首页 > 最新文献

Ethology最新文献

英文 中文
A Butterfly's Flash Coloration Distracts Predators—Read Future Textbook Knowledge in Ethology 蝴蝶的闪光色彩能分散捕食者的注意力--阅读未来的《选育学》教科书知识
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-07 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13520
Wolfgang Goymann
<p>A highlight of visiting a tropical rainforest is watching iridescent butterflies passing by, flashing their colours in the sunlight that makes it to the forest floor. It can be quite tricky to keep track of such a butterfly because typically only the upper side of the wings is iridescent, so that the colour only flashes when this side of a wing can be seen. People think that this kind of moving flash coloration makes it difficult for visual predators to follow the flight path of the butterfly. Hence, iridescent colours may help butterflies to distract predators. Computer simulations and experiments with humans as ‘predators’ suggest that this may be true, but as of now the flash colouration hypothesis had not been tested for real.</p><p>In this issue of Ethology, Vieira-Silva et al. (<span>2024</span>) conducted a series of clever experiments to finally test the flash colouration hypothesis in <i>Morpho helena</i> butterflies. This species displays an iridescent blue colour on the upper side of its wings (as can be seen on this issues' cover image) that flashes when they move through the forest.</p><p>In a first experiment, the authors painted the cryptic underside of the wings with a colour mimicking the iridescent blue of the upper side of the wing. Thereby, the butterflies become more constantly visible during flight, because now the blue colour is exposed all of the time. The underside of the wings of a control group got painted with a brown colour similar to the original cryptic colour, thereby controlling for the effect of catching and painting the butterflies. In a capture–recapture analysis, Vieira-Silva et al. (<span>2024</span>) found that blue-coloured butterflies were less likely to be seen again compared to the brown-coloured control individuals. Because <i>Morpho helena</i> butterflies do not move around far, the most parsimonious explanation for the lower resighting rates of the blue-coloured butterflies is that a higher proportion of them got eaten by predators.</p><p>In a second experiment, Vieira-Silva et al. (<span>2024</span>) tested if an overall cryptic colour as such would have reduced predation. To test this, they coloured the upper side of the wing—which normally has the blue flashing colouration—with brown colour, so that the butterflies become completely cryptic during flight. This treatment, however, did not affect recapture rates, suggesting that completely cryptic butterflies did not have an advantage over individuals that flashed their blue colour during flight. Hence, the distraction effect of a flashing blue colour likely has a similar effect than complete crypsis.</p><p>To show that the higher predation of butterflies with an underside coloured in blue was really due to moving butterflies and not because such butterflies became generally more visible to predators, the authors conducted a third experiment. To test if blue colouration affected predation in non-moving butterflies, they compared how likely dead mo
游览热带雨林的一大亮点是观赏彩蝶飞过,在阳光照射下闪烁着五彩斑斓的色彩。要追踪这样的蝴蝶可能相当棘手,因为通常只有翅膀的上侧才有彩虹色,所以只有当能看到翅膀的这一侧时,颜色才会闪烁。人们认为,这种移动闪烁的色彩使视觉捕食者难以跟踪蝴蝶的飞行路线。因此,彩虹色可能有助于蝴蝶分散捕食者的注意力。本期《动物学报》上,维埃拉-席尔瓦等人(2024 年)进行了一系列巧妙的实验,最终验证了闪色假说。在第一项实验中,作者在蝴蝶翅膀的隐蔽处涂上了与翅膀上侧的彩虹蓝相似的颜色。这样,蝴蝶在飞行过程中就更容易被看到了,因为现在蓝色一直暴露在外面。对照组的翅膀下侧涂上了与原始隐色相似的棕色,从而控制了捕捉和涂色对蝴蝶的影响。在一项捕获-再捕获分析中,Vieira-Silva 等人(2024 年)发现,与棕色对照组相比,蓝色蝴蝶再次出现的可能性较低。由于莫尔菲蝶(Morpho helena)的活动范围不大,因此蓝彩蝶再次被发现的几率较低的最合理解释是,它们中被捕食者吃掉的比例较高。在第二个实验中,Vieira-Silva 等人(2024 年)测试了整体隐色是否会减少捕食。为了测试这一点,他们在蝴蝶翅膀的上侧--通常是蓝色闪光色--涂上了棕色,这样蝴蝶在飞行过程中就完全隐蔽了。然而,这种处理方法并不影响捕获率,这表明完全隐蔽的蝴蝶与飞行时闪烁蓝色的个体相比并不具有优势。为了证明底色为蓝色的蝴蝶被捕食率更高的原因确实是蝴蝶在移动,而不是因为这些蝴蝶在捕食者面前更显眼,作者进行了第三个实验。为了测试蓝色是否会影响不动蝴蝶的捕食行为,他们比较了当蝴蝶翅膀底部涂上隐秘的棕色(模拟自然环境的对照组)或蓝色(与实验 1 类似)时,装死的蝴蝶受到捕食者攻击的可能性。对照组和蓝色蝴蝶坐骑受到的攻击没有区别,这表明不移动的蓝色蝴蝶并不比不移动的隐色蝴蝶吸引更多的捕食者。通过第三个实验,维埃拉-席尔瓦等人(2024 年)可以证明,实验 1 中的运动因素确实导致了更高的捕食率,而不仅仅是更显眼的颜色本身。本刊发表的一些研究成果已成为教科书知识或引文经典。我相信维埃拉-席尔瓦等人(2024 年)的这篇论文有可能成为关于捕食者分心的闪光颜色假说的经典引文。这项研究的实验设计既简单又巧妙。在统计分析日趋复杂的今天,这项研究仍然采用了非常简单的卡方检验统计方法。祝贺作者,并向他们致敬!
{"title":"A Butterfly's Flash Coloration Distracts Predators—Read Future Textbook Knowledge in Ethology","authors":"Wolfgang Goymann","doi":"10.1111/eth.13520","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13520","url":null,"abstract":"&lt;p&gt;A highlight of visiting a tropical rainforest is watching iridescent butterflies passing by, flashing their colours in the sunlight that makes it to the forest floor. It can be quite tricky to keep track of such a butterfly because typically only the upper side of the wings is iridescent, so that the colour only flashes when this side of a wing can be seen. People think that this kind of moving flash coloration makes it difficult for visual predators to follow the flight path of the butterfly. Hence, iridescent colours may help butterflies to distract predators. Computer simulations and experiments with humans as ‘predators’ suggest that this may be true, but as of now the flash colouration hypothesis had not been tested for real.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;In this issue of Ethology, Vieira-Silva et al. (&lt;span&gt;2024&lt;/span&gt;) conducted a series of clever experiments to finally test the flash colouration hypothesis in &lt;i&gt;Morpho helena&lt;/i&gt; butterflies. This species displays an iridescent blue colour on the upper side of its wings (as can be seen on this issues' cover image) that flashes when they move through the forest.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;In a first experiment, the authors painted the cryptic underside of the wings with a colour mimicking the iridescent blue of the upper side of the wing. Thereby, the butterflies become more constantly visible during flight, because now the blue colour is exposed all of the time. The underside of the wings of a control group got painted with a brown colour similar to the original cryptic colour, thereby controlling for the effect of catching and painting the butterflies. In a capture–recapture analysis, Vieira-Silva et al. (&lt;span&gt;2024&lt;/span&gt;) found that blue-coloured butterflies were less likely to be seen again compared to the brown-coloured control individuals. Because &lt;i&gt;Morpho helena&lt;/i&gt; butterflies do not move around far, the most parsimonious explanation for the lower resighting rates of the blue-coloured butterflies is that a higher proportion of them got eaten by predators.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;In a second experiment, Vieira-Silva et al. (&lt;span&gt;2024&lt;/span&gt;) tested if an overall cryptic colour as such would have reduced predation. To test this, they coloured the upper side of the wing—which normally has the blue flashing colouration—with brown colour, so that the butterflies become completely cryptic during flight. This treatment, however, did not affect recapture rates, suggesting that completely cryptic butterflies did not have an advantage over individuals that flashed their blue colour during flight. Hence, the distraction effect of a flashing blue colour likely has a similar effect than complete crypsis.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;To show that the higher predation of butterflies with an underside coloured in blue was really due to moving butterflies and not because such butterflies became generally more visible to predators, the authors conducted a third experiment. To test if blue colouration affected predation in non-moving butterflies, they compared how likely dead mo","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 12","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13520","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142641184","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Adaptive Significance of Long Mating With Repeated Intromissions in Zygogramma bicolorata
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-03 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13525
Rabi Sankar Pal, Anirban Bhowmick, Kunmun Naik, Bodhisatta Nandy

