首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
It's not just what we don't know: The mapping problem in the acquisition of negation 这不仅仅是我们不知道的:否定习得中的映射问题。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101592
Victor Gomes , Rebecca Doherty , Daniel Smits , Susan Goldin-Meadow , John C. Trueswell , Roman Feiman

How do learners learn what no and not mean when they are only presented with what is? Given its complexity, abstractness, and roles in logic, truth-functional negation might be a conceptual accomplishment. As a result, young children’s gradual acquisition of negation words might be due to their undergoing a gradual conceptual change that is necessary to represent those words’ logical meaning. However, it’s also possible that linguistic expressions of negation take time to learn because of children’s gradually increasing grasp of their language. To understand what no and not mean, children might first need to understand the rest of the sentences in which those words are used. We provide experimental evidence that conceptually equipped learners (adults) face the same acquisition challenges that children do when their access to linguistic information is restricted, which simulates how much language children understand at different points in acquisition. When watching a silenced video of naturalistic uses of negators by parents speaking to their children, adults could tell when the parent was prohibiting the child and struggled with inferring that negators were used to express logical negation. However, when provided with additional information about what else the parent said, guessing that the parent had expressed logical negation became easy for adults. Though our findings do not rule out that young learners also undergo conceptual change, they show that increasing understanding of language alone, with no accompanying conceptual change, can account for the gradual acquisition of negation words.

当学习者只被告知“是”时,他们如何学习“否”和“否”的含义?考虑到其复杂性、抽象性和在逻辑中的作用,真值功能否定可能是一种概念上的成就。因此,幼儿对否定词的逐渐习得可能是由于他们经历了一个渐进的概念变化,这是表达这些词的逻辑意义所必需的。然而,由于孩子们对语言的掌握程度逐渐提高,否定的语言表达也可能需要时间来学习。为了理解“不”和“不”的意思,孩子们可能首先需要理解这些单词所用的其余句子。我们提供的实验证据表明,当儿童获得语言信息受到限制时,具备概念的学习者(成年人)面临着与儿童相同的习得挑战,这模拟了儿童在不同的习得点上理解了多少语言。当观看父母对孩子说话时自然使用否定词的无声视频时,成年人可以判断出父母何时禁止孩子,并努力推断否定词是用来表达逻辑否定的。然而,当被提供关于父母还说了什么的额外信息时,成年人很容易猜测父母表达了合乎逻辑的否定。尽管我们的研究结果并不排除年轻学习者也会经历概念变化,但它们表明,只有对语言的理解不断增加,而没有伴随的概念变化,才能解释否定词的逐渐习得。
{"title":"It's not just what we don't know: The mapping problem in the acquisition of negation","authors":"Victor Gomes ,&nbsp;Rebecca Doherty ,&nbsp;Daniel Smits ,&nbsp;Susan Goldin-Meadow ,&nbsp;John C. Trueswell ,&nbsp;Roman Feiman","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101592","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101592","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>How do learners learn what <em>no</em> and <em>not</em> mean when they are only presented with what is? Given its complexity, abstractness, and roles in logic, truth-functional negation might be a conceptual accomplishment. As a result, young children’s gradual acquisition of negation words might be due to their undergoing a gradual conceptual change that is necessary to represent those words’ logical meaning. However, it’s also possible that linguistic expressions of negation take time to learn because of children’s gradually increasing grasp of their language. To understand what <em>no</em> and <em>not</em> mean, children might first need to understand the rest of the sentences in which those words are used. We provide experimental evidence that conceptually equipped learners (adults) face the same acquisition challenges that children do when their access to linguistic information is restricted, which simulates how much language children understand at different points in acquisition. When watching a silenced video of naturalistic uses of negators by parents speaking to their children, adults could tell when the parent was prohibiting the child and struggled with inferring that negators were used to express logical negation. However, when provided with additional information about what else the parent said, guessing that the parent had expressed logical negation became easy for adults. Though our findings do not rule out that young learners also undergo conceptual change, they show that increasing understanding of language alone, with no accompanying conceptual change, can account for the gradual acquisition of negation words.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"145 ","pages":"Article 101592"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10576150","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Norm conflicts and epistemic modals 规范冲突与认识模态。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101591
Niels Skovgaard-Olsen , John Cantwell

Statements containing epistemic modals (e.g., “by spring 2023 most European countries may have the Covid-19 pandemic under control”) are common expressions of epistemic uncertainty. In this paper, previous published findings (Knobe & Yalcin, 2014; Khoo & Phillips, 2018) on the opposition between Contextualism and Relativism for epistemic modals are re-examined. It is found that these findings contain a substantial degree of individual variation. To investigate whether participants differ in their interpretations of epistemic modals, an experiment with multiple phases and sessions is conducted to classify participants according to the three semantic theories of Relativism, Contextualism, and Objectivism. Through this study, some of the first empirical evidence for the kind of truth-value shifts postulated by semantic Relativism is presented. It is furthermore found that participants’ disagreement judgments match their truth evaluations and that participants are capable of distinguishing between truth and justification. In a second experimental session, it is investigated whether participants thus classified follow the norm of retraction which Relativism uses to account for argumentation with epistemic modals. Here the results are less favorable for Relativism. In a second experiment, these results are replicated and the normative beliefs of participants concerning the norm of retraction are investigated following work on measuring norms by Bicchieri (2017). Again, it is found that on average participants show no strong preferences concerning the norm of retraction for epistemic modals. Yet, it was found that participants who had committed to Objectivism and had training in logic applied the norm of retraction to might-statements. These results present a substantial challenge to the account of argumentation with epistemic modals presented in MacFarlane (2014), as discussed.

