首页 > 最新文献

Ethology最新文献

英文 中文
Pair-Coordinated Calling: Eurasian Magpies Respond Differently to Simulated Intruder Pairs That Overlap or Alternate Their Calls 成对协调的叫声:欧亚喜鹊对呼叫重叠或交替的模拟入侵者配对做出不同反应
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-21 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13515
Miriam Kuspiel, Sjouke A. Kingma, Heleen Vermeulen, Marc Naguib

Animal vocalisations are widely used to signal strength or motivation of a caller in competitive interactions, such as in territorial defence. Substantial understanding of signalling functions in territorial conflicts is based on singing by male songbirds. Yet, in many species, both pair members call during territorial conflicts, as well as in predator-induced situations, leading to complex signalling interactions in which calls overlap or alternate. This raises the question as to whether or not variation in how individuals in pairs time their calls is perceived as meaningful by receivers. Here, we tested with playback experiments whether Eurasian magpies (Pica pica), a species producing alarm calls (so-called chatter calls) in territorial defence, respond stronger to simulated pair-intruders who overlap their calls with each other than to those who alternate them. Magpies emitted a significantly longer first chatter calls in response to playback with overlapping calls but chattered significantly sooner and approached the loudspeakers significantly more closely in response to playbacks of alternating (and therefore longer) call sequences. These findings exemplify that the timing of calls by pair members matters, but in more complex ways than we predicted. The overlapping playback appeared to trigger a longer yet later initial chatter response and a weaker approach response, suggesting that the different ways in which magpies respond reflect different levels of arousal or defence strategies. The results may also reflect uncertainty by receivers due to a potential mismatch between signalled and perceived information: While overlapping calls may signal high arousal by both callers, a longer alternating sequence could be perceived as a more aroused longer signal. These findings expand on classical experiments on call function, suggesting that pairs can vary the message by coordinating their alarm calls in different ways, similar to how duetting pairs time their song contributions in advertisement signalling.

动物的发声被广泛用于在竞争性互动(如领地保卫战)中发出呼叫者的实力或动机信号。对领地冲突中信号功能的大量了解是基于雄性鸣禽的歌唱。然而,在许多物种中,一对成员都会在领地冲突期间以及捕食者诱发的情况下发出叫声,从而导致叫声重叠或交替的复杂信号互动。这就提出了一个问题:成对个体在鸣叫时间上的差异是否会被接收者认为是有意义的。在这里,我们通过回放实验测试了欧亚喜鹊(Pica pica)这种在领地防御中发出警报声(即所谓的喋喋不休的叫声)的物种是否会对相互重叠发出叫声的模拟成对入侵者做出比交替发出叫声者更强烈的反应。喜鹊对重放的重叠叫声的第一声鸣叫时间明显更长,但对重放的交替(因此更长)叫声序列的第一声鸣叫时间明显更短,而且靠近扬声器的距离明显更近。这些发现说明,鸟对成员的鸣叫时间很重要,但其复杂程度超出了我们的预测。重叠播放似乎引发了更长但更晚的初始鸣叫反应和更弱的接近反应,这表明喜鹊的不同反应方式反映了不同的唤醒水平或防御策略。这些结果也可能反映了接收者由于信号信息和感知信息之间的潜在不匹配而产生的不确定性:虽然重叠的鸣叫可能意味着双方都处于高度亢奋状态,但较长的交替序列可能被认为是更亢奋的较长信号。这些发现拓展了经典的鸣叫功能实验,表明成对鸣叫可以通过不同的方式协调它们的警报鸣叫来改变信息,这与广告信号中二重唱成对鸣叫的时间安排类似。
{"title":"Pair-Coordinated Calling: Eurasian Magpies Respond Differently to Simulated Intruder Pairs That Overlap or Alternate Their Calls","authors":"Miriam Kuspiel,&nbsp;Sjouke A. Kingma,&nbsp;Heleen Vermeulen,&nbsp;Marc Naguib","doi":"10.1111/eth.13515","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13515","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Animal vocalisations are widely used to signal strength or motivation of a caller in competitive interactions, such as in territorial defence. Substantial understanding of signalling functions in territorial conflicts is based on singing by male songbirds. Yet, in many species, both pair members call during territorial conflicts, as well as in predator-induced situations, leading to complex signalling interactions in which calls overlap or alternate. This raises the question as to whether or not variation in how individuals in pairs time their calls is perceived as meaningful by receivers. Here, we tested with playback experiments whether Eurasian magpies (<i>Pica pica</i>), a species producing alarm calls (so-called chatter calls) in territorial defence, respond stronger to simulated pair-intruders who overlap their calls with each other than to those who alternate them. Magpies emitted a significantly longer first chatter calls in response to playback with overlapping calls but chattered significantly sooner and approached the loudspeakers significantly more closely in response to playbacks of alternating (and therefore longer) call sequences. These findings exemplify that the timing of calls by pair members matters, but in more complex ways than we predicted. The overlapping playback appeared to trigger a longer yet later initial chatter response and a weaker approach response, suggesting that the different ways in which magpies respond reflect different levels of arousal or defence strategies. The results may also reflect uncertainty by receivers due to a potential mismatch between signalled and perceived information: While overlapping calls may signal high arousal by both callers, a longer alternating sequence could be perceived as a more aroused longer signal. These findings expand on classical experiments on call function, suggesting that pairs can vary the message by coordinating their alarm calls in different ways, similar to how duetting pairs time their song contributions in advertisement signalling.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 12","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13515","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142642395","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Agoutis: A Future Model for Ecologically Relevant Neuroscience and Physiology In Natura 阿古提鱼:自然界中与生态相关的神经科学和生理学的未来模型
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-18 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13514
J. I. Sanguinetti-Scheck, D. Gálvez

The overarching goal of neurobiology is to understand how complex behaviors are generated by the nervous system. The behavior of each species, and the brain that controls it, is shaped by the historical and current state of the environment that they inhabit. This fact is juxtaposed with the reductionist approach of neuroscience that isolates animals from their natural environment. Understanding how brains evolved to orchestrate the myriads of natural behaviors an animal performs in response to its environment requires an integrative approach to neuroscience that considers ecology, ethology, and evolution. Current technological developments are leading us to an inflection point at which studying brain functions in the wild is now possible. Ecological studies on how the environment affects behavior of animals (i.e., hibernation, foraging, food hoarding, and nest building) have framed a plurality of questions to be answered mechanistically, and yet, only few studies have addressed the relationship between the environment and the brain's anatomy and physiology. Neuroscience needs new animal models that allow us to tackle such questions in the wild. Here, we propose a new animal model for wild neuroscience, the agouti (Dasyprocta spp.), a large wild rodent playing a critical seasonal role in the maintenance of the central and south American rainforest ecosystems. We focus on how a rodent model, like the agouti, will allow for the investigation of large-scale brain dynamics during seasonal behaviors of ecological importance: scatter-hoarding and retrieval. We describe agouti evolution, ecology, and physiology as well as neuro-anatomical and neurophysiological studies, which have set the foundation for future neuroscience in natura. We suggest agoutis have the potential to be a groundbreaking model for wild neuroscience.

