首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Middle-schoolers' misconceptions in discretized nonsymbolic proportional reasoning explain fraction biases better than their continuous reasoning: Evidence from correlation and cluster analyses 中学生在离散非符号比例推理中的误解比他们的连续推理更能解释分数偏差:来自相关和聚类分析的证据
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101575
Roberto A. Abreu-Mendoza , Arthur B. Powell , K. Ann Renninger , Luis M. Rivera , John Vulic , Steve Weimar , Miriam Rosenberg-Lee

Early emerging nonsymbolic proportional skills have been posited as a foundational ability for later fraction learning. A positive relation between nonsymbolic and symbolic proportional reasoning has been reported, as well as successful nonsymbolic training and intervention programs enhancing fraction magnitude skills. However, little is known about the mechanisms underlying this relationship. Of particular interest are nonsymbolic representations, which can be in continuous formats that may emphasize proportional relations and in discretized formats that may prompt erroneous whole-number strategies and hamper access to fraction magnitudes. We assessed the proportional comparison skills of 159 middle-school students (mean age = 12.54 years, 43% females, 55% males, 2% other or prefer not to say) across three types of representations: (a) continuous, unsegmented bars, (b) discretized, segmented bars that allowed counting strategies, and (c) symbolic fractions. Using both correlational and cluster approaches, we also examined their relations to symbolic fraction comparison ability. Within each stimulus type, we varied proportional distance, and in the discretized and symbolic stimuli, we also manipulated whole-number congruency. We found that fraction distance across all formats modulated middle-schoolers' performance; however, whole-number information affected discretized and symbolic comparison performance. Further, continuous and discretized nonsymbolic performance was related to fraction comparison ability; however, discretized skills explained variance above and beyond the contributions of continuous skills. Finally, our cluster analyses revealed three nonsymbolic comparison profiles: students who chose the bars with the largest number of segments (whole-number bias), chance-level performers, and high performers. Crucially, students with a whole-number bias profile showed this bias in their fraction skills and failed to show any symbolic distance modulation. Together, our results indicate that the relation between nonsymbolic and symbolic proportional skills may be determined by the (mis)conceptions based on discretized representations, rather than understandings of proportional magnitudes, suggesting that interventions focusing on competence with discretized representations may show dividends for fraction understanding.

早期出现的非符号比例技能被认为是后期分数学习的基础能力。据报道,非符号和符号比例推理之间存在正相关关系,以及成功的非符号训练和干预计划提高了分数量级技能。然而,人们对这种关系背后的机制知之甚少。特别令人感兴趣的是非符号表示,它可以是强调比例关系的连续格式,也可以是可能引发错误整数策略并阻碍分数大小访问的离散格式。我们评估了159名中学生(平均年龄=12.54岁,43%为女性,55%为男性,2%为其他或不愿透露)在三种类型的表征中的比例比较技能:(a)连续的、未分段的小节,(b)允许计数策略的离散的、分段的小节和(c)符号分数。使用相关和聚类方法,我们还研究了它们与符号分数比较能力的关系。在每种刺激类型中,我们改变了比例距离,在离散化和符号化刺激中,我们还操纵了整数一致性。我们发现,所有形式的分数距离都会影响中学生的表现;然而,整数信息影响了离散化和符号化的比较性能。此外,连续和离散化的非符号性能与分数比较能力有关;然而,离散化技能解释了超出连续技能贡献的差异。最后,我们的聚类分析揭示了三个非符号比较特征:选择分段数量最多的酒吧的学生(整数偏差)、机会水平的表演者和高表演者。至关重要的是,具有整数偏差特征的学生在分数技能中表现出了这种偏差,并且没有表现出任何符号距离调制。总之,我们的结果表明,非符号和符号比例技能之间的关系可能是由基于离散表示的(错误)概念决定的,而不是由对比例大小的理解决定的,这表明专注于具有离散表示的能力的干预措施可能会对分数理解产生好处。
{"title":"Middle-schoolers' misconceptions in discretized nonsymbolic proportional reasoning explain fraction biases better than their continuous reasoning: Evidence from correlation and cluster analyses","authors":"Roberto A. Abreu-Mendoza ,&nbsp;Arthur B. Powell ,&nbsp;K. Ann Renninger ,&nbsp;Luis M. Rivera ,&nbsp;John Vulic ,&nbsp;Steve Weimar ,&nbsp;Miriam Rosenberg-Lee","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101575","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101575","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Early emerging nonsymbolic proportional skills have been posited as a foundational ability for later fraction learning. A positive relation between nonsymbolic and symbolic proportional reasoning has been reported, as well as successful nonsymbolic training and intervention programs enhancing fraction magnitude skills. However, little is known about the mechanisms underlying this relationship. Of particular interest are nonsymbolic representations, which can be in continuous formats that may emphasize proportional relations and in discretized formats that may prompt erroneous whole-number strategies and hamper access to fraction magnitudes. We assessed the proportional comparison skills of 159 middle-school students (mean age = 12.54 years, 43% females, 55% males, 2% other or prefer not to say) across three types of representations: (a) continuous, unsegmented bars, (b) discretized, segmented bars that allowed counting strategies, and (c) symbolic fractions. Using both correlational and cluster approaches, we also examined their relations to symbolic fraction comparison ability. Within each stimulus type, we varied proportional distance, and in the discretized and symbolic stimuli, we also manipulated whole-number congruency. We found that fraction distance across all formats modulated middle-schoolers' performance; however, whole-number information affected discretized and symbolic comparison performance. Further, continuous and discretized nonsymbolic performance was related to fraction comparison ability; however, discretized skills explained variance above and beyond the contributions of continuous skills. Finally, our cluster analyses revealed three nonsymbolic comparison profiles: students who chose the bars with the largest number of segments (whole-number bias), chance-level performers, and high performers. Crucially, students with a whole-number bias profile showed this bias in their fraction skills and failed to show any symbolic distance modulation. Together, our results indicate that the relation between nonsymbolic and symbolic proportional skills may be determined by the (mis)conceptions based on discretized representations, rather than understandings of proportional magnitudes, suggesting that interventions focusing on competence with discretized representations may show dividends for fraction understanding.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"143 ","pages":"Article 101575"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9605134","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Promoting climate actions: A cognitive-constraints approach 促进气候行动:一种认知约束方法
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101565
Junho Lee , Emily F. Wong , Patricia W. Cheng