Long matings are abundant in insects despite the range of the costs involved. The causes and consequences of the evolution of long mating remain an interesting problem for behavioural ecologists. We studied extraordinarily long mating that involves repeated intromissions interspersed with latent periods in the Parthenium beetle (Zygogramma bicolorata). We conducted a series of interrupted mating assays to examine the fitness consequences of different components of this curious mating behaviour. We tested multiple adaptive hypotheses concerning male fertility and competitive ability. We found that sperm transfer and fertility did not exhibit a linear increase with the number of intromissions. There was also no evidence of nutrient transfer by the males. Interestingly, our results showed that both sexes suffered a significant cost of long mating. Further, female remating behaviour was found to be modulated by the length of the previous mating. Additionally, males were observed performing a curious leg rubbing behaviour during the inter-intromission latent period, putatively serving as a copulatory courtship function that reduced female resistance to the continuance of mating. Therefore, we show that while the long mating may still serve a mate-guarding role, there are additional fitness effects of such behaviour that need careful consideration. Our study provides insights into the adaptive significance of long mating and its fitness consequences.

{"title":"Adaptive Significance of Long Mating With Repeated Intromissions in Zygogramma bicolorata","authors":"Rabi Sankar Pal,&nbsp;Anirban Bhowmick,&nbsp;Kunmun Naik,&nbsp;Bodhisatta Nandy","doi":"10.1111/eth.13525","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13525","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Long matings are abundant in insects despite the range of the costs involved. The causes and consequences of the evolution of long mating remain an interesting problem for behavioural ecologists. We studied extraordinarily long mating that involves repeated intromissions interspersed with latent periods in the Parthenium beetle (<i>Zygogramma bicolorata</i>). We conducted a series of interrupted mating assays to examine the fitness consequences of different components of this curious mating behaviour. We tested multiple adaptive hypotheses concerning male fertility and competitive ability. We found that sperm transfer and fertility did not exhibit a linear increase with the number of intromissions. There was also no evidence of nutrient transfer by the males. Interestingly, our results showed that both sexes suffered a significant cost of long mating. Further, female remating behaviour was found to be modulated by the length of the previous mating. Additionally, males were observed performing a curious leg rubbing behaviour during the inter-intromission latent period, putatively serving as a copulatory courtship function that reduced female resistance to the continuance of mating. Therefore, we show that while the long mating may still serve a mate-guarding role, there are additional fitness effects of such behaviour that need careful consideration. Our study provides insights into the adaptive significance of long mating and its fitness consequences.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"131 2","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-11-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143111427","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do Rearing Group-Size and Social Rank Influence the Affective State of a Cooperatively-Breeding Cichlid Fish? 饲养群体大小和社会等级是否影响合作繁殖慈鲷的情感状态?
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-31 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13516
Léa Langérôme, Océane La Loggia, Bettina Voser, Barbara Taborsky