神经生物学的首要目标是了解神经系统是如何产生复杂行为的。每个物种的行为以及控制行为的大脑,都是由它们所居住的环境的历史和现状决定的。这一事实与神经科学将动物与自然环境隔离开来的还原论方法形成了鲜明对比。要了解大脑是如何进化以协调动物为适应环境而做出的无数自然行为,就需要神经科学采用一种综合方法,将生态学、伦理学和进化论考虑在内。当前的技术发展正将我们引向一个拐点,即在野外研究大脑功能现已成为可能。关于环境如何影响动物行为(如冬眠、觅食、囤积食物和筑巢)的生态学研究提出了许多需要从机理上回答的问题,但只有少数研究涉及环境与大脑解剖学和生理学之间的关系。神经科学需要新的动物模型,让我们能够在野外解决这些问题。在这里,我们为野生神经科学提出了一种新的动物模型--阿古提(Dasyprocta spp.),这是一种大型野生啮齿类动物,在维持中美洲和南美洲雨林生态系统中扮演着重要的季节性角色。我们的研究重点是如何利用啮齿类动物模型(如阿古提)来研究具有重要生态意义的季节性行为(分散囤积和检索)期间的大规模大脑动态。我们介绍了金丝猴的进化、生态学和生理学以及神经解剖学和神经生理学研究,这些研究为未来的自然神经科学奠定了基础。我们认为无尾熊有可能成为野生神经科学的开创性模型。
{"title":"The Agoutis: A Future Model for Ecologically Relevant Neuroscience and Physiology In Natura","authors":"J. I. Sanguinetti-Scheck,&nbsp;D. Gálvez","doi":"10.1111/eth.13514","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13514","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The overarching goal of neurobiology is to understand how complex behaviors are generated by the nervous system. The behavior of each species, and the brain that controls it, is shaped by the historical and current state of the environment that they inhabit. This fact is juxtaposed with the reductionist approach of neuroscience that isolates animals from their natural environment. Understanding how brains evolved to orchestrate the myriads of natural behaviors an animal performs in response to its environment requires an integrative approach to neuroscience that considers ecology, ethology, and evolution. Current technological developments are leading us to an inflection point at which studying brain functions in the wild is now possible. Ecological studies on how the environment affects behavior of animals (i.e., hibernation, foraging, food hoarding, and nest building) have framed a plurality of questions to be answered mechanistically, and yet, only few studies have addressed the relationship between the environment and the brain's anatomy and physiology. Neuroscience needs new animal models that allow us to tackle such questions in the wild. Here, we propose a new animal model for wild neuroscience, the agouti (<i>Dasyprocta</i> spp.), a large wild rodent playing a critical seasonal role in the maintenance of the central and south American rainforest ecosystems. We focus on how a rodent model, like the agouti, will allow for the investigation of large-scale brain dynamics during seasonal behaviors of ecological importance: scatter-hoarding and retrieval. We describe agouti evolution, ecology, and physiology as well as neuro-anatomical and neurophysiological studies, which have set the foundation for future neuroscience in natura. We suggest agoutis have the potential to be a groundbreaking model for wild neuroscience.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 12","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13514","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142642151","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cooperative and Solitary Mobbing to Defend Offspring in Wild Gray Mouse Lemurs, Microcebus murinus 野生灰狐猴的合作与单独攻击以保护后代
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-16 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13512
Sacha C. Engelhardt, Claudia Fichtel, Peter M. Kappeler

Gray mouse lemur, Microcebus murinus, mothers either cooperatively breed plurally with kin or breed solitarily. We describe the first observations of cooperative and solitary mobbing to defend offspring by wild cooperative breeding gray mouse lemur mothers in Kirindy Forest, Western Madagascar. We observed four groups of cooperatively breeding mothers and their offspring daily between 18:00 and 04:00 from January 1 to 15, 2023. Cooperative mobbing was observed twice, and solitary mobbing by a single cooperative breeding mother was observed once. There was one Malagasy tree boa, Sanzinia madagascariensis, per mobbing event. Mothers solitarily mobbed by directly approaching within 1 m of the boa, walking and changing distance while within 1 m of the boa. In addition to the solitary mobbing behaviors, cooperatively mobbing mothers approached the boa together from the same or different directions or alternated, and they gathered around the boa. Mothers collaborated in the context of offspring defense from predators and performed different complementary tasks: mobbing while another provided alloparental care by guarding the offspring of the association, that is, babysitting. Mothers performed similar mobbing behaviors and occasionally synchronized in time or coordinate in time and in space to approach from different directions and gather around the boa. Mothers did not come in contact with the boas during mobbing but kept a distance of at least 20 cm. On two mobbing events, the boa rose up with its head facing the mobbing mothers and climbed down the tree after the mobbing. Cooperative mobbing to defend offspring likely evolved by kin selection but reciprocity and an interaction between kin selection and reciprocity cannot be ruled out, since gray mouse lemur mothers cooperatively breed with kin and basically allonurse reciprocally.