The present paper reports an experiment with a two-year-delayed (M = 695 days) follow-up that tests an approach to raising willingness to take political and personal climate actions. Many Americans still do not view climate change as a threat requiring urgent action. Moreover, among American conservatives, higher science literacy is paradoxically associated with higher anthropogenic climate-change skepticism. Our experimental materials were designed to harness the power of two central cognitive constraints — coherence and causal invariance, which map onto two narrative proclivities that anthropologists have identified as universal — to promote climate action across the political spectrum. Towards that goal, the essential role of these constraints in the causal-belief-formation process predicts that climate-change information would be more persuasive when it is embedded in a personal climate-action narrative, the evocation of which can benefit from exposure to parsimonious scientific explanations of indisputable everyday observations, juxtaposed with reasoners’ own, typically less coherent explanations, occurring in a context that engages their moral stance. Our brief one-time intervention, conducted in ten U.S. states with the highest level of climate skepticism, showed that across the political spectrum, our materials raised appreciation of science, openness to alternative views, and willingness to take climate actions in the immediate assessment. It also raised how likely were reports two years later of having taken those actions or would have taken them had the opportunity existed, suggesting a long-lasting effect. Our approach adopts the framework that conceptions of reality are representations, and adaptive solutions in that infinite space of representations require cognitive constraints to narrow the search.

本文报告了一项为期两年(M=695天)的延迟随访实验,该实验测试了一种提高采取政治和个人气候行动意愿的方法。许多美国人仍然不认为气候变化是一种需要紧急行动的威胁。此外,在美国保守派中,更高的科学素养与更高的人为气候变化怀疑论矛盾地联系在一起。我们的实验材料旨在利用两个核心认知约束的力量——连贯性和因果不变性,这两个认知约束映射到人类学家认为普遍存在的两种叙事倾向上——在政治光谱中促进气候行动。为了实现这一目标,这些制约因素在因果信念形成过程中的重要作用预测,当气候变化信息嵌入个人气候行动叙事中时,它将更有说服力,人们可以从对无可争议的日常观察的简约科学解释中受益,并与推理者自己的观点并置,通常不太连贯的解释,发生在涉及他们道德立场的背景下。我们在美国十个对气候持最高怀疑态度的州进行了短暂的一次性干预,结果表明,在各个政治派别中,我们的材料提高了对科学的认识、对其他观点的开放态度,以及在立即评估中采取气候行动的意愿。它还提出了两年后采取这些行动的可能性有多大,或者如果有机会就会采取这些行动,这表明会产生长期影响。我们的方法采用了现实概念是表征的框架,在表征的无限空间中的自适应解决方案需要认知约束来缩小搜索范围。
{"title":"Promoting climate actions: A cognitive-constraints approach","authors":"Junho Lee ,&nbsp;Emily F. Wong ,&nbsp;Patricia W. Cheng","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101565","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101565","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The present paper reports an experiment with a two-year-delayed (<em>M =</em> 695 days) follow-up that tests an approach to raising willingness to take political and personal climate actions. Many Americans still do not view climate change as a threat requiring urgent action. Moreover, among American conservatives, higher science literacy is paradoxically associated with higher anthropogenic climate-change skepticism. Our experimental materials were designed to harness the power of two central cognitive constraints — coherence and causal invariance, which map onto two narrative proclivities that anthropologists have identified as universal — to promote climate action across the political spectrum. Towards that goal, the essential role of these constraints in the causal-belief-formation process predicts that climate-change information would be more persuasive when it is embedded in a personal climate-action narrative, the evocation of which can benefit from exposure to parsimonious scientific explanations of indisputable everyday observations, juxtaposed with reasoners’ own, typically less coherent explanations, occurring in a context that engages their moral stance. Our brief one-time intervention, conducted in ten U.S. states with the highest level of climate skepticism, showed that across the political spectrum, our materials raised appreciation of science, openness to alternative views, and willingness to take climate actions in the immediate assessment. It also raised how likely were reports two years later of having taken those actions or would have taken them had the opportunity existed, suggesting a long-lasting effect. Our approach adopts the framework that conceptions of reality are representations, and adaptive solutions in that infinite space of representations require cognitive constraints to narrow the search.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"143 ","pages":"Article 101565"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9597771","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Heterogeneity of rules in Bayesian reasoning: A toolbox analysis 贝叶斯推理规则的异质性:工具箱分析
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101564
Jan K. Woike , Ralph Hertwig , Gerd Gigerenzer

