首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Psychophysiology最新文献

英文 中文
Probabilistic Noise and Human Electrophysiology 概率噪声与人体电生理
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-06-08 DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000319
W. Sannita
{"title":"Probabilistic Noise and Human Electrophysiology","authors":"W. Sannita","doi":"10.1027/0269-8803/a000319","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000319","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":50075,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-06-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46686440","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An Examination of the Orienting and Defensive Responses Using HR and HF-HRV 利用HR和HF-HRV研究定向和防御反应
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-06-08 DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000320
W. O’Brien, Tanya S. Watford, C. X. Lim, Tracy Sims, S. Jarukasemthawee, K. Pisitsungkagarn, P. Suvanbenjakule, Supalak Settawacharawanich
Abstract: Psychophysiological researchers have demonstrated that the orienting response is characterized by heart rate (HR) decreases and high-frequency heart rate variability (HF-HRV) increases. The defensive response is characterized by HR increases and HF-HRV decreases. In this study, HR and HF-HRV reactivity to a visual stimulus designed to elicit an orienting response (surgery video) and a cognitive task designed to elicit a defensive response (Trier Social Stress Test, math task) were measured among 81 undergraduate students. The order of stimuli presentation was counterbalanced. The expected pattern of reactivity was observed (decreased HR and increased HF-HRV during the video condition; increased HR and decreased HF-HRV during the cognitive math task). The order of stimulus presentation significantly influenced HF-HRV reactivity. When the video stimulus was presented first, a large and significant increase in HF-HRV was observed which indicated vagal activation. When the video stimulus followed the cognitive math task stimulus, lower levels of HF-HRV reactivity and vagal activation were observed. This suggests exposure to the cognitive math task elicited a defensive response that suppressed subsequent orienting responses. Theoretical, psychophysiological, and practical implications of the observed results and future psychophysiological research directions are discussed.
摘要:心理生理学研究人员已经证明,定向反应的特征是心率(HR)降低和高频心率变异性(HF-HRV)增加。防御反应的特点是HR增加,HF-HRV减少。在这项研究中,测量了81名本科生对旨在引发定向反应的视觉刺激(手术视频)和旨在引发防御反应的认知任务(特里尔社会压力测试,数学任务)的HR和HF-HRV反应。刺激呈现的顺序是平衡的。观察到预期的反应模式(在视频条件下,HR降低,HF-HRV增加;在认知数学任务中,HR增加,HF-HRV-减少)。刺激呈现的顺序显著影响HF-HRV的反应性。当首先呈现视频刺激时,观察到HF-HRV的大幅显著增加,这表明迷走神经激活。当视频刺激跟随认知数学任务刺激时,观察到HF-HRV反应性和迷走神经激活水平较低。这表明,接触认知数学任务会引发防御反应,从而抑制随后的定向反应。讨论了观测结果的理论、心理生理学和实践意义以及未来的心理生理学研究方向。
{"title":"An Examination of the Orienting and Defensive Responses Using HR and HF-HRV","authors":"W. O’Brien, Tanya S. Watford, C. X. Lim, Tracy Sims, S. Jarukasemthawee, K. Pisitsungkagarn, P. Suvanbenjakule, Supalak Settawacharawanich","doi":"10.1027/0269-8803/a000320","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000320","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract: Psychophysiological researchers have demonstrated that the orienting response is characterized by heart rate (HR) decreases and high-frequency heart rate variability (HF-HRV) increases. The defensive response is characterized by HR increases and HF-HRV decreases. In this study, HR and HF-HRV reactivity to a visual stimulus designed to elicit an orienting response (surgery video) and a cognitive task designed to elicit a defensive response (Trier Social Stress Test, math task) were measured among 81 undergraduate students. The order of stimuli presentation was counterbalanced. The expected pattern of reactivity was observed (decreased HR and increased HF-HRV during the video condition; increased HR and decreased HF-HRV during the cognitive math task). The order of stimulus presentation significantly influenced HF-HRV reactivity. When the video stimulus was presented first, a large and significant increase in HF-HRV was observed which indicated vagal activation. When the video stimulus followed the cognitive math task stimulus, lower levels of HF-HRV reactivity and vagal activation were observed. This suggests exposure to the cognitive math task elicited a defensive response that suppressed subsequent orienting responses. Theoretical, psychophysiological, and practical implications of the observed results and future psychophysiological research directions are discussed.","PeriodicalId":50075,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-06-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46073023","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Impact of Pain on Subsequent Effort and Cognitive Performance 疼痛对后续努力和认知表现的影响
4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000308
Nicolas Silvestrini, Corrado Corradi-Dell’Acqua
Abstract: Theoretical accounts of pain and empirical evidence indicate that pain and cognitive control share common neurocognitive processes. Numerous studies have examined the interactions between pain and cognitive performance when they occur simultaneously, typically showing analgesic effects on task performance and impaired performance due to pain. However, the sequential impact of pain on cognitive control and effort remains less clear. This study investigated the influence of a first task, including painful vs. non-painful thermal stimuli, on effort-related cardiac response and performance in a subsequent moderately difficult cognitive task. Drawing on the hypothesis that experiencing pain should recruit cognitive resources (such as attention and working memory) and reduce perceived ability, we predicted lower task performance and/or stronger compensatory effort in the subsequent cognitive task after the painful than after the non-painful first task. Results support our predictions regarding the effect of pain on subsequent cognitive performance, which was moderately lower after the painful task. However, such a decrease in task proficiency was not associated with a comparable decrease in perceived capacity or increase in effort-related cardiac reactivity. Nevertheless, further correlational analyses indicated that effort and perceived capacity were significantly related to pain ratings. Moderate pain was associated with stronger effort during the cognitive task, whereas high pain led to disengagement, that is, a low effort. Moreover, in line with our predictions, higher pain ratings were associated with a lower self-reported capacity to perform the cognitive task. We discuss these findings regarding the relationship between effort and performance, the impact of fatigue on motivation, and interindividual variability in these after-effects.