包含认知模态的声明(例如,“到2023年春季,大多数欧洲国家可能控制住新冠肺炎疫情”)是认知不确定性的常见表达。在这篇论文中,重新审视了先前发表的关于认知模态的语境主义和相对主义之间对立的研究结果(Knobe&Yalcin,2014;Khoo&Phillips,2018)。研究发现,这些发现包含了很大程度的个体差异。为了调查参与者对认识模态的解释是否不同,我们进行了一个多阶段和多阶段的实验,根据相对论、语境论和客观主义三种语义理论对参与者进行分类。通过这项研究,提供了语义相对主义所假设的那种真值转移的一些初步经验证据。研究还发现,参与者的分歧判断与他们对真理的评价相匹配,并且参与者能够区分真理和正当性。在第二个实验环节中,研究了这样分类的参与者是否遵循相对论用来解释认知模态论证的撤回规范。这里的结果不太利于相对主义。在第二个实验中,这些结果被复制,并且在Bicchieri(2017)测量规范的工作之后,对参与者关于撤回规范的规范信念进行了调查。再次,研究发现,平均而言,参与者对认知模态的回缩规范没有表现出强烈的偏好。然而,研究发现,那些致力于客观主义并接受过逻辑训练的参与者将撤回规范应用于可能的陈述。正如所讨论的,这些结果对MacFarlane(2014)中提出的使用认识模态的论证提出了实质性的挑战。
{"title":"Norm conflicts and epistemic modals","authors":"Niels Skovgaard-Olsen ,&nbsp;John Cantwell","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101591","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101591","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Statements containing epistemic modals (e.g., “by spring 2023 most European countries may have the Covid-19 pandemic under control”) are common expressions of epistemic uncertainty. In this paper, previous published findings (Knobe &amp; Yalcin, 2014; Khoo &amp; Phillips, 2018) on the opposition between Contextualism and Relativism for epistemic modals are re-examined. It is found that these findings contain a substantial degree of individual variation. To investigate whether participants differ in their interpretations of epistemic modals, an experiment with multiple phases and sessions is conducted to classify participants according to the three semantic theories of Relativism, Contextualism, and Objectivism. Through this study, some of the first empirical evidence for the kind of truth-value shifts postulated by semantic Relativism is presented. It is furthermore found that participants’ disagreement judgments match their truth evaluations and that participants are capable of distinguishing between truth and justification. In a second experimental session, it is investigated whether participants thus classified follow the norm of retraction which Relativism uses to account for argumentation with epistemic modals. Here the results are less favorable for Relativism. In a second experiment, these results are replicated and the normative beliefs of participants concerning the norm of retraction are investigated following work on measuring norms by Bicchieri (2017). Again, it is found that on average participants show no strong preferences concerning the norm of retraction for epistemic modals. Yet, it was found that participants who had committed to Objectivism and had training in logic applied the norm of retraction to might-statements. These results present a substantial challenge to the account of argumentation with epistemic modals presented in MacFarlane (2014), as discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"145 ","pages":"Article 101591"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10560201","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Psychological value theory: A computational cognitive model of charitable giving 心理价值理论:慈善捐赠的计算认知模型。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101593
Dale J. Cohen , Monica K. Campbell , Philip T. Quinlan

Charitable giving involves a complex economic and social decision because the giver expends resources for goods or services they will never receive. Although psychologists have identified numerous factors that influence charitable giving, there currently exists no unifying computational model of charitable choice. Here, we submit one such model, based within the strictures of Psychological Value Theory (PVT). In four experiments, we assess whether charitable giving is driven by the perceived Psychological Value of the recipient. Across all four experiments, we simultaneously predict response choice and response time with high accuracy. In a fifth experiment, we show that PVT predicts charitable giving more accurately than an account based on competence and warmth. PVT accurately predicts which charity a respondent will choose to donate to and separately, whether a respondent will choose to donate at all. PVT models the cognitive processes underlying charitable donations and it provides a computational framework for integrating known influences on charitable giving. For example, we show that in-group preference influences charitable giving by changing the Psychological Values of the options, rather than by bringing about a response bias toward the in-group.