The affective state of animals, that is, their mood and emotions, is altered by stressful (negative) or enriching (positive) experiences. In turn, the affective state influences decision making, thereby helping animals when coping with environmental challenges and opportunities. However, it is largely unknown how social experiences modulate the affective state. Here, we performed a judgement bias test to study the effects of rearing group-size and experimentally assigned current rank on the affective state of the cooperatively-breeding cichlid fish Neolamprologus pulcher. To assess affective state, we developed and validated a judgement bias test for this species. Fish learned to discriminate between a positive and a negative stimulus as shown by different latencies to approach the stimulus. Furthermore, the response curves to the stimuli conformed to the ones expected in judgement bias tests: fish showed an intermediate latency to approach an ambiguous stimulus, which significantly differed from the latencies to approach the positive and the negative stimulus. Unexpectedly, there were no significant effects of rearing group size and current social rank on the affective state of N. pulcher, despite known effects of these two social parameters on behaviours and physiology of this species. This may mean that observed behavioural and physiological differences in the treatment environments do not allow valid predictions about the affective state elicited by these environments. Alternatively, it may need more socioecologically relevant testing paradigms when evaluating the valence of social environments.

动物的情感状态,即它们的情绪和情绪,会因压力(消极)或丰富(积极)的经历而改变。反过来,情感状态影响决策,从而帮助动物应对环境挑战和机遇。然而,社会经验如何调节情感状态在很大程度上是未知的。本研究通过判断偏倚检验,研究了饲养群体规模和实验分配的当前等级对合作养殖的新幼鱼(Neolamprologus pulcher)情感状态的影响。为了评估情感状态,我们开发并验证了该物种的判断偏差测试。鱼学会了区分积极刺激和消极刺激,这可以从接近刺激的不同潜伏期中看出。此外,鱼对刺激的反应曲线符合判断偏差测试的预期:鱼对模糊刺激表现出中间潜伏期,这与接近积极和消极刺激的潜伏期有显著差异。出乎意料的是,饲养群体规模和当前社会等级对布袋小蜂的情感状态没有显著影响,尽管这两个社会参数对布袋小蜂的行为和生理有已知的影响。这可能意味着在治疗环境中观察到的行为和生理差异不允许对这些环境引发的情感状态进行有效预测。或者,在评估社会环境的效价时,可能需要更多的社会生态学相关的测试范式。
{"title":"Do Rearing Group-Size and Social Rank Influence the Affective State of a Cooperatively-Breeding Cichlid Fish?","authors":"Léa Langérôme,&nbsp;Océane La Loggia,&nbsp;Bettina Voser,&nbsp;Barbara Taborsky","doi":"10.1111/eth.13516","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13516","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The affective state of animals, that is, their mood and emotions, is altered by stressful (negative) or enriching (positive) experiences. In turn, the affective state influences decision making, thereby helping animals when coping with environmental challenges and opportunities. However, it is largely unknown how social experiences modulate the affective state. Here, we performed a judgement bias test to study the effects of rearing group-size and experimentally assigned current rank on the affective state of the cooperatively-breeding cichlid fish <i>Neolamprologus pulcher</i>. To assess affective state, we developed and validated a judgement bias test for this species. Fish learned to discriminate between a positive and a negative stimulus as shown by different latencies to approach the stimulus. Furthermore, the response curves to the stimuli conformed to the ones expected in judgement bias tests: fish showed an intermediate latency to approach an ambiguous stimulus, which significantly differed from the latencies to approach the positive and the negative stimulus. Unexpectedly, there were no significant effects of rearing group size and current social rank on the affective state of <i>N. pulcher</i>, despite known effects of these two social parameters on behaviours and physiology of this species. This may mean that observed behavioural and physiological differences in the treatment environments do not allow valid predictions about the affective state elicited by these environments. Alternatively, it may need more socioecologically relevant testing paradigms when evaluating the valence of social environments.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"131 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13516","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142862401","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Third-Party Affiliation in Domestic Dogs During and After a Human Conflict 家犬在与人类冲突期间和之后的第三方归属感
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-27 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13522
Laura Analía Rial, Camila Cavalli, Marina Victoria Dzik, Mariana Bentosela

Several behaviors occur in the aftermath of within-group conflicts. These include spontaneous affiliation toward the victim from an uninvolved third party. When third-party affiliations reduce the stress of the victim, this behavior has been defined as consolation. Given the absence of previous reports, the objective of the present study was to evaluate the presence of third-party post-conflict affiliation when dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) observe their owners arguing. We carried out two studies varying the intensity and the duration of the dispute. Affiliative behaviors toward each of the owners were registered, as well as stress-related behaviors. Our findings support the existence of third-party affiliation from dogs toward their owners during and after a conflict between them, evidenced as higher rates of victim-directed affiliative behaviors in the experimental condition versus the control, in both studies. Moreover, dogs exhibited more stress-related behaviors in the experimental condition compared to the control, but only in the second study, which suggests these stimuli were experienced as aversive, even though they were not aimed at the dogs. In addition, in the second study dogs displayed aggressor-directed behaviors that could be interpreted as appeasement. Finally, there was no evidence that the level of the bond between the dog and each owner acts as a modulator of affiliative behavior. Further studies are required to expand our understanding of these abilities of dogs and its effects on the emotional state of the victim.