摘要:疼痛的理论解释和经验证据表明,疼痛和认知控制具有共同的神经认知过程。许多研究已经研究了疼痛和认知表现之间的相互作用,当它们同时发生时,通常显示疼痛对任务表现和因疼痛而受损的表现有镇痛作用。然而,疼痛对认知控制和努力的连续影响仍然不太清楚。本研究调查了第一项任务(包括疼痛和非疼痛热刺激)对随后中等难度认知任务中与努力相关的心脏反应和表现的影响。基于经历疼痛会调动认知资源(如注意力和工作记忆)并降低感知能力的假设,我们预测在疼痛之后的后续认知任务中,与非疼痛的第一个任务相比,任务表现更低和/或更强的代偿努力。结果支持了我们关于疼痛对随后的认知表现的影响的预测,在痛苦的任务之后,认知表现适度降低。然而,这种任务熟练程度的降低与感知能力的降低或与努力相关的心脏反应性的增加无关。然而,进一步的相关分析表明,努力和感知能力与疼痛等级显著相关。在认知任务中,中度疼痛与更强的努力有关,而高度疼痛导致脱离,即低努力。此外,与我们的预测一致,较高的疼痛评分与较低的自我报告能力有关,以执行认知任务。我们将讨论努力与表现之间的关系,疲劳对动机的影响,以及这些后效的个体差异。
{"title":"The Impact of Pain on Subsequent Effort and Cognitive Performance","authors":"Nicolas Silvestrini, Corrado Corradi-Dell’Acqua","doi":"10.1027/0269-8803/a000308","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000308","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract: Theoretical accounts of pain and empirical evidence indicate that pain and cognitive control share common neurocognitive processes. Numerous studies have examined the interactions between pain and cognitive performance when they occur simultaneously, typically showing analgesic effects on task performance and impaired performance due to pain. However, the sequential impact of pain on cognitive control and effort remains less clear. This study investigated the influence of a first task, including painful vs. non-painful thermal stimuli, on effort-related cardiac response and performance in a subsequent moderately difficult cognitive task. Drawing on the hypothesis that experiencing pain should recruit cognitive resources (such as attention and working memory) and reduce perceived ability, we predicted lower task performance and/or stronger compensatory effort in the subsequent cognitive task after the painful than after the non-painful first task. Results support our predictions regarding the effect of pain on subsequent cognitive performance, which was moderately lower after the painful task. However, such a decrease in task proficiency was not associated with a comparable decrease in perceived capacity or increase in effort-related cardiac reactivity. Nevertheless, further correlational analyses indicated that effort and perceived capacity were significantly related to pain ratings. Moderate pain was associated with stronger effort during the cognitive task, whereas high pain led to disengagement, that is, a low effort. Moreover, in line with our predictions, higher pain ratings were associated with a lower self-reported capacity to perform the cognitive task. We discuss these findings regarding the relationship between effort and performance, the impact of fatigue on motivation, and interindividual variability in these after-effects.","PeriodicalId":50075,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135822408","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Time, Consciousness, and the Readiness Potential 时间、意识和准备潜力
4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000304
Walter G. Sannita
Time, Consciousness, and the Readiness PotentialWalter G. SannitaWalter G. SannitaWalter G. Sannita, Department of Neuroscience, Rehabilitation, Ophthalmology, Genetics, Maternal and Child Health, University of Genova, 3, Largo P. Daneo, 16132 Genova, Italy[email protected] Department of Neuroscience, Rehabilitation, Ophthalmology, Genetics and Mother/child sciences (DINOGMI), University of Genova, Italy Search for more papers by this authorPublished Online:June 27, 2022https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000304PDFView Full Text ToolsAdd to favoritesDownload CitationsTrack Citations ShareShare onFacebookTwitterLinkedInReddit SectionsMoreReferences Bader, F., & Wiener, M. (2021). Awareness of errors and feedback in human time estimation. Learning & Memory, 28(5), 171–177. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.053108.120 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Binda, P., & Morrone, M. C. (2018). Vision during saccadic eye movements. Annual Review of Vision Science, 4, 193–213. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-vision-091517-034317 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Boly, M., Seth, A. K., Wilke, M., Ingmundson, P., Baars, B., Laureys, S., Edelman, D. B., & Tsuchiya, N. (2013). Consciousness in humans and non-human animals: Recent advances and future directions. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article 625. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00625 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Buhusi, C. V., & Meck, W. H. (2009). Relative time sharing: New findings and an extension of the resource allocation model of temporal processing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological Sciences, 364(1525), 1875–1885. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0022 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Buzsáki, G., Geisler, C., Henze, D. A., & Wang, X. J. (2004). Interneuron Diversity series: Circuit complexity and axon wiring economy of cortical interneurons. Trends in Neurosciences, 27(4), 186–193. