首页 > 最新文献

Communications Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
A systematic review of volitional personality change research 意志性人格改变研究的系统回顾
Pub Date : 2024-11-30 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00167-5
Peter Haehner, Amanda Jo Wright, Wiebke Bleidorn
Personality traits predict a broad range of life outcomes, including relationship success, educational attainment, and health. As many people have the desire to change some aspects of their personality, volitional personality change (VPC) – self-directed trait changes in the direction of personal change goals – has recently gained increasing attention. This preregistered review aimed to provide an integrative overview of the emerging literature on VPC ( https://osf.io/ns79m ). Based on a systematic literature search on PsycINFO (October 1, 2024), we identified 30 empirical, longitudinal studies on VPC (N = 7719). We summarized the findings from these studies in a narrative integration and using meta-analytic tools and distinguished two research strands in the VPC literature: studies examining VPC without interventions and studies examining intervention-induced VPC. Simply having a goal to change one’s personality was only weakly related to actual personality changes. However, VPC interventions were successful in promoting desired personality changes (d = 0.22, 95% CI = [0.005, 0.433], 7 studies, 26 effect sizes). These personality changes seemed to last or even increase during follow-up periods (d = 0.37, 95% CI = [0.140, 0.591], 4 studies, 17 effect sizes) and were associated with changes in other variables such as well-being. Although the available evidence on VPC is still limited, the initial results on VPC are promising. Future research is needed to draw definitive conclusions on the generalizability, mechanisms, and practical implications of VPC. The authors received no funding to conduct this review. Meta-analytic evidence finds interventions aimed at altering personality traits were successful in promoting change. A review of nonintervention studies, finds having the goal to change personality is weakly related to success
性格特征预示着广泛的生活结果,包括关系成功、教育成就和健康。由于许多人都希望改变自己性格的某些方面,自愿性人格改变(VPC)——在个人改变目标方向上的自我导向的特质改变——最近得到了越来越多的关注。这篇预注册的综述旨在提供关于VPC的新兴文献的综合概述(https://osf.io/ns79m)。基于PsycINFO(2024年10月1日)的系统文献检索,我们确定了30个关于VPC的实证、纵向研究(N = 7719)。我们以叙事整合和使用元分析工具的方式总结了这些研究的结果,并区分了VPC文献中的两个研究方向:无干预的VPC研究和干预诱导的VPC研究。仅仅有一个改变人格的目标与实际的人格改变只有微弱的关系。然而,VPC干预在促进期望的人格改变方面是成功的(d = 0.22, 95% CI =[0.005, 0.433], 7项研究,26个效应量)。在随访期间,这些人格变化似乎持续甚至增加(d = 0.37, 95% CI =[0.140, 0.591], 4项研究,17个效应值),并与其他变量(如幸福感)的变化有关。尽管VPC的可用证据仍然有限,但VPC的初步结果是有希望的。未来的研究需要对VPC的普遍性、机制和实际意义得出明确的结论。作者没有获得进行本综述的资金。元分析证据发现,旨在改变人格特质的干预措施在促进变革方面是成功的。一项对非干预研究的回顾发现,以改变个性为目标与成功的关系很弱
{"title":"A systematic review of volitional personality change research","authors":"Peter Haehner, Amanda Jo Wright, Wiebke Bleidorn","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00167-5","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00167-5","url":null,"abstract":"Personality traits predict a broad range of life outcomes, including relationship success, educational attainment, and health. As many people have the desire to change some aspects of their personality, volitional personality change (VPC) – self-directed trait changes in the direction of personal change goals – has recently gained increasing attention. This preregistered review aimed to provide an integrative overview of the emerging literature on VPC ( https://osf.io/ns79m ). Based on a systematic literature search on PsycINFO (October 1, 2024), we identified 30 empirical, longitudinal studies on VPC (N = 7719). We summarized the findings from these studies in a narrative integration and using meta-analytic tools and distinguished two research strands in the VPC literature: studies examining VPC without interventions and studies examining intervention-induced VPC. Simply having a goal to change one’s personality was only weakly related to actual personality changes. However, VPC interventions were successful in promoting desired personality changes (d = 0.22, 95% CI = [0.005, 0.433], 7 studies, 26 effect sizes). These personality changes seemed to last or even increase during follow-up periods (d = 0.37, 95% CI = [0.140, 0.591], 4 studies, 17 effect sizes) and were associated with changes in other variables such as well-being. Although the available evidence on VPC is still limited, the initial results on VPC are promising. Future research is needed to draw definitive conclusions on the generalizability, mechanisms, and practical implications of VPC. The authors received no funding to conduct this review. Meta-analytic evidence finds interventions aimed at altering personality traits were successful in promoting change. A review of nonintervention studies, finds having the goal to change personality is weakly related to success","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-14"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-11-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00167-5.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142758087","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Behavioural determinants of physiologically-relevant light exposure 生理相关光照的行为决定因素
Pub Date : 2024-11-29 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00159-5
Anna M. Biller, Priji Balakrishnan, Manuel Spitschan
Light exposure triggers a range of physiological and behavioural responses that can improve and challenge health and well-being. Insights from laboratory studies have recently culminated in standards and guidelines for measuring and assessing healthy light exposure, and recommendations for healthy light levels. Implicit to laboratory paradigms is a simplistic input-output relationship between light and its effects on physiology. This simplified approach ignores that humans actively shape their light exposure through behaviour. This article presents a novel framework that conceptualises light exposure as an individual behaviour to meet specific, person-based needs. Key to healthy light exposure is shaping behaviour, beyond shaping technology. Biller et al explain that humans actively shape their lighting environment through behaviour to meet specific individual needs. They propose that achieving healthy light exposure relies on shaping behaviour.