How do people infer the Bayesian posterior probability from stated base rate, hit rate, and false alarm rate? This question is not only of theoretical relevance but also of practical relevance in medical and legal settings. We test two competing theoretical views: single-process theories versus toolbox theories. Single-process theories assume that a single process explains people’s inferences and have indeed been observed to fit people’s inferences well. Examples are Bayes’s rule, the representativeness heuristic, and a weighing-and-adding model. Their assumed process homogeneity implies unimodal response distributions. Toolbox theories, in contrast, assume process heterogeneity, implying multimodal response distributions. After analyzing response distributions in studies with laypeople and professionals, we find little support for the single-process theories tested. Using simulations, we find that a single process, the weighing-and-adding model, nevertheless can best fit the aggregate data and, surprisingly, also achieve the best out-of-sample prediction even though it fails to predict any single respondent’s inferences. To identify the potential toolbox of rules, we test how well candidate rules predict a set of over 10,000 inferences (culled from the literature) from 4,188 participants and 106 different Bayesian tasks. A toolbox of five non-Bayesian rules plus Bayes’s rule captures 64% of inferences. Finally, we validate the Five-Plus toolbox in three experiments that measure response times, self-reports, and strategy use. The most important conclusion from these analyses is that the fitting of single-process theories to aggregate data risks misidentifying the cognitive process. Antidotes to that risk are careful analyses of process and rule heterogeneity across people.

人们如何从规定的基本率、命中率和误报率推断贝叶斯后验概率?这个问题不仅具有理论意义,而且在医学和法律环境中也具有现实意义。我们测试了两种相互竞争的理论观点:单过程理论与工具箱理论。单过程理论假设单个过程可以解释人们的推断,并且确实被观察到很好地符合人们的推断。例如贝叶斯规则、代表性启发式算法和加权加法模型。它们假定的过程同质性意味着单峰响应分布。相反,工具箱理论假设过程异质性,意味着多模态响应分布。在分析了非专业人士和专业人士的研究中的反应分布后,我们发现测试的单过程理论几乎没有得到支持。通过模拟,我们发现单个过程,即加权和加法模型,仍然可以最好地拟合聚合数据,并且令人惊讶的是,即使它无法预测任何单个受访者的推断,也可以实现最佳的样本外预测。为了确定潜在的规则工具箱,我们测试了候选规则对4188名参与者和106个不同贝叶斯任务的10000多个推论(从文献中挑选)的预测效果。一个由五个非贝叶斯规则加上贝叶斯规则组成的工具箱可以捕获64%的推断。最后,我们在三个实验中验证了Five Plus工具箱,这些实验测量了反应时间、自我报告和策略使用。这些分析得出的最重要的结论是,将单过程理论应用于聚合数据可能会错误识别认知过程。这种风险的反方是对不同人群的过程和规则异质性的仔细分析。
{"title":"Heterogeneity of rules in Bayesian reasoning: A toolbox analysis","authors":"Jan K. Woike ,&nbsp;Ralph Hertwig ,&nbsp;Gerd Gigerenzer","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101564","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101564","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>How do people infer the Bayesian posterior probability from stated base rate, hit rate, and false alarm rate? This question is not only of theoretical relevance but also of practical relevance in medical and legal settings. We test two competing theoretical views: single-process theories versus toolbox theories. Single-process theories assume that a single process explains people’s inferences and have indeed been observed to fit people’s inferences well. Examples are Bayes’s rule, the representativeness heuristic, and a weighing-and-adding model. Their assumed process homogeneity implies unimodal response distributions. Toolbox theories, in contrast, assume process heterogeneity, implying multimodal response distributions. After analyzing response distributions in studies with laypeople and professionals, we find little support for the single-process theories tested. Using simulations, we find that a single process, the weighing-and-adding model, nevertheless can best fit the aggregate data and, surprisingly, also achieve the best out-of-sample prediction even though it fails to predict any single respondent’s inferences. To identify the potential toolbox of rules, we test how well candidate rules predict a set of over 10,000 inferences (culled from the literature) from 4,188 participants and 106 different Bayesian tasks. A toolbox of five non-Bayesian rules plus Bayes’s rule captures 64% of inferences. Finally, we validate the Five-Plus toolbox in three experiments that measure response times, self-reports, and strategy use. The most important conclusion from these analyses is that the fitting of single-process theories to aggregate data risks misidentifying the cognitive process. Antidotes to that risk are careful analyses of process and rule heterogeneity across people.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"143 ","pages":"Article 101564"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9604616","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Two pathways in vocabulary development: Large-scale differences in noun and verb semantic structure 词汇发展的两条途径:名词和动词语义结构的大规模差异
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101574
Justin B. Kueser , Sabrina Horvath , Arielle Borovsky

In adults, nouns and verbs have varied and multilevel semantic interrelationships. In children, evidence suggests that nouns and verbs also have semantic interrelationships, though the timing of the emergence of these relationships and their precise impact on later noun and verb learning are not clear. In this work, we ask whether noun and verb semantic knowledge in 16–30-month-old children tend to be semantically isolated from one another or semantically interacting from the onset of vocabulary development. Early word learning patterns were quantified using network science. We measured the semantic network structure for nouns and verbs in 3,804 16–30-month-old children at several levels of granularity using a large, open dataset of vocabulary checklist data. In a cross-sectional approach in Experiment 1, early nouns and verbs exhibited stronger network relationships with other nouns and verbs than expected across multiple network levels. Using a longitudinal approach in Experiment 2, we examined patterns of normative vocabulary development over time. Initial noun and verb learning was supported by strong semantic connections to other nouns, whereas later-learned words exhibited strong connections to verbs. Overall, these two experiments suggest that nouns and verbs demonstrate early semantic interactions and that these interactions impact later word learning. Early verb and noun learning is affected by the emergence of noun and verb semantic networks during early lexical development.

