Forty subjects with normal or corrected-to-normal vision participated in this experiment. By comparing the face-body compounds with emotional conflict and face-body compounds with neutral stimuli, we could investigate the source of the incongruent effect. For both behavioral and event-related potential (ERP) data, a 2 (bodily expression: happiness, fear) × 2 (congruence: congruent, incongruent) repeated-measure analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed to re-investigate the incongruent effect and a 3 (facial expression: fearful, happy, neutral) × 3 (bodily expression: fearful, happy, neutral) repeated-measure ANOVA was performed to clarify the source of the incongruent effect. As expected, both behavioral and ERP results have successfully repeated the incongruent effect. Specifically, the behavioral data showed that emotionally congruent versus incongruent face-body compounds were recognized more accurately ( p < .05). The ERP component of N2 was modulated by the emotional congruency between the facial and bodily expression showing that the emotionally incongruent compounds elicited greater N2 amplitudes than emotionally congruent compounds ( p < .05). No incongruent effect was found for P1 or P3 component ( p = .079, p = .99, respectively). Furthermore, by comparing the emotionally incongruent pairs with the neutral baseline, the present study suggests that the source of the incongruent effect might be from the happy face-fearful body compounds. We speculate that the emotion expressed by the fearful body was much more intensive than the emotion expressed by the happy body and thus caused a stronger interference in judging the facial expressions.
摘要在日常生活中,面部和身体发出的情绪信号通常被视为一个整体。先前的研究揭示了一种不一致效应,它指的是对情绪一致和不一致的脸-身体复合物的不同行为和神经反应。然而,究竟是哪一种“脸-体”化合物引起了不一致效应尚不清楚。在本研究中,我们加入中性面孔和中性身体刺激,形成新的脸-体复合物。40名视力正常或矫正至正常的受试者参加了本实验。通过比较具有情绪冲突的脸身复合物和具有中性刺激的脸身复合物,我们可以探讨不一致效应的来源。对于行为和事件相关电位(ERP)数据,采用2(身体表情:快乐、恐惧)× 2(一致、不一致)重复测量方差分析(ANOVA)来重新调查不一致效应,并采用3(面部表情:恐惧、快乐、中性)× 3(身体表情:恐惧、快乐、中性)重复测量方差分析(ANOVA)来澄清不一致效应的来源。正如预期的那样,行为和ERP结果都成功地重复了不一致效应。具体而言,行为数据显示情绪上一致的面部-身体复合物比不一致的面部-身体复合物更准确地被识别(p < 0.05)。N2的ERP分量受面部和身体表情的情绪一致性的调节,结果表明情绪不一致的化合物比情绪一致的化合物激发出更大的N2振幅(p < 0.05)。P1和P3成分未发现不一致效应(p = 0.079, p = 0.99)。此外,通过将情绪不一致的配对与中性基线进行比较,本研究表明,不一致效应的来源可能来自快乐-恐惧的身体化合物。我们推测,恐惧身体所表达的情绪比快乐身体所表达的情绪要强烈得多,因此对面部表情的判断产生了更强的干扰。
{"title":"In Identifying the Source of the Incongruent Effect","authors":"Tingji Chen, Yanting Sun, Chengzhi Feng, Wenfeng Feng","doi":"10.1027/0269-8803/a000290","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000290","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. Emotional signals from the face and body are normally perceived as an integrated whole in everyday life. Previous studies have revealed an incongruent effect which refers to distinctive behavioral and neural responses to emotionally congruent versus incongruent face-body compounds. However, it remains unknown which kind of the face-body compounds caused the incongruence effect. In the present study, we added neutral face and neutral body stimuli to form new face-body compounds. Forty subjects with normal or corrected-to-normal vision participated in this experiment. By comparing the face-body compounds with emotional conflict and face-body compounds with neutral stimuli, we could investigate the source of the incongruent effect. For both behavioral and event-related potential (ERP) data, a 2 (bodily expression: happiness, fear) × 2 (congruence: congruent, incongruent) repeated-measure analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed to re-investigate the incongruent effect and a 3 (facial expression: fearful, happy, neutral) × 3 (bodily expression: fearful, happy, neutral) repeated-measure ANOVA was performed to clarify the source of the incongruent effect. As expected, both behavioral and ERP results have successfully repeated the incongruent effect. Specifically, the behavioral data showed that emotionally congruent versus incongruent face-body compounds were recognized more accurately ( p < .05). The ERP component of N2 was modulated by the emotional congruency between the facial and bodily expression showing that the emotionally incongruent compounds elicited greater N2 amplitudes than emotionally congruent compounds ( p < .05). No incongruent effect was found for P1 or P3 component ( p = .079, p = .99, respectively). Furthermore, by comparing the emotionally incongruent pairs with the neutral baseline, the present study suggests that the source of the incongruent effect might be from the happy face-fearful body compounds. We speculate that the emotion expressed by the fearful body was much more intensive than the emotion expressed by the happy body and thus caused a stronger interference in judging the facial expressions.","PeriodicalId":50075,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Psychophysiology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-10-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46419371","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Preliminary Investigation of ERP Components of Attentional Bias in Anxious Adults using Temporospatial Principal Component Analysis. 利用时空主成分分析对焦虑成人注意偏倚的ERP成分进行初步研究。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2021-10-01 Epub Date: 2021-02-23 DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000275
