首页 > 最新文献

Ethology最新文献

英文 中文
Sex and age differences in the preference for materials for the communal nests of sociable weavers Philetairus socius 善于交际的织女 Philetairus socius 对公共巢穴材料偏好的性别和年龄差异
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-05-18 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13472
Nicolas J. Silva, Fantine Benoit, Andrew Elliott, Charlotte Rault, Pierre Colençon, Rita Covas, Claire Doutrelant

Many animals build structures that are used for shelter, reproduction or to capture prey. The type of material used to build these structures is likely to influence their solidity, thermoregulation capacity and, in some species, may influence the attractiveness of the builders. In the case of animal nests, evidence for preference of nesting material has been documented in several species but, to date, few field experiments have been conducted, and it was seldom investigated whether individuals' attributes affected those preferences. We investigated these preferences in relation to individual attributes on sociable weavers (Philetairus socius) that build communally one of the largest known nest structures, using dry grass. We conducted an experiment where we presented two piles of straws, long and short, to wild individuals. We recorded 900 h of video and used a deep learning method to automatically detect images where birds were present (266 colour-ringed individuals). Our results showed that males picked more straws than females and showed a preference for longer straws, while no preference was found for females. In addition, older males showed a preference for long straws compared to younger males. Finally, males displayed higher repeatability than females in their preference for longer straws. In conclusion, we show that choice of nesting material is not random, and that preferences are associated with individual attributes. Future studies should assess how building is associated with social status, mating and reproductive success of the individuals building, to investigate which of these factors could have shaped the evolution of these preferences.

许多动物建造用于栖息、繁殖或捕获猎物的结构。用于建造这些结构的材料类型可能会影响其坚固性和温度调节能力,在某些物种中,还可能影响建造者的吸引力。就动物巢而言,有证据表明一些物种对筑巢材料有偏好,但迄今为止,很少进行过野外实验,也很少研究个体的属性是否会影响这些偏好。我们研究了交际织女(Philetairus socius)的这些偏好与个体属性的关系,交际织女使用干草共同建造了已知最大的巢穴结构之一。我们进行了一项实验,向野生个体展示了长短两堆稻草。我们录制了 900 小时的视频,并使用深度学习方法自动检测鸟类出现的图像(266 只彩环个体)。我们的结果显示,雄性比雌性采摘更多的吸管,并表现出对较长吸管的偏好,而雌性则没有这种偏好。此外,年龄较大的雄性比年龄较小的雄性更偏爱长吸管。最后,在偏好长吸管方面,雄性比雌性表现出更高的重复性。总之,我们的研究表明,筑巢材料的选择不是随机的,其偏好与个体属性有关。未来的研究应该评估筑巢与筑巢个体的社会地位、交配和繁殖成功率之间的关系,以研究这些因素中哪些可能影响了这些偏好的进化。
{"title":"Sex and age differences in the preference for materials for the communal nests of sociable weavers Philetairus socius","authors":"Nicolas J. Silva,&nbsp;Fantine Benoit,&nbsp;Andrew Elliott,&nbsp;Charlotte Rault,&nbsp;Pierre Colençon,&nbsp;Rita Covas,&nbsp;Claire Doutrelant","doi":"10.1111/eth.13472","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13472","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Many animals build structures that are used for shelter, reproduction or to capture prey. The type of material used to build these structures is likely to influence their solidity, thermoregulation capacity and, in some species, may influence the attractiveness of the builders. In the case of animal nests, evidence for preference of nesting material has been documented in several species but, to date, few field experiments have been conducted, and it was seldom investigated whether individuals' attributes affected those preferences. We investigated these preferences in relation to individual attributes on sociable weavers (<i>Philetairus socius</i>) that build communally one of the largest known nest structures, using dry grass. We conducted an experiment where we presented two piles of straws, long and short, to wild individuals. We recorded 900 h of video and used a deep learning method to automatically detect images where birds were present (266 colour-ringed individuals). Our results showed that males picked more straws than females and showed a preference for longer straws, while no preference was found for females. In addition, older males showed a preference for long straws compared to younger males. Finally, males displayed higher repeatability than females in their preference for longer straws. In conclusion, we show that choice of nesting material is not random, and that preferences are associated with individual attributes. Future studies should assess how building is associated with social status, mating and reproductive success of the individuals building, to investigate which of these factors could have shaped the evolution of these preferences.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-05-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13472","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141062985","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Absence of female preference and the origin of a unisexual species, the Amazon molly (Poecilia formosa) 没有雌性偏好和单性物种亚马逊鲂(Poecilia formosa)的起源
IF 1.3 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-05-10 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13469
Caden Smith, Waldir Miron Berbel-Filho, Montrai Spikes, Frederic Fyon, Francisco Úbeda, Ingo Schlupp

The role of hybridization as a formative process in evolution has received much attention in the past few decades. A particularly fascinating outcome of hybrid speciation is the formation of asexual hybrid species. The Amazon molly (Poecilia formosa) is such a hybrid and originated from a P. mexicana mother and a P. latipinna father. Consequently, a heterospecific mating must have occurred leading to the Amazon molly, indicating a breakdown of any potential prezygotic isolation between parental species. Here we studied the female mate preferences of extant P. mexicana and P. latipinna from several populations using standard binary choice tests with males of both sexual species that were matched for size. Poecilia mexicana and P. latipinna can be crossed in the lab, however, the offspring are not asexual, but sexual F1s. In our study, we generated F1s and tested their mating preferences with sexual males of both P. mexicana and P. latipinna against F1 males. Overall, our results show that in extant P. mexicana and P. latipinna no female preference for conspecific males was detectable. Consequently, heterospecific matings are possible and not hindered by any apparent behavioral prezygotic isolation. If female preferences in these species were comparable around the time the Amazon molly originated as a hybrid species ca. 100,000 years ago, matings leading to hybrids would be very likely. F1 females also have no discernable mating preferences for either sexual males or F1 males. Such lack of prezygotic behavioral isolation could potentially lead to F2 individuals, backcrosses, and introgression.

