首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Learning to generalise but not segment an artificial language at 17 months predicts children’s language skills 3 years later 在17个月大的时候学会概括而不是分割一门人工语言,可以预测3年后孩子的语言技能。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-10-07 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101607
Padraic Monaghan , Seamus Donnelly , Katie Alcock , Amy Bidgood , Kate Cain , Samantha Durrant , Rebecca L.A. Frost , Lana S. Jago , Michelle S. Peter , Julian M. Pine , Heather Turnbull , Caroline F. Rowland

We investigated whether learning an artificial language at 17 months was predictive of children’s natural language vocabulary and grammar skills at 54 months. Children at 17 months listened to an artificial language containing non-adjacent dependencies, and were then tested on their learning to segment and to generalise the structure of the language. At 54 months, children were then tested on a range of standardised natural language tasks that assessed receptive and expressive vocabulary and grammar. A structural equation model demonstrated that learning the artificial language generalisation at 17 months predicted language abilities – a composite of vocabulary and grammar skills – at 54 months, whereas artificial language segmentation at 17 months did not predict language abilities at this age. Artificial language learning tasks – especially those that probe grammar learning – provide a valuable tool for uncovering the mechanisms driving children’s early language development.

我们调查了17个月时学习人工语言是否能预测54个月时儿童的自然语言词汇和语法技能。17个月大的孩子听一种包含非相邻依赖性的人工语言,然后测试他们对语言结构的分段和概括能力。在54个月大的时候,孩子们接受了一系列标准化的自然语言任务的测试,这些任务评估了接受和表达的词汇和语法。一个结构方程模型表明,在17个月时学习人工语言概括可以预测54个月时的语言能力,这是词汇和语法技能的组合,而在17个月中的人工语言分割不能预测这个年龄段的语言能力。人工语言学习任务——尤其是探究语法学习的任务——为揭示儿童早期语言发展的机制提供了一个有价值的工具。
{"title":"Learning to generalise but not segment an artificial language at 17 months predicts children’s language skills 3 years later","authors":"Padraic Monaghan ,&nbsp;Seamus Donnelly ,&nbsp;Katie Alcock ,&nbsp;Amy Bidgood ,&nbsp;Kate Cain ,&nbsp;Samantha Durrant ,&nbsp;Rebecca L.A. Frost ,&nbsp;Lana S. Jago ,&nbsp;Michelle S. Peter ,&nbsp;Julian M. Pine ,&nbsp;Heather Turnbull ,&nbsp;Caroline F. Rowland","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101607","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101607","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We investigated whether learning an artificial language at 17 months was predictive of children’s natural language vocabulary and grammar skills at 54 months. Children at 17 months listened to an artificial language containing non-adjacent dependencies, and were then tested on their learning to segment and to generalise the structure of the language. At 54 months, children were then tested on a range of standardised natural language tasks that assessed receptive and expressive vocabulary and grammar. A structural equation model demonstrated that learning the artificial language generalisation at 17 months predicted language abilities – a composite of vocabulary and grammar skills – at 54 months, whereas artificial language segmentation at 17 months did not predict language abilities at this age. Artificial language learning tasks – especially those that probe grammar learning – provide a valuable tool for uncovering the mechanisms driving children’s early language development.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"147 ","pages":"Article 101607"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-10-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41158737","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Syntactic theory of mathematical expressions 数学表达式的句法理论。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-09-23 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101606
Daiki Matsumoto , Tomoya Nakai

Mathematical expressions consist of recursive combinations of numbers, variables, and operators. According to theoretical linguists, the syntactic mechanisms of natural language also provide a basis for mathematics. To date, however, no theoretically rigorous investigation has been conducted to support such arguments. Therefore, this study uses a methodology based on theoretical linguistics to analyze the syntactic properties of mathematical expressions. Through a review of recent behavioral and neuroimaging studies on mathematical syntax, we report several inconsistencies with theoretical linguistics, such as the use of ternary structures. To address these, we propose that a syntactic category called Applicative plays a central role in analyzing mathematical expressions with seemingly ternary structures by combining binary structures. Besides basic arithmetic expressions, we also examine algebraic equations and complex expressions such as integral and differential calculi. This study is the first attempt at building a comprehensive framework for analyzing the syntactic structures of mathematical expressions.