慈善捐赠涉及一个复杂的经济和社会决策,因为捐赠者将资源用于他们永远不会得到的商品或服务。尽管心理学家已经确定了许多影响慈善捐赠的因素,但目前还没有统一的慈善选择计算模型。在这里,我们提出了一个这样的模型,基于心理价值理论(PVT)的限制。在四个实验中,我们评估慈善捐赠是否受到接受者感知的心理价值的驱动。在所有四个实验中,我们同时以高精度预测反应选择和反应时间。在第五个实验中,我们发现PVT比基于能力和热情的账户更准确地预测慈善捐赠。PVT准确地预测了受访者将选择向哪个慈善机构捐款,以及受访者是否会选择捐款。PVT对慈善捐赠背后的认知过程进行建模,并为整合已知的慈善捐赠影响提供了一个计算框架。例如,我们发现,群体内偏好通过改变选项的心理价值观来影响慈善捐赠,而不是通过对群体内偏好产生反应偏见。
{"title":"Psychological value theory: A computational cognitive model of charitable giving","authors":"Dale J. Cohen ,&nbsp;Monica K. Campbell ,&nbsp;Philip T. Quinlan","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101593","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101593","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Charitable giving involves a complex economic and social decision because the giver expends resources for goods or services they will never receive. Although psychologists have identified numerous factors that influence charitable giving, there currently exists no unifying computational model of charitable choice. Here, we submit one such model, based within the strictures of Psychological Value Theory (PVT). In four experiments, we assess whether charitable giving is driven by the perceived Psychological Value of the recipient. Across all four experiments, we simultaneously predict response choice and response time with high accuracy. In a fifth experiment, we show that PVT predicts charitable giving more accurately than an account based on competence and warmth. PVT accurately predicts which charity a respondent will choose to donate to and separately, whether a respondent will choose to donate at all. PVT models the cognitive processes underlying charitable donations and it provides a computational framework for integrating known influences on charitable giving. For example, we show that in-group preference influences charitable giving by changing the Psychological Values of the options, rather than by bringing about a response bias toward the in-group.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"145 ","pages":"Article 101593"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10211291","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Form to meaning mapping and the impact of explicit morpheme combination in novel word processing 形义映射与显性词素组合对小说词处理的影响。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101594
Rolando Bonandrini, Simona Amenta, Simone Sulpizio, Marco Tettamanti, Alessia Mazzucchelli, Marco Marelli

In the present study, we leveraged computational methods to explore the extent to which, relative to direct access to semantics from orthographic cues, the additional appreciation of morphological cues is advantageous while inducing the meaning of affixed pseudo-words. We re-analyzed data from a study on a lexical decision task for affixed pseudo-words. We considered a parsimonious model only including semantic variables (namely, semantic neighborhood density, entropy, magnitude, stem proximity) derived through a word-form-to-meaning approach (ngram-based). We then explored the extent to which the addition of equivalent semantic variables derived by combining semantic information from morphemes (combination-based) improved the fit of the statistical model explaining human data. Results suggest that semantic information can be extracted from arbitrary clusters of letters, yet a computational model of semantic access also including a combination-based strategy based on explicit morphological information better captures the cognitive mechanisms underlying human performance. This is particularly evident when participants recognize affixed pseudo-words as meaningful stimuli.

在本研究中,我们利用计算方法来探索相对于从正字法线索直接获取语义,在诱导附加伪词的含义时,对形态线索的额外欣赏在多大程度上是有利的。我们重新分析了一项关于附加伪词词汇决策任务的研究数据。我们考虑了一个简约模型,该模型仅包括通过从形式到意义的方法(基于ngram)导出的语义变量(即语义邻域密度、熵、幅度、词干接近度)。然后,我们探讨了通过组合语素的语义信息(基于组合)获得的等效语义变量的添加在多大程度上提高了解释人类数据的统计模型的拟合度。结果表明,语义信息可以从任意字母簇中提取,但语义访问的计算模型也包括基于明确形态信息的组合策略,可以更好地捕捉人类表现的认知机制。当参与者将附加的伪词识别为有意义的刺激时,这一点尤其明显。
{"title":"Form to meaning mapping and the impact of explicit morpheme combination in novel word processing","authors":"Rolando Bonandrini,&nbsp;Simona Amenta,&nbsp;Simone Sulpizio,&nbsp;Marco Tettamanti,&nbsp;Alessia Mazzucchelli,&nbsp;Marco Marelli","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101594","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101594","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In the present study, we leveraged computational methods to explore the extent to which, relative to direct access to semantics from orthographic cues, the additional appreciation of morphological cues is advantageous while inducing the meaning of affixed pseudo-words. We re-analyzed data from a study on a lexical decision task for affixed pseudo-words. We considered a parsimonious model only including semantic variables (namely, semantic neighborhood density, entropy, magnitude, stem proximity) derived through a word-form-to-meaning approach (<em>ngram</em>-based). We then explored the extent to which the addition of equivalent semantic variables derived by combining semantic information from morphemes (<em>combination</em>-based) improved the fit of the statistical model explaining human data. Results suggest that semantic information can be extracted from arbitrary clusters of letters, yet a computational model of semantic access also including a <em>combination</em>-based strategy based on explicit morphological information better captures the cognitive mechanisms underlying human performance. This is particularly evident when participants recognize affixed pseudo-words as meaningful stimuli.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"145 ","pages":"Article 101594"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10212250","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Serial attention to serial memory: The psychological refractory period in forward and backward cued recall 连续注意对连续记忆:前后线索回忆的心理不应期。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101583
Gordon D. Logan, Simon D. Lilburn, Jana E. Ulrich