杂交作为进化过程中的一个形成过程,在过去几十年里受到了广泛关注。杂交物种形成的一个特别迷人的结果是无性杂交物种的形成。亚马逊鲂(Poecilia formosa)就是这样一种杂交种,它的母本是 P. mexicana,父本是 P. latipinna。因此,一定发生了异种交配,导致了亚马逊鲂的出现,这表明亲本物种之间潜在的祖先隔离被打破了。在这里,我们使用标准的二元选择测试方法,研究了现存的墨西哥杓鹬和拉丁杓鹬的雌性交配偏好。Poecilia mexicana和P. latipinna可以在实验室中杂交,但后代不是无性的,而是有性的F1s。在我们的研究中,我们产生了 F1s,并用有性雄性 P. mexicana 和 P. latipinna 与 F1 雄性进行了交配偏好测试。总体而言,我们的结果表明,在现存的 P. mexicana 和 P. latipinna 中,检测不到雌性对同种雄性的偏好。因此,异种配对是可能的,而且不会受到任何明显的行为性同种前隔离的阻碍。如果这些物种的雌性偏好在亚马逊鲂作为杂交物种起源于约 10 万年前时具有可比性,那么导致杂交的交配将是非常可能的。F1雌性对有性雄性或F1雄性也没有明显的交配偏好。这种缺乏婚前行为隔离的情况有可能导致 F2个体、回交和引种。
{"title":"Absence of female preference and the origin of a unisexual species, the Amazon molly (Poecilia formosa)","authors":"Caden Smith,&nbsp;Waldir Miron Berbel-Filho,&nbsp;Montrai Spikes,&nbsp;Frederic Fyon,&nbsp;Francisco Úbeda,&nbsp;Ingo Schlupp","doi":"10.1111/eth.13469","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13469","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The role of hybridization as a formative process in evolution has received much attention in the past few decades. A particularly fascinating outcome of hybrid speciation is the formation of asexual hybrid species. The Amazon molly (<i>Poecilia formosa</i>) is such a hybrid and originated from a <i>P. mexicana</i> mother and a <i>P. latipinna</i> father. Consequently, a heterospecific mating must have occurred leading to the Amazon molly, indicating a breakdown of any potential prezygotic isolation between parental species. Here we studied the female mate preferences of extant <i>P. mexicana</i> and <i>P. latipinna</i> from several populations using standard binary choice tests with males of both sexual species that were matched for size. <i>Poecilia mexicana</i> and <i>P. latipinna</i> can be crossed in the lab, however, the offspring are not asexual, but sexual F<sub>1</sub>s. In our study, we generated F<sub>1</sub>s and tested their mating preferences with sexual males of both <i>P. mexicana</i> and <i>P. latipinna</i> against F<sub>1</sub> males. Overall, our results show that in extant <i>P. mexicana</i> and <i>P. latipinna</i> no female preference for conspecific males was detectable. Consequently, heterospecific matings are possible and not hindered by any apparent behavioral prezygotic isolation. If female preferences in these species were comparable around the time the Amazon molly originated as a hybrid species ca. 100,000 years ago, matings leading to hybrids would be very likely. F<sub>1</sub> females also have no discernable mating preferences for either sexual males or F<sub>1</sub> males. Such lack of prezygotic behavioral isolation could potentially lead to F<sub>2</sub> individuals, backcrosses, and introgression.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-05-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140935129","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Steps into a Small World: First glimpses on everyday moment-to-moment decision making in an ecologically meaningful multi-choice system for assessing animal preferences 步入小世界:评估动物偏好的具有生态意义的多选择系统中的日常决策窥探
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-10 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13468
Fiona Puls, Louisa-Mae Kosin, Fiona Garbisch, Chadi Touma, Christa Thöne-Reineke, Lorenz Gygax

The behaviour shown by an animal at any one time is the consolidated output of its behavioural control mechanism. Moreover, what animals “want” is viewed as (the most) important component for individual welfare. Accordingly, studying the motivation of animals helps understanding basic mechanisms and welfare related needs. However, studying wants of animals is notoriously difficult and many previous studies on the preferences of animals have been restricted in the sense that only two choice options were presented in an artificial test environment. Here, an extended approach, the “Small World” is presented, in which the choices of animals between eight ecologically relevant resources can be observed in a long-term test to reach conclusions with respect to everyday moment-to-moment decisions. In this sense, the system offers a quasi-natural environment. The approach was tested in three experiments with observations of individual female rats, small groups of female rats (Long Evans, Rattus norvegicus) and small groups of female chickens (Lohmann Brown, Gallus gallus domesticus). The animals oriented themselves quickly in the system and it was possible to collect multifaceted data on the use of the resources. These data included the faecal and urinary markings in the Small World cages, the daily frequency and duration of visits to these cages, the sequential analyses of the choices for and decisions among the resources, and the synchrony of the animals in the groups. Given the richness of these data and the lack of a stress response in the tested animals, the use of the Small World approach seems to be highly promising as an extension to previously used procedures. To further improve the approach and more directly reflect the subjective value of the different resources from the animals' point of view, the distances between the resources in a quasi-natural landscape shall be manipulated in future studies.