光照会引发一系列生理和行为反应,从而改善或挑战健康和幸福。最近,从实验室研究中获得的启示最终形成了测量和评估健康光照的标准和指南,以及健康光照水平的建议。实验室范例中隐含的是光及其对生理影响之间的简单输入输出关系。这种简化的方法忽视了人类通过行为主动塑造自己的光照。本文提出了一个新颖的框架,将光照概念化为一种个人行为,以满足特定的、基于个人的需求。健康光照的关键是塑造行为,而不是塑造技术。比勒等人解释说,人类通过行为积极塑造自己的照明环境,以满足特定的个人需求。他们提出,实现健康的光照依赖于塑造行为。
{"title":"Behavioural determinants of physiologically-relevant light exposure","authors":"Anna M. Biller, Priji Balakrishnan, Manuel Spitschan","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00159-5","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00159-5","url":null,"abstract":"Light exposure triggers a range of physiological and behavioural responses that can improve and challenge health and well-being. Insights from laboratory studies have recently culminated in standards and guidelines for measuring and assessing healthy light exposure, and recommendations for healthy light levels. Implicit to laboratory paradigms is a simplistic input-output relationship between light and its effects on physiology. This simplified approach ignores that humans actively shape their light exposure through behaviour. This article presents a novel framework that conceptualises light exposure as an individual behaviour to meet specific, person-based needs. Key to healthy light exposure is shaping behaviour, beyond shaping technology. Biller et al explain that humans actively shape their lighting environment through behaviour to meet specific individual needs. They propose that achieving healthy light exposure relies on shaping behaviour.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-11"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-11-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00159-5.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142737653","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Self-esteem modulates the similarity of the representation of the self in the brains of others 自尊能调节他人大脑中自我表征的相似性
Pub Date : 2024-11-27 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00148-8
Moriah S. Stendel, Taylor D. Guthrie, Victoria Guazzelli Williamson, Robert S. Chavez
Social neuroscientists have made marked progress in understanding the underlying neural mechanisms that contribute to self-esteem. However, these neural mechanisms have not been examined within the rich social contexts that theories in social psychology emphasize. Previous research has demonstrated that neural representations of the self are reflected in the brains of peers in a phenomenon called the ‘self-recapitulation effect’, but it remains unclear how these processes are influenced by self-esteem. In the current study, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging in a round-robin design within 19 independent groups of participants (total N = 107) to test how self-esteem modulates the representation of self-other similarity in multivariate brain response patterns during interpersonal perception. Our results replicate the self-recapitulation effect in a sample almost ten times the size of the original study and show that these effects are found within distributed brain systems underlying self-representation and social cognition. Furthermore, we extend these findings to demonstrate that individual differences in self-esteem modulate these responses within the medial prefrontal cortex, a region implicated in evaluative self-referential processing. These findings inform theoretical models of self-esteem in social psychology and suggest that greater self-esteem is associated with psychologically distanced self-evaluations from peer-evaluations in interpersonal appraisals. Using a round-robin design, this study replicated the “self-recapitulation effect” (where neural representations of the self are similar to those in close others’ brains) and revealed how self-esteem modulates this effect.