灰鼠狐猴,母狐猴要么与亲属合作繁殖,要么单独繁殖。在马达加斯加西部的Kirindy森林,我们描述了野生合作繁殖的灰鼠狐猴母亲为保护后代而进行的合作和单独袭击的第一次观察。我们于2023年1月1日至15日每天18:00 - 04:00对四组合作繁殖的母亲及其后代进行了观察。观察到2次集体围捕,1次单独围捕。每次袭击事件中都有一条马达加斯加树蟒(Sanzinia madagascar)。母亲们在离蟒蛇1米的范围内直接靠近,并在距离蟒蛇1米的范围内走动和改变距离。除了单独的围捕行为外,合作围捕的母亲也会从相同或不同的方向或交替的方向一起接近蟒蛇,并聚集在蟒蛇周围。母亲在保护后代免受捕食者侵害的背景下合作,并执行不同的互补任务:围捕,而另一个通过保护该协会的后代提供异亲代照顾,即照看孩子。母亲们也有类似的围捕行为,偶尔会在时间上同步或在时间和空间上协调,从不同的方向接近并聚集在蟒蛇周围。在围捕过程中,母熊没有接触到蟒蛇,但与它们保持至少20厘米的距离。在两次围捕事件中,蟒蛇站起来,头朝着围捕的母亲,并在围捕后爬下了树。保护后代的合作群体可能是由亲缘选择进化而来的,但不能排除互惠以及亲缘选择和互惠之间的相互作用,因为灰鼠狐猴母亲与亲属合作繁殖,基本上是相互哺育。
{"title":"Cooperative and Solitary Mobbing to Defend Offspring in Wild Gray Mouse Lemurs, Microcebus murinus","authors":"Sacha C. Engelhardt,&nbsp;Claudia Fichtel,&nbsp;Peter M. Kappeler","doi":"10.1111/eth.13512","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13512","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Gray mouse lemur, <i>Microcebus murinus</i>, mothers either cooperatively breed plurally with kin or breed solitarily. We describe the first observations of cooperative and solitary mobbing to defend offspring by wild cooperative breeding gray mouse lemur mothers in Kirindy Forest, Western Madagascar. We observed four groups of cooperatively breeding mothers and their offspring daily between 18:00 and 04:00 from January 1 to 15, 2023. Cooperative mobbing was observed twice, and solitary mobbing by a single cooperative breeding mother was observed once. There was one Malagasy tree boa, <i>Sanzinia madagascariensis</i>, per mobbing event. Mothers solitarily mobbed by directly approaching within 1 m of the boa, walking and changing distance while within 1 m of the boa. In addition to the solitary mobbing behaviors, cooperatively mobbing mothers approached the boa together from the same or different directions or alternated, and they gathered around the boa. Mothers collaborated in the context of offspring defense from predators and performed different complementary tasks: mobbing while another provided alloparental care by guarding the offspring of the association, that is, babysitting. Mothers performed similar mobbing behaviors and occasionally synchronized in time or coordinate in time and in space to approach from different directions and gather around the boa. Mothers did not come in contact with the boas during mobbing but kept a distance of at least 20 cm. On two mobbing events, the boa rose up with its head facing the mobbing mothers and climbed down the tree after the mobbing. Cooperative mobbing to defend offspring likely evolved by kin selection but reciprocity and an interaction between kin selection and reciprocity cannot be ruled out, since gray mouse lemur mothers cooperatively breed with kin and basically allonurse reciprocally.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"131 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13512","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142861587","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Are scientific journals delaying doctoral theses? 科学期刊是否耽误了博士论文?
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-16 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13507
Wolfgang Goymann
<p>In case you are surprised, such email messages are not uncommon. In fact, the first such email I received was quite effective in exerting moral pressure on me as an editor. The message attempted to hold me accountable for the timely submission of a thesis. Presumably, this even comes with a kind of obligation to also accept the manuscript, no matter how crappy it might be. Otherwise, I as editor would be responsible for a doctoral student's failure, thereby possibly ending a promising scientific career. But is this really so?</p><p>Editors of reputable scientific journals have an ethical obligation to readers and authors to accept and publish manuscripts on the basis of scientific quality and merit. Therefore—after an initial screening to assess the overall suitability for the journal—manuscripts must be peer-reviewed. Based on the reviewers' comments and the editor's own assessment, the editor then decides whether to accept the manuscript, whether it needs some revision, or whether it should be rejected. The possibility to reject a manuscript is essential in this process—unless you work for a predatory journal whose only interest is to generate revenue for the publisher.</p><p>In <i>Ethology</i>, roughly half of all submissions are eventually rejected, so any attempt to guarantee an author a (positive) decision and within a set period of time would be irresponsible and unethical to all other authors and readers of the journal, who trust in the journal's reputation and expect only high-quality behavioural research to be published by <i>Ethology</i>.</p><p>Presumably, most people who have written an email such as the one mentioned above are not even aware that what they do is unethical. They probably write such emails in an attempt to help their students finish their theses in time. But where does the expectation come from that a journal could make a (presumably) positive decision within a certain period of time? The problem may have to do with how many universities deal with the submission of dissertations.</p><p>When I was a doctoral student, it was still common practice at German universities to submit dissertations as monographs. My university was sort of progressive in that it allowed doctoral students to structure their dissertations into separate chapters, each of which could be published as a separate paper. However, the pressure on doctoral students to publish as early as possible and ideally before finishing their degree has strongly increased since then. As a consequence, my university also changed its policy: now theses have to be submitted as monographs or cumulatively as separate chapters, two of which have to be published or at least have to be accepted by a scientific journal before thesis submission. Most doctoral students in biology choose such a cumulative thesis, even if this comes at the cost of losing time with submitting and revising manuscripts. As a consequence, funding often runs out long before the chapters have been
如果你感到惊讶,这样的电子邮件并不少见。事实上,我收到的第一封此类邮件就对我这个编辑施加了相当有效的道德压力。该邮件试图让我对及时提交论文负责。据推测,这甚至意味着我也有义务接受稿件,不管它有多糟糕。否则,作为编辑的我就要为一个博士生的失败负责,从而可能断送他前途无量的科研事业。知名科学期刊的编辑对读者和作者负有道德义务,必须根据稿件的科学质量和价值来接受和发表稿件。因此,在经过初步筛选以评估稿件是否适合该期刊后,必须对稿件进行同行评审。编辑根据审稿人的意见和自己的评估,决定是否接受稿件,是否需要修改,或者是否应该退稿。在这一过程中,退稿的可能性至关重要--除非你供职于掠夺性期刊,其唯一的兴趣就是为出版商创收。在《文选》中,大约一半的投稿最终都会被拒,因此,任何试图保证作者在规定时间内得到(肯定的)决定的做法,都是对所有其他作者和期刊读者的不负责任和不道德,因为他们信任期刊的声誉,并期望《文选》只发表高质量的行为学研究成果。他们写这样的邮件可能是为了帮助学生及时完成论文。但是,期望期刊在一定时间内做出(可能是)积极决定的想法从何而来?这个问题可能与许多大学如何处理学位论文的提交有关。当我还是一名博士生时,德国大学的普遍做法仍然是以专著的形式提交学位论文。我所在的大学算是比较先进的,它允许博士生将论文分成独立的章节,每一章都可以作为单独的论文发表。然而,自那以后,博士生尽早发表论文(最好在完成学位之前)的压力与日俱增。因此,我所在的大学也改变了政策:现在论文必须以专著或单独章节的形式提交,其中两章必须发表,或至少在提交论文前被科学杂志接受。大多数生物学博士生都选择这种累积式论文,即使这样做的代价是在提交和修改手稿方面浪费时间。因此,往往在章节发表和论文提交之前,经费早已用完。在这个过程中,就出现了上文提到的电子邮件,试图让编辑对及时提交论文负起道德责任。但是,如果说有什么道德义务让博士生按时提交论文的话,那也是大学及其投稿规则的责任,而不是期刊或编辑的责任。最近,我的一个学生为发表他的第一章论文而苦苦挣扎。经过两轮同行评审,审稿人对所有改动都很满意,并建议发表,但随后期刊编辑又开始了四轮编辑评审,要求对稿件的内容和形式做进一步的实质性改动。我当时就在想,这个编辑是在妨碍我的学生按时完成他的论文(而且会耗尽经费)。但我不得不承认,这不是编辑的责任。作为编辑,他要对期刊发表的稿件的质量和完整性负责:毕竟,我们决定把我学生的作品投给这本期刊,是因为它有很高的声誉。我可能不同意这位编辑的微观管理方法,但可以肯定的是,我的学生及时提交论文的责任不在他......Wolfgang Goymann:构思;写作--原稿;写作--审阅和编辑。
{"title":"Are scientific journals delaying doctoral theses?","authors":"Wolfgang Goymann","doi":"10.1111/eth.13507","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13507","url":null,"abstract":"&lt;p&gt;In case you are surprised, such email messages are not uncommon. In fact, the first such email I received was quite effective in exerting moral pressure on me as an editor. The message attempted to hold me accountable for the timely submission of a thesis. Presumably, this even comes with a kind of obligation to also accept the manuscript, no matter how crappy it might be. Otherwise, I as editor would be responsible for a doctoral student's failure, thereby possibly ending a promising scientific career. But is this really so?&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;Editors of reputable scientific journals have an ethical obligation to readers and authors to accept and publish manuscripts on the basis of scientific quality and merit. Therefore—after an initial screening to assess the overall suitability for the journal—manuscripts must be peer-reviewed. Based on the reviewers' comments and the editor's own assessment, the editor then decides whether to accept the manuscript, whether it needs some revision, or whether it should be rejected. The possibility to reject a manuscript is essential in this process—unless you work for a predatory journal whose only interest is to generate revenue for the publisher.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;In &lt;i&gt;Ethology&lt;/i&gt;, roughly half of all submissions are eventually rejected, so any attempt to guarantee an author a (positive) decision and within a set period of time would be irresponsible and unethical to all other authors and readers of the journal, who trust in the journal's reputation and expect only high-quality behavioural research to be published by &lt;i&gt;Ethology&lt;/i&gt;.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;Presumably, most people who have written an email such as the one mentioned above are not even aware that what they do is unethical. They probably write such emails in an attempt to help their students finish their theses in time. But where does the expectation come from that a journal could make a (presumably) positive decision within a certain period of time? The problem may have to do with how many universities deal with the submission of dissertations.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;When I was a doctoral student, it was still common practice at German universities to submit dissertations as monographs. My university was sort of progressive in that it allowed doctoral students to structure their dissertations into separate chapters, each of which could be published as a separate paper. However, the pressure on doctoral students to publish as early as possible and ideally before finishing their degree has strongly increased since then. As a consequence, my university also changed its policy: now theses have to be submitted as monographs or cumulatively as separate chapters, two of which have to be published or at least have to be accepted by a scientific journal before thesis submission. Most doctoral students in biology choose such a cumulative thesis, even if this comes at the cost of losing time with submitting and revising manuscripts. As a consequence, funding often runs out long before the chapters have been ","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13507","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142447770","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of Nest-Site Availability on Male–Male Competition and the Foraging Costs of Egg Attendance in an Arachnid With Exclusive Paternal Care 巢穴的可利用性对雄性-雄性竞争的影响以及蛛形纲独享父性照料的觅卵成本
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-07 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13511
Laís A. Grossel, Rachel M. Werneck, Glauco Machado