动物在任何时候表现出的行为都是其行为控制机制的综合输出。此外,动物 "想要什么 "被视为个体福利的(最)重要组成部分。因此,研究动物的动机有助于了解基本机制和与福利相关的需求。然而,研究动物的 "想要 "是出了名的困难,以往许多关于动物偏好的研究都受到限制,即在人工测试环境中只提供两个选择选项。在这里,我们提出了一种扩展方法,即 "小世界",通过长期测试观察动物在八种生态相关资源之间的选择,从而得出日常决策的结论。从这个意义上说,该系统提供了一个准自然环境。该方法在三项实验中进行了测试,分别对单个雌鼠、小群雌鼠(Long Evans, Rattus norvegicus)和小群雌鸡(Lohmann Brown, Gallus gallus domesticus)进行了观察。动物在系统中很快就能找到自己的位置,因此可以收集到有关资源利用的多方面数据。这些数据包括 "小世界 "笼子里的粪便和尿液标记、每天访问这些笼子的频率和持续时间、对资源的选择和决定的顺序分析以及动物在群体中的同步性。考虑到这些数据的丰富性以及受测动物没有出现应激反应,使用 "小世界 "方法作为以前使用的程序的延伸似乎非常有前景。为了进一步改进该方法,并从动物的角度更直接地反映不同资源的主观价值,在未来的研究中,将对准自然景观中资源之间的距离进行操作。
{"title":"Steps into a Small World: First glimpses on everyday moment-to-moment decision making in an ecologically meaningful multi-choice system for assessing animal preferences","authors":"Fiona Puls,&nbsp;Louisa-Mae Kosin,&nbsp;Fiona Garbisch,&nbsp;Chadi Touma,&nbsp;Christa Thöne-Reineke,&nbsp;Lorenz Gygax","doi":"10.1111/eth.13468","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13468","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The behaviour shown by an animal at any one time is the consolidated output of its behavioural control mechanism. Moreover, what animals “want” is viewed as (the most) important component for individual welfare. Accordingly, studying the motivation of animals helps understanding basic mechanisms and welfare related needs. However, studying wants of animals is notoriously difficult and many previous studies on the preferences of animals have been restricted in the sense that only two choice options were presented in an artificial test environment. Here, an extended approach, the “Small World” is presented, in which the choices of animals between eight ecologically relevant resources can be observed in a long-term test to reach conclusions with respect to everyday moment-to-moment decisions. In this sense, the system offers a quasi-natural environment. The approach was tested in three experiments with observations of individual female rats, small groups of female rats (Long Evans, <i>Rattus norvegicus</i>) and small groups of female chickens (Lohmann Brown, <i>Gallus gallus domesticus</i>). The animals oriented themselves quickly in the system and it was possible to collect multifaceted data on the use of the resources. These data included the faecal and urinary markings in the Small World cages, the daily frequency and duration of visits to these cages, the sequential analyses of the choices for and decisions among the resources, and the synchrony of the animals in the groups. Given the richness of these data and the lack of a stress response in the tested animals, the use of the Small World approach seems to be highly promising as an extension to previously used procedures. To further improve the approach and more directly reflect the subjective value of the different resources from the animals' point of view, the distances between the resources in a quasi-natural landscape shall be manipulated in future studies.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13468","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140934862","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Of hormones (well, not really!), behavior, and observer bias 关于荷尔蒙(其实不是!)、行为和观察者的偏见
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-02 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13450
Wolfgang Goymann

In our laboratory, we measure hormones such as testosterone, melatonin, or corticosterone to relate hormone concentrations to the behavior of animals. Sometimes, we get samples of animals whose hormone concentrations had been altered by hormone implants or injections. Usually, those are measured to test if the hormonal manipulation had worked. I typically ask the experimenter which of the samples are from manipulated animals and which ones are from control animals. As an answer, I sometimes receive a raised eye brow and a questioning face. This kind of answer makes me happy because it shows my cooperation partner is aware of observer bias. I then explain that I would actually prefer not to know, which animals had been manipulated or not. However, our method to measure the hormones, the radioimmunoassay, requires me to know about it. The method is quite sensitive, but has a narrow range of concentrations in which we can reliably tell the correct hormone concentrations. Therefore, we need to adjust the dilution of the samples to remain in the expected range of concentrations we can measure with high precision. Hormone-treated samples might easily fall out of this range, if not diluted properly.

Presumably, the radioimmunoassay as a biochemical method is unlikely to produce observer bias, but this is different with behavioral observations, where our expectations as experimenters may inadvertently bias data collection. For this reason, good textbooks such as the classic Martin and Bateson (1985) or its latest edition (Bateson & Martin, 2021) caution against observer bias and also highlight the importance of testing for inter-observer reliability. A good way to do so is blinding observers to the treatment and apply established methods to test and improve inter-observer reliability.

About 12 years ago, Gordon Burghardt and colleagues investigated how major journals in animal behavior did with regard to reporting observer bias. They demonstrated that in 2010 major journals of our field (Animal Behaviour, Behaviour, Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology, and Ethology) had observer bias reporting rates of <10%, therefore lagging behind Infancy, a journal on human infant behavior with reporting rates of more than 75% (Burghardt et al., 2012). Journal of Comparative Psychology had reporting rates of 20%.

In this issue of Ethology, Todd Freeberg, Scott Benson, and Gordon Burghardt offer a follow-up study (2024), showing that all behavioral journals have improved on reporting. This is good news! However, with rates in the range of 50% our field still lags behind Infancy, where basically every study reports observer bias and tests for inter-observer reliability. Also, Journal of Comparative Psychology still does better with roughly 75% or articles reporting on observer bias. Admittedly, it is more difficult and sometimes impossible to condu