Guided by the conjecture that memory retrieval is attention turned inward, we examined serial attention in serial memory, combining the psychological refractory period (PRP) procedure from attention research with cued recall of two items from brief six-item lists. We report six experiments showing robust PRP effects in cued recall from memory (1–4) and cued report from perceptual displays (5–6), which suggest that memory retrieval requires the same attentional bottleneck as “retrieval” from perception. There were strong direction effects in each memory experiment. Response time (RT) was shorter and accuracy was higher when the cues occurred in the forward direction (left-to-right, top-to-bottom, first-to-last), replicating differences between forward and backward serial recall. Cue positions had strong effects on RT and accuracy in the memory experiments (1–4). The pattern suggested that subjects find cued items in memory by stepping through the list from the beginning or the end, with a preference for starting at the beginning. The perceptual experiments (5–6) showed weak effects of position that were more consistent with direct access. In all experiments, the distance between the cues in the list (lag) had weak effects, suggesting that subjects searched for each cue from the beginning or end of the list more often than they moved through the list from the first cue to the second. Direction, distance, and lag effects on RT and inter-response interval changed with SOA in a manner that suggested they affect bottleneck or pre-bottleneck processes that create and execute a plan for successive retrievals. We conclude that sequential retrieval from memory and sequential attention to perception engage the same computations and we show how computational models of memory can be interpreted as models of attention focused on memory.