社会神经科学家在了解促成自尊的潜在神经机制方面取得了显著进展。然而,这些神经机制尚未在社会心理学理论所强调的丰富社会背景下进行研究。以往的研究表明,自我的神经表征在同伴的大脑中得到反映,这种现象被称为 "自我重现效应",但这些过程如何受到自尊的影响仍不清楚。在本研究中,我们采用功能磁共振成像技术,在19个独立的参与者小组(总人数=107)中进行循环设计,以测试在人际感知过程中,自尊如何调节自他相似性在多变量大脑反应模式中的表征。我们的研究结果在规模几乎是原始研究十倍的样本中复制了自我重现效应,并表明这些效应存在于支撑自我表征和社会认知的分布式大脑系统中。此外,我们还扩展了这些研究结果,证明自尊的个体差异会调节内侧前额叶皮层中的这些反应,而内侧前额叶皮层是一个与评价性自我参照处理有关联的区域。这些发现为社会心理学中的自尊理论模型提供了参考,并表明在人际评价中,更强的自尊与心理上自我评价与同伴评价之间的距离有关。本研究采用循环设计,复制了 "自我重现效应"(自我的神经表征与亲密他人大脑中的表征相似),并揭示了自尊如何调节这种效应。
{"title":"Self-esteem modulates the similarity of the representation of the self in the brains of others","authors":"Moriah S. Stendel, Taylor D. Guthrie, Victoria Guazzelli Williamson, Robert S. Chavez","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00148-8","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00148-8","url":null,"abstract":"Social neuroscientists have made marked progress in understanding the underlying neural mechanisms that contribute to self-esteem. However, these neural mechanisms have not been examined within the rich social contexts that theories in social psychology emphasize. Previous research has demonstrated that neural representations of the self are reflected in the brains of peers in a phenomenon called the ‘self-recapitulation effect’, but it remains unclear how these processes are influenced by self-esteem. In the current study, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging in a round-robin design within 19 independent groups of participants (total N = 107) to test how self-esteem modulates the representation of self-other similarity in multivariate brain response patterns during interpersonal perception. Our results replicate the self-recapitulation effect in a sample almost ten times the size of the original study and show that these effects are found within distributed brain systems underlying self-representation and social cognition. Furthermore, we extend these findings to demonstrate that individual differences in self-esteem modulate these responses within the medial prefrontal cortex, a region implicated in evaluative self-referential processing. These findings inform theoretical models of self-esteem in social psychology and suggest that greater self-esteem is associated with psychologically distanced self-evaluations from peer-evaluations in interpersonal appraisals. Using a round-robin design, this study replicated the “self-recapitulation effect” (where neural representations of the self are similar to those in close others’ brains) and revealed how self-esteem modulates this effect.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-8"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-11-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00148-8.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142737656","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A dissociation between the use of implicit and explicit priors in perceptual inference 在知觉推理中使用隐性先验和显性先验之间的分离。
Pub Date : 2024-11-26 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00162-w
Caroline Bévalot, Florent Meyniel
The brain constantly uses prior knowledge of the statistics of its environment to shape perception. These statistics are often implicit (not directly observable) and learned incrementally from observation, but they can also be explicitly communicated to the observer, especially in humans. Here, we show that priors are used differently in human perceptual inference depending on whether they are explicit or implicit in the environment. Bayesian modeling of learning and perception revealed that the weight of the sensory likelihood in perceptual decisions was highly correlated across participants between tasks with implicit and explicit priors, and slightly stronger in the implicit task. By contrast, the weight of priors was much less correlated across tasks, and it was markedly smaller for explicit priors. The model comparison also showed that different computations underpinned perceptual decisions depending on the origin of the priors. This dissociation may resolve previously conflicting results about the appropriate use of priors in human perception. Whether priors are implicit or explicit affects the computations underlying perceptual decisions. The integration of priors and likelihood combination is closer to Bayesian integration when priors are implicit, but more akin to a simpler heuristic when they are explicit.