在我们的实验室里,我们测量睾酮、褪黑激素或皮质酮等激素,以便将激素浓度与动物的行为联系起来。有时,我们会得到一些动物的样本,这些动物的激素浓度因植入或注射激素而发生了改变。通常,我们会对这些样本进行测量,以检验激素操纵是否奏效。我通常会问实验者,哪些样本来自被操纵的动物,哪些样本来自对照组动物。作为回答,我有时会得到一个挑眉和质疑的表情。这种回答让我很开心,因为这表明我的合作者意识到了观察者的偏见。然后,我解释说,其实我更希望不知道哪些动物是否被操纵过。不过,我们测量荷尔蒙的方法--放射免疫分析法--要求我知道这一点。这种方法相当灵敏,但浓度范围很窄,我们无法可靠地判断激素的正确浓度。因此,我们需要调整样本的稀释度,使其保持在我们可以高精度测量的预期浓度范围内。据推测,放射免疫分析作为一种生化方法不太可能产生观察者偏差,但这与行为观察不同,我们作为实验者的期望可能会在无意中对数据收集产生偏差。因此,经典教科书马丁和贝特森(Martin and Bateson,1985 年)或其最新版本(Bateson & Martin,2021 年)都提醒我们注意观察者偏差,并强调测试观察者间可靠性的重要性。约 12 年前,Gordon Burghardt 及其同事调查了动物行为学主要期刊在报告观察者偏倚方面的情况。他们发现,2010 年本领域主要期刊(《动物行为学》、《行为学》、《行为生态学与社会生物学》和《选育学》)的观察者偏差报告率为 10%,因此落后于报告率超过 75% 的人类婴儿行为期刊《Infancy》(Burghardt 等人,2012 年)。托德-弗里伯格(Todd Freeberg)、斯科特-本森(Scott Benson)和戈登-伯格哈特(Gordon Burghardt)在本期《选育》杂志上提供了一项后续研究(2024 年),结果显示所有行为学期刊的报告率都有所提高。这是一个好消息!然而,我们的研究领域仍然落后于《婴儿学》(Infancy),后者基本上每项研究都会报告观察者偏差并测试观察者间的可靠性。此外,《比较心理学杂志》(Journal of Comparative Psychology)的情况仍然较好,约有 75% 的文章报告了观察者偏差。诚然,在动物行为研究中进行盲法观察比较困难,有时甚至是不可能的,尤其是在野外收集数据时,但可以肯定的是,盲法观察和报告仍有改进的余地。行为生态学与社会生物学》比包括《选集》在内的其他期刊略胜一筹,这可能是因为他们实施了一项投稿政策,要求作者报告观察者偏差。作为对 Freeberg 等人(2024 年)重新调查的回应,《选育》现在也决定要求作者报告他们是如何处理观察者偏差的。希望再过几年,Freeberg 及其同事能报告说,我们已经告别了稚嫩,我们的期刊在处理观察者偏见方面已经达到了成熟的状态。
{"title":"Of hormones (well, not really!), behavior, and observer bias","authors":"Wolfgang Goymann","doi":"10.1111/eth.13450","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13450","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In our laboratory, we measure hormones such as testosterone, melatonin, or corticosterone to relate hormone concentrations to the behavior of animals. Sometimes, we get samples of animals whose hormone concentrations had been altered by hormone implants or injections. Usually, those are measured to test if the hormonal manipulation had worked. I typically ask the experimenter which of the samples are from manipulated animals and which ones are from control animals. As an answer, I sometimes receive a raised eye brow and a questioning face. This kind of answer makes me happy because it shows my cooperation partner is aware of observer bias. I then explain that I would actually prefer not to know, which animals had been manipulated or not. However, our method to measure the hormones, the radioimmunoassay, requires me to know about it. The method is quite sensitive, but has a narrow range of concentrations in which we can reliably tell the correct hormone concentrations. Therefore, we need to adjust the dilution of the samples to remain in the expected range of concentrations we can measure with high precision. Hormone-treated samples might easily fall out of this range, if not diluted properly.</p><p>Presumably, the radioimmunoassay as a biochemical method is unlikely to produce observer bias, but this is different with behavioral observations, where our expectations as experimenters may inadvertently bias data collection. For this reason, good textbooks such as the classic Martin and Bateson (<span>1985</span>) or its latest edition (Bateson &amp; Martin, <span>2021</span>) caution against observer bias and also highlight the importance of testing for inter-observer reliability. A good way to do so is blinding observers to the treatment and apply established methods to test and improve inter-observer reliability.</p><p>About 12 years ago, Gordon Burghardt and colleagues investigated how major journals in animal behavior did with regard to reporting observer bias. They demonstrated that in 2010 major journals of our field (<i>Animal Behaviour</i>, <i>Behaviour</i>, <i>Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology</i>, and <i>Ethology</i>) had observer bias reporting rates of &lt;10%, therefore lagging behind <i>Infancy</i>, a journal on human infant behavior with reporting rates of more than 75% (Burghardt et al., <span>2012</span>). <i>Journal of Comparative Psychology</i> had reporting rates of 20%.</p><p>In this issue of Ethology, Todd Freeberg, Scott Benson, and Gordon Burghardt offer a follow-up study (2024), showing that all behavioral journals have improved on reporting. This is good news! However, with rates in the range of 50% our field still lags behind <i>Infancy</i>, where basically every study reports observer bias and tests for inter-observer reliability. Also, <i>Journal of Comparative Psychology</i> still does better with roughly 75% or articles reporting on observer bias. Admittedly, it is more difficult and sometimes impossible to condu","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13450","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140820485","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Obituary: Professor Wolfgang Wickler (November 18, 1931– January 12, 2024) 讣告沃尔夫冈-维克勒教授(1931 年 11 月 18 日- 2024 年 1 月 12 日)
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-02 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13458
Fritz Trillmich, Walter Arnold
{"title":"Obituary: Professor Wolfgang Wickler (November 18, 1931– January 12, 2024)","authors":"Fritz Trillmich,&nbsp;Walter Arnold","doi":"10.1111/eth.13458","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13458","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140820702","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Thanks to repetition, dustbathing detection can be automated combining accelerometry and wavelet analysis 由于重复性,结合加速度测量和小波分析,可自动进行粉尘浴检测
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-24 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13466
Rocio Guadalupe Fonseca, Maria Candelaria Bosch, Florencia Cecilia Spanevello, Maria Victoria de la Fuente, Raul Hector Marin, Lucas Barberis, Jackelyn Melissa Kembro, Ana Georgina Flesia