在记忆检索是注意力向内转向的猜想的指导下,我们将注意力研究中的心理不应期(PRP)过程与从简短的六个项目列表中提示回忆两个项目相结合,检验了连续记忆中的连续注意。我们报告了六个实验,显示了来自记忆的提示回忆(1-4)和来自感知显示的提示报告(5-6)中强大的PRP效应,这表明记忆检索需要与来自感知的“检索”相同的注意瓶颈。每个记忆实验都有很强的方向效应。当线索发生在正向(从左到右、从上到下、从前到后)时,反应时间(RT)更短,准确率更高,复制了正向和反向序列回忆之间的差异。提示位置对记忆实验中的RT和准确性有很强的影响(1-4)。该模式表明,受试者通过从头或尾遍历列表来找到记忆中的提示项目,并倾向于从头开始。感知实验(5-6)显示,位置的影响较弱,更符合直接访问。在所有实验中,列表中线索之间的距离(滞后)影响较弱,这表明受试者从列表的开头或结尾搜索每个线索的频率高于他们从第一个线索到第二个线索的搜索频率。方向、距离和滞后对RT和响应间隔的影响随着SOA的变化而变化,表明它们会影响创建和执行连续检索计划的瓶颈或瓶颈前流程。我们得出结论,从记忆的顺序检索和从注意力到感知的顺序检索涉及相同的计算,我们展示了记忆的计算模型如何被解释为专注于记忆的注意力模型。
{"title":"Serial attention to serial memory: The psychological refractory period in forward and backward cued recall","authors":"Gordon D. Logan,&nbsp;Simon D. Lilburn,&nbsp;Jana E. Ulrich","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101583","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101583","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Guided by the conjecture that memory retrieval is attention turned inward, we examined serial attention in serial memory, combining the psychological refractory period (PRP) procedure from attention research with cued recall of two items from brief six-item lists. We report six experiments showing robust PRP effects in cued recall from memory (1–4) and cued report from perceptual displays (5–6), which suggest that memory retrieval requires the same attentional bottleneck as “retrieval” from perception. There were strong direction effects in each memory experiment. Response time (RT) was shorter and accuracy was higher when the cues occurred in the forward direction (left-to-right, top-to-bottom, first-to-last), replicating differences between forward and backward serial recall. Cue positions had strong effects on RT and accuracy in the memory experiments (1–4). The pattern suggested that subjects find cued items in memory by stepping through the list from the beginning or the end, with a preference for starting at the beginning. The perceptual experiments (5–6) showed weak effects of position that were more consistent with direct access. In all experiments, the distance between the cues in the list (lag) had weak effects, suggesting that subjects searched for each cue from the beginning or end of the list more often than they moved through the list from the first cue to the second. Direction, distance, and lag effects on RT and inter-response interval changed with SOA in a manner that suggested they affect bottleneck or pre-bottleneck processes that create and execute a plan for successive retrievals. We conclude that sequential retrieval from memory and sequential attention to perception engage the same computations and we show how computational models of memory can be interpreted as models of attention focused on memory.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"145 ","pages":"Article 101583"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10574688","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A spatially continuous diffusion model of visual working memory 视觉工作记忆的空间连续扩散模型
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-08-31 DOI: 10.2139/ssrn.4352541
Alex Fennell, R. Ratcliff
We present results from five visual working memory (VWM) experiments in which participants were briefly shown between 2 and 6 colored squares. They were then cued to recall the color of one of the squares and they responded by choosing the color on a continuous color wheel. The experiments provided response proportions and response time (RT) measures as a function of angle for the choices. Current VWM models for this task include discrete models that assume an item is either within working memory or not and resource models that assume that memory strength varies as a function of the number of items. Because these models do not include processes that allow them to account for RT data, we implemented them within the spatially continuous diffusion model (SCDM, Ratcliff, 2018) and use the experimental data to evaluate these combined models. In the SCDM, evidence retrieved from memory is represented as a spatially continuous normal distribution and this drives the decision process until a criterion (represented as a 1-D line) is reached, which produces a decision. Noise in the accumulation process is represented by continuous Gaussian process noise over spatial position. The models that fit best from the discrete and resource-based classes converged on a common model that had a guessing component and that allowed the height of the normal memory-strength distribution to vary with number of items. The guessing component was implemented as a regular decision process driven by a flat evidence distribution, a zero-drift process. The combination of choice and RT data allows models that were not identifiable based on choice data alone to be discriminated.
我们介绍了五个视觉工作记忆(VWM)实验的结果,在这些实验中,参与者被短暂地展示在2到6个彩色方块之间。然后,他们被提示回忆其中一个方块的颜色,他们的反应是在一个连续的色轮上选择颜色。实验提供了反应比例和反应时间(RT)作为选择角度的函数。当前用于此任务的VWM模型包括离散模型,该模型假设一个项目要么在工作记忆中,要么不在工作记忆中,以及资源模型,该模型假设记忆强度随项目数量的变化而变化。由于这些模型不包括允许它们解释RT数据的过程,因此我们在空间连续扩散模型(SCDM, Ratcliff, 2018)中实现了它们,并使用实验数据来评估这些组合模型。在SCDM中,从记忆中检索到的证据被表示为空间连续的正态分布,这推动决策过程,直到达到一个标准(表示为1-D线),从而产生决策。累积过程中的噪声由空间位置上的连续高斯过程噪声表示。从离散的和基于资源的类别中,最适合的模型聚集在一个公共模型上,该模型具有猜测成分,并且允许正常记忆强度分布的高度随项目数量而变化。猜测组件被实现为一个规则的决策过程,由一个平坦的证据分布驱动,一个零漂移过程。选择数据和RT数据的组合允许仅基于选择数据无法识别的模型被区分开来。
{"title":"A spatially continuous diffusion model of visual working memory","authors":"Alex Fennell, R. Ratcliff","doi":"10.2139/ssrn.4352541","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.4352541","url":null,"abstract":"We present results from five visual working memory (VWM) experiments in which participants were briefly shown between 2 and 6 colored squares. They were then cued to recall the color of one of the squares and they responded by choosing the color on a continuous color wheel. The experiments provided response proportions and response time (RT) measures as a function of angle for the choices. Current VWM models for this task include discrete models that assume an item is either within working memory or not and resource models that assume that memory strength varies as a function of the number of items. Because these models do not include processes that allow them to account for RT data, we implemented them within the spatially continuous diffusion model (SCDM, Ratcliff, 2018) and use the experimental data to evaluate these combined models. In the SCDM, evidence retrieved from memory is represented as a spatially continuous normal distribution and this drives the decision process until a criterion (represented as a 1-D line) is reached, which produces a decision. Noise in the accumulation process is represented by continuous Gaussian process noise over spatial position. The models that fit best from the discrete and resource-based classes converged on a common model that had a guessing component and that allowed the height of the normal memory-strength distribution to vary with number of items. The guessing component was implemented as a regular decision process driven by a flat evidence distribution, a zero-drift process. The combination of choice and RT data allows models that were not identifiable based on choice data alone to be discriminated.","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-08-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48812020","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Keeping quantifier meaning in mind: Connecting semantics, cognition, and pragmatics 牢记量词意义:语义学、认知学和语用学的联系
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-08-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101584
Tyler Knowlton , John Trueswell , Anna Papafragou

A complete theory of the meaning of linguistic expressions needs to explain their semantic properties, their links to non-linguistic cognition, and their use in communication. Even though in principle interconnected, these areas are generally not pursued in tandem. We present a novel take on the semantics-cognition-pragmatics interface. We propose that formal semantic differences in expressions’ meanings lead those meanings to activate distinct cognitive systems, which in turn have downstream effects on when speakers prefer to use those expressions. As a case study, we focus on the quantifiers “each” and “every”, which can be used to talk about the same state of the world, but have been argued to differ in meaning. In particular, we adopt a mentalistic proposal about these quantifiers on which “each” has a purely individualistic meaning that interfaces with the psychological system for representing object-files, whereas “every” has a meaning that implicates a group and interfaces with the psychological system for representing ensembles. In seven experiments, we demonstrate that this account correctly predicts both known and newly-observed constraints on how “each” and “every” are pragmatically used. More generally, this integrated approach to semantics, cognition, and pragmatics suggests that canonical patterns of language use can be affected in predictable ways by fine-grained differences in semantic meanings and the cognitive systems to which those meanings connect.

