首页 > 最新文献

Communications Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Intentional looks facilitate faster responding in observers 有意的注视有助于观察者更快地做出反应
Pub Date : 2024-09-30 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00137-x
Florence Mayrand, Sarah D. McCrackin, Jelena Ristic
Humans construct rich representations of other people’s mental states. Here we investigated how intentionality in eye gaze affected perception and responses to gaze. Observers viewed videos of human gazers looking left or right. Unbeknownst to the observers, the gazers could either choose where to look (self-chosen gaze) or were explicitly instructed where to look (computer-instructed gaze). In Experiment 1, observers reported the direction of the gazer’s upcoming look before the eye movement was initiated. Faster responses were found for self-chosen relative to computer-instructed gaze. In Experiments 2 and 3, observers responded by reporting the location of a peripheral target that appeared at the gazed-at or not gazed-at location. Faster responses were found for gazed-at relative to not gazed-at targets and at longer cue-target intervals for self-chosen relative to computer-instructed gaze. The examination of the eye movement kinematics indicated that self-chosen gaze shifts were marked by a larger magnitude of motion within the eye region prior to the eye movement occurring relative to computer-instructed ones. Thus, perceived intentionality in eye gaze facilitates responses in observers with the information about mental states communicated via subtle properties of eye motion. When observing individuals who either chose or were instructed where to direct their gaze, naïve observers were quicker to respond to self-chosen gazes.
人类构建了丰富的他人心理状态表征。在这里,我们研究了眼睛注视的有意性如何影响对注视的感知和反应。观察者观看了人类注视者向左或向右注视的视频。在观察者不知情的情况下,注视者可以选择注视的方向(自选注视),也可以明确指示注视的方向(计算机指示注视)。在实验 1 中,观察者在启动眼球运动之前报告了注视者即将注视的方向。结果发现,相对于计算机指导的注视,自我选择的注视反应更快。在实验 2 和 3 中,观察者通过报告出现在注视或未注视位置的外围目标的位置来做出反应。结果发现,相对于不注视目标,注视目标的反应速度更快;相对于计算机引导的注视,自选目标的反应速度更快。对眼球运动运动学的研究表明,相对于计算机指导的眼球运动,自我选择的注视移动在眼球运动发生前眼球区域内的运动幅度更大。因此,眼球注视的感知意向性有助于观察者通过眼球运动的微妙特性传达的心理状态信息做出反应。在观察那些选择或被指示将目光投向何处的个体时,天真的观察者会更快地对自我选择的目光做出反应。
{"title":"Intentional looks facilitate faster responding in observers","authors":"Florence Mayrand, Sarah D. McCrackin, Jelena Ristic","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00137-x","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00137-x","url":null,"abstract":"Humans construct rich representations of other people’s mental states. Here we investigated how intentionality in eye gaze affected perception and responses to gaze. Observers viewed videos of human gazers looking left or right. Unbeknownst to the observers, the gazers could either choose where to look (self-chosen gaze) or were explicitly instructed where to look (computer-instructed gaze). In Experiment 1, observers reported the direction of the gazer’s upcoming look before the eye movement was initiated. Faster responses were found for self-chosen relative to computer-instructed gaze. In Experiments 2 and 3, observers responded by reporting the location of a peripheral target that appeared at the gazed-at or not gazed-at location. Faster responses were found for gazed-at relative to not gazed-at targets and at longer cue-target intervals for self-chosen relative to computer-instructed gaze. The examination of the eye movement kinematics indicated that self-chosen gaze shifts were marked by a larger magnitude of motion within the eye region prior to the eye movement occurring relative to computer-instructed ones. Thus, perceived intentionality in eye gaze facilitates responses in observers with the information about mental states communicated via subtle properties of eye motion. When observing individuals who either chose or were instructed where to direct their gaze, naïve observers were quicker to respond to self-chosen gazes.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-8"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00137-x.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142329452","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Egalitarian preferences in young children depend on the genders of the interacting partners 幼儿的平等主义偏好取决于互动伙伴的性别
Pub Date : 2024-09-25 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00139-9
Marijn van Wingerden, Lina Oberließen, Tobias Kalenscher
In decisions between equal and unequal resource distributions, women are often believed to be more prosocial than men. Previous research showed that fairness attitudes develop in childhood, but their—possibly gendered, developmental trajectory remains unclear. We hypothesised that gender-related fairness attitudes might depend not only on the gender of the Allocator, but also on that of the Recipient. To examine this, we tested 332 three to 8-year-old children in a paired resource allocation task, with both boys and girls acting as Allocators and Recipients. We indeed found gender-related effects: girls more than boys aimed to reduce advantageous inequity, and Allocators of both genders were more averse against male Recipients being better off. Notably, older girls exhibited an envy bias, i.e., they tolerated disadvantageous inequity more when the resource allocation was in favour of other girls than when it favoured boys. We also observed a gender-related spite gap in boys aged 7-8: unlike girls, boys treated other boys with spite, i.e., they valued unfair distributions in their own favour over equal outcomes, especially if rejecting advantageous inequity was costly. This pattern hints at contextualised gender-related fairness preferences that evolve with age that could depend on same- and cross-gender past interaction experiences. Gender-related differences in resource distribution depend on both the gender of the Allocator and of the Recipient. Girls tried to reduce advantageous inequality more than boys but tolerated disadvantageous inequity more if it favoured another girl whereas boys were more competitive with other boys.