Birds from at least a dozen orders engage in dustbathing, including Galliformes. Dustbathing is generally considered a behavioural need for poultry. It involves a precise and orderly sequence of movements repeated over time. The most characteristic movement involves tossing the dust with the wings and undulating the body beneath the dust shower. Thus, repetitive changes in body position during dustbathing could be automatically detected through data processing of body-mounted accelerometer recordings. The approach was tested in 13 adult male Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) fitted with a body mounted triaxial accelerometer. Behaviour was video-recorded for at least 6 h. Observations showed that when the animal lies on its left- or right-side during dustbathing, the lateral (swaying) component of the acceleration vector adopts values of +1 or −1, respectively. Analysis shows that the bird repeats these shifts in body position every 25–60 s. The wavelet analysis (i.e. complex Morlet continuous wavelet transform (CWT)) detected this oscillatory behaviour within the time series as higher power values. This characteristic was used to automate the detection of dustbathing events, for which a threshold value for the maximum power value estimated was established for the corresponding range of scales between 25 and 60 s. The overall general accuracy of this classification method for dustbathing detection was 80%, with individual variations falling within the range of 66%–100%. Finally, an example of the potential of this method in the study of temporal dynamics, such as daily rhythms of dustbathing, is provided. Our results show that combining accelerometry and wavelet analysis could be useful for the assessment of intra- and inter-individual variability in dustbathing dynamics over long-term studies, even within large complex environments, such as natural habitats or breeding facilities. Moreover, this approach could open doors for future in-depth studies exploring the relationship between dustbathing and poultry welfare.

至少有十几种鸟类会进行尘浴,包括胆形目。一般认为,洗尘是家禽的一种行为需要。它包括一连串精确而有序的动作,随着时间的推移不断重复。最有特点的动作是用翅膀甩动灰尘,并在灰尘淋浴下起伏身体。因此,可以通过对安装在身体上的加速度计记录进行数据处理,自动检测出尘浴过程中身体位置的重复变化。该方法在 13 只成年雄性日本鹌鹑(Coturnix japonica)身上进行了测试,鹌鹑身上安装了一个三轴加速度计。观察结果表明,当鹌鹑左侧卧或右侧卧洗尘时,加速度矢量的横向(摇摆)分量分别为+1或-1。分析表明,鸟类每隔 25-60 秒就会重复这些身体位置的变化。小波分析(即复莫雷特连续小波变换 (CWT))以较高的功率值检测到时间序列中的这种振荡行为。这种特征被用于自动检测尘浴事件,为此,在 25 至 60 秒之间的相应时间范围内为估计的最大功率值设定了一个阈值。这种分类方法对尘浴检测的总体准确率为 80%,个体差异在 66%-100%之间。最后,我们举例说明了这种方法在研究时间动态(如每天的沐尘节律)方面的潜力。我们的研究结果表明,将加速度测量与小波分析相结合,有助于评估长期研究中个体内和个体间的嗜尘动态变异性,即使是在大型复杂环境中,如自然栖息地或繁殖设施。此外,这种方法还能为今后深入研究浴尘与家禽福利之间的关系打开大门。
{"title":"Thanks to repetition, dustbathing detection can be automated combining accelerometry and wavelet analysis","authors":"Rocio Guadalupe Fonseca,&nbsp;Maria Candelaria Bosch,&nbsp;Florencia Cecilia Spanevello,&nbsp;Maria Victoria de la Fuente,&nbsp;Raul Hector Marin,&nbsp;Lucas Barberis,&nbsp;Jackelyn Melissa Kembro,&nbsp;Ana Georgina Flesia","doi":"10.1111/eth.13466","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13466","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Birds from at least a dozen orders engage in dustbathing, including Galliformes. Dustbathing is generally considered a behavioural need for poultry. It involves a precise and orderly sequence of movements repeated over time. The most characteristic movement involves tossing the dust with the wings and undulating the body beneath the dust shower. Thus, repetitive changes in body position during dustbathing could be automatically detected through data processing of body-mounted accelerometer recordings. The approach was tested in 13 adult male Japanese quail (<i>Coturnix japonica</i>) fitted with a body mounted triaxial accelerometer. Behaviour was video-recorded for at least 6 h. Observations showed that when the animal lies on its left- or right-side during dustbathing, the lateral (swaying) component of the acceleration vector adopts values of +1 or −1, respectively. Analysis shows that the bird repeats these shifts in body position every 25–60 s. The wavelet analysis (i.e. complex Morlet continuous wavelet transform (CWT)) detected this oscillatory behaviour within the time series as higher power values. This characteristic was used to automate the detection of dustbathing events, for which a threshold value for the maximum power value estimated was established for the corresponding range of scales between 25 and 60 s. The overall general accuracy of this classification method for dustbathing detection was 80%, with individual variations falling within the range of 66%–100%. Finally, an example of the potential of this method in the study of temporal dynamics, such as daily rhythms of dustbathing, is provided. Our results show that combining accelerometry and wavelet analysis could be useful for the assessment of intra- and inter-individual variability in dustbathing dynamics over long-term studies, even within large complex environments, such as natural habitats or breeding facilities. Moreover, this approach could open doors for future in-depth studies exploring the relationship between dustbathing and poultry welfare.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140662586","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social buffering of behavioural stress response in two fish species, Nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus) and koi carp (Cyprinus carpio) 尼罗罗非鱼(Oreochromis niloticus)和锦鲤(Cyprinus carpio)这两种鱼类行为应激反应的社会缓冲作用
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-21 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13464
Santiago Pintos, Tyrone Lucon-Xiccato, Luisa María Vera, Luis Conceição, Cristiano Bertolucci, Javier Sánchez-Vázquez, Paulo Rema