Resh S Gupta, Autumn Kujawa, David R Vago

Threat-related attention bias is thought to contribute to the development and maintenance of anxiety disorders. Dot-probe studies using event-related potentials (ERPs) have indicated that several early ERP components are modulated by threatening and emotional stimuli in anxious populations, suggesting enhanced allocation of attention to threat and emotion at earlier stages of processing. However, ERP components selected for examination and analysis in these studies vary widely and remain inconsistent. The present study used temporospatial principal component analysis (PCA) to systematically identify ERP components elicited to face pair cues and probes in a dot-probe task in anxious adults. Cue-locked components sensitive to emotion included an early occipital C1 component enhanced for happy versus angry face pair cues and an early parieto-occipital P1 component enhanced for happy versus angry face pair cues. Probe-locked components sensitive to congruency included a parieto-occipital P2 component enhanced for incongruent probes (probes replacing neutral faces) versus congruent probes (probes replacing emotional faces). Split-half correlations indicated that the mean value around the PCA-derived peaks were reliably measured in the ERP waveforms. These results highlight promising neurophysiological markers for attentional bias research that can be extended to designs comparing anxious and healthy comparison groups. Results from a secondary exploratory PCA analysis investigating the effects of emotional face position and analyses on behavioral reaction time data are also presented.

威胁相关的注意偏差被认为有助于焦虑症的发展和维持。使用事件相关电位(ERP)的点探测研究表明,在焦虑人群中,一些早期ERP成分受到威胁和情绪刺激的调节,这表明在处理的早期阶段,对威胁和情绪的注意分配得到了加强。然而,在这些研究中选择的ERP组件进行检查和分析差异很大,并且仍然不一致。本研究采用时空主成分分析(PCA)系统地识别了焦虑成人在点探针任务中面对对线索和探针所引发的ERP成分。对情绪敏感的线索锁定成分包括对快乐和愤怒的面孔线索增强的早期枕部C1成分,以及对快乐和愤怒的面孔线索增强的早期顶叶-枕部P1成分。对一致性敏感的探针锁定成分包括顶枕P2成分,对于不一致探针(探针取代中性面孔)和一致探针(探针取代情绪化面孔)增强。二分相关表明,在ERP波形中可靠地测量了pca衍生峰周围的平均值。这些结果突出了注意偏倚研究中有希望的神经生理标记,可以扩展到比较焦虑组和健康组的设计中。本文还介绍了一项二次探索性PCA分析的结果,该分析调查了情绪面部位置的影响,并分析了行为反应时间数据。
{"title":"A Preliminary Investigation of ERP Components of Attentional Bias in Anxious Adults using Temporospatial Principal Component Analysis.","authors":"Resh S Gupta,&nbsp;Autumn Kujawa,&nbsp;David R Vago","doi":"10.1027/0269-8803/a000275","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000275","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Threat-related attention bias is thought to contribute to the development and maintenance of anxiety disorders. Dot-probe studies using event-related potentials (ERPs) have indicated that several early ERP components are modulated by threatening and emotional stimuli in anxious populations, suggesting enhanced allocation of attention to threat and emotion at earlier stages of processing. However, ERP components selected for examination and analysis in these studies vary widely and remain inconsistent. The present study used temporospatial principal component analysis (PCA) to systematically identify ERP components elicited to face pair cues and probes in a dot-probe task in anxious adults. Cue-locked components sensitive to emotion included an early occipital C1 component enhanced for happy versus angry face pair cues and an early parieto-occipital P1 component enhanced for happy versus angry face pair cues. Probe-locked components sensitive to congruency included a parieto-occipital P2 component enhanced for incongruent probes (probes replacing neutral faces) versus congruent probes (probes replacing emotional faces). Split-half correlations indicated that the mean value around the PCA-derived peaks were reliably measured in the ERP waveforms. These results highlight promising neurophysiological markers for attentional bias research that can be extended to designs comparing anxious and healthy comparison groups. Results from a secondary exploratory PCA analysis investigating the effects of emotional face position and analyses on behavioral reaction time data are also presented.</p>","PeriodicalId":50075,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Psychophysiology","volume":"35 4","pages":"223-236"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8559726/pdf/nihms-1680662.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39588285","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Cardiovascular and Self-Reported Recovery in Two-Shift Systems 两班制的心血管和自我报告恢复
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2021-09-23 DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000284