在团队内部冲突之后会出现几种行为。这些包括来自无关的第三方对受害者的自发联系。当与第三方的关系减轻了受害者的压力时,这种行为被定义为安慰。鉴于之前的报道缺失,本研究的目的是评估当狗(犬狼疮)观察到它们的主人争吵时,是否存在第三方冲突后关系。我们进行了两项研究,改变了争论的强度和持续时间。对每位主人的附属行为以及与压力相关的行为都进行了记录。我们的研究结果支持狗在它们之间的冲突期间和之后对它们的主人存在第三方从属关系,在两项研究中,实验条件下与对照组相比,受害者导向的从属行为的比例更高。此外,与对照组相比,狗在实验条件下表现出更多与压力相关的行为,但这只是在第二项研究中,这表明这些刺激是令人厌恶的,即使它们不是针对狗的。此外,在第二项研究中,狗表现出攻击者导向的行为,这可以解释为绥靖。最后,没有证据表明狗和每个主人之间的联系程度会调节依恋行为。需要进一步的研究来扩大我们对狗的这些能力及其对受害者情绪状态的影响的理解。
{"title":"Third-Party Affiliation in Domestic Dogs During and After a Human Conflict","authors":"Laura Analía Rial,&nbsp;Camila Cavalli,&nbsp;Marina Victoria Dzik,&nbsp;Mariana Bentosela","doi":"10.1111/eth.13522","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13522","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Several behaviors occur in the aftermath of within-group conflicts. These include spontaneous affiliation toward the victim from an uninvolved third party. When third-party affiliations reduce the stress of the victim, this behavior has been defined as consolation. Given the absence of previous reports, the objective of the present study was to evaluate the presence of third-party post-conflict affiliation when dogs (<i>Canis lupus familiaris</i>) observe their owners arguing. We carried out two studies varying the intensity and the duration of the dispute. Affiliative behaviors toward each of the owners were registered, as well as stress-related behaviors. Our findings support the existence of third-party affiliation from dogs toward their owners during and after a conflict between them, evidenced as higher rates of victim-directed affiliative behaviors in the experimental condition versus the control, in both studies. Moreover, dogs exhibited more stress-related behaviors in the experimental condition compared to the control, but only in the second study, which suggests these stimuli were experienced as aversive, even though they were not aimed at the dogs. In addition, in the second study dogs displayed aggressor-directed behaviors that could be interpreted as appeasement. Finally, there was no evidence that the level of the bond between the dog and each owner acts as a modulator of affiliative behavior. Further studies are required to expand our understanding of these abilities of dogs and its effects on the emotional state of the victim.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"131 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142862158","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Females Guarded by Sneaker Males Experience Higher Predation in the Two-Spotted Spider Mite 在双斑蜘蛛螨中,被Sneaker雄性看守的雌性蜘蛛有更高的捕食率
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-27 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13524
Taito Sano, Tanvi Gurjar, Martijn Egas, Yukie Sato

Males often employ different reproductive tactics to gain access to females based on their condition and the surrounding environment. Predation risk is expected to have a significant influence on the frequencies of alternative reproductive tactics because these tactics typically differ in activity, which may result in differences in predation rate. In theory, such predation effects can explain the evolution as well as the maintenance of alternative reproductive tactics. Yet, there is little experimental work testing how predation risk affects alternative reproductive tactics. To assess such effects of predation, here we report on experiments with the two-spotted spider mite Tetranychus urticae. The two-spotted spider mite is a small arthropod herbivore species, in which males exhibit precopulatory mate guarding by mounting preadult moulting (and hence immobile) females. Two reproductive tactics are observed during mate guarding: The fighting tactic involves attacking other males to drive them away, while the sneaking tactic involves mounting the females and remaining motionless, even when contacted by other males. In this study, we exposed pairs of male and female spider mites to a predatory mite (Phytoseiulus persimilis) and observed their survival and male response to a predator when females were guarded by either fighter or sneaker males. We found that predation risk was not significantly different between fighter and sneaker males. However, the immobile females were more often preyed upon when guarded by sneakers than when guarded by fighters. We attribute this indirect effect of predation risk to the sneakers continuing to mount females even when a predator is nearby.

雄性通常会根据自己的状况和周围环境采取不同的繁殖策略来接近雌性。捕食风险预计会对不同繁殖策略的频率产生重大影响,因为这些策略的活动通常不同,这可能导致捕食率的差异。从理论上讲,这种捕食效应可以解释替代性繁殖策略的进化和维持。然而,很少有实验工作测试捕食风险如何影响其他繁殖策略。为了评估这种捕食的影响,在这里,我们报告了双斑叶螨的实验。双斑蜘蛛螨是一种小型节肢动物食草物种,其中雄性蜘蛛螨通过骑在成年前蜕皮(因此无法移动)的雌性上来保护交配前的配偶。在守护配偶的过程中,观察到两种繁殖策略:战斗策略包括攻击其他雄性,将它们赶走,而偷偷摸摸的策略包括骑在雌性身上,即使有其他雄性接触,也保持静止不动。在这项研究中,我们将雄性和雌性蜘蛛螨对暴露于一种掠食性螨(Phytoseiulus persimilis)中,观察了雌性蜘蛛螨在被战斗或运动鞋雄性保护时的存活率和雄性对掠食性螨的反应。我们发现好斗雄性和运动鞋雄性的捕食风险没有显著差异。然而,在运动鞋的保护下,一动不动的雌性比在战士的保护下更容易被捕食。我们将这种捕食风险的间接影响归因于即使捕食者就在附近,球鞋也会继续骑在雌性身上。
{"title":"Females Guarded by Sneaker Males Experience Higher Predation in the Two-Spotted Spider Mite","authors":"Taito Sano,&nbsp;Tanvi Gurjar,&nbsp;Martijn Egas,&nbsp;Yukie Sato","doi":"10.1111/eth.13524","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13524","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Males often employ different reproductive tactics to gain access to females based on their condition and the surrounding environment. Predation risk is expected to have a significant influence on the frequencies of alternative reproductive tactics because these tactics typically differ in activity, which may result in differences in predation rate. In theory, such predation effects can explain the evolution as well as the maintenance of alternative reproductive tactics. Yet, there is little experimental work testing how predation risk affects alternative reproductive tactics. To assess such effects of predation, here we report on experiments with the two-spotted spider mite <i>Tetranychus urticae</i>. The two-spotted spider mite is a small arthropod herbivore species, in which males exhibit precopulatory mate guarding by mounting preadult moulting (and hence immobile) females. Two reproductive tactics are observed during mate guarding: The fighting tactic involves attacking other males to drive them away, while the sneaking tactic involves mounting the females and remaining motionless, even when contacted by other males. In this study, we exposed pairs of male and female spider mites to a predatory mite (<i>Phytoseiulus persimilis</i>) and observed their survival and male response to a predator when females were guarded by either fighter or sneaker males. We found that predation risk was not significantly different between fighter and sneaker males. However, the immobile females were more often preyed upon when guarded by sneakers than when guarded by fighters. We attribute this indirect effect of predation risk to the sneakers continuing to mount females even when a predator is nearby.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"131 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13524","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142862159","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Complexity in Chick-a-Dee Calls of Mountain Chickadees (Poecile gambeli): Call Variation Associated With Flock Size and Flight
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-27 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13523
Zaharia A. Selman, Todd M. Freeberg