在平等和不平等的资源分配决策中,人们通常认为女性比男性更亲社会。以往的研究表明,公平态度在儿童时期就已经形成,但其可能与性别有关的发展轨迹仍不清楚。我们假设,与性别相关的公平态度可能不仅取决于分配者的性别,还取决于接受者的性别。为了研究这一点,我们对 332 名三至八岁的儿童进行了配对资源分配任务测试,由男孩和女孩分别担任分配者和接受者。我们确实发现了与性别相关的效应:女孩比男孩更希望减少有利的不公平现象,而男女分配者都更不希望男性接受者过得更好。值得注意的是,年龄较大的女孩表现出一种嫉妒偏差,即当资源分配有利于其他女孩时,她们对不利的不公平现象的容忍度要高于有利于男孩的情况。我们还在 7-8 岁的男孩身上观察到了与性别相关的怨恨差距:与女孩不同的是,男孩对待其他男孩的态度是怨恨的,也就是说,他们更看重对自己有利的不公平分配,而不是平等的结果,尤其是在拒绝有利的不公平分配需要付出代价的情况下。这种模式表明,与性别相关的公平偏好会随着年龄的增长而发生变化,这可能取决于同性别和跨性别的过往互动经验。资源分配中与性别相关的差异取决于分配者和接受者的性别。女孩比男孩更努力减少有利的不公平现象,但如果不利的不公平现象有利于另一个女孩,女孩则更能容忍,而男孩则更喜欢与其他男孩竞争。
{"title":"Egalitarian preferences in young children depend on the genders of the interacting partners","authors":"Marijn van Wingerden, Lina Oberließen, Tobias Kalenscher","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00139-9","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00139-9","url":null,"abstract":"In decisions between equal and unequal resource distributions, women are often believed to be more prosocial than men. Previous research showed that fairness attitudes develop in childhood, but their—possibly gendered, developmental trajectory remains unclear. We hypothesised that gender-related fairness attitudes might depend not only on the gender of the Allocator, but also on that of the Recipient. To examine this, we tested 332 three to 8-year-old children in a paired resource allocation task, with both boys and girls acting as Allocators and Recipients. We indeed found gender-related effects: girls more than boys aimed to reduce advantageous inequity, and Allocators of both genders were more averse against male Recipients being better off. Notably, older girls exhibited an envy bias, i.e., they tolerated disadvantageous inequity more when the resource allocation was in favour of other girls than when it favoured boys. We also observed a gender-related spite gap in boys aged 7-8: unlike girls, boys treated other boys with spite, i.e., they valued unfair distributions in their own favour over equal outcomes, especially if rejecting advantageous inequity was costly. This pattern hints at contextualised gender-related fairness preferences that evolve with age that could depend on same- and cross-gender past interaction experiences. Gender-related differences in resource distribution depend on both the gender of the Allocator and of the Recipient. Girls tried to reduce advantageous inequality more than boys but tolerated disadvantageous inequity more if it favoured another girl whereas boys were more competitive with other boys.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-17"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00139-9.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142324785","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Older adults are relatively more susceptible to impulsive social influence than young adults 相对来说,老年人比年轻人更容易受到冲动的社会影响。
Pub Date : 2024-09-23 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00134-0
Zhilin Su, Mona M. Garvert, Lei Zhang, Sanjay G. Manohar, Todd A. Vogel, Louisa Thomas, Joshua H. Balsters, Masud Husain, Matthew A. J. Apps, Patricia L. Lockwood
People differ in their levels of impulsivity and patience, and these preferences are heavily influenced by others. Previous research suggests that susceptibility to social influence may vary with age, but the mechanisms and whether people are more influenced by patience or impulsivity remain unknown. Here, using a delegated inter-temporal choice task and Bayesian computational models, we tested susceptibility to social influence in young (aged 18–36, N = 76) and older (aged 60–80, N = 78) adults. Participants completed a temporal discounting task and then learnt the preferences of two other people (one more impulsive and one more patient) before making their choices again. We used the signed Kullback-Leibler divergence to quantify the magnitude and direction of social influence. We found that, compared to young adults, older adults were relatively more susceptible to impulsive social influence. Factor analyses showed that older adults with higher self-reported levels of affective empathy and emotional motivation were particularly susceptible to impulsive influence. Importantly, older and young adults showed similar learning accuracy about others’ preferences, and their baseline impulsivity did not differ. Together, these findings suggest highly affectively empathetic and emotionally motivated older adults may be at higher risk for impulsive decisions, due to their susceptibility to social influence. Older adults were more influenced by impulsive economic decisions made by others in comparison to young adults. More empathetic and emotionally motivated older adults were the most influenced by others’ impulsive economic choices.