L. Stieler, B. Hunger, R. Seibt
Abstract. Recovery is necessary to maintain workers’ health and efficiency. Shift work has been associated with delayed recovery processes. The objective of this study was to examine the cardiovascular and self-reported recovery measures of German shift and day workers in the hotel and catering industry. Furthermore, it aimed to clarify to what extent shift groups differentiate given additional factors that influence recovery (ability to relax, lifestyle). The sample group consisted of 64 alternative shift workers (two-shift system with a low proportion of night work) and 96-day workers employed in the hotel and catering industry. Blood pressure monitoring was conducted for 24 hr during a working day, including the phases of work, leisure, and sleep, to assess cardiovascular reduction during leisure and sleep. The blood pressure status was measured over the course of a 4-day self-assessment period. Self-reported measures, including quality of sleep (Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, PSQI), work-life balance (WPC), ability to relax (FABA), and lifestyle factors (physical activity, smoking, drinking), were assessed through questionnaires. Sixty-one participants (36%) were hypertensives. There were no significant effects of cardiovascular recovery regarding the reduction of blood pressure for the calculated differences between WORK – LEISURE, WORK – SLEEP, and LEISURE – SLEEP among shift and day workers. Shift workers reported a significantly less favorable work-life balance ( p = .017), a decreased ability to relax ( p = .001), and less regular physical activity ( p = .003). The workload within the two-shift system of the hotel and catering industry seems to have a lesser effect on cardiovascular means than on self-reported measures of recovery. The decisive factor is the ability to relax, which means psychological detachment from work. To enable a comprehensive recovery, an optimally designed recovery cycle is necessary for shift systems.
摘要恢复对于保持工人的健康和效率是必要的。轮班工作与延迟的恢复过程有关。本研究的目的是检查德国酒店和餐饮业轮班和日工的心血管和自我报告的恢复措施。此外,它旨在澄清在影响恢复的其他因素(放松能力、生活方式)的情况下,轮班组的差异在多大程度上。样本组包括64名替代轮班工人(两班制,夜间工作比例低)和96名受雇于酒店和餐饮行业的日薪工人。在工作日(包括工作、休闲和睡眠阶段)进行24小时血压监测,以评估休闲和睡眠期间心血管减少情况。在为期4天的自我评估期内测量血压状况。自我报告措施,包括睡眠质量(匹兹堡睡眠质量指数,PSQI)、工作与生活平衡(WPC)、放松能力(FABA)和生活方式因素(体育活动、吸烟、饮酒),通过问卷进行评估。61名参与者(36%)为高血压患者。在轮班工人和白班工人中,计算出的工作-休闲、工作-睡眠和休闲-睡眠之间的差异对降低血压的心血管恢复没有显著影响。倒班工人报告的工作与生活的平衡明显较差(p = 0.017),放松的能力下降(p = 0.001),定期体育活动减少(p = 0.003)。酒店和餐饮业两班制内的工作量对心血管疾病的影响似乎小于对自我报告的康复措施的影响。决定因素是放松的能力,这意味着心理上脱离工作。为了实现全面的回收,一个优化设计的回收周期对于移位系统是必要的。
{"title":"Cardiovascular and Self-Reported Recovery in Two-Shift Systems","authors":"L. Stieler, B. Hunger, R. Seibt","doi":"10.1027/0269-8803/a000284","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000284","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. Recovery is necessary to maintain workers’ health and efficiency. Shift work has been associated with delayed recovery processes. The objective of this study was to examine the cardiovascular and self-reported recovery measures of German shift and day workers in the hotel and catering industry. Furthermore, it aimed to clarify to what extent shift groups differentiate given additional factors that influence recovery (ability to relax, lifestyle). The sample group consisted of 64 alternative shift workers (two-shift system with a low proportion of night work) and 96-day workers employed in the hotel and catering industry. Blood pressure monitoring was conducted for 24 hr during a working day, including the phases of work, leisure, and sleep, to assess cardiovascular reduction during leisure and sleep. The blood pressure status was measured over the course of a 4-day self-assessment period. Self-reported measures, including quality of sleep (Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, PSQI), work-life balance (WPC), ability to relax (FABA), and lifestyle factors (physical activity, smoking, drinking), were assessed through questionnaires. Sixty-one participants (36%) were hypertensives. There were no significant effects of cardiovascular recovery regarding the reduction of blood pressure for the calculated differences between WORK – LEISURE, WORK – SLEEP, and LEISURE – SLEEP among shift and day workers. Shift workers reported a significantly less favorable work-life balance ( p = .017), a decreased ability to relax ( p = .001), and less regular physical activity ( p = .003). The workload within the two-shift system of the hotel and catering industry seems to have a lesser effect on cardiovascular means than on self-reported measures of recovery. The decisive factor is the ability to relax, which means psychological detachment from work. To enable a comprehensive recovery, an optimally designed recovery cycle is necessary for shift systems.","PeriodicalId":50075,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Psychophysiology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43297146","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The impact of pain on subsequent effort and cognitive performance 疼痛对后续努力和认知表现的影响