一个完整的语言表达意义理论需要解释其语义特性、与非语言认知的联系以及在交际中的使用。尽管原则上相互关联,但这些领域通常不会同时进行。我们提出了一个新颖的语义认知语用界面。我们认为,表达意义的形式语义差异会导致这些意义激活不同的认知系统,进而对说话者何时更喜欢使用这些表达产生下游影响。作为一个案例研究,我们关注的是量词“each”和“every”,它们可以用来谈论世界的相同状态,但被认为意义不同。特别是,我们对这些量词采用了一种唯心主义的建议,其中“每个”具有纯粹的个人主义含义,与表示对象文件的心理系统接口,而“每个”则具有暗示一个群体并与表示集合的心理系统接口的含义。在七个实验中,我们证明了这种描述正确地预测了关于如何务实地使用“每个”和“每个”的已知和新观察到的约束。更普遍地说,这种对语义、认知和语用学的综合方法表明,语言使用的规范模式可以以可预测的方式受到语义和这些含义所连接的认知系统的细粒度差异的影响。
{"title":"Keeping quantifier meaning in mind: Connecting semantics, cognition, and pragmatics","authors":"Tyler Knowlton ,&nbsp;John Trueswell ,&nbsp;Anna Papafragou","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101584","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101584","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>A complete theory of the meaning of linguistic expressions needs to explain their semantic properties, their links to non-linguistic cognition, and their use in communication. Even though in principle interconnected, these areas are generally not pursued in tandem. We present a novel take on the semantics-cognition-pragmatics interface. We propose that formal semantic differences in expressions’ meanings lead those meanings to activate distinct cognitive systems, which in turn have downstream effects on when speakers prefer to use those expressions. As a case study, we focus on the quantifiers “each” and “every”, which can be used to talk about the same state of the world, but have been argued to differ in meaning. In particular, we adopt a mentalistic proposal about these quantifiers on which “each” has a purely individualistic meaning that interfaces with the psychological system for representing object-files, whereas “every” has a meaning that implicates a group and interfaces with the psychological system for representing ensembles. In seven experiments, we demonstrate that this account correctly predicts both known and newly-observed constraints on how “each” and “every” are pragmatically used. More generally, this integrated approach to semantics, cognition, and pragmatics suggests that canonical patterns of language use can be affected in predictable ways by fine-grained differences in semantic meanings and the cognitive systems to which those meanings connect.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"144 ","pages":"Article 101584"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9858810","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What’s so hard about hierarchical control? Pinpointing processing constraints within cue-based and serial-order control structures 等级控制有什么难的?在基于线索和串行顺序的控制结构中精确定位处理约束
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-08-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101582
Melissa E. Moss , Ulrich Mayr

Most task spaces require a hierarchical structure, where decisions on one level are contingent on previous decisions made on one or more higher levels. While it is a truism that increasing the number of hierarchical levels makes it harder to solve a given task, the exact nature of this “number-of-levels” effect is not clear. On the one hand, processing costs might be strictly “local,” incurred only when higher-level settings need to be updated, while otherwise lower-level decisions are insulated from the presence of higher-level settings (local updating costs with ballistic control). On the other hand, maintaining and integrating more complex hierarchical structures could require overall greater representational resources, negatively affecting each individual decision within the represented task space (global integration/maintenance costs). Further, navigation through hierarchical structures can be guided either through prompts in the environment (cue-based), or through sequential plans (serial-order based), with potentially distinct maintenance and updating demands. In two experiments, we assessed performance as a function of hierarchical level and format (serial-order vs. cue-based). Model comparisons showed that the pattern of costs in the serial-order format was consistent with a global maintenance/integration account. In contrast, in the cue-based format, costs arose at updating points and when one additional relevant level beyond the current decision was relevant, while additional levels produced no further costs. This overall constellation of costs can be explained by assuming that each decision requires checking the immediately relevant higher-level context for that decision. For cue-based control, this context involves the “next-level-up” rule, whereas in the serial-order format, each trial requires updating of the current position within the sequence, which in turn requires integration across all relevant hierarchical levels.