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2004.02.007 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Celesia, G. G., & Sannita, W. G. (2013). Can patients in vegetative state experience pain and have conscious awareness? Neurology, 80(4), 328–329. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e31827f0928 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Cook, N. D., Carvalho, G. B., & Damasio, A. (2014). From membrane excitability to metazoan psychology. Trends in Neurosciences, 37(12), 698–705. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2014.07.011 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar De Corte, B. J., Navarro, V. M., & Wasserman, E. A. (2017). Non-cortical magnitude coding of space and time by pigeons. Current Biology, 27(23), R1264–R1265. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.10.029 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Deecke, L., Weinberg, H., & Brickett, P. (1982). Magnetic fields of the human brain accompanying voluntary movement: Bereitschaftsmagnetfeld. Experimental Brain Research, 48(1), 144–148. https://doi.org/10
(2008)。时间感知的专用和内在模型。认知科学趋势,12(7),273-280。https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.04.002在文章ecrossref中的首次引用,Google Scholar Ivry, R. B.和Spencer, R. M.(2004)。时间的神经表征。神经生物学杂志,14(2),225-232。https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.013在文章ecrossref中首次引用,谷歌学者Koch, C.(2018)。什么是意识?科学美国人,318(6),60-64。https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0618-60文章ecrossref首次引用,谷歌学者Koch, C, Massimini, M., Boly, M., & Tononi, G.(2016)。意识的神经关联:进展与问题。自然评论:神经科学,17(5),307-321。https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.22文章ecrossref首次引用,谷歌学者Kornhuber, h.h., & Deecke, l.r.(2016)。人类主动和被动运动中的脑电位变化:准备电位和再传入电位。[j] .中国生物医学工程学报,2016,33(4):559 - 564。https://doi.org/10.1007/s00424-016-1852-3文章首次被引用于ecrossref,谷歌学者Kotchoubey, B., Vogel, D., Lang, S., & m<e:1> ller, F.(2014)。什么样的意识是最小的?脑损伤,28(9),1156-1163。https://doi.org/10.3109/02699052.2014.920523在文章ecrossref中首次引用,Google Scholar Libet, B.(1985)。无意识的大脑主动性和有意识的意志在自愿行动中的作用。心理科学学报,2009(4):529 - 536。https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00044903在文章ecrossref中的首次引用,Google Scholar Libet, B., Gleason, C. A., Wright, E. W., & Pearl, D. K.(1983)。与大脑活动的开始有关的有意识的行动意图的时间(准备-电位)。无意识地开始一种自由自愿的行为。中国生物医学工程学报,2009(3),623-642。https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/106.3.623文章首次被引用于ecrossref, Google Scholar Madl, T., Baars, b.j., & Franklin, S.(2011)。认知周期的时间。科学通报,6(4),Article e14803。https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0014803在文章ecrossref中首次引用,谷歌学者Mauk, m.d, & Buonomano, d.v.(2004)。时间处理的神经基础。神经科学年鉴,27,307-340。https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144247文章首次引用于ecrosref, Google Scholar Meck, W. H., Penney, T. B., & Pouthas, V.(2008)。动物和人类的皮质纹状体时间表征。神经生物学杂志,18(2),145-152。https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2008.08.002文章首次引用ecrosref, Google Scholar Merchant, H., Harrington, D. L, & Meck, W. H.(2013)。感知和估计时间的神经基础。神经科学进展,36,313-336。https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-062012-170349文章首次引用ecrosref,谷歌学者Monti, m.m.s annita, w.g.(编辑)。(2016)。意识障碍中的脑功能和反应。施普林格。文章首次引用于ecrosref, Google Scholar Morrone, m.c., Cicchini, m.m., & Burr, d.c.(2010)。时间和运动的空间地图。脑科学进展,2016(2),391 - 391。https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-010-2334-z在文章ecrossref中首次引用,Google Scholar Neafsey, E. J.(2021)。有意识的意图和人类行为:战备潜力的起落和利贝特时钟的回顾。《意识与认知》,1994,第103171条。https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2021.103171在文章ecrossref中的首次引用,谷歌学者Ross, J., Morrone, m.c., Goldberg, m.e., & Burr, d.c.(2001)。扫视时视觉感知的变化。神经科学进展,24(2),113-121。https://doi.org/10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01685-4在文章ecrossref中首次引用,谷歌学者sanita, w.g.(2008)。神经元功能多样性与集体行为。生物物理学报,34(3),267-278。https://doi.org/10.1007/s10867-008-9097-9在文章ecrossref中首次引用,谷歌学者sanita, w.g.(2014)。在意识障碍中研究人脑生理学。神经病学前沿,5,第211篇。https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2014.00211在ecrossref中首次被引用,谷歌学者Scharnowski, F., Rees, G., & Walsh, V.(2013)。时间与大脑:神经相对论:从神经元而非观察者的角度来研究大脑的时间结构。认知科学动态,17(2),51-52。https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.12.005文章首次引用ecrosref, Google Scholar Schiff, N. D., Ribary, U., Moreno, D. R., Beattie, B., Kronberg, E., Blasberg, R., Giacino, J., McCagg, C., Fins, J., Llinás, R., & Plum, F.(2002)。残留的大脑活动和行为碎片可以留在持续的植物人大脑中。脑科学,125(6),1210-1234。https://doi.org/10.10