大脑会不断利用有关环境统计数据的先验知识来塑造感知。这些统计信息通常是隐含的(无法直接观察到),并从观察中逐步学习,但它们也可以明确地传达给观察者,尤其是人类。在这里,我们展示了在人类的感知推理中,先验的使用方式是不同的,这取决于先验在环境中是显性的还是隐性的。学习和感知的贝叶斯建模显示,感知决策中感官可能性的权重在隐性和显性先验任务中高度相关,在隐性任务中稍强。相比之下,先验的权重在不同任务中的相关性要小得多,而在显性先验中则明显较小。模型比较还表明,根据先验的来源不同,感知决策所依赖的计算也不同。这种差异可能会解决之前关于在人类感知中适当使用先验的相互矛盾的结果。
{"title":"A dissociation between the use of implicit and explicit priors in perceptual inference","authors":"Caroline Bévalot, Florent Meyniel","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00162-w","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00162-w","url":null,"abstract":"The brain constantly uses prior knowledge of the statistics of its environment to shape perception. These statistics are often implicit (not directly observable) and learned incrementally from observation, but they can also be explicitly communicated to the observer, especially in humans. Here, we show that priors are used differently in human perceptual inference depending on whether they are explicit or implicit in the environment. Bayesian modeling of learning and perception revealed that the weight of the sensory likelihood in perceptual decisions was highly correlated across participants between tasks with implicit and explicit priors, and slightly stronger in the implicit task. By contrast, the weight of priors was much less correlated across tasks, and it was markedly smaller for explicit priors. The model comparison also showed that different computations underpinned perceptual decisions depending on the origin of the priors. This dissociation may resolve previously conflicting results about the appropriate use of priors in human perception. Whether priors are implicit or explicit affects the computations underlying perceptual decisions. The integration of priors and likelihood combination is closer to Bayesian integration when priors are implicit, but more akin to a simpler heuristic when they are explicit.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-11"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-11-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00162-w.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142735499","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Adaptive empathic response selection is sensitive to multiple dimensions of social interaction 适应性移情反应选择对社会互动的多个方面都很敏感。
Pub Date : 2024-11-26 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00164-8
Elena Kozakevich Arbel, Simone G. Shamay-Tsoory, Uri Hertz
During empathic response selection, individuals draw from both past experiences and social cues, including the distressed person’s identity, their emotional state, and the cause of distress. To study how these social dimensions influence empathic-response learning we integrated a multidimensional learning paradigm, computational modelling, and adaptive empathy framework. Participants identified effective empathic responses across two blocks of distress scenarios, with one social dimension altered between blocks. We anticipated two learning patterns: dimension-sensitive, treating each change as a new learning experience, and dimension-insensitive, relying on previous experience as a baseline. We found that participants were sensitive to changes in person, emotional state, and distress cause, but to different degree. The person dimension was the most salient, suggesting that the distressed person’s identity is the primary reference point when interacting with others. Our findings provide a quantitative evaluation of the weight given to different dimensions of social interactions, which may help understand how people perceive and react in such scenarios. The Stage 1 protocol for this Registered Report was accepted in principle on 8 May 2024. The protocol, as accepted by the journal, can be found at https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.25827334.v1 . When providing emotional support and deciding on an empathic reaction, responders were sensitive to changes in the person requiring empathy, the emotional state of that person, and the cause of their distress. The identity of the person needing support was the most salient factor.
在移情反应的选择过程中,个体会借鉴过去的经验和社会线索,包括受困者的身份、情绪状态和受困原因。为了研究这些社会维度如何影响共情反应学习,我们整合了多维学习范式、计算建模和适应性共情框架。受试者在两组苦恼情景中找出有效的共情反应,在两组情景之间改变一个社会维度。我们预计会出现两种学习模式:维度敏感型和维度不敏感型,前者将每一次变化都视为一次新的学习体验,而后者则将以往的经验作为基线。我们发现,参与者对人、情绪状态和困扰原因的变化都很敏感,但敏感程度不同。人的维度最为突出,这表明在与他人互动时,受困扰者的身份是主要参考点。我们的研究结果提供了对社会互动中不同维度所占权重的量化评估,这可能有助于理解人们在此类情景中的感知和反应。
{"title":"Adaptive empathic response selection is sensitive to multiple dimensions of social interaction","authors":"Elena Kozakevich Arbel, Simone G. Shamay-Tsoory, Uri Hertz","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00164-8","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00164-8","url":null,"abstract":"During empathic response selection, individuals draw from both past experiences and social cues, including the distressed person’s identity, their emotional state, and the cause of distress. To study how these social dimensions influence empathic-response learning we integrated a multidimensional learning paradigm, computational modelling, and adaptive empathy framework. Participants identified effective empathic responses across two blocks of distress scenarios, with one social dimension altered between blocks. We anticipated two learning patterns: dimension-sensitive, treating each change as a new learning experience, and dimension-insensitive, relying on previous experience as a baseline. We found that participants were sensitive to changes in person, emotional state, and distress cause, but to different degree. The person dimension was the most salient, suggesting that the distressed person’s identity is the primary reference point when interacting with others. Our findings provide a quantitative evaluation of the weight given to different dimensions of social interactions, which may help understand how people perceive and react in such scenarios. The Stage 1 protocol for this Registered Report was accepted in principle on 8 May 2024. The protocol, as accepted by the journal, can be found at https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.25827334.v1 . When providing emotional support and deciding on an empathic reaction, responders were sensitive to changes in the person requiring empathy, the emotional state of that person, and the cause of their distress. The identity of the person needing support was the most salient factor.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-14"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-11-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00164-8.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142735500","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Parent-child relationship quality predicts higher subjective well-being in adulthood across a diverse group of countries 不同国家的亲子关系质量可预测成年后更高的主观幸福感