大多数任务空间都需要层次结构,其中一个级别上的决策取决于以前在一个或多个更高级别上做出的决策。虽然增加层级数量会使解决给定任务变得更加困难,但这种“层级数量”效应的确切性质尚不清楚。一方面,处理成本可能是严格的“本地”成本,只有在需要更新更高级别的设置时才会产生,而在其他方面,较低级别的决策与更高级别设置的存在隔绝(弹道控制的本地更新成本)。另一方面,维护和整合更复杂的层级结构可能需要更大的总体代表性资源,从而对所代表的任务空间内的每个单独决策产生负面影响(全球整合/维护成本)。此外,通过分层结构的导航可以通过环境中的提示(基于提示)或通过顺序计划(基于序列顺序)进行指导,具有潜在的不同维护和更新需求。在两个实验中,我们将性能评估为层次级别和格式的函数(序列顺序与基于线索)。模型比较表明,序列订单格式的成本模式与全球维护/集成账户一致。相反,在基于提示的格式中,当当前决策之外的一个额外的相关级别相关时,更新点会产生成本,而额外的级别不会产生进一步的成本。可以通过假设每个决策都需要检查该决策的直接相关的更高层上下文来解释这种总体成本组合。对于基于线索的控制,这种上下文涉及“下一级”规则,而在序列顺序格式中,每次试验都需要更新序列中的当前位置,这反过来又需要跨所有相关层次进行集成。
{"title":"What’s so hard about hierarchical control? Pinpointing processing constraints within cue-based and serial-order control structures","authors":"Melissa E. Moss ,&nbsp;Ulrich Mayr","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101582","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101582","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Most task spaces require a hierarchical structure, where decisions on one level are contingent on previous decisions made on one or more higher levels. While it is a truism that increasing the number of hierarchical levels makes it harder to solve a given task, the exact nature of this “number-of-levels” effect is not clear. On the one hand, processing costs might be strictly “local,” incurred only when higher-level settings need to be updated, while otherwise lower-level decisions are insulated from the presence of higher-level settings (local updating costs with ballistic control). On the other hand, maintaining and integrating more complex hierarchical structures could require overall greater representational resources, negatively affecting each individual decision within the represented task space (global integration/maintenance costs). Further, navigation through hierarchical structures can be guided either through prompts in the environment (cue-based), or through sequential plans (serial-order based), with potentially distinct maintenance and updating demands. In two experiments, we assessed performance as a function of hierarchical level and format (serial-order vs. cue-based). Model comparisons showed that the pattern of costs in the serial-order format was consistent with a global maintenance/integration account. In contrast, in the cue-based format, costs arose at updating points and when one additional relevant level beyond the current decision was relevant, while additional levels produced no further costs. This overall constellation of costs can be explained by assuming that each decision requires checking the immediately relevant higher-level context for that decision. For cue-based control, this context involves the “next-level-up” rule, whereas in the serial-order format, each trial requires updating of the current position within the sequence, which in turn requires integration across all relevant hierarchical levels.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"144 ","pages":"Article 101582"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10044804","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Events and objects are similar cognitive entities 事件和对象是相似的认知实体
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101573
Anna Papafragou , Yue Ji

Logico-semantic theories have long noted parallels between the linguistic representation of temporal entities (events) and spatial entities (objects): bounded (or telic) predicates such as fix a car resemble count nouns such as sandcastle because they are “atoms” that have well-defined boundaries, contain discrete minimal parts and cannot be divided arbitrarily. By contrast, unbounded (or atelic) phrases such as drive a car resemble mass nouns such as sand in that they are unspecified for atomic features. Here, we demonstrate for the first time the parallels in the perceptual-cognitive representation of events and objects even in entirely non-linguistic tasks. Specifically, after viewers form categories of bounded or unbounded events, they can extend the category to objects or substances respectively (Experiments 1 and 2). Furthermore, in a training study, people successfully learn event-to-object mappings that respect atomicity (i.e., grouping bounded events with objects and unbounded events with substances) but fail to acquire the opposite, atomicity-violating mappings (Experiment 3). Finally, viewers can spontaneously draw connections between events and objects without any prior training (Experiment 4). These striking similarities between the mental representation of events and objects have implications for current theories of event cognition, as well as the relationship between language and thought.

逻辑语义理论长期以来一直注意到时间实体(事件)和空间实体(对象)的语言表示之间的相似之处:有界(或telic)谓词(如fix a car)类似于计数名词(如sandcastle),因为它们是“原子”,具有明确的边界,包含离散的最小部分,不能任意划分。相比之下,像“开车”这样的无界(或atelic)短语类似于“沙子”这样的大众名词,因为它们没有指定原子特征。在这里,我们首次证明了事件和对象的感知认知表征的相似性,即使在完全非语言的任务中也是如此。具体来说,在观看者形成有界或无界事件的类别后,他们可以将该类别分别扩展到对象或物质(实验1和2)。此外,在一项训练研究中,人们成功地学习了尊重原子性的事件到对象映射(即,将有界事件与对象分组,将无界事件与物质分组),但未能获得相反的违反原子性的映射(实验3)。最后,观看者可以在没有任何事先训练的情况下自发地绘制事件和对象之间的联系(实验4)。事件和物体的心理表征之间的这些惊人的相似之处对当前的事件认知理论以及语言和思维之间的关系都有启示。
{"title":"Events and objects are similar cognitive entities","authors":"Anna Papafragou ,&nbsp;Yue Ji","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101573","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101573","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Logico-semantic theories have long noted parallels between the linguistic representation of temporal entities (events) and spatial entities (objects): bounded (or telic) predicates such as <em>fix a car</em> resemble count nouns such as <em>sandcastle</em> because they are “atoms” that have well-defined boundaries, contain discrete minimal parts and cannot be divided arbitrarily. By contrast, unbounded (or atelic) phrases such as <em>drive a car</em> resemble mass nouns such as <em>sand</em> in that they are unspecified for atomic features. Here, we demonstrate for the first time the parallels in the <em>perceptual-cognitive</em> representation of events and objects even in entirely non-linguistic tasks. Specifically, after viewers form categories of bounded or unbounded events, they can extend the category to objects or substances respectively (Experiments 1 and 2). Furthermore, in a training study, people successfully learn event-to-object mappings that respect atomicity (i.e., grouping bounded events with objects and unbounded events with substances) but fail to acquire the opposite, atomicity-violating mappings (Experiment 3). Finally, viewers can spontaneously draw connections between events and objects without any prior training (Experiment 4). These striking similarities between the mental representation of events and objects have implications for current theories of event cognition, as well as the relationship between language and thought.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"143 ","pages":"Article 101573"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9604614","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The role of rehearsal and reminding in the recall of categorized word lists 复述和提醒在分类词表回忆中的作用
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101563
Geoff Ward , Lydia Tan