{"title":"Time, Consciousness, and the Readiness Potential","authors":"Walter G. Sannita","doi":"10.1027/0269-8803/a000304","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000304","url":null,"abstract":"Time, Consciousness, and the Readiness PotentialWalter G. SannitaWalter G. SannitaWalter G. Sannita, Department of Neuroscience, Rehabilitation, Ophthalmology, Genetics, Maternal and Child Health, University of Genova, 3, Largo P. Daneo, 16132 Genova, Italy[email protected] Department of Neuroscience, Rehabilitation, Ophthalmology, Genetics and Mother/child sciences (DINOGMI), University of Genova, Italy Search for more papers by this authorPublished Online:June 27, 2022https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000304PDFView Full Text ToolsAdd to favoritesDownload CitationsTrack Citations ShareShare onFacebookTwitterLinkedInReddit SectionsMoreReferences Bader, F., & Wiener, M. (2021). Awareness of errors and feedback in human time estimation. Learning & Memory, 28(5), 171–177. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.053108.120 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Binda, P., & Morrone, M. C. (2018). Vision during saccadic eye movements. Annual Review of Vision Science, 4, 193–213. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-vision-091517-034317 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Boly, M., Seth, A. K., Wilke, M., Ingmundson, P., Baars, B., Laureys, S., Edelman, D. B., & Tsuchiya, N. (2013). Consciousness in humans and non-human animals: Recent advances and future directions. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article 625. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00625 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Buhusi, C. V., & Meck, W. H. (2009). Relative time sharing: New findings and an extension of the resource allocation model of temporal processing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological Sciences, 364(1525), 1875–1885. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0022 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Buzsáki, G., Geisler, C., Henze, D. A., & Wang, X. J. (2004). Interneuron Diversity series: Circuit complexity and axon wiring economy of cortical interneurons. Trends in Neurosciences, 27(4), 186–193. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2004.02.007 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Celesia, G. G., & Sannita, W. G. (2013). Can patients in vegetative state experience pain and have conscious awareness? Neurology, 80(4), 328–329. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e31827f0928 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Cook, N. D., Carvalho, G. B., & Damasio, A. (2014). From membrane excitability to metazoan psychology. Trends in Neurosciences, 37(12), 698–705. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2014.07.011 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar De Corte, B. J., Navarro, V. M., & Wasserman, E. A. (2017). Non-cortical magnitude coding of space and time by pigeons. Current Biology, 27(23), R1264–R1265. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.10.029 First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Deecke, L., Weinberg, H., & Brickett, P. (1982). Magnetic fields of the human brain accompanying voluntary movement: Bereitschaftsmagnetfeld. Experimental Brain Research, 48(1), 144–148. https://doi.org/10","PeriodicalId":50075,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136186803","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Type of Trauma Exposure Impacts Neural Reactivity to Errors 创伤暴露类型影响神经对错误的反应
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-01-19 DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000318
Emily E. Jones, Kayla A. Kreutzer, Charles A. Manzler, Emily G. Evans, S. Gorka
Abstract. Studies suggest that individuals with a history of trauma exposure display abnormal reactivity to threat, though the pattern of findings across prior studies has been inconsistent. At least two factors likely contribute to previous discrepant findings: (1) the type of index trauma event and (2) the type of threat paradigm. Accordingly, the current study aimed to examine the impact of trauma type on a specific psychophysiological index of threat sensitivity – error negativity (Ne), also described as error-related negativity (ERN). Young adults were classified into three groups: lifetime history of interpersonal trauma (i.e., sexual assault, physical assault, or immediate family violence; n = 30), lifetime history of a non-interpersonal trauma (e.g., accidents, natural disasters; n = 30), or no lifetime history of trauma ( n = 64). All participants completed a well-validated flanker task designed to elicit the Ne/ERN during continuous electroencephalographic (EEG) data collection. Results indicated that individuals with non-interpersonal trauma exposure displayed reduced Ne/ERN amplitude compared with the other two groups (who did not differ from each other). Broadly, these findings highlight the importance of trauma type and theory suggesting different forms of trauma may result in different neurobiological profiles. These findings also add to a growing literature indicating that non-interpersonal traumas may be uniquely associated with blunted threat sensitivity and deficiencies in self-monitoring.