Pub Date : 2024-11-22 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00161-x
Jonathan T. Rothwell, Telli Davoodi
Theoretically and empirically, parenting is closely related to the psychological health of offspring, but long-term effects and possible international differences are not well established. In a pre-registered multilevel modeling analysis using data from the Global Flourishing Study, we tested whether retrospective parent-child relationship quality predicts adult well-being in a representative sample of 202,898 adults living in 21 countries and one territory. We developed and validated indexes of flourishing and mental health. Retrospective parent-child relationship quality predicted both with substantial effect sizes for flourishing (std mean effect = 0.21, 95% CI 0.19–0.23) and mental health (std mean effect = 0.18, 95% CI 0.17–0.20). A positive association between relationship quality and flourishing was found in all 22 areas (significant in 21). Parental religiosity positively predicted relationship quality (std mean effect = 0.09, 95% CI 0.06–0.11). In higher income and more secular countries, relationship quality was lower, but the well-being benefits were higher. Parental religiosity predicted higher relationship quality in every country in the sample. Cross-cultural evidence indicates an association between higher recalled parent-child relationship quality and adult flourishing as well as current mental health. Relationship quality was higher with reports of greater parental religiosity
从理论和经验上讲,养育子女与后代的心理健康密切相关,但长期影响和可能存在的国际差异尚未得到很好的证实。我们利用全球幸福研究(Global Flourishing Study)的数据进行了一项预先登记的多层次模型分析,测试了生活在 21 个国家和 1 个地区的 202,898 名成年人的代表性样本中,回顾性亲子关系质量是否能预测成年人的幸福感。我们开发并验证了幸福指数和心理健康指数。回顾性亲子关系质量对幸福感(std mean effect = 0.21, 95% CI 0.19-0.23)和心理健康(std mean effect = 0.18, 95% CI 0.17-0.20)都有显著的预测效果。在所有 22 个领域中,关系质量与蓬勃发展之间都存在正相关(在 21 个领域中显著)。父母的宗教信仰对人际关系质量有积极的预测作用(std 平均效应 = 0.09,95% CI 0.06-0.11)。在收入较高和世俗化程度较高的国家,关系质量较低,但幸福感较高。在样本中的每个国家,父母的宗教信仰都预示着较高的关系质量。跨文化证据表明,较高的亲子关系质量与成人的蓬勃发展以及当前的心理健康之间存在关联。父母宗教信仰越高,关系质量越高
{"title":"Parent-child relationship quality predicts higher subjective well-being in adulthood across a diverse group of countries","authors":"Jonathan T. Rothwell, Telli Davoodi","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00161-x","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00161-x","url":null,"abstract":"Theoretically and empirically, parenting is closely related to the psychological health of offspring, but long-term effects and possible international differences are not well established. In a pre-registered multilevel modeling analysis using data from the Global Flourishing Study, we tested whether retrospective parent-child relationship quality predicts adult well-being in a representative sample of 202,898 adults living in 21 countries and one territory. We developed and validated indexes of flourishing and mental health. Retrospective parent-child relationship quality predicted both with substantial effect sizes for flourishing (std mean effect = 0.21, 95% CI 0.19–0.23) and mental health (std mean effect = 0.18, 95% CI 0.17–0.20). A positive association between relationship quality and flourishing was found in all 22 areas (significant in 21). Parental religiosity positively predicted relationship quality (std mean effect = 0.09, 95% CI 0.06–0.11). In higher income and more secular countries, relationship quality was lower, but the well-being benefits were higher. Parental religiosity predicted higher relationship quality in every country in the sample. Cross-cultural evidence indicates an association between higher recalled parent-child relationship quality and adult flourishing as well as current mental health. Relationship quality was higher with reports of greater parental religiosity","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-16"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00161-x.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142692160","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sensory stimulation enhances visual working memory capacity 感官刺激可增强视觉工作记忆能力
Pub Date : 2024-11-18 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00158-6
Indre Pileckyte, Salvador Soto-Faraco
Visual working memory (vWM) plays a crucial role in visual information processing and higher cognitive functions; however, it has a very limited capacity. Recently, several studies have successfully modulated vWM capacity in humans using entrainment with transcranial alternate current stimulation (tACS) by targeting parietal theta in a frequency-specific manner. In the current study, we aim to expand upon these findings by utilizing sensory instead of electrical stimulation. Across six behavioral experiments (combined N = 209), we applied rhythmic visual and auditory sensory stimulation at 4 Hz and 7 Hz, aiming to modulate vWM capacity. Collectively, the results showed an overall robust improvement with sensory stimulation at either frequency, compared to baseline. However, contrary to our prediction, 7 Hz stimulation tended to slightly outperform 4 Hz stimulation. Importantly, the observed facilitatory effect was mainly driven by the low-capacity sub-group of participants. Follow-up experiments using the Attention Network Test (ANT) and pupillometry measures did not find evidence that this effect could be directly attributed to modulation of phasic or tonic arousal. We speculate that our results differed from those obtained with tACS due to targeting functionally different theta oscillations, or the modulation of participants’ temporal expectations. Visual or auditory stimulation at 4 Hz and 7 Hz improved visual working memory performance. This effect was more pronounced in individuals with lower visual working memory capacity.