Natural cavities are monopolizable resources used as nest sites across many species exhibiting exclusive male care. However, our understanding of how the availability of this type of reproductive site influences male–male competition and the foraging costs associated with parental care is limited and based on studies of fish. The harvestman Magnispina neptunus offers the opportunity to explore these questions in invertebrates because males defend natural cavities used as oviposition sites and provide exclusive parental care. We hypothesized that low nest availability would increase male–male competition and the foraging costs of egg attendance. To test this hypothesis, we collected observational data in the field and conducted a laboratory experiment with two groups, low and high nest availability. Field data indicate natural cavity scarcity, as only 50% of the males were nest holders. Larger males were more likely to hold nests in the field, but body size did not predict nest ownership in the laboratory. In the field, nests with openings smaller than average were equally occupied by small and large males, while nests with openings larger than average were occupied almost exclusively by large males. The nest opening in the laboratory experiment was adjusted to be equal to the average found in the field, which may have increased nest defensibility, regardless of male size. Thus, prior residency may outweigh body size in determining nest possession. Because nest owners seem to have an advantage over intruders, the costs of abandoning a nest are likely high. This may explain why the frequency of nest takeovers and foraging outside the nests did not differ between experimental groups. In conclusion, although underexplored in empirical studies, external factors, such as the opening size of cavities defended by males of many species with resource-defense mating systems, may play a key role in the defensibility of nest sites.