摘要研究表明,有创伤暴露史的个体对威胁表现出异常的反应,尽管先前研究的发现模式并不一致。至少有两个因素可能导致先前的差异发现:(1)指数创伤事件的类型和(2)威胁范式的类型。因此,本研究旨在探讨创伤类型对威胁敏感性的特定心理生理指标-错误否定性(Ne)的影响,也称为错误相关否定性(ERN)。年轻人被分为三组:有人际创伤史(即性侵犯、身体攻击或直系家庭暴力);N = 30),非人际创伤的终生史(如事故、自然灾害;N = 30),或终生无创伤史(N = 64)。所有参与者都完成了一个经过验证的侧翼任务,该任务旨在在连续脑电图(EEG)数据收集期间引发Ne/ERN。结果表明,与其他两组相比,非人际创伤暴露个体的Ne/ERN振幅降低(两者之间没有差异)。总的来说,这些发现强调了创伤类型和理论的重要性,表明不同形式的创伤可能导致不同的神经生物学特征。这些发现也增加了越来越多的文献表明,非人际创伤可能与迟钝的威胁敏感性和自我监控缺陷有关。
{"title":"Type of Trauma Exposure Impacts Neural Reactivity to Errors","authors":"Emily E. Jones, Kayla A. Kreutzer, Charles A. Manzler, Emily G. Evans, S. Gorka","doi":"10.1027/0269-8803/a000318","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000318","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. Studies suggest that individuals with a history of trauma exposure display abnormal reactivity to threat, though the pattern of findings across prior studies has been inconsistent. At least two factors likely contribute to previous discrepant findings: (1) the type of index trauma event and (2) the type of threat paradigm. Accordingly, the current study aimed to examine the impact of trauma type on a specific psychophysiological index of threat sensitivity – error negativity (Ne), also described as error-related negativity (ERN). Young adults were classified into three groups: lifetime history of interpersonal trauma (i.e., sexual assault, physical assault, or immediate family violence; n = 30), lifetime history of a non-interpersonal trauma (e.g., accidents, natural disasters; n = 30), or no lifetime history of trauma ( n = 64). All participants completed a well-validated flanker task designed to elicit the Ne/ERN during continuous electroencephalographic (EEG) data collection. Results indicated that individuals with non-interpersonal trauma exposure displayed reduced Ne/ERN amplitude compared with the other two groups (who did not differ from each other). Broadly, these findings highlight the importance of trauma type and theory suggesting different forms of trauma may result in different neurobiological profiles. These findings also add to a growing literature indicating that non-interpersonal traumas may be uniquely associated with blunted threat sensitivity and deficiencies in self-monitoring.","PeriodicalId":50075,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-01-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45249109","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Age Differences in Learning-Related Neurophysiological Changes 学习相关神经生理变化的年龄差异
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-01-19 DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000317
Angelica M. Tinga, N. Menger, Tycho T. de Back, M. Louwerse
Abstract. Research in young adults has demonstrated that neurophysiological measures are able to provide insight into learning processes. However, to date, it remains unclear whether neurophysiological changes during learning in older adults are comparable to those in younger adults. The current study addressed this issue by exploring age differences in changes over time in a range of neurophysiological outcome measures collected during visuomotor sequence learning. Specifically, measures of electroencephalography (EEG), skin conductance, heart rate, heart rate variability, respiration rate, and eye-related measures, in addition to behavioral performance measures, were collected in younger ( Mage = 27.24 years) and older adults ( Mage = 58.06 years) during learning. Behavioral responses became more accurate over time in both age groups during visuomotor sequence learning. Yet, older adults needed more time in each trial to enhance the precision of their movement. Changes in EEG during learning demonstrated a stronger increase in theta power in older compared to younger adults and a decrease in gamma power in older adults while increasing slightly in younger adults. No such differences between the two age groups were found on other neurophysiological outcome measures, suggesting changes in brain activity during learning to be more sensitive to age differences than changes in peripheral physiology. Additionally, differences in which neurophysiological outcomes were associated with behavioral performance on the learning task were found between younger and older adults. This indicates that the neurophysiological underpinnings of learning may differ between younger and older adults. Therefore, the current findings highlight the importance of taking age into account when aiming to gain insight into behavioral performance through neurophysiology during learning.
摘要对年轻人的研究表明,神经生理学测量能够提供对学习过程的洞察。然而,到目前为止,尚不清楚老年人在学习过程中的神经生理变化是否与年轻人具有可比性。目前的研究通过探索视觉运动序列学习期间收集的一系列神经生理结果测量值随时间变化的年龄差异来解决这个问题。具体而言,除了行为表现测量外,还收集了学习期间年轻人(年龄27.24岁)和老年人(年龄58.06岁)的脑电图(EEG)、皮肤电导、心率、心率变异性、呼吸率和眼睛相关测量。随着时间的推移,在视觉运动序列学习过程中,两个年龄组的行为反应都变得更加准确。然而,老年人在每次试验中需要更多的时间来提高他们的运动精度。学习过程中脑电图的变化表明,老年人的θ波功率比年轻人增加得更多,老年人的伽马波功率减少,而年轻人则略有增加。在其他神经生理结果测量中,两个年龄组之间没有发现这种差异,这表明在学习过程中大脑活动的变化对年龄差异比周围生理的变化更敏感。此外,在学习任务中的神经生理结果与行为表现之间的差异在年轻人和老年人之间被发现。这表明学习的神经生理基础可能在年轻人和老年人之间有所不同。因此,目前的研究结果强调了在学习过程中通过神经生理学来了解行为表现时考虑年龄的重要性。
{"title":"Age Differences in Learning-Related Neurophysiological Changes","authors":"Angelica M. Tinga, N. Menger, Tycho T. de Back, M. Louwerse","doi":"10.1027/0269-8803/a000317","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000317","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. Research in young adults has demonstrated that neurophysiological measures are able to provide insight into learning processes. However, to date, it remains unclear whether neurophysiological changes during learning in older adults are comparable to those in younger adults. The current study addressed this issue by exploring age differences in changes over time in a range of neurophysiological outcome measures collected during visuomotor sequence learning. Specifically, measures of electroencephalography (EEG), skin conductance, heart rate, heart rate variability, respiration rate, and eye-related measures, in addition to behavioral performance measures, were collected in younger ( Mage = 27.24 years) and older adults ( Mage = 58.06 years) during learning. Behavioral responses became more accurate over time in both age groups during visuomotor sequence learning. Yet, older adults needed more time in each trial to enhance the precision of their movement. Changes in EEG during learning demonstrated a stronger increase in theta power in older compared to younger adults and