视觉工作记忆(vWM)在视觉信息处理和高级认知功能中起着至关重要的作用;然而,它的容量却非常有限。最近,有几项研究利用经颅交替电流刺激(tACS),以特定频率的方式针对顶叶θ,成功地调节了人类的视觉工作记忆能力。在当前的研究中,我们旨在利用感官刺激而不是电刺激来扩展这些发现。在六次行为实验(总人数= 209)中,我们分别采用了4赫兹和7赫兹的有节奏视觉和听觉感官刺激,旨在调节vWM能力。总的来说,实验结果表明,与基线相比,两种频率的感官刺激都能显著改善患者的记忆力。然而,与我们的预测相反,7赫兹刺激往往略优于4赫兹刺激。重要的是,观察到的促进作用主要是由低能力亚组参与者驱动的。使用注意力网络测试(ANT)和瞳孔测量法进行的后续实验没有发现证据表明,这种效应可直接归因于对阶段性或强直性唤醒的调节。我们推测,我们的结果与 tACS 的结果不同,是因为我们针对的是功能不同的 Theta 振荡,或者是对参与者时间预期的调节。4赫兹和7赫兹的视觉或听觉刺激提高了视觉工作记忆能力。这种效果在视觉工作记忆能力较低的个体中更为明显。
{"title":"Sensory stimulation enhances visual working memory capacity","authors":"Indre Pileckyte, Salvador Soto-Faraco","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00158-6","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00158-6","url":null,"abstract":"Visual working memory (vWM) plays a crucial role in visual information processing and higher cognitive functions; however, it has a very limited capacity. Recently, several studies have successfully modulated vWM capacity in humans using entrainment with transcranial alternate current stimulation (tACS) by targeting parietal theta in a frequency-specific manner. In the current study, we aim to expand upon these findings by utilizing sensory instead of electrical stimulation. Across six behavioral experiments (combined N = 209), we applied rhythmic visual and auditory sensory stimulation at 4 Hz and 7 Hz, aiming to modulate vWM capacity. Collectively, the results showed an overall robust improvement with sensory stimulation at either frequency, compared to baseline. However, contrary to our prediction, 7 Hz stimulation tended to slightly outperform 4 Hz stimulation. Importantly, the observed facilitatory effect was mainly driven by the low-capacity sub-group of participants. Follow-up experiments using the Attention Network Test (ANT) and pupillometry measures did not find evidence that this effect could be directly attributed to modulation of phasic or tonic arousal. We speculate that our results differed from those obtained with tACS due to targeting functionally different theta oscillations, or the modulation of participants’ temporal expectations. Visual or auditory stimulation at 4 Hz and 7 Hz improved visual working memory performance. This effect was more pronounced in individuals with lower visual working memory capacity.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-18"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-11-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00158-6.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142665215","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Psychoacoustic and Archeoacoustic nature of ancient Aztec skull whistles 古代阿兹台克人头骨哨音的心理声学和考古声学性质
Pub Date : 2024-11-11 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00157-7
Sascha Frühholz, Pablo Rodriguez, Mathilde Bonard, Florence Steiner, Marine Bobin
Many ancient cultures used musical tools for social and ritual procedures, with the Aztec skull whistle being a unique exemplar from postclassic Mesoamerica. Skull whistles can produce softer hiss-like but also aversive and scream-like sounds that were potentially meaningful either for sacrificial practices, mythological symbolism, or intimidating warfare of the Aztecs. However, solid psychoacoustic evidence for any theory is missing, especially how human listeners cognitively and affectively respond to skull whistle sounds. Using psychoacoustic listening and classification experiments, we show that skull whistle sounds are predominantly perceived as aversive and scary and as having a hybrid natural-artificial origin. Skull whistle sounds attract mental attention by affectively mimicking other aversive and startling sounds produced by nature and technology. They were psychoacoustically classified as a hybrid mix of being voice- and scream-like but also originating from technical mechanisms. Using human neuroimaging, we furthermore found that skull whistle sounds received a specific decoding of the affective significance in the neural auditory system of human listeners, accompanied by higher-order auditory cognition and symbolic evaluations in fronto-insular-parietal brain systems. Skull whistles thus seem unique sound tools with specific psycho-affective effects on listeners, and Aztec communities might have capitalized on the scary and scream-like nature of skull whistles. A series of psychoacoustic and neuroimaging studies reveal the effect that the sound of Aztec skull whistles has on modern listeners; the sound, which is perceived as a mixture of voice-like, scream-like, and technological, triggers affective processing.