人们的冲动和耐心程度各不相同,而这些偏好在很大程度上受他人影响。以往的研究表明,人们对社会影响的敏感性可能会随着年龄的增长而变化,但其机制以及人们更容易受耐心还是冲动的影响仍是未知数。在这里,我们使用委托时际选择任务和贝叶斯计算模型,测试了年轻人(18-36 岁,76 人)和老年人(60-80 岁,78 人)对社会影响的易感性。参与者在完成时间折现任务后,会了解另外两个人(一个更冲动,一个更耐心)的偏好,然后再次做出选择。我们使用带符号的库尔巴克-莱伯勒发散来量化社会影响的大小和方向。我们发现,与年轻人相比,老年人更容易受到冲动型社会影响的影响。因子分析显示,自我报告情感共鸣和情感动机水平较高的老年人特别容易受到冲动性影响。重要的是,老年人和年轻人对他人偏好的学习准确性相似,而且他们的基线冲动性没有差异。总之,这些研究结果表明,具有高度情感共鸣和情感动机的老年人由于容易受到社会影响,做出冲动性决定的风险可能更高。
{"title":"Older adults are relatively more susceptible to impulsive social influence than young adults","authors":"Zhilin Su, Mona M. Garvert, Lei Zhang, Sanjay G. Manohar, Todd A. Vogel, Louisa Thomas, Joshua H. Balsters, Masud Husain, Matthew A. J. Apps, Patricia L. Lockwood","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00134-0","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00134-0","url":null,"abstract":"People differ in their levels of impulsivity and patience, and these preferences are heavily influenced by others. Previous research suggests that susceptibility to social influence may vary with age, but the mechanisms and whether people are more influenced by patience or impulsivity remain unknown. Here, using a delegated inter-temporal choice task and Bayesian computational models, we tested susceptibility to social influence in young (aged 18–36, N = 76) and older (aged 60–80, N = 78) adults. Participants completed a temporal discounting task and then learnt the preferences of two other people (one more impulsive and one more patient) before making their choices again. We used the signed Kullback-Leibler divergence to quantify the magnitude and direction of social influence. We found that, compared to young adults, older adults were relatively more susceptible to impulsive social influence. Factor analyses showed that older adults with higher self-reported levels of affective empathy and emotional motivation were particularly susceptible to impulsive influence. Importantly, older and young adults showed similar learning accuracy about others’ preferences, and their baseline impulsivity did not differ. Together, these findings suggest highly affectively empathetic and emotionally motivated older adults may be at higher risk for impulsive decisions, due to their susceptibility to social influence. Older adults were more influenced by impulsive economic decisions made by others in comparison to young adults. More empathetic and emotionally motivated older adults were the most influenced by others’ impulsive economic choices.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-13"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00134-0.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142309595","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Individual differences in belief updating and phasic arousal are related to psychosis proneness 信念更新和阶段性唤醒的个体差异与精神病易感性有关。
Pub Date : 2024-09-23 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00140-2
Peter R. Murphy, Katarina Krkovic, Gina Monov, Natalia Kudlek, Tania Lincoln, Tobias H. Donner
Many decisions entail the updating of beliefs about the state of the environment by accumulating noisy sensory evidence. This form of probabilistic reasoning may go awry in psychosis. Computational theory shows that optimal belief updating in environments subject to hidden changes in their state requires a dynamic modulation of the evidence accumulation process. Recent empirical findings implicate transient responses of pupil-linked central arousal systems to individual evidence samples in this modulation. Here, we analyzed behavior and pupil responses during evidence accumulation in a changing environment in a community sample of human participants. We also assessed their subclinical psychotic experiences (psychosis proneness). Participants most prone to psychosis showed overall less flexible belief updating profiles, with diminished behavioral impact of evidence samples occurring late during decision formation. These same individuals also exhibited overall smaller pupil responses and less reliable pupil encoding of computational variables governing the dynamic belief updating. Our findings provide insights into the cognitive and physiological bases of psychosis proneness and open paths to unraveling the pathophysiology of psychotic disorders. When making decisions, a non-clinical sample of individuals who were more prone to symptoms of psychosis were less likely to update their beliefs based on late-arriving information. This was associated with weaker decision-related pupil responses.