在成年人中,名词和动词具有多种多样、多层次的语义相互关系。在儿童中,有证据表明名词和动词也有语义上的相互关系,尽管这些关系出现的时间及其对后来名词和动词学习的确切影响尚不清楚。在这项工作中,我们询问16-30个月大的儿童的名词和动词语义知识从词汇发展开始是否倾向于在语义上相互隔离或语义上相互作用。早期的单词学习模式是使用网络科学进行量化的。我们使用词汇清单数据的大型开放数据集,在几个粒度级别上测量了3804名16-30个月大儿童的名词和动词的语义网络结构。在实验1的横断面方法中,早期名词和动词在多个网络层面上与其他名词和动词表现出比预期更强的网络关系。在实验2中,我们采用纵向方法,考察了规范性词汇随时间的发展模式。最初的名词和动词学习得到了与其他名词的强烈语义联系的支持,而后来学习的单词表现出与动词的强烈联系。总的来说,这两个实验表明,名词和动词表现出早期的语义交互作用,这些交互作用影响后期的单词学习。在早期词汇发展过程中,名词和动词语义网络的出现影响了早期动词和名词的学习。
{"title":"Two pathways in vocabulary development: Large-scale differences in noun and verb semantic structure","authors":"Justin B. Kueser ,&nbsp;Sabrina Horvath ,&nbsp;Arielle Borovsky","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101574","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101574","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In adults, nouns and verbs have varied and multilevel semantic interrelationships. In children, evidence suggests that nouns and verbs also have semantic interrelationships, though the timing of the emergence of these relationships and their precise impact on later noun and verb learning are not clear. In this work, we ask whether noun and verb semantic knowledge in 16–30-month-old children tend to be semantically isolated from one another or semantically interacting from the onset of vocabulary development. Early word learning patterns were quantified using network science. We measured the semantic network structure for nouns and verbs in 3,804 16–30-month-old children at several levels of granularity using a large, open dataset of vocabulary checklist data. In a cross-sectional approach in Experiment 1, early nouns and verbs exhibited stronger network relationships with other nouns and verbs than expected across multiple network levels. Using a longitudinal approach in Experiment 2, we examined patterns of normative vocabulary development over time. Initial noun and verb learning was supported by strong semantic connections to other nouns, whereas later-learned words exhibited strong connections to verbs. Overall, these two experiments suggest that nouns and verbs demonstrate early semantic interactions and that these interactions impact later word learning. Early verb and noun learning is affected by the emergence of noun and verb semantic networks during early lexical development.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"143 ","pages":"Article 101574"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9962333","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Analogical inferences mediated by relational categories 由关系范畴介导的类推推理
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101561
Ricardo A. Minervino , Adrián Margni , Máximo Trench

The standard approach posits that analogical inferences are generated by copying unmapped base relations, substituting mapped target entities for source entities, and generating slots for base entities that have not found a correspondence in the target. In the present study we argue that this mechanism does not adequately explain the generation of inferences mediated by relational categories. Experiment 1 revealed that for analogies in which the gist of the information to be transferred is better captured by relational categories than by explicit relations, inferences are more concerned with reinstantiating the base relational category than with ensuring that the relation of the inference resembles that of the base. Experiment 2 replicated this finding with analogies between situations maintaining a higher degree of semantic and contextual distance. The following experiments addressed whether there are further restrictions that guide a more fine-grained selection of exemplars. Experiment 3 revealed that when no relevant differences exist between compared situations, the exemplars included in analogical inferences tend to match the base exemplars along salient dimensions of the relational category to which both exemplars belong. In turn, Experiment 4 replicated this finding with analogies between situations maintaining some degree of semantic and contextual distance. The study adds to a growing literature recognizing the role of categorization in analogical reasoning. The challenges posed by the present results to the traditional view of analogical inference are discussed, as well as the prospects of the categorial mechanism for explaining other types of analogies not included in the present study.