The chick-a-dee call of chickadees, tits, and titmice is a vocal system used in a wide range of social contexts by both sexes throughout the year and is one of the more structurally complicated vocal systems outside of human language. Relatively little is known about the chick-a-dee calls of mountain chickadees, Poecile gambeli, however. This is an important species for increasing our comparative understanding of variation in chick-a-dee calls as they are one of the chickadee species with the largest naturally occurring flock sizes. Flock size relates to the social complexity of flocks, and the social complexity hypothesis for communication predicts that individuals in more complex social groups should communicate with greater complexity than individuals in simpler social groups. Correlational and experimental evidence in support of the hypothesis has been found in the calls of a wide range of species, including Carolina chickadees, P. carolinensis. Here, we provide the first description of the variation in note composition and note-ordering rules in calls from mountain chickadee flocks in California and Colorado. California flocks were found to be significantly larger than Colorado flocks. Analysis of note-type usage and transition probabilities between note types found that calls of California birds were more complex than calls of Colorado birds, supporting a key prediction of the social complexity hypothesis for communication. We also found relatively high rates of reversals of note-ordering rules in mountain chickadee calls, which might help explain the complexity of the chick-a-dee calls of this species. Additionally, birds in flight produced calls with different note compositions when compared to perched birds. Generally, the note-type ordering and transition probabilities of calls of mountain chickadees seem comparable to other better-studied chickadee species, although their frequent note-type order rule reversals suggest potential syntax-like properties in this call system.

{"title":"Complexity in Chick-a-Dee Calls of Mountain Chickadees (Poecile gambeli): Call Variation Associated With Flock Size and Flight","authors":"Zaharia A. Selman,&nbsp;Todd M. Freeberg","doi":"10.1111/eth.13523","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13523","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>The <i>chick-a-dee</i> call of chickadees, tits, and titmice is a vocal system used in a wide range of social contexts by both sexes throughout the year and is one of the more structurally complicated vocal systems outside of human language. Relatively little is known about the <i>chick-a-dee</i> calls of mountain chickadees, <i>Poecile gambeli</i>, however. This is an important species for increasing our comparative understanding of variation in <i>chick-a-dee</i> calls as they are one of the chickadee species with the largest naturally occurring flock sizes. Flock size relates to the social complexity of flocks, and the social complexity hypothesis for communication predicts that individuals in more complex social groups should communicate with greater complexity than individuals in simpler social groups. Correlational and experimental evidence in support of the hypothesis has been found in the calls of a wide range of species, including Carolina chickadees, <i>P. carolinensis</i>. Here, we provide the first description of the variation in note composition and note-ordering rules in calls from mountain chickadee flocks in California and Colorado. California flocks were found to be significantly larger than Colorado flocks. Analysis of note-type usage and transition probabilities between note types found that calls of California birds were more complex than calls of Colorado birds, supporting a key prediction of the social complexity hypothesis for communication. We also found relatively high rates of reversals of note-ordering rules in mountain chickadee calls, which might help explain the complexity of the <i>chick-a-dee</i> calls of this species. Additionally, birds in flight produced calls with different note compositions when compared to perched birds. Generally, the note-type ordering and transition probabilities of calls of mountain chickadees seem comparable to other better-studied chickadee species, although their frequent note-type order rule reversals suggest potential syntax-like properties in this call system.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"131 2","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143120234","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Call for Your Life: Acoustic Structure and Age-Sex Differences in Distress Calls of Red-Necked Nightjars 呼唤生命:红颈夜鸦窘迫叫声的声学结构和年龄性别差异
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-25 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13513
Javier Sierro, Diego Gil, Pedro Sáez-Gómez, Paula Hidalgo-Rodríguez, Julio Rabadán-González, Carlos Camacho

Predatory interactions result in strong selection pressures acting on multiple aspects of animal behaviour. Anti-predator strategies are therefore common in most animals, typically signalling at various stages of a predation event. Many species of caprimulgids perform conspicuous anti-predator displays, including stereotyped behaviours and vocal signals. Here, we described distress calls of red-necked nightjars (Caprimulgus ruficollis), produced when birds are trapped and unable to escape during a predatory interaction. Distress calls are harsh, low-frequency guttural vocalisations with irregular amplitude modulations. The age and sex of birds partially explained the acoustic variation observed, whereas size-related morphological features were poor predictors of the acoustic structure. Age-sex differences in distress calls may respond to physiological constraints associated with sexual dimorphism and/or developmental variation. Alternatively, directional selection associated with differential predation risk on each age-sex class may have resulted in the observed differences in distress calls. The extremely deep voice and the harsh quality of distress calls fit the structure of aggressive signals and may resemble those produced by a larger animal. We propose that these calls serve as a last resort strategy to reduce post-encounter risk of predation, either as a startling effect to facilitate escape or to attract other predators that could intimidate the captor.