数学表达式由数字、变量和运算符的递归组合组成。理论语言学家认为,自然语言的句法机制也为数学提供了基础。然而,迄今为止,还没有进行理论上严格的调查来支持这种论点。因此,本研究采用基于理论语言学的方法来分析数学表达式的句法特性。通过回顾最近关于数学语法的行为和神经成像研究,我们报告了一些与理论语言学不一致的地方,例如三元结构的使用。为了解决这些问题,我们提出了一个名为“应用性”的句法类别,通过组合二元结构,在分析具有看似三元结构的数学表达式中发挥着核心作用。除了基本的算术表达式,我们还研究代数方程和复杂的表达式,如积分和微分演算。本研究首次尝试建立一个分析数学表达式句法结构的综合框架。
{"title":"Syntactic theory of mathematical expressions","authors":"Daiki Matsumoto ,&nbsp;Tomoya Nakai","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101606","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101606","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Mathematical expressions consist of recursive combinations of numbers, variables, and operators. According to theoretical linguists, the syntactic mechanisms of natural language also provide a basis for mathematics. To date, however, no theoretically rigorous investigation has been conducted to support such arguments. Therefore, this study uses a methodology based on theoretical linguistics to analyze the syntactic properties of mathematical expressions. Through a review of recent behavioral and neuroimaging studies on mathematical syntax, we report several inconsistencies with theoretical linguistics, such as the use of ternary structures. To address these, we propose that a syntactic category called Applicative plays a central role in analyzing mathematical expressions with seemingly ternary structures by combining binary structures. Besides basic arithmetic expressions, we also examine algebraic equations and complex expressions such as integral and differential calculi. This study is the first attempt at building a comprehensive framework for analyzing the syntactic structures of mathematical expressions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"146 ","pages":"Article 101606"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41136052","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How trial-to-trial learning shapes mappings in the mental lexicon: Modelling lexical decision with linear discriminative learning 试-试学习如何塑造心理词汇中的映射:用线性判别学习建模词汇决策。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-09-14 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101598
Maria Heitmeier , Yu-Ying Chuang , R. Harald Baayen

Trial-to-trial effects have been found in a number of studies, indicating that processing a stimulus influences responses in subsequent trials. A special case are priming effects which have been modelled successfully with error-driven learning (Marsolek, 2008), implying that participants are continuously learning during experiments. This study investigates whether trial-to-trial learning can be detected in an unprimed lexical decision experiment. We used the Discriminative Lexicon Model (DLM; Baayen et al., 2019), a model of the mental lexicon with meaning representations from distributional semantics, which models error-driven incremental learning with the Widrow-Hoff rule. We used data from the British Lexicon Project (BLP; Keuleers et al., 2012) and simulated the lexical decision experiment with the DLM on a trial-by-trial basis for each subject individually. Then, reaction times were predicted with Generalized Additive Models (GAMs), using measures derived from the DLM simulations as predictors. We extracted measures from two simulations per subject (one with learning updates between trials and one without), and used them as input to two GAMs. Learning-based models showed better model fit than the non-learning ones for the majority of subjects. Our measures also provide insights into lexical processing and individual differences. This demonstrates the potential of the DLM to model behavioural data and leads to the conclusion that trial-to-trial learning can indeed be detected in unprimed lexical decision. Our results support the possibility that our lexical knowledge is subject to continuous changes.