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2021-08-27 DOI: 10.31234/osf.io/gwaup
N. Silvestrini, Corrado Corradi-Dell’Acqua
Theoretical accounts of pain and empirical evidence indicate that pain and cognitive control share common neurocognitive processes. Numerous studies have examined the interactions between pain and cognitive performance when they occur simultaneously, typically showing analgesic effects of task performance and impaired performance due to pain. However, the sequential impact of pain on cognitive control and effort remains less clear. This study investigated the influence of a first task including painful vs. non-painful thermal stimuli on effort-related cardiac response and performance in a subsequent moderately difficult cognitive task. Drawing on the hypothesis that experiencing pain should recruit cognitive resources and reduce perceived ability, we predicted lower task performance and/or stronger compensatory effort in the subsequent cognitive task after the painful than after the non-painful first task. Results support our predictions regarding the effect of pain on subsequent cognitive performance, which was moderately lower after the painful task. However, such a decrease in task proficiency was not associated with a comparable decrease in perceived capacity or increase in effort-related cardiac reactivity. Nevertheless, further correlational analyses indicated that effort mobilization and perceived capacity were significantly related to pain ratings. Moderate pain was associated with stronger effort during the cognitive task whereas high pain led to disengagement, i.e., a low effort. Moreover, in line with our predictions, higher pain ratings were associated with lower self-reported capacity to perform the cognitive task. We discuss these findings regarding the relationship between effort and performance; the impact of fatigue on motivation; and interindividual variability in these after-effects.
疼痛的理论解释和经验证据表明,疼痛和认知控制有着共同的神经认知过程。许多研究考察了疼痛和认知表现同时发生时的相互作用,通常显示了任务表现和因疼痛而受损的表现的镇痛作用。然而,疼痛对认知控制和努力的顺序影响仍然不太清楚。本研究调查了第一项任务(包括疼痛和非疼痛的热刺激)对努力相关心脏反应和随后中等难度认知任务表现的影响。根据经历疼痛应该募集认知资源并降低感知能力的假设,我们预测,与非疼痛的第一项任务相比,在疼痛后的后续认知任务中,任务表现更低和/或补偿努力更强。结果支持了我们关于疼痛对后续认知表现影响的预测,在完成疼痛任务后,认知表现会适度降低。然而,这种任务熟练度的下降与感知能力的类似下降或与努力相关的心脏反应性的增加无关。然而,进一步的相关分析表明,努力动员和感知能力与疼痛评分显著相关。中度疼痛与认知任务中更强的努力有关,而高度疼痛导致脱离,即低努力。此外,与我们的预测一致,较高的疼痛评分与较低的自我报告执行认知任务的能力有关。我们讨论了这些关于努力和绩效之间关系的发现;疲劳对动机的影响;以及这些后遗症的个体间变异性。
{"title":"The impact of pain on subsequent effort and cognitive performance","authors":"N. Silvestrini, Corrado Corradi-Dell’Acqua","doi":"10.31234/osf.io/gwaup","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/gwaup","url":null,"abstract":"Theoretical accounts of pain and empirical evidence indicate that pain and cognitive control share common neurocognitive processes. Numerous studies have examined the interactions between pain and cognitive performance when they occur simultaneously, typically showing analgesic effects of task performance and impaired performance due to pain. However, the sequential impact of pain on cognitive control and effort remains less clear. This study investigated the influence of a first task including painful vs. non-painful thermal stimuli on effort-related cardiac response and performance in a subsequent moderately difficult cognitive task. Drawing on the hypothesis that experiencing pain should recruit cognitive resources and reduce perceived ability, we predicted lower task performance and/or stronger compensatory effort in the subsequent cognitive task after the painful than after the non-painful first task. Results support our predictions regarding the effect of pain on subsequent cognitive performance, which was moderately lower after the painful task. However, such a decrease in task proficiency was not associated with a comparable decrease in perceived capacity or increase in effort-related cardiac reactivity. Nevertheless, further correlational analyses indicated that effort mobilization and perceived capacity were significantly related to pain ratings. Moderate pain was associated with stronger effort during the cognitive task whereas high pain led to disengagement, i.e., a low effort. Moreover, in line with our predictions, higher pain ratings were associated with lower self-reported capacity to perform the cognitive task. We discuss these findings regarding the relationship between effort and performance; the impact of fatigue on motivation; and interindividual variability in these after-effects.","PeriodicalId":50075,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Psychophysiology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-08-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44065986","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
期刊
Journal of Psychophysiology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1