捕食互动会对动物行为的多个方面产生强大的选择压力。因此,反捕食策略在大多数动物中都很常见,通常是在捕食事件的不同阶段发出信号。许多种类的毛冠鱼都有明显的反捕食表现,包括刻板行为和声音信号。在这里,我们描述了红颈夜鸦(Caprimulgus ruficollis)的求救鸣叫,这种鸣叫是在捕食过程中鸟类被困无法逃脱时发出的。求救鸣叫是一种刺耳的低频肠鸣音,具有不规则的振幅调制。鸟类的年龄和性别可以部分解释所观察到的声学变化,而与体型相关的形态特征则不能很好地预测声学结构。求救鸣声的年龄-性别差异可能与性二型和/或发育变异相关的生理限制有关。另外,与各年龄-性别类别不同的捕食风险相关的定向选择也可能导致了所观察到的求救信号的差异。求救信号的声音极度低沉,音质刺耳,符合攻击性信号的结构,可能与大型动物发出的求救信号相似。我们认为,这些求救信号是降低捕食后风险的最后手段,可以起到惊吓作用,帮助捕食者逃脱,也可以吸引其他捕食者来恐吓捕食者。
{"title":"Call for Your Life: Acoustic Structure and Age-Sex Differences in Distress Calls of Red-Necked Nightjars","authors":"Javier Sierro,&nbsp;Diego Gil,&nbsp;Pedro Sáez-Gómez,&nbsp;Paula Hidalgo-Rodríguez,&nbsp;Julio Rabadán-González,&nbsp;Carlos Camacho","doi":"10.1111/eth.13513","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13513","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Predatory interactions result in strong selection pressures acting on multiple aspects of animal behaviour. Anti-predator strategies are therefore common in most animals, typically signalling at various stages of a predation event. Many species of caprimulgids perform conspicuous anti-predator displays, including stereotyped behaviours and vocal signals. Here, we described distress calls of red-necked nightjars (<i>Caprimulgus ruficollis</i>), produced when birds are trapped and unable to escape during a predatory interaction. Distress calls are harsh, low-frequency guttural vocalisations with irregular amplitude modulations. The age and sex of birds partially explained the acoustic variation observed, whereas size-related morphological features were poor predictors of the acoustic structure. Age-sex differences in distress calls may respond to physiological constraints associated with sexual dimorphism and/or developmental variation. Alternatively, directional selection associated with differential predation risk on each age-sex class may have resulted in the observed differences in distress calls. The extremely deep voice and the harsh quality of distress calls fit the structure of aggressive signals and may resemble those produced by a larger animal. We propose that these calls serve as a last resort strategy to reduce post-encounter risk of predation, either as a startling effect to facilitate escape or to attract other predators that could intimidate the captor.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 12","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13513","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142642365","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Wolf Spider Tigrosa helluo Uses Visual Associative and Beacon Landmarks During Water Maze Navigation Tasks 狼蛛虎蛛在水迷宫导航任务中使用视觉联想和灯塔地标
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-25 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13518
Riko Weidman, Kelsey Persons, Matthew Persons

Wolf spiders can learn simple spatial navigation tasks. Previous studies have shown that the wolf spider Tigrosa helluo can use environmental edge features (reference frame landmarks) to learn the location of a dry target in flooded T-mazes; however, the relative importance of different types or numbers of landmark cues to spatial learning remains unknown. We used a modified open arena water maze and recorded the ability of adult female T. helluo to find a target reward (a dark and dry cup) among cups that were identical to the target but flooded. We measured variation in spatial learning by measuring time to target with no landmark (control), with a beacon (a landmark that is part of the target), with an associative cue (a landmark associated with a specific navigational action), and with both a beacon and an associative cue (N = 92 subjects, n = 23 per landmark cue treatment). For each treatment, we tested females for five trials each on four consecutive days, with the last trial on the fourth day having an altered target location, totaling 19 training trials and one reversal trial (1840 trials). We found that spiders took significantly less time to find the target over subsequent trials within a day and learned more quickly when landmark cues were present, but we found no difference in the type or number of landmark features in the meantime to target entrance. After learning a target location, moving the landmark significantly increased the mean time to target entrance in the combined beacon and associative cue treatment relative to other treatments. Our results indicate that wolf spiders use visual beacons and associative cue landmarks alone or in combination and that performance improves across trials when landmarks are present and deteriorates more when multiple landmarks are moved.