在许多研究中发现了试验对试验的影响,表明处理刺激会影响后续试验的反应。一个特殊的情况是启动效应,该效应已通过错误驱动学习成功建模(Marsolek,2008),这意味着参与者在实验中不断学习。这项研究调查了在一个没有时间的词汇决策实验中是否可以检测到试对试学习。我们使用了判别词典模型(DLM;Baayen等人,2019),这是一个具有分布语义意义表示的心理词典模型,它使用Widrow-Hoff规则对错误驱动的增量学习进行建模。我们使用了英国词汇项目(BLP;Keuleers等人,2012)的数据,并在逐个试验的基础上,对每个受试者分别模拟了DLM的词汇决策实验。然后,使用广义加性模型(GAM)预测反应时间,使用DLM模拟得出的测量值作为预测因子。我们从每个受试者的两个模拟中提取了测量值(一个在试验之间有学习更新,另一个没有),并将其用作两个GAM的输入。对于大多数科目,基于学习的模型比非学习的模型显示出更好的模型拟合性。我们的测量还提供了对词汇处理和个体差异的见解。这证明了DLM对行为数据建模的潜力,并得出了这样的结论,即在未定时的词汇决策中确实可以检测到试对试学习。我们的研究结果支持了我们的词汇知识不断变化的可能性。
{"title":"How trial-to-trial learning shapes mappings in the mental lexicon: Modelling lexical decision with linear discriminative learning","authors":"Maria Heitmeier ,&nbsp;Yu-Ying Chuang ,&nbsp;R. Harald Baayen","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101598","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101598","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Trial-to-trial effects have been found in a number of studies, indicating that processing a stimulus influences responses in subsequent trials. A special case are priming effects which have been modelled successfully with error-driven learning (Marsolek, 2008), implying that participants are continuously learning during experiments. This study investigates whether trial-to-trial learning can be detected in an unprimed lexical decision experiment. We used the Discriminative Lexicon Model (DLM; Baayen et al., 2019), a model of the mental lexicon with meaning representations from distributional semantics, which models error-driven incremental learning with the Widrow-Hoff rule. We used data from the British Lexicon Project (BLP; Keuleers et al., 2012) and simulated the lexical decision experiment with the DLM on a trial-by-trial basis for each subject individually. Then, reaction times were predicted with Generalized Additive Models (GAMs), using measures derived from the DLM simulations as predictors. We extracted measures from two simulations per subject (one with learning updates between trials and one without), and used them as input to two GAMs. Learning-based models showed better model fit than the non-learning ones for the majority of subjects. Our measures also provide insights into lexical processing and individual differences. This demonstrates the potential of the DLM to model behavioural data and leads to the conclusion that trial-to-trial learning can indeed be detected in unprimed lexical decision. Our results support the possibility that our lexical knowledge is subject to continuous changes.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"146 ","pages":"Article 101598"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10589761/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10273410","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Testing formal cognitive models of classification and old-new recognition in a real-world high-dimensional category domain 在现实世界的高维类别领域中测试分类和新旧识别的形式认知模型。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101596
Brian J. Meagher, Robert M. Nosofsky

Categorization and old-new recognition memory are closely linked topics in the cognitive-psychology literature and there have been extensive past efforts at developing unified formal modeling accounts of these fundamental psychological processes. However, the existing formal-modeling literature has almost exclusively used small sets of simplified stimuli and artificial category structures. The present work extends this literature by collecting both categorization and old-new recognition judgments on a large set of high-dimensional stimuli that form real-world category structures: namely, a set of 540 images of rocks belonging to the geologically-defined categories igneous, metamorphic and sedimentary. Participants first engaged in a learning phase in which they classified large sets of training instances into these real-world categories. This was followed by a test phase in which they classified both training and novel transfer items into the learned categories and also judged whether each item was old or new. We attempted to model both the classification and recognition test data at the level of individual items. Ultimately, the categorization data were well fit by both an exemplar and clustering model, but not by a prototype model. Only the exemplar model was able to provide a reasonable first-order account of the old-new recognition data; however, the standard version of the model failed to capture the variability in hit rates within the class of old-training items themselves. An extended hybrid-similarity version of the exemplar model that made allowance for boosts in self-similarity due to matching distinctive features yielded much improved accounts of the old-new recognition data. The study is among the first to test cognitive-process models on their ability to account quantitatively for old-new recognition of real-world, high-dimensional stimuli at the level of individual items.