许多决策都需要通过积累嘈杂的感官证据来更新对环境状态的信念。这种形式的概率推理可能会在精神病中出错。计算理论表明,要在状态发生隐性变化的环境中实现最佳的信念更新,需要对证据积累过程进行动态调节。最近的实证研究发现,与瞳孔相连的中枢唤醒系统对个体证据样本的瞬时反应与这种调节有关。在这里,我们分析了在不断变化的环境中,人类参与者在证据积累过程中的行为和瞳孔反应。我们还评估了他们的亚临床精神病经历(精神病倾向)。最易患精神病的参与者总体上表现出较低的信念更新灵活性,在决策形成的后期,证据样本对行为的影响减弱。这些人还表现出整体较小的瞳孔反应,以及对支配动态信念更新的计算变量较不可靠的瞳孔编码。我们的研究结果为了解精神病易感性的认知和生理基础提供了见解,并为揭示精神病性障碍的病理生理学开辟了道路。
{"title":"Individual differences in belief updating and phasic arousal are related to psychosis proneness","authors":"Peter R. Murphy, Katarina Krkovic, Gina Monov, Natalia Kudlek, Tania Lincoln, Tobias H. Donner","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00140-2","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00140-2","url":null,"abstract":"Many decisions entail the updating of beliefs about the state of the environment by accumulating noisy sensory evidence. This form of probabilistic reasoning may go awry in psychosis. Computational theory shows that optimal belief updating in environments subject to hidden changes in their state requires a dynamic modulation of the evidence accumulation process. Recent empirical findings implicate transient responses of pupil-linked central arousal systems to individual evidence samples in this modulation. Here, we analyzed behavior and pupil responses during evidence accumulation in a changing environment in a community sample of human participants. We also assessed their subclinical psychotic experiences (psychosis proneness). Participants most prone to psychosis showed overall less flexible belief updating profiles, with diminished behavioral impact of evidence samples occurring late during decision formation. These same individuals also exhibited overall smaller pupil responses and less reliable pupil encoding of computational variables governing the dynamic belief updating. Our findings provide insights into the cognitive and physiological bases of psychosis proneness and open paths to unraveling the pathophysiology of psychotic disorders. When making decisions, a non-clinical sample of individuals who were more prone to symptoms of psychosis were less likely to update their beliefs based on late-arriving information. This was associated with weaker decision-related pupil responses.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-14"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00140-2.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142309594","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Diminished pain sensitivity mediates the relationship between psychopathic traits and reduced learning from pain 疼痛敏感性降低是精神变态特质与痛觉学习能力降低之间关系的介导因素
Pub Date : 2024-09-14 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00133-1
Dimana V. Atanassova, Christoph Mathys, Andreea O. Diaconescu, Victor I. Madariaga, Joukje M. Oosterman, Inti A. Brazil
Individuals with elevated psychopathic traits exhibit decision-making deficits linked to a failure to learn from negative outcomes. We investigated how reduced pain sensitivity affects reinforcement-based decision-making in individuals with varying levels of psychopathic traits, as measured by the Self-Report Psychopathy Scale-Short Form. Using computational modelling, we estimated the latent cognitive processes in a community non-offender sample (n = 111) that completed a task with choices leading to painful and non-painful outcomes. Higher psychopathic traits were associated with reduced pain sensitivity and disturbances in reinforcement learning from painful outcomes. In a Structural Equation Model, a superordinate psychopathy factor was associated with a faster return to original stimulus-outcome associations as pain tolerance increased. This provides evidence directly linking reduced pain sensitivity and learning from painful outcomes with elevated psychopathic traits. Our results offer insights into the computational mechanisms of maladaptive decision-making in psychopathy and antisocial behavior. Higher levels of psychopathic traits were associated with reduced pain sensitivity as well as a greater tendency to ignore new evidence and maintain prior expectations in pain learning situations.