许多古代文化都将音乐工具用于社会和仪式程序,阿兹台克人的骷髅哨就是后古典中美洲的一个独特范例。骷髅哨声可以发出较柔和的嘶嘶声,也可以发出令人厌恶的尖叫声,这些声音对阿兹特克人的祭祀活动、神话象征或战争恐吓都有潜在意义。然而,任何理论都缺乏可靠的心理声学证据,尤其是人类听众如何对骷髅哨声做出认知和情感反应。通过心理声学聆听和分类实验,我们发现骷髅哨声主要被视为厌恶和可怕的声音,并且具有自然和人工的混合来源。骷髅哨声通过模仿自然和技术产生的其他令人厌恶和惊恐的声音来吸引心理注意。这些声音在心理声学上被归类为声音和尖叫的混合体,但也源自技术机制。通过人类神经影像学研究,我们进一步发现,骷髅口哨声在人类听者的神经听觉系统中具有特定的情感意义解码,并伴随着前脑-岛叶-顶叶大脑系统中的高阶听觉认知和符号评估。因此,骷髅哨声似乎是一种独特的声音工具,对听众具有特定的心理情感效应,阿兹台克人可能利用了骷髅哨声的恐怖和尖叫性质。一系列心理声学和神经影像学研究揭示了阿兹台克人的骷髅哨声对现代听众的影响;这种声音被认为是声音、尖叫和技术的混合体,会引发情感处理。
{"title":"Psychoacoustic and Archeoacoustic nature of ancient Aztec skull whistles","authors":"Sascha Frühholz, Pablo Rodriguez, Mathilde Bonard, Florence Steiner, Marine Bobin","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00157-7","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00157-7","url":null,"abstract":"Many ancient cultures used musical tools for social and ritual procedures, with the Aztec skull whistle being a unique exemplar from postclassic Mesoamerica. Skull whistles can produce softer hiss-like but also aversive and scream-like sounds that were potentially meaningful either for sacrificial practices, mythological symbolism, or intimidating warfare of the Aztecs. However, solid psychoacoustic evidence for any theory is missing, especially how human listeners cognitively and affectively respond to skull whistle sounds. Using psychoacoustic listening and classification experiments, we show that skull whistle sounds are predominantly perceived as aversive and scary and as having a hybrid natural-artificial origin. Skull whistle sounds attract mental attention by affectively mimicking other aversive and startling sounds produced by nature and technology. They were psychoacoustically classified as a hybrid mix of being voice- and scream-like but also originating from technical mechanisms. Using human neuroimaging, we furthermore found that skull whistle sounds received a specific decoding of the affective significance in the neural auditory system of human listeners, accompanied by higher-order auditory cognition and symbolic evaluations in fronto-insular-parietal brain systems. Skull whistles thus seem unique sound tools with specific psycho-affective effects on listeners, and Aztec communities might have capitalized on the scary and scream-like nature of skull whistles. A series of psychoacoustic and neuroimaging studies reveal the effect that the sound of Aztec skull whistles has on modern listeners; the sound, which is perceived as a mixture of voice-like, scream-like, and technological, triggers affective processing.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-14"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-11-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00157-7.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142600831","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Three diverse motives for information sharing 信息共享的三种不同动机。
Pub Date : 2024-11-06 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00144-y
Valentina Vellani, Moshe Glickman, Tali Sharot
Knowledge is distributed over many individuals. Thus, humans are tasked with informing one another for the betterment of all. But as information can alter people’s action, affect and cognition in both positive and negative ways, deciding whether to share information can be a particularly difficult problem. Here, we examine how people integrate potentially conflicting consequences of knowledge, to decide whether to inform others. We show that participants (Exp1: N = 114, Pre-registered replication: N = 102) use their own information-seeking preferences to solve complex information-sharing decisions. In particular, when deciding whether to inform others, participants consider the usefulness of information in directing action, its valence and the receiver’s uncertainty level, and integrate these assessments into a calculation of the value of information that explains information sharing decisions. A cluster analysis revealed that participants were clustered into groups based on the different weights they assign to these three factors. Within individuals, the relative influence of each of these factors was stable across information-seeking and information-sharing decisions. These results suggest that people put themselves in a receiver position to determine whether to inform others and can help predict when people will share information. Individuals consider the usefulness, emotional valence, and prior uncertainty when deciding both when to seek information for themselves and when to share information with others.