分类和新旧识别记忆是认知心理学文献中密切相关的主题,过去曾在开发这些基本心理过程的统一形式建模方面做出过大量努力。然而,现有的形式建模文献几乎只使用了少量的简化刺激和人工类别结构。本工作通过收集一大组高维刺激的分类和新旧识别判断来扩展这一文献,这些刺激形成了现实世界的类别结构:即一组540张岩石图像,属于地质定义的火成岩、变质岩和沉积岩类别。参与者首先进入了一个学习阶段,在这个阶段,他们将大量的训练实例分类到这些现实世界的类别中。接下来是测试阶段,他们将训练和新的迁移项目分为学习类别,并判断每个项目是旧的还是新的。我们试图在单个项目的水平上对分类和识别测试数据进行建模。最终,样本模型和聚类模型都很好地拟合了分类数据,但原型模型没有。只有示例模型能够提供新旧识别数据的合理一阶说明;然而,该模型的标准版本未能捕捉到旧训练项目类别中命中率的可变性。样本模型的扩展混合相似性版本考虑到了由于匹配不同特征而提高的自相似性,从而大大改进了新旧识别数据的描述。这项研究是首批测试认知过程模型在单个项目水平上定量解释对现实世界高维刺激的新旧识别能力的研究之一。
{"title":"Testing formal cognitive models of classification and old-new recognition in a real-world high-dimensional category domain","authors":"Brian J. Meagher,&nbsp;Robert M. Nosofsky","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101596","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101596","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Categorization and old-new recognition memory are closely linked topics in the cognitive-psychology literature and there have been extensive past efforts at developing unified formal modeling accounts of these fundamental psychological processes. However, the existing formal-modeling literature has almost exclusively used small sets of simplified stimuli and artificial category structures. The present work extends this literature by collecting both categorization and old-new recognition judgments on a large set of high-dimensional stimuli that form real-world category structures: namely, a set of 540 images of rocks belonging to the geologically-defined categories <em>igneous</em>, <em>metamorphic</em> and <em>sedimentary</em>. Participants first engaged in a learning phase in which they classified large sets of training instances into these real-world categories. This was followed by a test phase in which they classified both training and novel transfer items into the learned categories and also judged whether each item was old or new. We attempted to model both the classification and recognition test data at the level of individual items. Ultimately, the categorization data were well fit by both an exemplar and clustering model, but not by a prototype model. Only the exemplar model was able to provide a reasonable first-order account of the old-new recognition data; however, the standard version of the model failed to capture the variability in hit rates within the class of old-training items themselves. An extended hybrid-similarity version of the exemplar model that made allowance for boosts in self-similarity due to matching distinctive features yielded much improved accounts of the old-new recognition data. The study is among the first to test cognitive-process models on their ability to account quantitatively for old-new recognition of real-world, high-dimensional stimuli at the level of individual items.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"145 ","pages":"Article 101596"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10265216","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A spatially continuous diffusion model of visual working memory 视觉工作记忆的空间连续扩散模型。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101595
Alex Fennell, Roger Ratcliff

We present results from five visual working memory (VWM) experiments in which participants were briefly shown between 2 and 6 colored squares. They were then cued to recall the color of one of the squares and they responded by choosing the color on a continuous color wheel. The experiments provided response proportions and response time (RT) measures as a function of angle for the choices. Current VWM models for this task include discrete models that assume an item is either within working memory or not and resource models that assume that memory strength varies as a function of the number of items. Because these models do not include processes that allow them to account for RT data, we implemented them within the spatially continuous diffusion model (SCDM, Ratcliff, 2018) and use the experimental data to evaluate these combined models. In the SCDM, evidence retrieved from memory is represented as a spatially continuous normal distribution and this drives the decision process until a criterion (represented as a 1-D line) is reached, which produces a decision. Noise in the accumulation process is represented by continuous Gaussian process noise over spatial position. The models that fit best from the discrete and resource-based classes converged on a common model that had a guessing component and that allowed the height of the normal memory-strength distribution to vary with number of items. The guessing component was implemented as a regular decision process driven by a flat evidence distribution, a zero-drift process. The combination of choice and RT data allows models that were not identifiable based on choice data alone to be discriminated.