具有高度精神变态特质的个体会表现出决策缺陷,这与他们无法从负面结果中吸取教训有关。我们研究了疼痛敏感性降低如何影响具有不同程度精神变态特质的个体基于强化的决策,这些特质是通过精神变态自评量表-简表来测量的。通过计算建模,我们估算了社区非罪犯样本(n = 111)的潜在认知过程,他们在完成一项任务时,会选择痛苦和非痛苦的结果。较高的精神变态特质与疼痛敏感性降低以及从痛苦结果中强化学习的干扰有关。在结构方程模型中,随着疼痛耐受性的增加,心理变态的上位因子与更快地恢复原刺激-结果关联有关。这提供了将疼痛敏感性降低和从痛苦结果中学习与精神变态特质升高直接联系起来的证据。我们的研究结果提供了对精神变态和反社会行为中适应不良决策的计算机制的见解。较高水平的精神变态特质与疼痛敏感性降低以及在疼痛学习情境中更倾向于忽视新证据和保持先前预期有关。
{"title":"Diminished pain sensitivity mediates the relationship between psychopathic traits and reduced learning from pain","authors":"Dimana V. Atanassova, Christoph Mathys, Andreea O. Diaconescu, Victor I. Madariaga, Joukje M. Oosterman, Inti A. Brazil","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00133-1","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00133-1","url":null,"abstract":"Individuals with elevated psychopathic traits exhibit decision-making deficits linked to a failure to learn from negative outcomes. We investigated how reduced pain sensitivity affects reinforcement-based decision-making in individuals with varying levels of psychopathic traits, as measured by the Self-Report Psychopathy Scale-Short Form. Using computational modelling, we estimated the latent cognitive processes in a community non-offender sample (n = 111) that completed a task with choices leading to painful and non-painful outcomes. Higher psychopathic traits were associated with reduced pain sensitivity and disturbances in reinforcement learning from painful outcomes. In a Structural Equation Model, a superordinate psychopathy factor was associated with a faster return to original stimulus-outcome associations as pain tolerance increased. This provides evidence directly linking reduced pain sensitivity and learning from painful outcomes with elevated psychopathic traits. Our results offer insights into the computational mechanisms of maladaptive decision-making in psychopathy and antisocial behavior. Higher levels of psychopathic traits were associated with reduced pain sensitivity as well as a greater tendency to ignore new evidence and maintain prior expectations in pain learning situations.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-12"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00133-1.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142233240","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cognitive neuroscience: the brain’s symphony in hearing speech and music 认知神经科学:大脑聆听语言和音乐的交响乐
Pub Date : 2024-09-13 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00136-y
Troby Ka-Yan Lui
New research shows that the brain employs similar anatomical regions but specific neural oscillatory patterns during speech and music perception.
新的研究表明,在感知语音和音乐时,大脑会使用相似的解剖区域和特定的神经振荡模式。
{"title":"Cognitive neuroscience: the brain’s symphony in hearing speech and music","authors":"Troby Ka-Yan Lui","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00136-y","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00136-y","url":null,"abstract":"New research shows that the brain employs similar anatomical regions but specific neural oscillatory patterns during speech and music perception.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-1"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00136-y.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142231146","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A behavioral signature for quantifying the social value of interpersonal relationships with specific others 量化与特定他人的人际关系的社会价值的行为特征。
Pub Date : 2024-09-06 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00132-2
João F. Guassi Moreira, Carolyn Parkinson
The idea that individuals ascribe value to social phenomena, broadly construed, is well-established. Despite the ubiquity of this concept, defining social value in the context of interpersonal relationships remains elusive. This is notable because while prominent theories of human social behavior acknowledge the role of value-based processes, they mostly emphasize the value of individual actions an agent may choose to take in a given environment. Comparatively little is known about how humans value their interpersonal relationships. To address this, we devised a method for engineering a behavioral signature of social value in several independent samples (total N = 1111). Incorporating the concept of opportunity cost from economics and data-driven quantitative methods, we derived this signature by sourcing and weighting a range of social behaviors based on how likely individuals are to prioritize them in the face of limited resources. We examined how strongly the signature was expressed in self-reported social behaviors with specific relationship partners (a parent, close friend, and acquaintance). Social value scores track with other aspects of these relationships (e.g., relationship quality, aversion to losing relationship partners), are predictive of decision preferences on a range of tasks, and display good psychometric properties. These results provide greater mechanistic specificity in delineating human value-based behavior in social contexts and help parse the motivational relevance of the different facets that comprise interpersonal relationships. The social value of interpersonal relationships was quantified by participants reporting the likelihood of engaging in various free time activities with different social partners and then applying a model-based signature of individual activity value to these ratings. The ensuing scores correlated with self-reported relationship quality and social loss aversion.