a decrease in gamma power in older adults while increasing slightly in younger adults. No such differences between the two age groups were found on other neurophysiological outcome measures, suggesting changes in brain activity during learning to be more sensitive to age differences than changes in peripheral physiology. Additionally, differences in which neurophysiological outcomes were associated with behavioral performance on the learning task were found between younger and older adults. This indicates that the neurophysiological underpinnings of learning may differ between younger and older adults. Therefore, the current findings highlight the importance of taking age into account when aiming to gain insight into behavioral performance through neurophysiology during learning.","PeriodicalId":50075,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-01-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44592524","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Psychophysiological Stress Response of Novel Students in Chemical Laboratory Practices 新生在化学实验室实习中的心理生理应激反应
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-12-13 DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000314
L. Redondo-Flórez, V. Clemente-Suárez
Abstract. The aim of the present study was to analyze the psychophysiological stress response of novel pharmacy and biotechnology students in chemistry laboratory practices. Autonomic stress response was analyzed in 66 students by considering heart rate variability (HRV) values before, during, and after their first laboratory practice, as well as their distress perception before and after this activity. In the laboratory practice, students had to deal with toxic products, sharp objects, or irritating substances while being examined by the teachers. Consistent with a large anticipatory anxiety response at the beginning of the practice, results showed low HRV values, generally related to a typical sympathetic modulation. This sympathetic effect persisted during the entire laboratory practice. Moreover, just after the end of the practice, there was an attenuation of the sympathetic activation and a takeover by the parasympathetic system, as revealed by the increase of HRV parameters. Laboratory practices performed by novel Pharmacy and Biotechnology students did not produce the expected habituation response at psychophysiological level since, in addition to an anticipatory anxiety response observed prior to the laboratory practice, a high sympathetic autonomic activation was observed during the whole practice. In brief, using HRV parameters could be a useful tool to analyze stress responses in students and to help them with new strategies to improve routine evaluation procedures.
摘要本研究的目的是分析新药学和生物技术专业学生在化学实验室实践中的心理生理应激反应。通过考虑第一次实验室练习之前、期间和之后的心率变异性(HRV)值,以及他们在该活动前后的痛苦感知,对66名学生的自主应激反应进行了分析。在实验室实践中,学生在接受老师检查时必须处理有毒产品、尖锐物体或刺激性物质。与实践开始时的大量预期焦虑反应一致,结果显示HRV值较低,通常与典型的交感神经调节有关。这种同情效应在整个实验室实践中一直存在。此外,就在练习结束后,交感神经激活减弱,副交感神经系统接管,HRV参数的增加表明了这一点。新药学和生物技术专业学生进行的实验室实践没有在心理生理水平上产生预期的习惯反应,因为除了在实验室实践前观察到的预期焦虑反应外,在整个实践过程中还观察到高度的交感自主激活。简言之,使用HRV参数可以成为分析学生压力反应的有用工具,并帮助他们制定新的策略来改进日常评估程序。
{"title":"Psychophysiological Stress Response of Novel Students in Chemical Laboratory Practices","authors":"L. Redondo-Flórez, V. Clemente-Suárez","doi":"10.1027/0269-8803/a000314","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000314","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. The aim of the present study was to analyze the psychophysiological stress response of novel pharmacy and biotechnology students in chemistry laboratory practices. Autonomic stress response was analyzed in 66 students by considering heart rate variability (HRV) values before, during, and after their first laboratory practice, as well as their distress perception before and after this activity. In the laboratory practice, students had to deal with toxic products, sharp objects, or irritating substances while being examined by the teachers. Consistent with a large anticipatory anxiety response at the beginning of the practice, results showed low HRV values, generally related to a typical sympathetic modulation. This sympathetic effect persisted during the entire laboratory practice. Moreover, just after the end of the practice, there was an attenuation of the sympathetic activation and a takeover by the parasympathetic system, as revealed by the increase of HRV parameters. Laboratory practices performed by novel Pharmacy and Biotechnology students did not produce the expected habituation response at psychophysiological level since, in addition to an anticipatory anxiety response observed prior to the laboratory practice, a high sympathetic autonomic activation was observed during the whole practice. In brief, using HRV parameters could be a useful tool to analyze stress responses in students and to help them with new strategies to improve routine evaluation procedures.","PeriodicalId":50075,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-12-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49172434","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Disgust-Eliciting Pathogen Threats and Salivary Immune Responses 引起恶心的病原体威胁和唾液免疫反应
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-12-13 DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000316
Lahai A M Wicks, D. Baldwin, Matthew T. Richesin
Abstract. This study aimed to examine the effects of different sensory modality presentations of disgusting stimuli on aspects of immunity (behavioral and physiological) and affect in men and women. Sixty-four college students participated, and all participants were randomly assigned to one of four groups based on sensory modality stimuli type (visual or olfactory) and manipulation (experimental or control). Results indicated that olfactory stimuli were rated as more disgusting than visual stimuli. When collapsed across sensory modality, females tended to self-report greater disgust sensitivity than males. Overall, there was a significant negative association between disgust ratings and changes in resting heart rate (HR). Baseline salivary immunoglobulin A (SIgA) concentration was significantly positively correlated with disgust ratings in men. Our findings suggest that the behavioral and physiological immune systems are indeed coordinated, but future studies are needed to examine the extent to which multisensory disgusting cues influence immune system responses as a function of gender.