标准方法假设通过复制未映射的基本关系、用映射的目标实体替换源实体以及为在目标中未找到对应关系的基本实体生成槽来生成类比推理。在本研究中,我们认为这种机制不能充分解释由关系类别介导的推论的产生。实验1表明,对于关系类别比显式关系更好地捕捉要传递的信息的要点的类比,推理更关心的是重新实例化基础关系类别,而不是确保推理的关系与基础的关系相似。实验2通过在保持较高语义和上下文距离的情况之间进行类比来复制这一发现。以下实验讨论了是否有进一步的限制来指导更细粒度的样本选择。实验3表明,当比较情境之间不存在相关差异时,类比推理中包含的样本倾向于沿着两个样本所属关系类别的显著维度与基础样本匹配。反过来,实验4通过在保持一定程度的语义和上下文距离的情况之间进行类比来复制这一发现。这项研究增加了越来越多的文献认识到分类在类比推理中的作用。讨论了当前结果对传统类比推理观点提出的挑战,以及解释本研究中未包括的其他类型类比的分类机制的前景。
{"title":"Analogical inferences mediated by relational categories","authors":"Ricardo A. Minervino ,&nbsp;Adrián Margni ,&nbsp;Máximo Trench","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101561","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101561","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The standard approach posits that analogical inferences are generated by copying unmapped base relations, substituting mapped target entities for source entities, and generating slots for base entities that have not found a correspondence in the target. In the present study we argue that this mechanism does not adequately explain the generation of inferences mediated by relational categories. Experiment 1 revealed that for analogies in which the gist of the information to be transferred is better captured by relational categories than by explicit relations, inferences are more concerned with reinstantiating the base relational category than with ensuring that the relation of the inference resembles that of the base. Experiment 2 replicated this finding with analogies between situations maintaining a higher degree of semantic and contextual distance. The following experiments addressed whether there are further restrictions that guide a more fine-grained selection of exemplars. Experiment 3 revealed that when no relevant differences exist between compared situations, the exemplars included in analogical inferences tend to match the base exemplars along salient dimensions of the relational category to which both exemplars belong. In turn, Experiment 4 replicated this finding with analogies between situations maintaining some degree of semantic and contextual distance. The study adds to a growing literature recognizing the role of categorization in analogical reasoning. The challenges posed by the present results to the traditional view of analogical inference are discussed, as well as the prospects of the categorial mechanism for explaining other types of analogies not included in the present study.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"142 ","pages":"Article 101561"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9854417","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Information acquisition and decision strategies in intertemporal choice 跨期选择中的信息获取与决策策略
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101562
Lisheng He , Daniel Wall , Crystal Reeck , Sudeep Bhatia

Intertemporal decision models describe choices between outcomes with different delays. While these models mainly focus on predicting choices, they make implicit assumptions about how people acquire and process information. A link between information processing and choice model predictions is necessary for a complete mechanistic account of decision making. We establish this link by fitting 18 intertemporal choice models to experimental datasets with both choice and information acquisition data. First, we show that choice models have highly correlated fits: people that behave according to one model also behave according to other models that make similar information processing assumptions. Second, we develop and fit an attention model to information acquisition data. Critically, the attention model parameters predict which type of intertemporal choice models best describes a participant’s choices. Overall, our results relate attentional processes to models of intertemporal choice, providing a stepping stone towards a complete mechanistic account of intertemporal decision making.

时间间决策模型描述了在具有不同延迟的结果之间的选择。虽然这些模型主要侧重于预测选择,但它们对人们如何获取和处理信息做出了隐含的假设。信息处理和选择模型预测之间的联系对于决策的完整机制描述是必要的。我们通过将18个跨期选择模型拟合到具有选择和信息获取数据的实验数据集来建立这种联系。首先,我们证明了选择模型具有高度相关的拟合:根据一个模型行事的人也根据做出类似信息处理假设的其他模型行事。其次,我们开发了一个注意力模型,并将其拟合到信息获取数据中。至关重要的是,注意力模型参数预测哪种类型的跨期选择模型最能描述参与者的选择。总的来说,我们的研究结果将注意力过程与跨期选择模型联系起来,为跨期决策的完整机制解释提供了垫脚石。
{"title":"Information acquisition and decision strategies in intertemporal choice","authors":"Lisheng He ,&nbsp;Daniel Wall ,&nbsp;Crystal Reeck ,&nbsp;Sudeep Bhatia","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101562","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101562","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Intertemporal decision models describe choices between outcomes with different delays. While these models mainly focus on predicting choices, they make implicit assumptions about how people acquire and process information. A link between information processing and choice model predictions is necessary for a complete mechanistic account of decision making. We establish this link by fitting 18 intertemporal choice models to experimental datasets with both choice and information acquisition data. First, we show that choice models have highly correlated fits: people that behave according to one model also behave according to other models that make similar information processing assumptions. Second, we develop and fit an attention model to information acquisition data. Critically, the attention model parameters predict which type of intertemporal choice models best describes a participant’s choices. Overall, our results relate attentional processes to models of intertemporal choice, providing a stepping stone towards a complete mechanistic account of intertemporal decision making.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"142 ","pages":"Article 101562"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9487694","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Relational and lexical similarity in analogical reasoning and recognition memory: Behavioral evidence and computational evaluation 类比推理和识别记忆中的关系和词汇相似性:行为证据和计算评价
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101550
Nicholas Ichien , Katherine L. Alfred , Sophia Baia , David J.M. Kraemer , Keith J. Holyoak , Silvia A. Bunge , Hongjing Lu

We examined the role of different types of similarity in both analogical reasoning and recognition memory. On recognition tasks, people more often falsely report having seen a recombined word pair (e.g., flower: garden) if it instantiates the same semantic relation (e.g., is a part of) as a studied word pair (e.g., house: town). This phenomenon, termed relational luring, has been interpreted as evidence that explicit relation representations—known to play a central role in analogical reasoning—also impact episodic memory. We replicate and extend previous studies, showing that relation-based false alarms in recognition memory occur after participants encode word pairs either by making relatedness judgments about individual words presented sequentially, or by evaluating analogies between pairs of word pairs. To test alternative explanations of relational luring, we implemented an established model of recognition memory, the Generalized Context Model (GCM). Within this basic framework, we compared representations of word pairs based on similarities derived either from explicit relations or from lexical semantics (i.e., individual word meanings). In two experiments on recognition memory, best-fitting values of GCM parameters enabled both similarity models (even the model based solely on lexical semantics) to predict relational luring with comparable accuracy. However, the model based on explicit relations proved more robust to parameter variations than that based on lexical similarity. We found this same pattern of modeling results when applying GCM to an independent set of data reported by Popov, Hristova, and Anders (2017). In accord with previous work, we also found that explicit relation representations are necessary for modeling analogical reasoning. Our findings support the possibility that explicit relations, which are central to analogical reasoning, also play an important role in episodic memory.