从广义上讲,个人赋予社会现象以价值,这一观点已经深入人心。尽管这一概念无处不在,但在人际关系中如何定义社会价值却仍然难以捉摸。值得注意的是,尽管著名的人类社会行为理论承认基于价值的过程的作用,但它们大多强调个体在特定环境中可能选择采取的行动的价值。相对而言,人们对人类如何重视人际关系知之甚少。为了解决这个问题,我们设计了一种方法,在多个独立样本(总样本数 = 1111)中设计出社会价值的行为特征。结合经济学中的机会成本概念和数据驱动的定量方法,我们根据个人在面对有限资源时优先考虑社交行为的可能性,对一系列社交行为进行了筛选和加权,从而得出了这一特征。我们研究了该特征在与特定关系伙伴(父母、密友和熟人)的自我报告社交行为中的表现强度。社会价值得分与这些关系的其他方面(如关系质量、对失去关系伙伴的厌恶)相一致,可预测一系列任务中的决策偏好,并显示出良好的心理测量特性。这些结果为描述人类在社会环境中以价值为基础的行为提供了更高的机制特异性,并有助于分析构成人际关系的不同方面的动机相关性。
{"title":"A behavioral signature for quantifying the social value of interpersonal relationships with specific others","authors":"João F. Guassi Moreira, Carolyn Parkinson","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00132-2","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00132-2","url":null,"abstract":"The idea that individuals ascribe value to social phenomena, broadly construed, is well-established. Despite the ubiquity of this concept, defining social value in the context of interpersonal relationships remains elusive. This is notable because while prominent theories of human social behavior acknowledge the role of value-based processes, they mostly emphasize the value of individual actions an agent may choose to take in a given environment. Comparatively little is known about how humans value their interpersonal relationships. To address this, we devised a method for engineering a behavioral signature of social value in several independent samples (total N = 1111). Incorporating the concept of opportunity cost from economics and data-driven quantitative methods, we derived this signature by sourcing and weighting a range of social behaviors based on how likely individuals are to prioritize them in the face of limited resources. We examined how strongly the signature was expressed in self-reported social behaviors with specific relationship partners (a parent, close friend, and acquaintance). Social value scores track with other aspects of these relationships (e.g., relationship quality, aversion to losing relationship partners), are predictive of decision preferences on a range of tasks, and display good psychometric properties. These results provide greater mechanistic specificity in delineating human value-based behavior in social contexts and help parse the motivational relevance of the different facets that comprise interpersonal relationships. The social value of interpersonal relationships was quantified by participants reporting the likelihood of engaging in various free time activities with different social partners and then applying a model-based signature of individual activity value to these ratings. The ensuing scores correlated with self-reported relationship quality and social loss aversion.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-17"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00132-2.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142147302","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Science convergence in affective research is associated with impactful multidisciplinary appeal rather than multidisciplinary content 情感研究中的科学融合与具有影响力的多学科吸引力而非多学科内容有关
Pub Date : 2024-09-04 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00129-x
Vitalii Zhukov, Alexander M. Petersen, Daniel Dukes, David Sander, Panagiotis Tsiamyrtzis, Ioannis Pavlidis
Affectivism is a research trend dedicated to the study of emotions and their role in cognition and human behavior. Affectivism both complements and competes with cognitivism, which typically neglects affect in explaining behavior. By the nature of their subject, both affectivism and cognitivism constitute fertile grounds for studying the confluence of conceptual knowledge from diverse disciplines, which is often credited with major breakthroughs and is known as convergence science. Analyzing over half a million relevant publications from PubMed, selected according to psychologist chosen MeSH terms, we find that affectivism yields higher impact than cognitivism, as measured through normalized citations. Importantly, this higher impact is strongly associated with higher multidisciplinarity in the citations of affectivism publications but lower multidisciplinarity in the papers themselves. Hence, the case of affectivism suggests that research content of low topical diversity but broad value can generate strong and wide-ranging scholarly impact, feeding downstream convergence. Affective research generates more diverse citations that cover a higher variety of research fields when compared to cognitive research. This occurs despite a more narrow focus of topics included in the original affective articles themselves
情感主义是一种研究趋势,致力于研究情感及其在认知和人类行为中的作用。情感主义与认知主义既互补又竞争,后者在解释行为时通常会忽视情感。由于其研究对象的性质,情感主义和认知主义都是研究不同学科概念知识融合的沃土。通过分析根据心理学家选择的 MeSH 术语从 PubMed 上选取的 50 多万篇相关出版物,我们发现情感主义比认知主义产生了更大的影响(通过归一化引文来衡量)。重要的是,这种较高的影响力与情感主义出版物被引用时较高的多学科性密切相关,但论文本身的多学科性较低。因此,情感主义的案例表明,专题多样性较低但价值广泛的研究内容可以产生强大而广泛的学术影响,促进下游的融合。与认知研究相比,情感研究产生了更多样化的引文,涵盖了更多的研究领域。尽管原创情感文章本身所包含的主题范围较窄,但仍出现了这种情况