狼蛛可以学习简单的空间导航任务。先前的研究表明,狼蛛Tigrosa helluo可以利用环境边缘特征(参考框架地标)来学习洪水迷宫中干燥目标的位置;然而,不同类型或数量的地标线索对空间学习的相对重要性尚不清楚。我们使用了一个改进的开放式竞技场水迷宫,记录了成年雌性T. helluo在与目标相同但被水淹没的杯子中找到目标奖励(一个黑暗而干燥的杯子)的能力。我们通过测量无路标(对照组)、路标(路标是目标的一部分)、联想线索(路标与特定导航动作相关)以及路标和联想线索的时间来测量空间学习的变化(N = 92名受试者,每个路标线索治疗N = 23名)。对于每个治疗,我们对女性进行连续4天的5次试验,第4天的最后一次试验改变目标位置,共计19次训练试验和1次逆转试验(1840次试验)。我们发现,在随后的试验中,蜘蛛在一天内找到目标的时间明显更短,并且在有地标性线索时学习得更快,但我们发现,在目标入口的这段时间里,地标性特征的类型或数量没有差异。在学习目标位置后,在信标和联想线索联合处理中,移动地标显著增加了到达目标入口的平均时间。我们的研究结果表明,狼蛛单独或结合使用视觉信标和联想线索标志,当标志存在时,它们的表现在试验中有所提高,而当多个标志被移动时,它们的表现会更差。
{"title":"The Wolf Spider Tigrosa helluo Uses Visual Associative and Beacon Landmarks During Water Maze Navigation Tasks","authors":"Riko Weidman,&nbsp;Kelsey Persons,&nbsp;Matthew Persons","doi":"10.1111/eth.13518","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13518","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Wolf spiders can learn simple spatial navigation tasks. Previous studies have shown that the wolf spider <i>Tigrosa helluo</i> can use environmental edge features (reference frame landmarks) to learn the location of a dry target in flooded T-mazes; however, the relative importance of different types or numbers of landmark cues to spatial learning remains unknown. We used a modified open arena water maze and recorded the ability of adult female <i>T. helluo</i> to find a target reward (a dark and dry cup) among cups that were identical to the target but flooded. We measured variation in spatial learning by measuring time to target with no landmark (control), with a beacon (a landmark that is part of the target), with an associative cue (a landmark associated with a specific navigational action), and with both a beacon and an associative cue (<i>N</i> = 92 subjects, <i>n</i> = 23 per landmark cue treatment). For each treatment, we tested females for five trials each on four consecutive days, with the last trial on the fourth day having an altered target location, totaling 19 training trials and one reversal trial (1840 trials). We found that spiders took significantly less time to find the target over subsequent trials within a day and learned more quickly when landmark cues were present, but we found no difference in the type or number of landmark features in the meantime to target entrance. After learning a target location, moving the landmark significantly increased the mean time to target entrance in the combined beacon and associative cue treatment relative to other treatments. Our results indicate that wolf spiders use visual beacons and associative cue landmarks alone or in combination and that performance improves across trials when landmarks are present and deteriorates more when multiple landmarks are moved.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"131 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142862088","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Relevance of Flash Coloration Against Avian Predation in a Morpho Butterfly: A Field Experiment in a Tropical Rainforest 斑蝶的闪光色彩与鸟类捕食的相关性:热带雨林中的现场实验
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-24 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13517
Aline Vieira-Silva, Gabriel B. Evora, André V. L. Freitas, Paulo S. Oliveira

The flash coloration hypothesis postulates that otherwise cryptically colored animals suddenly displaying conspicuous colors during movement confuse predators, reducing capture. Morpho helenor butterflies have contrasting colors on dorsal (iridescent blue) and ventral (brown) wing surfaces, resulting in sequential blue “flashes” during flight. We tested whether this flashing pattern reduces avian predation on M. helenor in Atlantic rainforest by changing the flashing effect in three experiments. In Experiment 1, we added a blue band to the ventral wing. In Experiment 2, we covered the dorsal wing's blue band with a brown band. Control groups in each experiment were painted such that wing color patterns remained unaltered. Survivorship was evaluated through mark-recapture censuses and beak marks on the wings. Results show that survivorship of treated butterflies in Experiment 1 decrease markedly compared to unaltered control individuals, while survivorship of treated butterflies in Experiment 2 did not differ compared to control individuals. In Experiment 3, we detected scant predation on treated (blue band added to ventral wing) and control butterflies (brown band added to ventral wing) on the forest floor (wings closed), corroborating that flash coloration is an important protective mechanism during flight. Our field experiments provide the first evidence, to our knowledge, that flash coloration in bright blue Morpho butterflies is an effective defense mechanism against avian predators in a tropical rainforest.

闪光着色假说认为,原本颜色隐秘的动物在运动过程中突然显示出明显的颜色,会迷惑捕食者,从而减少捕获量。Morpho helenor 蝴蝶的背翅(彩蓝)和腹翅(棕色)表面颜色对比鲜明,因此在飞行过程中会连续 "闪烁 "蓝色。我们在三个实验中通过改变闪烁效果来测试这种闪烁模式是否会减少大西洋雨林中鸟类对 M. helenor 的捕食。在实验 1 中,我们在腹翅上添加了一条蓝带。在实验 2 中,我们在背翅的蓝色条带上覆盖了一条棕色条带。每个实验的对照组都涂上了颜色,使翅膀的颜色图案保持不变。存活率通过标记重捕普查和翅膀上的喙痕进行评估。结果表明,实验 1 中经过处理的蝴蝶的存活率与未经改变的对照组相比明显下降,而实验 2 中经过处理的蝴蝶的存活率与对照组相比没有差异。在实验 3 中,我们在森林地面(翅膀闭合)上发现,处理过的蝴蝶(腹翅上有蓝色条带)和对照组蝴蝶(腹翅上有棕色条带)很少被捕食,这证实了闪光着色是蝴蝶在飞行过程中的一种重要保护机制。据我们所知,我们的野外实验首次证明了亮蓝莫弗蝶的闪光着色是热带雨林中一种有效的抵御鸟类捕食者的防御机制。
{"title":"The Relevance of Flash Coloration Against Avian Predation in a Morpho Butterfly: A Field Experiment in a Tropical Rainforest","authors":"Aline Vieira-Silva,&nbsp;Gabriel B. Evora,&nbsp;André V. L. Freitas,&nbsp;Paulo S. Oliveira","doi":"10.1111/eth.13517","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13517","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>The flash coloration hypothesis postulates that otherwise cryptically colored animals suddenly displaying conspicuous colors during movement confuse predators, reducing capture. <i>Morpho helenor</i> butterflies have contrasting colors on dorsal (iridescent blue) and ventral (brown) wing surfaces, resulting in sequential blue “flashes” during flight. We tested whether this flashing pattern reduces avian predation on <i>M</i>. <i>helenor</i> in Atlantic rainforest by changing the flashing effect in three experiments. In Experiment 1, we added a blue band to the ventral wing. In Experiment 2, we covered the dorsal wing's blue band with a brown band. Control groups in each experiment were painted such that wing color patterns remained unaltered. Survivorship was evaluated through mark-recapture censuses and beak marks on the wings. Results show that survivorship of treated butterflies in Experiment 1 decrease markedly compared to unaltered control individuals, while survivorship of treated butterflies in Experiment 2 did not differ compared to control individuals. In Experiment 3, we detected scant predation on treated (blue band added to ventral wing) and control butterflies (brown band added to ventral wing) on the forest floor (wings closed), corroborating that flash coloration is an important protective mechanism during flight. Our field experiments provide the first evidence, to our knowledge, that flash coloration in bright blue <i>Morpho</i> butterflies is an effective defense mechanism against avian predators in a tropical rainforest.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 12","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142642426","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Condition-Dependent Female Aggression and Its Effects on Mating Success and Sexual Cannibalism in Jumping Spiders 条件依赖性雌性攻击及其对跳蛛交配成功率和性食肉的影响
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-22 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13519
Michael E. Vickers, Marianne W. Robertson, Travis E. Wilcoxen