天然洞穴是一种可垄断的资源,在许多物种中被用作巢穴,表现出专由雄性照料的特点。然而,我们对这种生殖场所的可用性如何影响雄性之间的竞争以及与父母照料相关的觅食成本的了解是有限的,而且是基于对鱼类的研究。由于雄性动物会保护作为产卵地点的天然洞穴,并提供专有的亲代照料,因此采食者Magnispina neptunus为在无脊椎动物中探讨这些问题提供了机会。我们假设,巢的可用性低会加剧雄性之间的竞争,增加产卵的觅食成本。为了验证这一假设,我们在野外收集了观察数据,并在实验室进行了巢穴可用性低和巢穴可用性高两组实验。野外数据表明自然巢穴稀缺,因为只有50%的雄性是巢的持有者。在野外,体型较大的雄性更有可能筑巢,但在实验室中,体型大小并不能预测巢的拥有情况。在野外,巢口小于平均值的巢被大小雄性平均占有,而巢口大于平均值的巢几乎只被大雄性占有。在实验室实验中,巢的开口被调整到与野外发现的平均值相等,这可能会增加巢的可防御性,而与雄性的大小无关。因此,在决定是否拥有巢时,先前的居住地可能比体型更重要。由于巢的拥有者似乎比入侵者更有优势,因此放弃巢的成本可能很高。这也许可以解释为什么不同实验组之间在巢外占巢和觅食的频率没有差异。总之,尽管在实证研究中探索不足,但外部因素,如许多具有资源防御交配系统的物种的雄性所防御的洞穴的开口大小,可能对巢穴的可防御性起着关键作用。
{"title":"Effects of Nest-Site Availability on Male–Male Competition and the Foraging Costs of Egg Attendance in an Arachnid With Exclusive Paternal Care","authors":"Laís A. Grossel,&nbsp;Rachel M. Werneck,&nbsp;Glauco Machado","doi":"10.1111/eth.13511","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13511","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Natural cavities are monopolizable resources used as nest sites across many species exhibiting exclusive male care. However, our understanding of how the availability of this type of reproductive site influences male–male competition and the foraging costs associated with parental care is limited and based on studies of fish. The harvestman <i>Magnispina neptunus</i> offers the opportunity to explore these questions in invertebrates because males defend natural cavities used as oviposition sites and provide exclusive parental care. We hypothesized that low nest availability would increase male–male competition and the foraging costs of egg attendance. To test this hypothesis, we collected observational data in the field and conducted a laboratory experiment with two groups, low and high nest availability. Field data indicate natural cavity scarcity, as only 50% of the males were nest holders. Larger males were more likely to hold nests in the field, but body size did not predict nest ownership in the laboratory. In the field, nests with openings smaller than average were equally occupied by small and large males, while nests with openings larger than average were occupied almost exclusively by large males. The nest opening in the laboratory experiment was adjusted to be equal to the average found in the field, which may have increased nest defensibility, regardless of male size. Thus, prior residency may outweigh body size in determining nest possession. Because nest owners seem to have an advantage over intruders, the costs of abandoning a nest are likely high. This may explain why the frequency of nest takeovers and foraging outside the nests did not differ between experimental groups. In conclusion, although underexplored in empirical studies, external factors, such as the opening size of cavities defended by males of many species with resource-defense mating systems, may play a key role in the defensibility of nest sites.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 12","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142641577","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Uncoupled Evolutionary Patterns in Spectral and Temporal Components of Acoustic Signals in Anurans Associated With Streams 与溪流有关的无尾类动物声信号的频谱和时间成分的非耦合进化模式
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-03 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13510
Fernando Vargas-Salinas, Carlos A. Londoño-Guarnizo, Sebastián Duarte-Marín, Olga L. Torres-Suárez, Adolfo Amézquita

The use of high-pitched auditory signals by species communicating alongside noisy streams has been often considered an adaptation, yet studies testing this hypothesis have yielded inconclusive results. The major challenge has been to quantify the proportion of across-species signal variation that could be attributed to either common history (phylogenetic load) or adaptation. We further advance in this approach by comparing the phylogenetic load between different components of anuran calls. Because stream noise allegedly represents a stronger selective pressure for call frequency than for call temporal traits, we predicted a weaker phylogenetic signal in call frequency, particularly in the taxa that breed alongside streams. We first built a phylogenetic hypothesis using four mitochondrial genes on each of three clades: the genus Hyloscirtus and the family Centrolenidae, which call alongside streams and the subfamily Phyllomedusinae, known to call at lentic water bodies. In parallel, we compiled data on the advertisement calls of 154 species and used them to calculate Blomberg's K values as a proxy for the phylogenetic load (signal) of the call traits. The phylogenetic signal was weaker in spectral than in temporal call traits within the Hyloscirtus calls and weak or absent in both spectral and temporal components of Phyllomedusine calls. Against our expectations, the phylogenetic signal was strong in call frequency, but absent in call temporal components of the centrolenid calls. Our results support uncoupled evolution between spectral and temporal components of anuran calls. They also indicate that the selective role of abiotic noise varies among taxa and that other factors must be invoked to fully understand among-species variation in advertisement calls.

在嘈杂的溪流边进行交流的物种使用高音调听觉信号通常被认为是一种适应,但对这一假设进行检验的研究却没有得出结论。主要的挑战是量化可归因于共同历史(系统发育负荷)或适应的跨物种信号变异的比例。我们通过比较无尾类叫声不同成分之间的系统发育负荷,进一步推进了这一研究方法。由于溪流噪声据称对鸣叫频率的选择压力大于对鸣叫时间特征的选择压力,我们预测鸣叫频率的系统发育信号较弱,尤其是在溪流边繁殖的类群中。我们首先利用三个支系中每个支系的四个线粒体基因建立了一个系统发育假说,这三个支系分别是:在溪流边鸣叫的 Hyloscirtus 属和 Centrolenidae 科,以及已知在湖泊水体中鸣叫的 Phyllomedusinae 亚科。与此同时,我们还收集了 154 个物种的广告鸣叫数据,并利用这些数据计算了布隆伯格 K 值,以此来代表鸣叫特征的系统发生负荷(信号)。在Hyloscirtus的叫声中,系统发生信号在频谱上比在时间上弱,而在Phyllomedusine的叫声中,频谱和时间部分的系统发生信号都很弱或不存在。与我们的预期相反,系统发生学信号在百灵鸟的叫声频率中很强,但在叫声时间成分中却不存在。我们的研究结果支持无尾类叫声的频谱和时间成分之间的非耦合进化。这些结果还表明,非生物噪声在不同类群中的选择性作用是不同的,要全面了解广告叫声的种间变异,还必须考虑其他因素。
{"title":"Uncoupled Evolutionary Patterns in Spectral and Temporal Components of Acoustic Signals in Anurans Associated With Streams","authors":"Fernando Vargas-Salinas,&nbsp;Carlos A. Londoño-Guarnizo,&nbsp;Sebastián Duarte-Marín,&nbsp;Olga L. Torres-Suárez,&nbsp;Adolfo Amézquita","doi":"10.1111/eth.13510","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13510","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>The use of high-pitched auditory signals by species communicating alongside noisy streams has been often considered an adaptation, yet studies testing this hypothesis have yielded inconclusive results. The major challenge has been to quantify the proportion of across-species signal variation that could be attributed to either common history (phylogenetic load) or adaptation. We further advance in this approach by comparing the phylogenetic load between different components of anuran calls. Because stream noise allegedly represents a stronger selective pressure for call frequency than for call temporal traits, we predicted a weaker phylogenetic signal in call frequency, particularly in the taxa that breed alongside streams. We first built a phylogenetic hypothesis using four mitochondrial genes on each of three clades: the genus <i>Hyloscirtus</i> and the family Centrolenidae, which call alongside streams and the subfamily Phyllomedusinae, known to call at lentic water bodies. In parallel, we compiled data on the advertisement calls of 154 species and used them to calculate Blomberg's <i>K</i> values as a proxy for the phylogenetic load (signal) of the call traits. The phylogenetic signal was weaker in spectral than in temporal call traits within the <i>Hyloscirtus</i> calls and weak or absent in both spectral and temporal components of Phyllomedusine calls. Against our expectations, the phylogenetic signal was strong in call frequency, but absent in call temporal components of the centrolenid calls. Our results support uncoupled evolution between spectral and temporal components of anuran calls. They also indicate that the selective role of abiotic noise varies among taxa and that other factors must be invoked to fully understand among-species variation in advertisement calls.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-10-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142447522","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Personality may modulate learning and memory differences in two taxa of the African striped mouse genus Rhabdomys 性格可能调节非洲条纹鼠属两个类群的学习和记忆差异
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-23 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13509
Candice N. Neves, Neville Pillay