我们研究了不同类型的相似性在类比推理和识别记忆中的作用。在识别任务中,如果一个重新组合的词对(例如,花:花园)与一个被研究词对(如,房子:城镇)实例化了相同的语义关系(例如,是的一部分),人们更经常谎报看到了这个词对。这种现象被称为关系引诱,被解释为显式关系表征——已知在类比推理中起着核心作用——也会影响情景记忆的证据。我们复制和扩展了以前的研究,表明在参与者通过对顺序呈现的单个单词进行相关判断或通过评估成对单词之间的类比来对单词对进行编码后,识别记忆中基于关系的假警报就会发生。为了测试关系引诱的替代解释,我们实现了一个已建立的识别记忆模型,即广义上下文模型(GCM)。在这个基本框架内,我们比较了基于相似性的词对表示,这些相似性要么来自显式关系,要么来自词汇语义(即单个词义)。在两个关于识别记忆的实验中,GCM参数的最佳拟合值使两个相似性模型(甚至是仅基于词汇语义的模型)都能够以相当的精度预测关系引诱。然而,基于显式关系的模型比基于词汇相似性的模型对参数变化更具鲁棒性。当将GCM应用于Popov、Hristova和Anders(2017)报告的一组独立数据时,我们发现了同样的建模结果模式。与之前的工作一致,我们还发现显式关系表示对于模拟推理的建模是必要的。我们的研究结果支持这样一种可能性,即作为类比推理核心的外显关系在情景记忆中也发挥着重要作用。
{"title":"Relational and lexical similarity in analogical reasoning and recognition memory: Behavioral evidence and computational evaluation","authors":"Nicholas Ichien ,&nbsp;Katherine L. Alfred ,&nbsp;Sophia Baia ,&nbsp;David J.M. Kraemer ,&nbsp;Keith J. Holyoak ,&nbsp;Silvia A. Bunge ,&nbsp;Hongjing Lu","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101550","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101550","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We examined the role of different types of similarity in both analogical reasoning and recognition memory. On recognition tasks, people more often falsely report having seen a recombined word pair (e.g., <em>flower: garden</em>) if it instantiates the same semantic relation (e.g., <em>is a part of</em>) as a studied word pair (e.g., <em>house: town</em>). This phenomenon, termed <em>relational luring</em>, has been interpreted as evidence that explicit relation representations—known to play a central role in analogical reasoning—also impact episodic memory. We replicate and extend previous studies, showing that relation-based false alarms in recognition memory occur after participants encode word pairs either by making relatedness judgments about individual words presented sequentially, or by evaluating analogies between pairs of word pairs. To test alternative explanations of relational luring, we implemented an established model of recognition memory, the Generalized Context Model (GCM). Within this basic framework, we compared representations of word pairs based on similarities derived either from explicit relations or from lexical semantics (i.e., individual word meanings). In two experiments on recognition memory, best-fitting values of GCM parameters enabled <em>both</em> similarity models (even the model based solely on lexical semantics) to predict relational luring with comparable accuracy. However, the model based on explicit relations proved more robust to parameter variations than that based on lexical similarity. We found this same pattern of modeling results when applying GCM to an independent set of data reported by <span>Popov, Hristova, and Anders (2017)</span>. In accord with previous work, we also found that explicit relation representations are necessary for modeling analogical reasoning. Our findings support the possibility that explicit relations, which are central to analogical reasoning, also play an important role in episodic memory.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"141 ","pages":"Article 101550"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9484031","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The spatiotemporal gradient of intrusion errors in continuous outcome source memory: Source retrieval is affected by both guessing and intrusions 连续结果源记忆中入侵错误的时空梯度:猜测和入侵对源检索均有影响
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101552
Jason Zhou, Adam F. Osth, Philip L. Smith

Previous research has characterized source retrieval as a thresholded process, which fails on a proportion of trials and leads to guessing, as opposed to a continuous process, in which response precision varies across trials but is never zero. The thresholded view of source retrieval is largely based on the observation of heavy tailed distributions of response errors, thought to reflect a large proportion of “memoryless” trials. In this study, we investigate whether these errors might instead reflect systematic intrusions from other list items which can mimic source guessing. Using the circular diffusion model of decision making, which accounts for both response errors and RTs we found that intrusions account for some, but not all, errors in a continuous-report source memory task. We found that intrusion errors were more likely to come from items studied in nearby locations and times, and were well-described by a spatiotemporal gradient model, but not from semantically or perceptually similar cues. Our findings support a thresholded view of source retrieval but suggest that previous work has overestimated the proportion of guesses which have been conflated with intrusions.