知识分布在许多人身上。因此,人类的任务是相互通报信息,以造福全人类。但是,由于信息会以积极或消极的方式改变人们的行动、情感和认知,因此决定是否分享信息可能是一个特别困难的问题。在这里,我们研究了人们如何整合知识可能带来的冲突性后果,以决定是否告知他人。我们发现,参与者(Exp1: N = 114, Pre-registered replication: N = 102)会利用自身的信息搜寻偏好来解决复杂的信息共享决策。特别是,在决定是否告知他人时,参与者会考虑信息对指导行动的有用性、信息的价值和接收者的不确定性水平,并将这些评估整合到信息价值的计算中,从而解释信息共享决策。聚类分析显示,根据参与者对这三个因素所赋予的不同权重,他们被分为不同的组别。在个体内部,每个因素的相对影响力在信息寻求和信息共享决策中都是稳定的。这些结果表明,人们在决定是否告知他人时,会将自己置于接收者的位置,这有助于预测人们何时会分享信息。
{"title":"Three diverse motives for information sharing","authors":"Valentina Vellani, Moshe Glickman, Tali Sharot","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00144-y","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00144-y","url":null,"abstract":"Knowledge is distributed over many individuals. Thus, humans are tasked with informing one another for the betterment of all. But as information can alter people’s action, affect and cognition in both positive and negative ways, deciding whether to share information can be a particularly difficult problem. Here, we examine how people integrate potentially conflicting consequences of knowledge, to decide whether to inform others. We show that participants (Exp1: N = 114, Pre-registered replication: N = 102) use their own information-seeking preferences to solve complex information-sharing decisions. In particular, when deciding whether to inform others, participants consider the usefulness of information in directing action, its valence and the receiver’s uncertainty level, and integrate these assessments into a calculation of the value of information that explains information sharing decisions. A cluster analysis revealed that participants were clustered into groups based on the different weights they assign to these three factors. Within individuals, the relative influence of each of these factors was stable across information-seeking and information-sharing decisions. These results suggest that people put themselves in a receiver position to determine whether to inform others and can help predict when people will share information. Individuals consider the usefulness, emotional valence, and prior uncertainty when deciding both when to seek information for themselves and when to share information with others.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-10"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-11-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11541573/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142592476","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
People are increasingly bored in our digital age 在数字时代,人们越来越感到无聊。
Pub Date : 2024-11-03 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00155-9
Katy Y. Y. Tam, Michael Inzlicht
In an era where entertainment is effortlessly at our fingertips, one would assume that people are less bored than ever. Yet, reports of boredom are higher now than compared to the past. This rising trend is concerning because chronic boredom can undermine well-being, learning, and behaviour. Understanding why this is happening is crucial to prevent further negative impacts. In this Perspective, we explore one possible reason—digital media use makes people more bored. We propose that digital media increases boredom through dividing attention, elevating desired level of engagement, reducing sense of meaning, heightening opportunity costs, and serving as an ineffective boredom coping strategy. In recent years, there has been an increase in both reports of boredom and greater use of digital media. Digital media may exacerbate boredom via multiple pathways including dividing attention and reducing sense of meaning.
在这个娱乐活动唾手可得的时代,人们应该比以往任何时候都更少感到无聊。然而,与过去相比,现在关于无聊的报告却更多了。这种上升趋势令人担忧,因为长期感到无聊会损害身心健康、学习和行为。了解出现这种情况的原因对于防止进一步的负面影响至关重要。在本《视角》中,我们将探讨一个可能的原因--数字媒体的使用使人们更加无聊。我们提出,数字媒体会通过分散注意力、提高期望的参与度、降低意义感、增加机会成本以及作为一种无效的无聊应对策略来增加无聊感。
{"title":"People are increasingly bored in our digital age","authors":"Katy Y. Y. Tam, Michael Inzlicht","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00155-9","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00155-9","url":null,"abstract":"In an era where entertainment is effortlessly at our fingertips, one would assume that people are less bored than ever. Yet, reports of boredom are higher now than compared to the past. This rising trend is concerning because chronic boredom can undermine well-being, learning, and behaviour. Understanding why this is happening is crucial to prevent further negative impacts. In this Perspective, we explore one possible reason—digital media use makes people more bored. We propose that digital media increases boredom through dividing attention, elevating desired level of engagement, reducing sense of meaning, heightening opportunity costs, and serving as an ineffective boredom coping strategy. In recent years, there has been an increase in both reports of boredom and greater use of digital media. Digital media may exacerbate boredom via multiple pathways including dividing attention and reducing sense of meaning.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-9"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-11-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11532334/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142570881","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Communications Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1