Different environments place different cognitive demands on constituent taxa. Learning and memory involve cognitive processes with associated costs, and it is expected that different levels of learning will occur in taxa from different environments. Greater memory loads linked to increased environmental complexity require greater learning and memory capacities. We investigated the variation in learning and memory in sister taxa of striped mice (genus Rhabdomys). We studied two populations each of the mesic grassland-occurring R. d. chakae and the mostly arid-occurring R. pumilio. We conducted two sets of experiments. (1) In a novel object recognition (NOR) test, we assessed memory by recording the duration of exploration of similar and novel objects by test mice. (2) In an associative learning task, we assessed whether mice could associate specific scents with or without a food incentive or with different quantities of the food incentive in previous training phases. We measured the latency of mice to contact scents in a two-sample choice in the test phase. In the NOR test, R. pumilio spent less time investigating similar objects in a training trial than R. d. chakae but increased absolute exploration of the novel object when presented with a novel and a familiar object in the retention trial, suggesting a sensitization to the novel object by R. pumilio. In the associative learning experiments, R. pumilio approached the stimuli faster than R. d. chakae, whereas mice from both taxa preferred scents associated with a seed versus no seed and scents associated with 5 seeds versus 1 seed. The data provide evidence of taxon-level differences in learning and memory, likely related to environmentally modulated personality differences between the taxa.

不同的环境对组成类群的生物提出了不同的认知要求。学习和记忆的认知过程需要付出相应的代价,因此不同环境中的类群需要不同程度的学习。与环境复杂性增加有关的更大记忆负荷需要更强的学习和记忆能力。我们研究了条纹鼠(Rhabdomys 属)姊妹类群在学习和记忆方面的变异。我们研究了中生代草原条纹鼠(R. d. chakae)和干旱地区条纹鼠(R. pumilio)的两个种群。我们进行了两组实验。(1)在新物体识别(NOR)测试中,我们通过记录受试小鼠探索相似和新物体的持续时间来评估记忆力。(2)在联想学习任务中,我们评估了小鼠是否能将特定气味与之前训练阶段的食物诱因或不同数量的食物诱因联系起来。在测试阶段,我们测量了小鼠在双样本选择中接触气味的潜伏期。在NOR测试中,与R. d. chakae相比,R. pumilio在训练试验中花在研究相似物体上的时间较少,但在保留试验中,当出现一个新物体和一个熟悉物体时,R. pumilio对新物体的绝对研究时间增加,这表明R. pumilio对新物体敏感。在联想学习实验中,R. pumilio 比 R. d. chakae 接近刺激物的速度更快,而这两个类群的小鼠都更喜欢与种子相关的气味而不是没有种子的气味,以及与 5 粒种子相关的气味而不是 1 粒种子的气味。这些数据提供了分类群学习和记忆水平差异的证据,很可能与分类群之间受环境调节的个性差异有关。
{"title":"Personality may modulate learning and memory differences in two taxa of the African striped mouse genus Rhabdomys","authors":"Candice N. Neves,&nbsp;Neville Pillay","doi":"10.1111/eth.13509","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13509","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Different environments place different cognitive demands on constituent taxa. Learning and memory involve cognitive processes with associated costs, and it is expected that different levels of learning will occur in taxa from different environments. Greater memory loads linked to increased environmental complexity require greater learning and memory capacities. We investigated the variation in learning and memory in sister taxa of striped mice (genus <i>Rhabdomys</i>). We studied two populations each of the mesic grassland-occurring <i>R. d. chakae</i> and the mostly arid-occurring <i>R. pumilio.</i> We conducted two sets of experiments. (1) In a novel object recognition (NOR) test, we assessed memory by recording the duration of exploration of similar and novel objects by test mice. (2) In an associative learning task, we assessed whether mice could associate specific scents with or without a food incentive or with different quantities of the food incentive in previous training phases. We measured the latency of mice to contact scents in a two-sample choice in the test phase. In the NOR test, <i>R. pumilio</i> spent less time investigating similar objects in a training trial than <i>R. d. chakae</i> but increased absolute exploration of the novel object when presented with a novel and a familiar object in the retention trial, suggesting a sensitization to the novel object by <i>R. pumilio</i>. In the associative learning experiments, <i>R. pumilio</i> approached the stimuli faster than <i>R. d. chakae</i>, whereas mice from both taxa preferred scents associated with a seed versus no seed and scents associated with 5 seeds versus 1 seed. The data provide evidence of taxon-level differences in learning and memory, likely related to environmentally modulated personality differences between the taxa.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13509","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142449228","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Male and female age affects the reproductive potential of two tephritid flies 雄蝇和雌蝇的年龄影响两种表蝇的生殖潜力
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-11 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13506
Viviana Díaz, Antonella Giudice, Alfonsina Palladini, Andrea Moyano, Gisela Castillo, Diana Pérez-Staples, Solana Abraham