摘要本研究旨在研究恶心刺激的不同感官形态对免疫(行为和生理)方面的影响及其对男性和女性的影响。64名大学生参与了这项研究,所有参与者根据感官刺激方式(视觉或嗅觉)和操作方式(实验或对照)随机分为四组。结果表明,嗅觉刺激比视觉刺激更令人厌恶。当感觉模态崩溃时,女性倾向于自我报告比男性更强烈的厌恶敏感性。总的来说,厌恶评分和静息心率(HR)的变化之间存在显著的负相关。基线唾液免疫球蛋白A (SIgA)浓度与男性厌恶评分显著正相关。我们的研究结果表明,行为和生理免疫系统确实是协调的,但未来的研究需要检验多感官恶心线索对免疫系统反应的影响程度。
{"title":"Disgust-Eliciting Pathogen Threats and Salivary Immune Responses","authors":"Lahai A M Wicks, D. Baldwin, Matthew T. Richesin","doi":"10.1027/0269-8803/a000316","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000316","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. This study aimed to examine the effects of different sensory modality presentations of disgusting stimuli on aspects of immunity (behavioral and physiological) and affect in men and women. Sixty-four college students participated, and all participants were randomly assigned to one of four groups based on sensory modality stimuli type (visual or olfactory) and manipulation (experimental or control). Results indicated that olfactory stimuli were rated as more disgusting than visual stimuli. When collapsed across sensory modality, females tended to self-report greater disgust sensitivity than males. Overall, there was a significant negative association between disgust ratings and changes in resting heart rate (HR). Baseline salivary immunoglobulin A (SIgA) concentration was significantly positively correlated with disgust ratings in men. Our findings suggest that the behavioral and physiological immune systems are indeed coordinated, but future studies are needed to examine the extent to which multisensory disgusting cues influence immune system responses as a function of gender.","PeriodicalId":50075,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-12-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45879393","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sleep and Daily Positive Emotions – Is Heart Rate Variability a Mediator? 睡眠和日常积极情绪——心率变异是一个中介因素吗?
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-12-13 DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000315
A. Ballesio, A. Zagaria, Andrea Salaris, M. Terrasi, C. Lombardo, C. Ottaviani
Abstract. Sleep quality is considered a basic dimension of emotional health. The psychophysiological mechanisms underlying the associations between sleep quality and positive emotions are still largely unknown, yet autonomic regulation may play a role. This study employed a two-day ecological momentary assessment methodology in a sample of young adults to investigate whether subjective sleep quality reported in the morning was associated with daily positive emotional experience and whether this association was mediated by heart rate variability (HRV), a measure of cardiac vagal tone. Sleep quality was assessed using an electronic sleep diary upon awakening, while resting HRV and positive emotions were inspected at random times throughout the day using photoplethysmography and an electronic diary, respectively. Relevant confounding variables such as smoking, alcohol intake, and physical exercise between each measurement were also assessed. The sample included 121 participants (64.8% females, Mage = 25.97 ± 5.32 years). After controlling for relevant confounders including health behaviors and psychiatric comorbidities, mediation analysis revealed that greater sleep quality positively predicted daily HRV (β = .289, p < .001) which, in turn, had a direct influence on positive emotions (β = .244, p = .006). Also, sleep quality directly predicted positive emotional experience (β = .272, p = .001). Lastly, the model showed an indirect effect between sleep quality and positive emotions via HRV (β = .071, 95% BCI [.011, .146]). Results support the view of HRV as a process variable linking sleep to positive emotions. Experimental data is needed to consolidate the present findings.