The presence of conspecifics plays a crucial role in mitigating stress responses in social teleost species and holds potential for enhancing welfare in captive fish. While studies on social buffering effects have typically focussed on single species, marked interspecific differences can exist. Here, we conducted an analysis of social buffering of stress response in two of the most extensively farmed fish, the Nile tilapia and the koi carp. Subjects were exposed to a behavioural stress response assay (open-field test) in three conditions simulating increasing levels of social enrichment: isolation, pairs, or shoals of five fish. We obtained five stress indicators from the assay: thigmotaxis, freezing, activity, erratic movements and interindividual distance in conditions with more than one fish. In both species, erratic movements significantly decreased with increasing levels of social enrichment, suggesting a similar social buffering effect. However, other indicators revealed species differences. Koi carp, but not Nile tilapia, showed a socially-mediated reduction in thigmotaxis, whereas Nile tilapia, but not Koi carp, showed a socially-mediated reduction in freezing behaviour. Furthermore, social enrichment determined opposite effects on the activity of the two species: Nile tilapia were more active as group size increased, whereas the opposite trend was found in koi carp. Finally, Nile tilapia showed increased interindividual distance with increasing social group size, whereas no changes were observed for koi carp. Our study indicates that the buffering effects of social enrichment on the behavioural stress response do not completely overlap between different fish species, highlighting the importance of developing finely-tuned species-specific enrichments and welfare indicators.

同种生物的存在对减轻社会性远摄鱼的应激反应起着至关重要的作用,并有可能提高圈养鱼类的福利。虽然有关社会缓冲作用的研究通常集中在单一物种上,但种间也可能存在明显的差异。在这里,我们对尼罗罗非鱼和锦鲤这两种最广泛养殖的鱼类的社会缓冲压力反应进行了分析。受试者在三种模拟社会富集程度递增的条件下(隔离、成对或五条鱼组成的鱼群)接受行为应激反应测定(开阔地测试)。我们从该试验中获得了五项应激指标:移行、冻结、活动、不规则运动以及在有多条鱼的情况下的个体间距离。在两种鱼中,随着社会富集程度的增加,不规则运动明显减少,这表明存在类似的社会缓冲作用。然而,其他指标则显示出物种差异。锦鲤(而非尼罗罗非鱼)表现出了由社会介导的滞游行为的减少,而尼罗罗非鱼(而非锦鲤)则表现出了由社会介导的冻结行为的减少。此外,社会富集对两种鱼类的活动产生了相反的影响:随着群体规模的扩大,尼罗罗非鱼更加活跃,而锦鲤则相反。最后,尼罗罗非鱼的个体间距离随着群体大小的增加而增加,而锦鲤则没有变化。我们的研究表明,社会富集对行为应激反应的缓冲作用在不同鱼类物种之间并不完全重合,这凸显了开发针对特定物种的富集和福利指标的重要性。
{"title":"Social buffering of behavioural stress response in two fish species, Nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus) and koi carp (Cyprinus carpio)","authors":"Santiago Pintos,&nbsp;Tyrone Lucon-Xiccato,&nbsp;Luisa María Vera,&nbsp;Luis Conceição,&nbsp;Cristiano Bertolucci,&nbsp;Javier Sánchez-Vázquez,&nbsp;Paulo Rema","doi":"10.1111/eth.13464","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13464","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The presence of conspecifics plays a crucial role in mitigating stress responses in social teleost species and holds potential for enhancing welfare in captive fish. While studies on social buffering effects have typically focussed on single species, marked interspecific differences can exist. Here, we conducted an analysis of social buffering of stress response in two of the most extensively farmed fish, the Nile tilapia and the koi carp. Subjects were exposed to a behavioural stress response assay (open-field test) in three conditions simulating increasing levels of social enrichment: isolation, pairs, or shoals of five fish. We obtained five stress indicators from the assay: thigmotaxis, freezing, activity, erratic movements and interindividual distance in conditions with more than one fish. In both species, erratic movements significantly decreased with increasing levels of social enrichment, suggesting a similar social buffering effect. However, other indicators revealed species differences. Koi carp, but not Nile tilapia, showed a socially-mediated reduction in thigmotaxis, whereas Nile tilapia, but not Koi carp, showed a socially-mediated reduction in freezing behaviour. Furthermore, social enrichment determined opposite effects on the activity of the two species: Nile tilapia were more active as group size increased, whereas the opposite trend was found in koi carp. Finally, Nile tilapia showed increased interindividual distance with increasing social group size, whereas no changes were observed for koi carp. Our study indicates that the buffering effects of social enrichment on the behavioural stress response do not completely overlap between different fish species, highlighting the importance of developing finely-tuned species-specific enrichments and welfare indicators.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13464","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140678137","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of urbanization on painted turtle (Chrysemys picta) behaviour 城市化对彩龟(Chrysemys picta)行为的影响
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-21 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13467
Sebastian Blanchett, Audrey Turcotte, Gabriel Blouin-Demers

Urbanization is a significant driver of the global biodiversity crisis. Turtles are particularly impacted by urbanization because of the vulnerability of riparian habitats to habitat loss and road mortality. Behaviour plays a crucial role in determining the success of urban animals. Behavioural responses to urbanization, however, are rarely studied in turtles even though many turtles are at-risk and sometimes live in urban areas. Therefore, we evaluated behavioural changes in painted turtles (Chrysemys picta) living in wetlands surrounded by a gradient of urbanization. We tested the consistency of painted turtle behaviour in the laboratory and examined the behaviour of painted turtles from 24 wetland sites across an urbanization gradient in Ottawa, Ontario, Canada. We assessed: (i) aggression by measuring the number of active defensive behaviours the turtles performed in response to handling, (ii) boldness by measuring the amount of time the turtles took to emerge from their shells and move from their initial locations in a circular arena and (iii) activity by measuring the amount of time the turtles spent moving in the same circular arena. We found that all behaviours were consistent in the laboratory. We also found that as the level of urbanization increased, turtles were more aggressive and bolder. Urbanization affects painted turtle behaviour, but further research is required to understand the mechanisms responsible and the conservation implications.