先前的研究将来源检索描述为一个有阈值的过程,它在一定比例的试验中失败,并导致猜测,而不是一个连续的过程,在这个过程中,不同试验的响应精度不同,但永远不会为零。源检索的阈值观点主要基于对响应误差的重尾分布的观察,被认为反映了很大一部分“无记忆”试验。在这项研究中,我们调查了这些错误是否反映了来自其他列表项的系统入侵,这些列表项可以模拟来源猜测。使用决策的循环扩散模型,该模型同时考虑了响应错误和RT,我们发现入侵导致了连续报告源内存任务中的一些错误,但不是全部错误。我们发现,入侵错误更有可能来自于在附近位置和时间研究的项目,并且通过时空梯度模型得到了很好的描述,但不是来自语义或感知相似的线索。我们的发现支持了对源检索的阈值化观点,但表明之前的工作高估了与入侵混为一谈的猜测比例。
{"title":"The spatiotemporal gradient of intrusion errors in continuous outcome source memory: Source retrieval is affected by both guessing and intrusions","authors":"Jason Zhou,&nbsp;Adam F. Osth,&nbsp;Philip L. Smith","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101552","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101552","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous research has characterized source retrieval as a thresholded process, which fails on a proportion of trials and leads to guessing, as opposed to a continuous process, in which response precision varies across trials but is never zero. The thresholded view of source retrieval is largely based on the observation of heavy tailed distributions of response errors, thought to reflect a large proportion of “memoryless” trials. In this study, we investigate whether these errors might instead reflect systematic intrusions from other list items which can mimic source guessing. Using the circular diffusion model of decision making, which accounts for both response errors and RTs we found that intrusions account for some, but not all, errors in a continuous-report source memory task. We found that intrusion errors were more likely to come from items studied in nearby locations and times, and were well-described by a spatiotemporal gradient model, but not from semantically or perceptually similar cues. Our findings support a thresholded view of source retrieval but suggest that previous work has overestimated the proportion of guesses which have been conflated with intrusions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"141 ","pages":"Article 101552"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9487641","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Probability and intentional action 概率和有意行为
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101551
Spencer R. Ericson , Stephanie Denison , John Turri , Ori Friedman

How does probability affect attributions of intentionality? In five experiments (total N = 1410), we provide evidence for a probability raising account holding that people are more likely to see the outcome of an agent’s action as intentional if the agent does something to increase the odds of that outcome. Experiment 1 found that high probability without probability raising does not suffice for strong attributions of intentionality. Participants were more likely to conclude a girl intentionally obtained a desired gumball from a single gumball machine when it offered favorable odds for getting that kind of gumball compared with when it offered poor odds, but their attributions of intentionality were lukewarm. Experiments 2 and 3 then found stronger attributions of intentionality when the girl raised her probability of success by choosing to use machines offering favorable odds over machines offering poor odds. Finally, Experiments 4 and 5 examined whether these effects of probability raising might reduce to consideration of agents’ beliefs and expectations. We found that although these mental states do matter, probability raising matters too—people attribute intentional actions to agents who increase their odds of success, rather than to agents who merely become convinced that success is likely. We discuss the implications of these findings for claims that control and skill contribute to attributions of intentional action.

概率如何影响意向性的归因?在五个实验中(总共N=1410),我们为一种提高概率的说法提供了证据,即如果代理人做了一些事情来增加结果的几率,人们更有可能认为代理人的行为是故意的。实验1发现,没有概率提升的高概率不足以实现意向性的强归因。参与者更有可能得出结论,女孩是故意从一台口香糖机上获得想要的口香糖的,当它提供了获得这种口香糖的有利几率时,而当它提供的几率很低时,但他们对意向的归因并不热烈。实验2和3发现,当女孩选择使用赔率高的机器而不是赔率低的机器来提高成功概率时,意向性的归因更强。最后,实验4和5检验了概率提升的这些影响是否会减少到考虑主体的信念和期望。我们发现,尽管这些心理状态确实很重要,但提高概率也很重要——人们将有意的行为归因于增加成功几率的代理人,而不是仅仅相信有可能成功的代理人。我们讨论了这些发现对控制和技能有助于故意行为归因的说法的影响。
{"title":"Probability and intentional action","authors":"Spencer R. Ericson ,&nbsp;Stephanie Denison ,&nbsp;John Turri ,&nbsp;Ori Friedman","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101551","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101551","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>How does probability affect attributions of intentionality? In five experiments (total N = 1410), we provide evidence for a probability raising account holding that people are more likely to see the outcome of an agent’s action as intentional if the agent does something to increase the odds of that outcome. Experiment 1 found that high probability without probability raising does not suffice for strong attributions of intentionality. Participants were more likely to conclude a girl intentionally obtained a desired gumball from a single gumball machine when it offered favorable odds for getting that kind of gumball compared with when it offered poor odds, but their attributions of intentionality were lukewarm. Experiments 2 and 3 then found stronger attributions of intentionality when the girl raised her probability of success by choosing to use machines offering favorable odds over machines offering poor odds. Finally, Experiments 4 and 5 examined whether these effects of probability raising might reduce to consideration of agents’ beliefs and expectations. We found that although these mental states do matter, probability raising matters too—people attribute intentional actions to agents who increase their odds of success, rather than to agents who merely become convinced that success is likely. We discuss the implications of these findings for claims that control and skill contribute to attributions of intentional action.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"141 ","pages":"Article 101551"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9536974","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Contextuality and context-sensitivity in probabilistic models of cognition 认知概率模型中的情境性和情境敏感性
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-02-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101529
P.D. Bruza, L. Fell, P. Hoyte, S. Dehdashti, A. Obeid, A. Gibson, C. Moreira