我们展示了五个视觉工作记忆(VWM)实验的结果,在这些实验中,参与者被简单地展示在2到6个彩色方块之间。然后,他们被提示回忆其中一个方块的颜色,并通过选择连续色轮上的颜色来做出反应。实验提供了作为选择角度函数的响应比例和响应时间(RT)测量。当前用于该任务的VWM模型包括假设项目是否在工作记忆中的离散模型,以及假设记忆强度随项目数量变化的资源模型。由于这些模型不包括允许它们考虑RT数据的过程,我们在空间连续扩散模型(SCDM,Ratcliff,2018)中实现了它们,并使用实验数据来评估这些组合模型。在SCDM中,从存储器中检索到的证据被表示为空间连续的正态分布,这驱动决策过程,直到达到标准(表示为1-D线),从而产生决策。累积过程中的噪声由空间位置上的连续高斯过程噪声表示。最适合离散类和基于资源的类的模型集中在一个通用模型上,该模型具有猜测组件,并允许正态记忆强度分布的高度随项目数量而变化。猜测组件被实现为一个由平坦证据分布驱动的常规决策过程,一个零漂移过程。选择和RT数据的组合允许对仅基于选择数据无法识别的模型进行区分。
{"title":"A spatially continuous diffusion model of visual working memory","authors":"Alex Fennell,&nbsp;Roger Ratcliff","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101595","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101595","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We present results from five visual working memory (VWM) experiments in which participants were briefly shown between 2 and 6 colored squares. They were then cued to recall the color of one of the squares and they responded by choosing the color on a continuous color wheel. The experiments provided response proportions and response time (RT) measures as a function of angle for the choices. Current VWM models for this task include discrete models that assume an item is either within working memory or not and resource models that assume that memory strength varies as a function of the number of items. Because these models do not include processes that allow them to account for RT data, we implemented them within the spatially continuous diffusion model (SCDM, Ratcliff, 2018) and use the experimental data to evaluate these combined models. In the SCDM, evidence retrieved from memory is represented as a spatially continuous normal distribution and this drives the decision process until a criterion (represented as a 1-D line) is reached, which produces a decision. Noise in the accumulation process is represented by continuous Gaussian process noise over spatial position. The models that fit best from the discrete and resource-based classes converged on a common model that had a guessing component and that allowed the height of the normal memory-strength distribution to vary with number of items. The guessing component was implemented as a regular decision process driven by a flat evidence distribution, a zero-drift process. The combination of choice and RT data allows models that were not identifiable based on choice data alone to be discriminated.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"145 ","pages":"Article 101595"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10546276/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10201267","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
It's not just what we don't know: The mapping problem in the acquisition of negation 这不仅仅是我们不知道的:否定习得中的映射问题。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101592
Victor Gomes , Rebecca Doherty , Daniel Smits , Susan Goldin-Meadow , John C. Trueswell , Roman Feiman

How do learners learn what no and not mean when they are only presented with what is? Given its complexity, abstractness, and roles in logic, truth-functional negation might be a conceptual accomplishment. As a result, young children’s gradual acquisition of negation words might be due to their undergoing a gradual conceptual change that is necessary to represent those words’ logical meaning. However, it’s also possible that linguistic expressions of negation take time to learn because of children’s gradually increasing grasp of their language. To understand what no and not mean, children might first need to understand the rest of the sentences in which those words are used. We provide experimental evidence that conceptually equipped learners (adults) face the same acquisition challenges that children do when their access to linguistic information is restricted, which simulates how much language children understand at different points in acquisition. When watching a silenced video of naturalistic uses of negators by parents speaking to their children, adults could tell when the parent was prohibiting the child and struggled with inferring that negators were used to express logical negation. However, when provided with additional information about what else the parent said, guessing that the parent had expressed logical negation became easy for adults. Though our findings do not rule out that young learners also undergo conceptual change, they show that increasing understanding of language alone, with no accompanying conceptual change, can account for the gradual acquisition of negation words.