城市化是全球生物多样性危机的一个重要驱动因素。由于河岸栖息地容易受到栖息地丧失和道路死亡的影响,乌龟尤其受到城市化的影响。行为在决定城市动物成功与否方面起着至关重要的作用。然而,尽管许多乌龟面临危险,有时还生活在城市地区,但很少有人研究乌龟对城市化的行为反应。因此,我们评估了生活在被城市化梯度包围的湿地中的彩龟(Chrysemys picta)的行为变化。我们在实验室测试了彩龟行为的一致性,并考察了加拿大安大略省渥太华市城市化梯度中 24 个湿地的彩龟行为。我们对以下行为进行了评估:(i) 攻击行为,即测量彩龟在应对操作时主动防御行为的数量;(ii) 胆量,即测量彩龟从壳中出来并从圆形场地中的初始位置移动所需的时间;(iii) 活动,即测量彩龟在同一圆形场地中移动所需的时间。我们发现,所有行为在实验室中都是一致的。我们还发现,随着城市化水平的提高,乌龟的攻击性更强,胆子更大。城市化会影响彩龟的行为,但还需要进一步的研究来了解其中的机制和对保护的影响。
{"title":"The impact of urbanization on painted turtle (Chrysemys picta) behaviour","authors":"Sebastian Blanchett,&nbsp;Audrey Turcotte,&nbsp;Gabriel Blouin-Demers","doi":"10.1111/eth.13467","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13467","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Urbanization is a significant driver of the global biodiversity crisis. Turtles are particularly impacted by urbanization because of the vulnerability of riparian habitats to habitat loss and road mortality. Behaviour plays a crucial role in determining the success of urban animals. Behavioural responses to urbanization, however, are rarely studied in turtles even though many turtles are at-risk and sometimes live in urban areas. Therefore, we evaluated behavioural changes in painted turtles (<i>Chrysemys picta</i>) living in wetlands surrounded by a gradient of urbanization. We tested the consistency of painted turtle behaviour in the laboratory and examined the behaviour of painted turtles from 24 wetland sites across an urbanization gradient in Ottawa, Ontario, Canada. We assessed: (i) aggression by measuring the number of active defensive behaviours the turtles performed in response to handling, (ii) boldness by measuring the amount of time the turtles took to emerge from their shells and move from their initial locations in a circular arena and (iii) activity by measuring the amount of time the turtles spent moving in the same circular arena. We found that all behaviours were consistent in the laboratory. We also found that as the level of urbanization increased, turtles were more aggressive and bolder. Urbanization affects painted turtle behaviour, but further research is required to understand the mechanisms responsible and the conservation implications.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13467","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140678126","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Tap dancing frogs: Posterior toe tapping and feeding in Dendrobates tinctorius 跳踢踏舞的青蛙Dendrobates tinctorius的后趾敲击和进食
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-16 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13465
Thomas Q. Parrish, Eva K. Fischer

Animals have myriad adaptations to help them hunt and feed in the most efficient and effective manner. One mysterious behavior related to hunting and feeding is the posterior toe tapping behavior of some frogs. Biologists and hobbyists alike have long noticed this behavior, but there is little empirical data to explain its causes and consequences. To test the hypothesis that tapping is related to feeding and modulated by environmental context, we conducted a series of related experiments in the Dyeing poison frog, Dendrobates tinctorius. We first confirmed that tap rate was higher during feeding as has been observed in other species. Interestingly, this effect was heightened in the presence of a conspecific. We next asked whether frogs tapped less under conditions when prey were visible, but inaccessible. Finally, we asked whether D. tinctorius adjusted tap rate based on substrate characteristics and whether prey capture success was higher when tapping. In addition to confirming an association between tapping and feeding, our work demonstrates modulation of toe tapping based on social context, prey accessibility, and substrate characteristics. Based on our findings, we suggest that tapping could act to induce prey movement and thereby facilitate prey detection and capture by frogs.

动物有无数的适应性,可以帮助它们以最有效率和最有效果的方式捕猎和觅食。与捕食和觅食有关的一种神秘行为是某些青蛙的后趾敲击行为。长期以来,生物学家和爱好者都注意到了这种行为,但几乎没有经验数据可以解释其原因和后果。为了验证拍打行为与摄食有关并受环境影响的假设,我们在染毒蛙(Dendrobates tinctorius)身上进行了一系列相关实验。我们首先证实,与在其他物种中观察到的情况一样,摄食时拍打率较高。有趣的是,在有同种蛙在场的情况下,这种效应会增强。接下来,我们询问在猎物可见但不可及的情况下,蛙类的敲击次数是否会减少。最后,我们想知道丁香蛙是否会根据底质特征调整拍击频率,以及拍击时捕获猎物的成功率是否更高。我们的研究不仅证实了敲击与捕食之间的联系,还证明了脚趾敲击会根据社会环境、猎物可及性和底质特征进行调节。根据我们的研究结果,我们认为轻拍可以诱导猎物移动,从而促进蛙类发现和捕获猎物。
{"title":"Tap dancing frogs: Posterior toe tapping and feeding in Dendrobates tinctorius","authors":"Thomas Q. Parrish,&nbsp;Eva K. Fischer","doi":"10.1111/eth.13465","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13465","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Animals have myriad adaptations to help them hunt and feed in the most efficient and effective manner. One mysterious behavior related to hunting and feeding is the posterior toe tapping behavior of some frogs. Biologists and hobbyists alike have long noticed this behavior, but there is little empirical data to explain its causes and consequences. To test the hypothesis that tapping is related to feeding and modulated by environmental context, we conducted a series of related experiments in the Dyeing poison frog, <i>Dendrobates tinctorius</i>. We first confirmed that tap rate was higher during feeding as has been observed in other species. Interestingly, this effect was heightened in the presence of a conspecific. We next asked whether frogs tapped less under conditions when prey were visible, but inaccessible. Finally, we asked whether <i>D. tinctorius</i> adjusted tap rate based on substrate characteristics and whether prey capture success was higher when tapping. In addition to confirming an association between tapping and feeding, our work demonstrates modulation of toe tapping based on social context, prey accessibility, and substrate characteristics. Based on our findings, we suggest that tapping could act to induce prey movement and thereby facilitate prey detection and capture by frogs.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13465","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140569389","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Changes to the acoustic properties of Gromphadorhina portentosa defensive sounds when exposed to the molting hormone, 20-hydroxyecdysone 当暴露于蜕皮激素--20-羟基蜕皮激素时Gromphadorhina portentosa防御声音的声学特性变化
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-05 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13463
David J. Parker, Hrithik Basak, Patricia Foltynski, Lindsey Swierk