摘要睡眠质量被认为是情绪健康的一个基本方面。睡眠质量和积极情绪之间联系的心理生理机制在很大程度上仍然未知,但自主调节可能发挥作用。这项研究对年轻人样本采用了为期两天的生态瞬时评估方法,以调查早上报告的主观睡眠质量是否与日常积极情绪体验有关,以及这种联系是否由心率变异性(HRV)介导,心率变异性是衡量心脏迷走神经张力的一种指标。醒来后使用电子睡眠日记评估睡眠质量,同时分别使用光电体积描记术和电子日记在一天中随机检查静息HRV和积极情绪。还评估了每次测量之间的相关混杂变量,如吸烟、饮酒和体育锻炼。该样本包括121名参与者(64.8%的女性,Mage=25.97±5.32岁)。在控制了包括健康行为和精神合并症在内的相关混杂因素后,中介分析显示,更高的睡眠质量可以积极预测每日HRV(β=.289,p<.001),而这反过来又对积极情绪产生了直接影响(β=.244,p=.006)。此外,睡眠质量直接预测积极情绪体验(β=0.272,p=0.001)。最后,该模型通过HRV显示了睡眠质量与积极情绪之间的间接影响(β=0.071,95%脑机接口[0.011,.146])。结果支持HRV作为将睡眠与积极情绪联系起来的过程变量的观点。需要实验数据来巩固目前的发现。
{"title":"Sleep and Daily Positive Emotions – Is Heart Rate Variability a Mediator?","authors":"A. Ballesio, A. Zagaria, Andrea Salaris, M. Terrasi, C. Lombardo, C. Ottaviani","doi":"10.1027/0269-8803/a000315","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000315","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. Sleep quality is considered a basic dimension of emotional health. The psychophysiological mechanisms underlying the associations between sleep quality and positive emotions are still largely unknown, yet autonomic regulation may play a role. This study employed a two-day ecological momentary assessment methodology in a sample of young adults to investigate whether subjective sleep quality reported in the morning was associated with daily positive emotional experience and whether this association was mediated by heart rate variability (HRV), a measure of cardiac vagal tone. Sleep quality was assessed using an electronic sleep diary upon awakening, while resting HRV and positive emotions were inspected at random times throughout the day using photoplethysmography and an electronic diary, respectively. Relevant confounding variables such as smoking, alcohol intake, and physical exercise between each measurement were also assessed. The sample included 121 participants (64.8% females, Mage = 25.97 ± 5.32 years). After controlling for relevant confounders including health behaviors and psychiatric comorbidities, mediation analysis revealed that greater sleep quality positively predicted daily HRV (β = .289, p < .001) which, in turn, had a direct influence on positive emotions (β = .244, p = .006). Also, sleep quality directly predicted positive emotional experience (β = .272, p = .001). Lastly, the model showed an indirect effect between sleep quality and positive emotions via HRV (β = .071, 95% BCI [.011, .146]). Results support the view of HRV as a process variable linking sleep to positive emotions. Experimental data is needed to consolidate the present findings.","PeriodicalId":50075,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-12-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48018999","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Documentation of Peripheral Auditory Function in Studies of the Auditory P300 Response P300听觉反应研究中外周听觉功能的记录
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-11-07 DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000312
Janushca van der Merwe, Leigh Biagio-de Jager, F. Mahomed-Asmail, James W. Hall
Abstract. A critical review was conducted to examine whether the peripheral hearing status of participants with neurological and psychological disorders was documented in published clinical studies of the auditory P300 response. Literature searches were conducted with three databases: PubMed, PsycINFO, and Scopus. Studies of participants with seven neurological or psychological disorders were included in the study. Each disorder was coupled with the main search phrase in separate searches on each database. Of the total 102 papers which met the inclusion criteria, the majority (64%) did not describe the peripheral hearing sensitivity of participants. In this review with studies that included participants at risk for hearing impairment, particularly age-related hearing loss, only a single publication adequately described formal hearing evaluation. Peripheral hearing status is rarely defined in studies of the P300 response. The inclusion of participants with a hearing loss likely affects the validity of findings for these studies. We recommend formal hearing assessment prior to inclusion of participants in studies of the auditory P300 response. The findings of this study may increase the awareness among researchers outside the field of audiology of the effects of peripheral hearing loss on the auditory P300.
摘要我们进行了一项重要的综述,以检查患有神经和心理疾病的参与者的外周听力状况是否在已发表的P300听觉反应的临床研究中有记录。文献检索使用三个数据库:PubMed、PsycINFO和Scopus。研究对象包括患有七种神经或心理疾病的参与者。在每个数据库的单独搜索中,每种疾病都与主要搜索短语相关联。在符合纳入标准的102篇论文中,大多数(64%)没有描述参与者的外周听力敏感性。在本综述中,研究纳入了有听力损害风险的参与者,特别是与年龄相关的听力损失,只有一篇出版物充分描述了正式的听力评估。在P300反应的研究中,外周听力状态很少被定义。纳入有听力损失的参与者可能会影响这些研究结果的有效性。我们建议在参与P300听觉反应研究之前进行正式的听力评估。本研究的发现可能会增加听力学领域以外的研究人员对周围性听力损失对听觉P300的影响的认识。
{"title":"Documentation of Peripheral Auditory Function in Studies of the Auditory P300 Response","authors":"Janushca van der Merwe, Leigh Biagio-de Jager, F. Mahomed-Asmail, James W. Hall","doi":"10.1027/0269-8803/a000312","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000312","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract. A critical review was conducted to examine whether the peripheral hearing status of participants with neurological and psychological disorders was documented in published clinical studies of the auditory P300 response. Literature searches were conducted with three databases: PubMed, PsycINFO, and Scopus. Studies of participants with seven neurological or psychological disorders were included in the study. Each disorder was coupled with the main search phrase in separate searches on each database. Of the total 102 papers which met the inclusion criteria, the majority (64%) did not describe the peripheral hearing sensitivity of participants. In this review with studies that included participants at risk for hearing impairment, particularly age-related hearing loss, only a single publication adequately described formal hearing evaluation. Peripheral hearing status is rarely defined in studies of the P300 response. The inclusion of participants with a hearing loss likely affects the validity of findings for these studies. We recommend formal hearing assessment prior to inclusion of participants in studies of the auditory P300 response. The findings of this study may increase the awareness among researchers outside the field of audiology of the effects of peripheral hearing loss on the auditory P300.","PeriodicalId":50075,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Psychophysiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-11-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42478561","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Journal of Psychophysiology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1