In insects, aging produces deterioration in physiological and cellular functioning, affecting their reproductive potential. Anastrepha fraterculus and Ceratitis capitata are two fruit fly species where overwintering adults resume their reproductive activity in spring, giving old individuals the possibility of mating with young adults. Most age studies focus on male reproductive capacity; however, we lack information on how the interaction between the ages of both sexes can determine post-mating processes. Here, we studied sex and age effects on (i) female fecundity and fertility, (ii) failure to leave viable offspring (reproductive failure), and (iii) female remating behavior. We found that young pairs of both species had higher fecundity, but young C. capitata males mated with old females had the lowest fecundity. This suggests that overwintering flies in this species will not substantially contribute to the next generation. We also found in C. capitata more prevalent reproductive failure in hetero-age combinations, which could be due to age recognition between mates, resulting perhaps in differential ejaculate allocation. Copula duration was positively associated with female age, yet it was longer for older A. fraterculus females and shorter for C. capitata females. Female remating was lower when young females mated with old males in C. capitata. This would imply that males perceive young females of “good quality” and thus invest and transfer all the ejaculate possible to ensure the delay of renewal of female receptivity. Aging does not always cause a decline in reproductive potential, which may be important in species that overwinter as adults. Complex interactions between female physiology and male ejaculate senescence can impact postcopulatory behaviors that affect reproductive success for both sexes.

在昆虫中,衰老会导致生理和细胞功能退化,影响其繁殖潜力。Anastrepha fraterculus 和 Ceratitis capitata 是两个果蝇物种,其越冬成虫在春季恢复生殖活动,使老年个体有可能与年轻成虫交配。大多数年龄研究集中于雄性的生殖能力;然而,我们缺乏有关两性年龄之间的相互作用如何决定交配后过程的信息。在这里,我们研究了性别和年龄对(i)雌性繁殖力和生育能力、(ii)未能留下可存活后代(繁殖失败)和(iii)雌性再交配行为的影响。我们发现,两种苍蝇的年轻配对繁殖力都较高,但与老龄雌蝇交配的年轻雄蝇繁殖力最低。这表明该物种的越冬蝇对下一代的贡献不大。我们还发现,在异龄组合中,C. capitata的繁殖失败率更高,这可能是由于配偶之间的年龄识别,从而导致射精分配的差异。交配持续时间与雌性年龄呈正相关,但年龄较大的A. fraterculus雌性交配持续时间较长,而C. capitata雌性交配持续时间较短。C.capitata的年轻雌性与年老雄性交配时,雌性的再交配率较低。这意味着雄性认为年轻雌性 "质量好",因此会尽可能投入和转移所有射精,以确保推迟雌性接受能力的恢复。衰老并不总是导致繁殖潜力下降,这对于以成体越冬的物种可能很重要。雌性生理机能和雄性射精衰老之间复杂的相互作用会影响交配后的行为,从而影响雌雄的繁殖成功率。
{"title":"Male and female age affects the reproductive potential of two tephritid flies","authors":"Viviana Díaz,&nbsp;Antonella Giudice,&nbsp;Alfonsina Palladini,&nbsp;Andrea Moyano,&nbsp;Gisela Castillo,&nbsp;Diana Pérez-Staples,&nbsp;Solana Abraham","doi":"10.1111/eth.13506","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13506","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In insects, aging produces deterioration in physiological and cellular functioning, affecting their reproductive potential. <i>Anastrepha fraterculus</i> and <i>Ceratitis capitata</i> are two fruit fly species where overwintering adults resume their reproductive activity in spring, giving old individuals the possibility of mating with young adults. Most age studies focus on male reproductive capacity; however, we lack information on how the interaction between the ages of both sexes can determine post-mating processes. Here, we studied sex and age effects on (i) female fecundity and fertility, (ii) failure to leave viable offspring (reproductive failure), and (iii) female remating behavior. We found that young pairs of both species had higher fecundity, but young <i>C. capitata</i> males mated with old females had the lowest fecundity. This suggests that overwintering flies in this species will not substantially contribute to the next generation. We also found in <i>C. capitata</i> more prevalent reproductive failure in hetero-age combinations, which could be due to age recognition between mates, resulting perhaps in differential ejaculate allocation. Copula duration was positively associated with female age, yet it was longer for older <i>A. fraterculus</i> females and shorter for <i>C. capitata</i> females. Female remating was lower when young females mated with old males in <i>C. capitata</i>. This would imply that males perceive young females of “good quality” and thus invest and transfer all the ejaculate possible to ensure the delay of renewal of female receptivity. Aging does not always cause a decline in reproductive potential, which may be important in species that overwinter as adults. Complex interactions between female physiology and male ejaculate senescence can impact postcopulatory behaviors that affect reproductive success for both sexes.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-09-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142184415","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Polistes dominula spatial learning abilities while foraging Polistes dominula觅食时的空间学习能力
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-11 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13505
Sabrina Moreyra, Mariana Lozada

The eusocial “paper wasp” Polistes dominula is an invasive species that has successfully established worldwide. In the present study, we investigate the foraging behaviour of P. dominula wasps when revisiting previously gathered resources. We recorded the number of learning flights and food manipulation efficiency performed by a single target wasp during several consecutive collecting visits. Additionally, we analysed P. dominula relocating behaviour when dealing with changes in food position. We assessed whether wasps choose the previously visited dish containing food or opt for a novel baited dish placed 60 cm away. Then, we trained wasps to collect food either once or three times and compared the time taken to find the novel food position when it was displaced 60 cm away. This is the first study to demonstrate relocating behaviour in P. dominula in which wasps rapidly learned to return to certain food sites. A significant reduction in learning flights after only one collecting visit was observed. Likewise, wasps learned to manipulate the resource spending less time after each experience. Moreover, when two food sources were offered, after just one visit, the majority of wasps collected the resource from the previously rewarded dish. Furthermore, when food was displaced, foragers found the new food location more rapidly after one visit than after three consecutive collecting experiences. Our findings contribute to the understanding of P. dominula behavioural plasticity while collecting food sources in anthropized environments, which highlight the importance of considering such learning experiences.