Steroid hormones play a pivotal role in shaping arthropod phenotypes, with 20-hydroxyecdysone (20E) serving as a key regulator of molting, a vulnerable period in an insect's lifecycle. Despite its critical role in arthropod growth and development, the influence of 20E on arthropod behavior, particularly defensive strategies, remains poorly understood. We investigated the impact of 20E on the bioacoustic characteristics of hisses in the Madagascar hissing cockroach (Gromphadorhina portentosa), a social species with multiple complex acoustic signals. With increased 20E, we predicted that hiss production would be more likely and more defensive (i.e., longer hisses with greater intensity (dB) and reduced frequency (Hz)). We injected male G. portentosa with either a low- (35 μg) or high-dose (70 μg) of 20E or a control (0 μg 20E), and we measured the presence/absence of hissing responses and their bioacoustic characteristics following a standardized tactile stimulus. Contrary to our prediction, there was no difference in the likelihood of hissing or hiss duration with 20E administration. However, administering 20E resulted in reduced hiss intensity and increased hiss frequency (as measured by peak and center frequencies), suggesting potential shifts from defensive to aggressive signaling. Our study contributes to the limited knowledge of the behavioral effects of 20E, suggesting that some arthropods may experience increased aggression or energetic limitations to defense during molting. Behavioral changes elicited by hormones have important implications for both fundamental ecology and applied pest management.

类固醇激素在节肢动物表型的形成过程中起着关键作用,其中 20-羟基蜕皮激素(20E)是蜕皮的关键调节因子,而蜕皮是昆虫生命周期中的一个脆弱时期。尽管 20E 在节肢动物的生长和发育过程中发挥着关键作用,但人们对其对节肢动物行为,尤其是防御策略的影响仍然知之甚少。我们研究了 20E 对马达加斯加嘶嘶声蜚蠊(Gromphadorhina portentosa)嘶嘶声生物声学特征的影响。我们预测,随着 20E 的增加,嘶嘶声的产生将更有可能且更具防御性(即嘶嘶声更长、强度(dB)更大、频率(Hz)更低)。我们给雄性孔雀鱼注射了低剂量(35 μg)或高剂量(70 μg)的 20E 或对照组(0 μg 20E),并在标准化触觉刺激后测量了有无嘶嘶声反应及其生物声学特征。与我们的预测相反,施用 20E 后,嘶嘶声的可能性或嘶嘶声持续时间没有差异。然而,施用 20E 会导致嘶嘶声强度降低和嘶嘶声频率增加(根据峰值和中心频率测量),这表明嘶嘶声信号可能从防御性转向攻击性。我们的研究丰富了有关 20E 行为效应的有限知识,表明一些节肢动物在蜕皮期间可能会遇到攻击性增强或防御能量受限的情况。激素引起的行为变化对基础生态学和应用害虫管理都有重要影响。
{"title":"Changes to the acoustic properties of Gromphadorhina portentosa defensive sounds when exposed to the molting hormone, 20-hydroxyecdysone","authors":"David J. Parker,&nbsp;Hrithik Basak,&nbsp;Patricia Foltynski,&nbsp;Lindsey Swierk","doi":"10.1111/eth.13463","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13463","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Steroid hormones play a pivotal role in shaping arthropod phenotypes, with 20-hydroxyecdysone (20E) serving as a key regulator of molting, a vulnerable period in an insect's lifecycle. Despite its critical role in arthropod growth and development, the influence of 20E on arthropod behavior, particularly defensive strategies, remains poorly understood. We investigated the impact of 20E on the bioacoustic characteristics of hisses in the Madagascar hissing cockroach (<i>Gromphadorhina portentosa</i>), a social species with multiple complex acoustic signals. With increased 20E, we predicted that hiss production would be more likely and more defensive (i.e., longer hisses with greater intensity (dB) and reduced frequency (Hz)). We injected male <i>G. portentosa</i> with either a low- (35 μg) or high-dose (70 μg) of 20E or a control (0 μg 20E), and we measured the presence/absence of hissing responses and their bioacoustic characteristics following a standardized tactile stimulus. Contrary to our prediction, there was no difference in the likelihood of hissing or hiss duration with 20E administration. However, administering 20E resulted in reduced hiss intensity and increased hiss frequency (as measured by peak and center frequencies), suggesting potential shifts from defensive to aggressive signaling. Our study contributes to the limited knowledge of the behavioral effects of 20E, suggesting that some arthropods may experience increased aggression or energetic limitations to defense during molting. Behavioral changes elicited by hormones have important implications for both fundamental ecology and applied pest management.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13463","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140569260","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Ethology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1