Most theories of free recall emphasize the importance of retrieval in explaining temporal and semantic regularities in recall; rehearsal mechanisms are often absent or limit rehearsal to a subset of what was last rehearsed. However, in three experiments using the overt rehearsal method, we show clear evidence that just-presented items act as retrieval cues during encoding (study-phase retrieval) with prior related items rehearsed despite well over a dozen intervening items. Experiment 1 examined free recall of categorized and uncategorized lists of 32 words. In Experiments 2 and 3, we presented categorized lists of 24, 48, and 64 words for free recall or cued recall, with the category exemplars blocked in successive list positions (Experiment 2) or randomized throughout the list (Experiment 3). The probability of rehearsing a prior word was affected by its semantic similarity to the just-presented item, and the frequency and recency of its prior rehearsals. These rehearsal data suggest alternative interpretations to well-known recall phenomena. With randomized designs, the serial position curves were reinterpreted by when words were last rehearsed (which contributed to the list length effects), and semantic clustering and temporal contiguity effects at output were reinterpreted by whether words were co-rehearsed during study. The contrast with the blocked designs suggests that recall is sensitive to the relative (not absolute) recency of targeted list items. We discuss the benefits of incorporating rehearsal machinery into computational models of episodic memory, and suggest that the same retrieval processes that generate the recalls are used to generate the rehearsals.

大多数自由回忆理论都强调检索在解释回忆的时间和语义规律方面的重要性;排练机制通常不存在,或者将排练限制在上次排练的一个子集。然而,在使用公开排练方法的三个实验中,我们显示了明确的证据,即在编码(研究阶段检索)过程中,尽管有十几个干预项目,但刚刚呈现的项目充当了检索线索,先前的相关项目也进行了排练。实验1检查了32个单词的分类和非分类列表的自由回忆。在实验2和3中,我们提出了24个、48个和64个单词的分类列表,用于自由回忆或提示回忆,类别样本被阻塞在连续的列表位置(实验2)或在整个列表中随机化(实验3)。排练先前单词的概率受其与刚呈现的项目的语义相似性以及先前排练的频率和最近性的影响。这些预演数据为众所周知的回忆现象提供了另一种解释。在随机设计中,序列位置曲线通过单词最后一次排练的时间来重新解释(这有助于列表长度效应),输出时的语义聚类和时间邻接效应通过单词在研究期间是否共同排练来重新解释。与封锁设计的对比表明,召回对目标列表项目的相对(而不是绝对)最近性很敏感。我们讨论了将排练机制纳入情景记忆计算模型的好处,并建议使用与生成回忆相同的检索过程来生成排练。
{"title":"The role of rehearsal and reminding in the recall of categorized word lists","authors":"Geoff Ward ,&nbsp;Lydia Tan","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101563","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101563","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Most theories of free recall emphasize the importance of retrieval in explaining temporal and semantic regularities in recall; rehearsal mechanisms are often absent or limit rehearsal to a subset of what was last rehearsed. However, in three experiments using the overt rehearsal method, we show clear evidence that just-presented items act as retrieval cues during encoding (study-phase retrieval) with prior related items rehearsed despite well over a dozen intervening items. Experiment 1 examined free recall of categorized and uncategorized lists of 32 words. In Experiments 2 and 3, we presented categorized lists of 24, 48, and 64 words for free recall or cued recall, with the category exemplars blocked in successive list positions (Experiment 2) or randomized throughout the list (Experiment 3). The probability of rehearsing a prior word was affected by its semantic similarity to the just-presented item, and the frequency and recency of its prior rehearsals. These rehearsal data suggest alternative interpretations to well-known recall phenomena. With randomized designs, the serial position curves were reinterpreted by when words were last rehearsed (which contributed to the list length effects), and semantic clustering and temporal contiguity effects at output were reinterpreted by whether words were co-rehearsed during study. The contrast with the blocked designs suggests that recall is sensitive to the relative (not absolute) recency of targeted list items. We discuss the benefits of incorporating rehearsal machinery into computational models of episodic memory, and suggest that the same retrieval processes that generate the recalls are used to generate the rehearsals.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"143 ","pages":"Article 101563"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9978563","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
期刊
Cognitive Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1