{"title":"Science convergence in affective research is associated with impactful multidisciplinary appeal rather than multidisciplinary content","authors":"Vitalii Zhukov, Alexander M. Petersen, Daniel Dukes, David Sander, Panagiotis Tsiamyrtzis, Ioannis Pavlidis","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00129-x","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00129-x","url":null,"abstract":"Affectivism is a research trend dedicated to the study of emotions and their role in cognition and human behavior. Affectivism both complements and competes with cognitivism, which typically neglects affect in explaining behavior. By the nature of their subject, both affectivism and cognitivism constitute fertile grounds for studying the confluence of conceptual knowledge from diverse disciplines, which is often credited with major breakthroughs and is known as convergence science. Analyzing over half a million relevant publications from PubMed, selected according to psychologist chosen MeSH terms, we find that affectivism yields higher impact than cognitivism, as measured through normalized citations. Importantly, this higher impact is strongly associated with higher multidisciplinarity in the citations of affectivism publications but lower multidisciplinarity in the papers themselves. Hence, the case of affectivism suggests that research content of low topical diversity but broad value can generate strong and wide-ranging scholarly impact, feeding downstream convergence. Affective research generates more diverse citations that cover a higher variety of research fields when compared to cognitive research. This occurs despite a more narrow focus of topics included in the original affective articles themselves","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-9"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00129-x.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142130474","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A systematic review and Bayesian meta-analysis provide evidence for an effect of acute physical activity on cognition in young adults 系统综述和贝叶斯荟萃分析为急性体育锻炼对青壮年认知能力的影响提供了证据
Pub Date : 2024-08-28 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00124-2
Jordan Garrett, Carly Chak, Tom Bullock, Barry Giesbrecht
Physical exercise is a potential intervention for enhancing cognitive function across the lifespan. However, while studies employing long-term exercise interventions consistently show positive effects on cognition, studies using single acute bouts have produced mixed results. Here, a systematic review and meta-analysis was conducted to determine the impact of acute exercise on cognitive task performance in healthy young adults. A Bayesian hierarchical model quantified probabilistic evidence for a modulatory relationship by synthesizing 651 effect sizes from 113 studies from PsychInfo and Google Scholar representing 4,390 participants. Publication bias was mitigated using the trim-and-fill method. Acute exercise was found to have a small beneficial effect on cognition (g = 0.13 ± 0.04; BF = 3.67) and decrease reaction time. A meta-analysis restricted to executive function tasks revealed improvements in working memory and inhibition. Meta-analytic estimates were consistent across multiple priors and likelihood functions. Physical activities were categorized based on exercise type (e.g., cycling) because many activities have aerobic and anaerobic components, but this approach may limit comparison to studies that categorize activities based on metabolic demands. The current study provides an updated synthesis of the existing literature and insights into the robustness of acute exercise-induced effects on cognition. Funding provided by the United States Army Research Office. A single instance of exercise improves cognitive task performance especially in regard to reaction time. Cycling and high-intensity interval training (HIIT) were found to be particularly beneficial.
体育锻炼是增强人一生认知功能的潜在干预措施。然而,虽然采用长期运动干预的研究一直显示出对认知的积极影响,但采用单次急性运动的研究结果却好坏参半。在此,我们进行了一项系统回顾和荟萃分析,以确定急性运动对健康年轻人认知任务表现的影响。贝叶斯层次模型通过综合来自PsychInfo和谷歌学术的113项研究的651个效应大小(代表4390名参与者),量化了调节关系的概率证据。采用修剪填充法减轻了发表偏差。研究发现,急性运动对认知能力(g = 0.13 ± 0.04; BF = 3.67)和反应时间的减少有微小的益处。仅限于执行功能任务的荟萃分析显示,工作记忆和抑制能力有所改善。元分析估计值在多个先验和似然函数中保持一致。体育活动根据运动类型(如骑自行车)进行分类,因为许多活动都有有氧和无氧成分,但这种方法可能会限制与根据代谢需求对活动进行分类的研究进行比较。目前的研究对现有文献进行了最新综述,并深入探讨了急性运动对认知影响的稳健性。美国陆军研究办公室提供资助。单次运动可提高认知任务的表现,尤其是在反应时间方面。研究发现,骑自行车和高强度间歇训练(HIIT)尤其有益。
{"title":"A systematic review and Bayesian meta-analysis provide evidence for an effect of acute physical activity on cognition in young adults","authors":"Jordan Garrett, Carly Chak, Tom Bullock, Barry Giesbrecht","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00124-2","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00124-2","url":null,"abstract":"Physical exercise is a potential intervention for enhancing cognitive function across the lifespan. However, while studies employing long-term exercise interventions consistently show positive effects on cognition, studies using single acute bouts have produced mixed results. Here, a systematic review and meta-analysis was conducted to determine the impact of acute exercise on cognitive task performance in healthy young adults. A Bayesian hierarchical model quantified probabilistic evidence for a modulatory relationship by synthesizing 651 effect sizes from 113 studies from PsychInfo and Google Scholar representing 4,390 participants. Publication bias was mitigated using the trim-and-fill method. Acute exercise was found to have a small beneficial effect on cognition (g = 0.13 ± 0.04; BF = 3.67) and decrease reaction