当学习者只被告知“是”时,他们如何学习“否”和“否”的含义?考虑到其复杂性、抽象性和在逻辑中的作用,真值功能否定可能是一种概念上的成就。因此,幼儿对否定词的逐渐习得可能是由于他们经历了一个渐进的概念变化,这是表达这些词的逻辑意义所必需的。然而,由于孩子们对语言的掌握程度逐渐提高,否定的语言表达也可能需要时间来学习。为了理解“不”和“不”的意思,孩子们可能首先需要理解这些单词所用的其余句子。我们提供的实验证据表明,当儿童获得语言信息受到限制时,具备概念的学习者(成年人)面临着与儿童相同的习得挑战,这模拟了儿童在不同的习得点上理解了多少语言。当观看父母对孩子说话时自然使用否定词的无声视频时,成年人可以判断出父母何时禁止孩子,并努力推断否定词是用来表达逻辑否定的。然而,当被提供关于父母还说了什么的额外信息时,成年人很容易猜测父母表达了合乎逻辑的否定。尽管我们的研究结果并不排除年轻学习者也会经历概念变化,但它们表明,只有对语言的理解不断增加,而没有伴随的概念变化,才能解释否定词的逐渐习得。
{"title":"It's not just what we don't know: The mapping problem in the acquisition of negation","authors":"Victor Gomes ,&nbsp;Rebecca Doherty ,&nbsp;Daniel Smits ,&nbsp;Susan Goldin-Meadow ,&nbsp;John C. Trueswell ,&nbsp;Roman Feiman","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101592","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101592","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>How do learners learn what <em>no</em> and <em>not</em> mean when they are only presented with what is? Given its complexity, abstractness, and roles in logic, truth-functional negation might be a conceptual accomplishment. As a result, young children’s gradual acquisition of negation words might be due to their undergoing a gradual conceptual change that is necessary to represent those words’ logical meaning. However, it’s also possible that linguistic expressions of negation take time to learn because of children’s gradually increasing grasp of their language. To understand what <em>no</em> and <em>not</em> mean, children might first need to understand the rest of the sentences in which those words are used. We provide experimental evidence that conceptually equipped learners (adults) face the same acquisition challenges that children do when their access to linguistic information is restricted, which simulates how much language children understand at different points in acquisition. When watching a silenced video of naturalistic uses of negators by parents speaking to their children, adults could tell when the parent was prohibiting the child and struggled with inferring that negators were used to express logical negation. However, when provided with additional information about what else the parent said, guessing that the parent had expressed logical negation became easy for adults. Though our findings do not rule out that young learners also undergo conceptual change, they show that increasing understanding of language alone, with no accompanying conceptual change, can account for the gradual acquisition of negation words.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"145 ","pages":"Article 101592"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10576150","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Norm conflicts and epistemic modals 规范冲突与认识模态。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101591
Niels Skovgaard-Olsen , John Cantwell

Statements containing epistemic modals (e.g., “by spring 2023 most European countries may have the Covid-19 pandemic under control”) are common expressions of epistemic uncertainty. In this paper, previous published findings (Knobe & Yalcin, 2014; Khoo & Phillips, 2018) on the opposition between Contextualism and Relativism for epistemic modals are re-examined. It is found that these findings contain a substantial degree of individual variation. To investigate whether participants differ in their interpretations of epistemic modals, an experiment with multiple phases and sessions is conducted to classify participants according to the three semantic theories of Relativism, Contextualism, and Objectivism. Through this study, some of the first empirical evidence for the kind of truth-value shifts postulated by semantic Relativism is presented. It is furthermore found that participants’ disagreement judgments match their truth evaluations and that participants are capable of distinguishing between truth and justification. In a second experimental session, it is investigated whether participants thus classified follow the norm of retraction which Relativism uses to account for argumentation with epistemic modals. Here the results are less favorable for Relativism. In a second experiment, these results are replicated and the normative beliefs of participants concerning the norm of retraction are investigated following work on measuring norms by Bicchieri (2017). Again, it is found that on average participants show no strong preferences concerning the norm of retraction for epistemic modals. Yet, it was found that participants who had committed to Objectivism and had training in logic applied the norm of retraction to might-statements. These results present a substantial challenge to the account of argumentation with epistemic modals presented in MacFarlane (2014), as discussed.