社会性 "纸黄蜂 "Polistes dominula是一种入侵物种,已在全球成功立足。在本研究中,我们调查了 P. dominula 黄蜂重访以前收集的资源时的觅食行为。我们记录了一只目标黄蜂在连续几次采集过程中的学习飞行次数和操纵食物的效率。此外,我们还分析了栉蜂在应对食物位置变化时的迁移行为。我们评估了黄蜂是选择之前去过的装有食物的盘子,还是选择放置在 60 厘米外的一个新的有诱饵的盘子。然后,我们训练黄蜂采集食物一次或三次,并比较了当食物被移到 60 厘米以外时,黄蜂找到新食物位置所需的时间。这是首次研究证明栉水母的迁移行为,在这种行为中,黄蜂迅速学会了返回特定的食物地点。仅在一次采集后,就观察到学习飞行次数明显减少。同样,黄蜂在每次经历后花费较少的时间就学会了操纵资源。此外,当提供两种食物来源时,大多数黄蜂只需访问一次就能从先前获得奖励的盘子中收集资源。此外,当食物被转移时,觅食者在一次访问后找到新食物地点的速度比连续三次采集后更快。我们的研究结果有助于了解栉蜂在人类化环境中采集食物源时的行为可塑性,并强调了考虑这种学习经历的重要性。
{"title":"Polistes dominula spatial learning abilities while foraging","authors":"Sabrina Moreyra,&nbsp;Mariana Lozada","doi":"10.1111/eth.13505","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13505","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The eusocial “paper wasp” <i>Polistes dominula</i> is an invasive species that has successfully established worldwide. In the present study, we investigate the foraging behaviour of <i>P. dominula</i> wasps when revisiting previously gathered resources. We recorded the number of learning flights and food manipulation efficiency performed by a single target wasp during several consecutive collecting visits. Additionally, we analysed <i>P. dominula</i> relocating behaviour when dealing with changes in food position. We assessed whether wasps choose the previously visited dish containing food or opt for a novel baited dish placed 60 cm away. Then, we trained wasps to collect food either once or three times and compared the time taken to find the novel food position when it was displaced 60 cm away. This is the first study to demonstrate relocating behaviour in <i>P. dominula</i> in which wasps rapidly learned to return to certain food sites. A significant reduction in learning flights after only one collecting visit was observed. Likewise, wasps learned to manipulate the resource spending less time after each experience. Moreover, when two food sources were offered, after just one visit, the majority of wasps collected the resource from the previously rewarded dish. Furthermore, when food was displaced, foragers found the new food location more rapidly after one visit than after three consecutive collecting experiences. Our findings contribute to the understanding of <i>P. dominula</i> behavioural plasticity while collecting food sources in anthropized environments, which highlight the importance of considering such learning experiences.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-09-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142224088","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Intra-individual modulations and inter-individual variations of female signals in the domestic canary (Serinus canaria) 金丝雀雌性信号的个体内调节和个体间变化
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-10 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13502
Camille Le Gal, Sébastien Derégnaucourt, Mathieu Amy

During courtship, animals perform conspicuous and elaborate signals. In birds, courtship involved often mutual engagement by both partners but most research on courtship behaviours has focused on male signals despite of growing interest for female signals in recent years. Here, we show that female domestic canaries (Serinus canaria) have the ability to modulate their sexual response to male songs. To do so, we exposed females to two types of song (very attractive and moderately attractive songs) during two consecutive reproductive cycles. We measured both visual (copulation solicitation displays, CSD) and vocal signals (copulation solicitation trills, CST; contact calls, CC and simple trills, ST) emitted by the females during song broadcast. We observed that females could modify the characteristics of their signals (duration and the number of elements of CSD, duration, frequency and number of notes of calls) depending on song attractiveness and the number of times they were exposed to a male's song. We also found that some females always emitted more signals than others (i.e. stable inter-individual differences) regardless of the song attractiveness and across reproductive cycles. Further studies are necessary to check whether female signals constitute sexual ornaments and if they could stimulate male canaries during courtship.

在求偶过程中,动物会发出明显而复杂的信号。在鸟类中,求偶通常需要双方的共同参与,但尽管近年来对雌性求偶信号的研究越来越多,但大多数关于求偶行为的研究都集中在雄性信号上。在这里,我们发现雌性金丝雀(Serinus canaria)有能力调节其对雄性歌曲的性反应。为此,我们在两个连续的繁殖周期中让雌性金丝雀接触两种类型的歌曲(极具吸引力的歌曲和中等吸引力的歌曲)。我们测量了雌性在播放歌曲时发出的视觉信号(交配引诱显示,CSD)和声音信号(交配引诱颤音,CST;接触叫声,CC和简单颤音,ST)。我们观察到,雌性会根据雄性歌声的吸引力和接触雄性歌声的次数来改变其信号的特征(CSD的持续时间和元素数量,呼叫的持续时间、频率和音符数量)。我们还发现,无论鸣声的吸引力如何,在不同的繁殖周期,一些雌鸟总是比其他雌鸟发出更多的信号(即稳定的个体间差异)。有必要进行进一步的研究,以检验雌性信号是否构成性装饰品,以及是否能在求偶过程中刺激雄性金丝雀。
{"title":"Intra-individual modulations and inter-individual variations of female signals in the domestic canary (Serinus canaria)","authors":"Camille Le Gal,&nbsp;Sébastien Derégnaucourt,&nbsp;Mathieu Amy","doi":"10.1111/eth.13502","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13502","url":null,"abstract":"<p>During courtship, animals perform conspicuous and elaborate signals. In birds, courtship involved often mutual engagement by both partners but most research on courtship behaviours has focused on male signals despite of growing interest for female signals in recent years. Here, we show that female domestic canaries (<i>Serinus canaria</i>) have the ability to modulate their sexual response to male songs. To do so, we exposed females to two types of song (very attractive and moderately attractive songs) during two consecutive reproductive cycles. We measured both visual (copulation solicitation displays, CSD) and vocal signals (copulation solicitation trills, CST; contact calls, CC and simple trills, ST) emitted by the females during song broadcast. We observed that females could modify the characteristics of their signals (duration and the number of elements of CSD, duration, frequency and number of notes of calls) depending on song attractiveness and the number of times they were exposed to a male's song. We also found that some females always emitted more signals than others (i.e. stable inter-individual differences) regardless of the song attractiveness and across reproductive cycles. Further studies are necessary to check whether female signals constitute sexual ornaments and if they could stimulate male canaries during courtship.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-09-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142184453","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Ethology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1