time. A meta-analysis restricted to executive function tasks revealed improvements in working memory and inhibition. Meta-analytic estimates were consistent across multiple priors and likelihood functions. Physical activities were categorized based on exercise type (e.g., cycling) because many activities have aerobic and anaerobic components, but this approach may limit comparison to studies that categorize activities based on metabolic demands. The current study provides an updated synthesis of the existing literature and insights into the robustness of acute exercise-induced effects on cognition. Funding provided by the United States Army Research Office. A single instance of exercise improves cognitive task performance especially in regard to reaction time. Cycling and high-intensity interval training (HIIT) were found to be particularly beneficial.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-21"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00124-2.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142123434","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Model-free decision-making underlies motor errors in rapid sequential movements under threat 无模型决策是威胁下快速连续运动中出现运动错误的基础
Pub Date : 2024-08-27 DOI: 10.1038/s44271-024-00123-3
Pranav Sankhe, Masahiko Haruno
Our movements, especially sequential ones, are usually goal-directed, i.e., coupled with task-level goals. Consequently, cognitive strategies for decision-making and motor performance are likely to influence each other. However, evidence linking decision-making strategies and motor performance remains elusive. Here, we designed a modified version of the two-step task, named the two-step sequential movement task, where participants had to conduct rapid sequential finger movements to obtain rewards (n = 40). In the shock session, participants received an electrical shock if they made an erroneous or slow movement, while in the no-shock session, they only received zero reward. We found that participants who prioritised model-free decision-making committed more motor errors in the presence of the shock stimulus (shock sessions) than those who prioritised model-based decision-making. Using a mediation analysis, we also revealed a strong link between the balance of the model-based and the model-free learning strategies and sequential movement performances. These results suggested that model-free decision-making produces more motor errors than model-based decision-making in rapid sequential movements under the threat of stressful stimuli. Reinforcement learning strategies and motor performance are linked. Participants show poorer motor performance when they adopt or shift towards a model-free strategy under threat of electric shocks than when they use a model-based strategy.
我们的运动,尤其是连续运动,通常都是以目标为导向的,即与任务层面的目标相结合。因此,决策的认知策略和运动表现很可能会相互影响。然而,将决策策略和运动表现联系起来的证据仍然缺乏。在这里,我们设计了一个改良版的两步任务,命名为两步连续运动任务,参与者必须进行快速的手指连续运动以获得奖励(n = 40)。在电击环节,如果参与者做出错误或缓慢的动作,就会受到电击;而在无电击环节,参与者只能获得零奖励。我们发现,在电击刺激下(电击环节),优先考虑无模型决策的参与者比优先考虑基于模型决策的参与者犯了更多的运动错误。通过中介分析,我们还发现基于模型和无模型学习策略的平衡与连续运动表现之间存在密切联系。这些结果表明,在压力刺激的威胁下进行快速连续动作时,无模型决策比有模型决策会产生更多的运动错误。强化学习策略与运动表现有关。与使用基于模型的策略相比,受试者在电击威胁下采用或转向无模型策略时的运动表现更差。
{"title":"Model-free decision-making underlies motor errors in rapid sequential movements under threat","authors":"Pranav Sankhe, Masahiko Haruno","doi":"10.1038/s44271-024-00123-3","DOIUrl":"10.1038/s44271-024-00123-3","url":null,"abstract":"Our movements, especially sequential ones, are usually goal-directed, i.e., coupled with task-level goals. Consequently, cognitive strategies for decision-making and motor performance are likely to influence each other. However, evidence linking decision-making strategies and motor performance remains elusive. Here, we designed a modified version of the two-step task, named the two-step sequential movement task, where participants had to conduct rapid sequential finger movements to obtain rewards (n = 40). In the shock session, participants received an electrical shock if they made an erroneous or slow movement, while in the no-shock session, they only received zero reward. We found that participants who prioritised model-free decision-making committed more motor errors in the presence of the shock stimulus (shock sessions) than those who prioritised model-based decision-making. Using a mediation analysis, we also revealed a strong link between the balance of the model-based and the model-free learning strategies and sequential movement performances. These results suggested that model-free decision-making produces more motor errors than model-based decision-making in rapid sequential movements under the threat of stressful stimuli. Reinforcement learning strategies and motor performance are linked. Participants show poorer motor performance when they adopt or shift towards a model-free strategy under threat of electric shocks than when they use a model-based strategy.","PeriodicalId":501698,"journal":{"name":"Communications Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1-10"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.nature.com/articles/s44271-024-00123-3.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142123433","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Communications Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1