包含认知模态的声明(例如,“到2023年春季,大多数欧洲国家可能控制住新冠肺炎疫情”)是认知不确定性的常见表达。在这篇论文中,重新审视了先前发表的关于认知模态的语境主义和相对主义之间对立的研究结果(Knobe&Yalcin,2014;Khoo&Phillips,2018)。研究发现,这些发现包含了很大程度的个体差异。为了调查参与者对认识模态的解释是否不同,我们进行了一个多阶段和多阶段的实验,根据相对论、语境论和客观主义三种语义理论对参与者进行分类。通过这项研究,提供了语义相对主义所假设的那种真值转移的一些初步经验证据。研究还发现,参与者的分歧判断与他们对真理的评价相匹配,并且参与者能够区分真理和正当性。在第二个实验环节中,研究了这样分类的参与者是否遵循相对论用来解释认知模态论证的撤回规范。这里的结果不太利于相对主义。在第二个实验中,这些结果被复制,并且在Bicchieri(2017)测量规范的工作之后,对参与者关于撤回规范的规范信念进行了调查。再次,研究发现,平均而言,参与者对认知模态的回缩规范没有表现出强烈的偏好。然而,研究发现,那些致力于客观主义并接受过逻辑训练的参与者将撤回规范应用于可能的陈述。正如所讨论的,这些结果对MacFarlane(2014)中提出的使用认识模态的论证提出了实质性的挑战。
{"title":"Norm conflicts and epistemic modals","authors":"Niels Skovgaard-Olsen ,&nbsp;John Cantwell","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101591","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101591","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Statements containing epistemic modals (e.g., “by spring 2023 most European countries may have the Covid-19 pandemic under control”) are common expressions of epistemic uncertainty. In this paper, previous published findings (Knobe &amp; Yalcin, 2014; Khoo &amp; Phillips, 2018) on the opposition between Contextualism and Relativism for epistemic modals are re-examined. It is found that these findings contain a substantial degree of individual variation. To investigate whether participants differ in their interpretations of epistemic modals, an experiment with multiple phases and sessions is conducted to classify participants according to the three semantic theories of Relativism, Contextualism, and Objectivism. Through this study, some of the first empirical evidence for the kind of truth-value shifts postulated by semantic Relativism is presented. It is furthermore found that participants’ disagreement judgments match their truth evaluations and that participants are capable of distinguishing between truth and justification. In a second experimental session, it is investigated whether participants thus classified follow the norm of retraction which Relativism uses to account for argumentation with epistemic modals. Here the results are less favorable for Relativism. In a second experiment, these results are replicated and the normative beliefs of participants concerning the norm of retraction are investigated following work on measuring norms by Bicchieri (2017). Again, it is found that on average participants show no strong preferences concerning the norm of retraction for epistemic modals. Yet, it was found that participants who had committed to Objectivism and had training in logic applied the norm of retraction to might-statements. These results present a substantial challenge to the account of argumentation with epistemic modals presented in MacFarlane (2014), as discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"145 ","pages":"Article 101591"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10560201","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Psychological value theory: A computational cognitive model of charitable giving 心理价值理论:慈善捐赠的计算认知模型。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101593
Dale J. Cohen , Monica K. Campbell , Philip T. Quinlan

Charitable giving involves a complex economic and social decision because the giver expends resources for goods or services they will never receive. Although psychologists have identified numerous factors that influence charitable giving, there currently exists no unifying computational model of charitable choice. Here, we submit one such model, based within the strictures of Psychological Value Theory (PVT). In four experiments, we assess whether charitable giving is driven by the perceived Psychological Value of the recipient. Across all four experiments, we simultaneously predict response choice and response time with high accuracy. In a fifth experiment, we show that PVT predicts charitable giving more accurately than an account based on competence and warmth. PVT accurately predicts which charity a respondent will choose to donate to and separately, whether a respondent will choose to donate at all. PVT models the cognitive processes underlying charitable donations and it provides a computational framework for integrating known influences on charitable giving. For example, we show that in-group preference influences charitable giving by changing the Psychological Values of the options, rather than by bringing about a response bias toward the in-group.

慈善捐赠涉及一个复杂的经济和社会决策,因为捐赠者将资源用于他们永远不会得到的商品或服务。尽管心理学家已经确定了许多影响慈善捐赠的因素,但目前还没有统一的慈善选择计算模型。在这里,我们提出了一个这样的模型,基于心理价值理论(PVT)的限制。在四个实验中,我们评估慈善捐赠是否受到接受者感知的心理价值的驱动。在所有四个实验中,我们同时以高精度预测反应选择和反应时间。在第五个实验中,我们发现PVT比基于能力和热情的账户更准确地预测慈善捐赠。PVT准确地预测了受访者将选择向哪个慈善机构捐款,以及受访者是否会选择捐款。PVT对慈善捐赠背后的认知过程进行建模,并为整合已知的慈善捐赠影响提供了一个计算框架。例如,我们发现,群体内偏好通过改变选项的心理价值观来影响慈善捐赠,而不是通过对群体内偏好产生反应偏见。
{"title":"Psychological value theory: A computational cognitive model of charitable giving","authors":"Dale J. Cohen ,&nbsp;Monica K. Campbell ,&nbsp;Philip T. Quinlan","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101593","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101593","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Charitable giving involves a complex economic and social decision because the giver expends resources for goods or services they will never receive. Although psychologists have identified numerous factors that influence charitable giving, there currently exists no unifying computational model of charitable choice. Here, we submit one such model, based within the strictures of Psychological Value Theory (PVT). In four experiments, we assess whether charitable giving is driven by the perceived Psychological Value of the recipient. Across all four experiments, we simultaneously predict response choice and response time with high accuracy. In a fifth experiment, we show that PVT predicts charitable giving more accurately than an account based on competence and warmth. PVT accurately predicts which charity a respondent will choose to donate to and separately, whether a respondent will choose to donate at all. PVT models the cognitive processes underlying charitable donations and it provides a computational framework for integrating known influences on charitable giving. For example, we show that in-group preference influences charitable giving by changing the Psychological Values of the options, rather than by bringing about a response bias toward the in-group.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"145 ","pages":"Article 101593"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10211291","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Form to meaning mapping and the impact of explicit morpheme combination in novel word processing 形义映射与显性词素组合对小说词处理的影响。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101594
Rolando Bonandrini, Simona Amenta, Simone Sulpizio, Marco Tettamanti, Alessia Mazzucchelli, Marco Marelli