The context-sensitivity of cognition has been demonstrated across a wide range of cognitive functions such as perception, memory, judgement and decision making. A related term, ‘contextuality’, has appeared from the field of quantum cognition, with mounting empirical evidence demonstrating that cognitive phenomena are sometimes contextual. Contextuality is a subtle notion that influences how we must view the properties of the cognitive phenomenon being studied. This article addresses the questions: What does it mean for a cognitive phenomenon to be contextual? What are the implications of contextuality for probabilistic models of cognition? How does contextuality differ from context-sensitivity? Starting from George Boole’s “conditions of possible experience”, we argue that a probabilistic model of a cognitive phenomenon is necessarily subject to an assumption of realism. By this we mean that the phenomenon being studied is assumed to have cognitive properties with a definite value independent of observation. In contrast, quantum cognition holds that a cognitive property maybe indeterminate, i.e., its properties do not have well established values prior to observation. We argue that indeterminacy is sufficient for incompatibility between cognitive properties. In turn, incompatibility is necessary for their contextuality. The significance of this argument for cognitive psychology is the following:if a cognitive phenomenon is found to be contextual, then there is reason to believe it may be indeterminate. We illustrate by means of two crowdsourced experiments how context-sensitivity and contextuality of cognitive properties in the form of facial trait judgements can be characterized from empirical data. Finally, we conceptually and formally contrast contextuality with context-sensitivity. We propose that both involve a form of context dependence, with causality being the differentiating factor: the context dependence in context-sensitivity has a causal basis, whereas the context dependence in contextuality is acausal. The resulting implications for probabilistic models of cognition are discussed.

认知的上下文敏感性已经在感知、记忆、判断和决策等广泛的认知功能中得到了证明。量子认知领域出现了一个相关的术语“情境性”,越来越多的经验证据表明,认知现象有时是情境性的。语境是一个微妙的概念,它影响我们如何看待所研究的认知现象的性质。这篇文章解决了以下问题:认知现象的语境意味着什么?情境性对认知的概率模型有什么影响?情境性与情境敏感性有何不同?从乔治·布尔的“可能经验的条件”出发,我们认为认知现象的概率模型必然服从现实主义的假设。我们的意思是,被研究的现象被认为具有认知特性,具有独立于观察的确定值。相反,量子认知认为认知性质可能是不确定的,即其性质在观察之前没有很好的确定值。我们认为,不确定性足以导致认知特性之间的不相容性。反过来,不兼容性对于它们的上下文来说是必要的。这一论点对认知心理学的意义如下:如果一个认知现象被发现是上下文的,那么有理由相信它可能是不确定的。我们通过两个众包实验说明了如何从经验数据中表征面部特征判断形式的认知属性的上下文敏感性和上下文性。最后,我们从概念上和形式上对比语境性和语境敏感性。我们认为两者都涉及一种形式的语境依赖,因果关系是区分因素:语境敏感性中的语境依赖有因果基础,而语境性中的语境依存是非因果的。讨论了由此产生的认知概率模型的含义。
{"title":"Contextuality and context-sensitivity in probabilistic models of cognition","authors":"P.D. Bruza,&nbsp;L. Fell,&nbsp;P. Hoyte,&nbsp;S. Dehdashti,&nbsp;A. Obeid,&nbsp;A. Gibson,&nbsp;C. Moreira","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101529","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2022.101529","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The context-sensitivity of cognition has been demonstrated across a wide range of cognitive functions such as perception, memory, judgement and decision making. A related term, ‘contextuality’, has appeared from the field of quantum cognition, with mounting empirical evidence demonstrating that cognitive phenomena are sometimes contextual. Contextuality is a subtle notion that influences how we must view the properties of the cognitive phenomenon being studied. This article addresses the questions: What does it mean for a cognitive phenomenon to be contextual? What are the implications of contextuality for probabilistic models of cognition? How does contextuality differ from context-sensitivity? Starting from George Boole’s “conditions of possible experience”, we argue that a probabilistic model of a cognitive phenomenon is necessarily subject to an assumption of realism. By this we mean that the phenomenon being studied is assumed to have cognitive properties with a definite value independent of observation. In contrast, quantum cognition holds that a cognitive property maybe indeterminate, i.e., its properties do not have well established values prior to observation. We argue that indeterminacy is sufficient for incompatibility between cognitive properties. In turn, incompatibility is necessary for their contextuality. The significance of this argument for cognitive psychology is the following:if a cognitive phenomenon is found to be contextual, then there is reason to believe it may be indeterminate. We illustrate by means of two crowdsourced experiments how context-sensitivity and contextuality of cognitive properties in the form of facial trait judgements can be characterized from empirical data. Finally, we conceptually and formally contrast contextuality with context-sensitivity. We propose that both involve a form of context dependence, with causality being the differentiating factor: the context dependence in context-sensitivity has a causal basis, whereas the context dependence in contextuality is acausal. The resulting implications for probabilistic models of cognition are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"140 ","pages":"Article 101529"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9482222","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
期刊
Cognitive Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1