Pre-copulatory sexual cannibalism is an extreme form of aggression toward potential mates. In this study, we examined condition-dependent female aggression and its effects on mating success and frequency of pre-copulatory sexual cannibalism in the jumping spider Phidippus audax Hentz 1845. We examined two of the leading hypotheses of why sexual cannibalism may occur, (1) female mate choice and (2) adaptive female foraging. We separated 137 adult virgin female P. audax into three feeding treatments: (1) well-fed spiders, (2) 14-day food deprived, and (3) 28-day food deprived. We recorded weight loss (14- and 28-day treatments) and survival rates. We conducted mating trials to measure the proportion of non-aggressive and aggressive male and female behaviors, male mating success, and pre-copulatory sexual cannibalism. Females deprived of food had higher weight loss and lower survival rates than well-fed females. In addition, food deprived female spiders exhibited higher proportions of aggressive behaviors and pre-copulatory sexual cannibalism, and lower mating success compared to well-fed spiders. We found that as male size increased females were less likely to cannibalize males, but we found no effect of male body condition on whether a male mated with or was cannibalized by a female. Our results are consistent with the adaptive foraging hypothesis and corroborates prior research on pre-copulatory sexual cannibalism.

交配前性同类相食是对潜在配偶的一种极端攻击。本研究以跳蛛菲迪普斯·奥达斯·亨茨1845为研究对象,研究了条件依赖性雌性攻击行为及其对交配成功率和交配前性同类相食频率的影响。我们研究了为什么会发生性同类相食的两个主要假设,(1)雌性配偶选择和(2)适应性雌性觅食。我们将137只成年雌性奥达蜘蛛分为3个摄食处理:(1)摄食充足,(2)14天不摄食,(3)28天不摄食。我们记录了体重减轻(治疗14天和28天)和生存率。我们进行了交配试验,以测量非攻击性和攻击性雄性和雌性行为的比例,雄性交配成功率和交配前性同类相食。被剥夺食物的雌性比吃饱的雌性体重下降得更快,存活率也更低。此外,与食物充足的蜘蛛相比,缺乏食物的雌性蜘蛛表现出更高的攻击行为和交配前性同类相食的比例,而交配成功率较低。我们发现,随着雄性体型的增加,雌性吃掉雄性的可能性降低,但我们发现雄性身体状况对雄性是否与雌性交配或被雌性吃掉没有影响。我们的研究结果与适应性觅食假说一致,并证实了先前关于交配前性同类相食的研究。
{"title":"Condition-Dependent Female Aggression and Its Effects on Mating Success and Sexual Cannibalism in Jumping Spiders","authors":"Michael E. Vickers,&nbsp;Marianne W. Robertson,&nbsp;Travis E. Wilcoxen","doi":"10.1111/eth.13519","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13519","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Pre-copulatory sexual cannibalism is an extreme form of aggression toward potential mates. In this study, we examined condition-dependent female aggression and its effects on mating success and frequency of pre-copulatory sexual cannibalism in the jumping spider <i>Phidippus audax</i> Hentz 1845. We examined two of the leading hypotheses of why sexual cannibalism may occur, (1) female mate choice and (2) adaptive female foraging. We separated 137 adult virgin female <i>P. audax</i> into three feeding treatments: (1) well-fed spiders, (2) 14-day food deprived, and (3) 28-day food deprived. We recorded weight loss (14- and 28-day treatments) and survival rates. We conducted mating trials to measure the proportion of non-aggressive and aggressive male and female behaviors, male mating success, and pre-copulatory sexual cannibalism. Females deprived of food had higher weight loss and lower survival rates than well-fed females. In addition, food deprived female spiders exhibited higher proportions of aggressive behaviors and pre-copulatory sexual cannibalism, and lower mating success compared to well-fed spiders. We found that as male size increased females were less likely to cannibalize males, but we found no effect of male body condition on whether a male mated with or was cannibalized by a female. Our results are consistent with the adaptive foraging hypothesis and corroborates prior research on pre-copulatory sexual cannibalism.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"131 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13519","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142861997","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Ethology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1