In the present study, we leveraged computational methods to explore the extent to which, relative to direct access to semantics from orthographic cues, the additional appreciation of morphological cues is advantageous while inducing the meaning of affixed pseudo-words. We re-analyzed data from a study on a lexical decision task for affixed pseudo-words. We considered a parsimonious model only including semantic variables (namely, semantic neighborhood density, entropy, magnitude, stem proximity) derived through a word-form-to-meaning approach (ngram-based). We then explored the extent to which the addition of equivalent semantic variables derived by combining semantic information from morphemes (combination-based) improved the fit of the statistical model explaining human data. Results suggest that semantic information can be extracted from arbitrary clusters of letters, yet a computational model of semantic access also including a combination-based strategy based on explicit morphological information better captures the cognitive mechanisms underlying human performance. This is particularly evident when participants recognize affixed pseudo-words as meaningful stimuli.

在本研究中,我们利用计算方法来探索相对于从正字法线索直接获取语义,在诱导附加伪词的含义时,对形态线索的额外欣赏在多大程度上是有利的。我们重新分析了一项关于附加伪词词汇决策任务的研究数据。我们考虑了一个简约模型,该模型仅包括通过从形式到意义的方法(基于ngram)导出的语义变量(即语义邻域密度、熵、幅度、词干接近度)。然后,我们探讨了通过组合语素的语义信息(基于组合)获得的等效语义变量的添加在多大程度上提高了解释人类数据的统计模型的拟合度。结果表明,语义信息可以从任意字母簇中提取,但语义访问的计算模型也包括基于明确形态信息的组合策略,可以更好地捕捉人类表现的认知机制。当参与者将附加的伪词识别为有意义的刺激时,这一点尤其明显。
{"title":"Form to meaning mapping and the impact of explicit morpheme combination in novel word processing","authors":"Rolando Bonandrini,&nbsp;Simona Amenta,&nbsp;Simone Sulpizio,&nbsp;Marco Tettamanti,&nbsp;Alessia Mazzucchelli,&nbsp;Marco Marelli","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101594","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101594","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In the present study, we leveraged computational methods to explore the extent to which, relative to direct access to semantics from orthographic cues, the additional appreciation of morphological cues is advantageous while inducing the meaning of affixed pseudo-words. We re-analyzed data from a study on a lexical decision task for affixed pseudo-words. We considered a parsimonious model only including semantic variables (namely, semantic neighborhood density, entropy, magnitude, stem proximity) derived through a word-form-to-meaning approach (<em>ngram</em>-based). We then explored the extent to which the addition of equivalent semantic variables derived by combining semantic information from morphemes (<em>combination</em>-based) improved the fit of the statistical model explaining human data. Results suggest that semantic information can be extracted from arbitrary clusters of letters, yet a computational model of semantic access also including a <em>combination</em>-based strategy based on explicit morphological information better captures the cognitive mechanisms underlying human performance. This is particularly evident when participants recognize affixed pseudo-words as meaningful stimuli.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"145 ","pages":"Article 101594"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10212250","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Serial attention to serial memory: The psychological refractory period in forward and backward cued recall 连续注意对连续记忆:前后线索回忆的心理不应期。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101583
Gordon D. Logan, Simon D. Lilburn, Jana E. Ulrich

Guided by the conjecture that memory retrieval is attention turned inward, we examined serial attention in serial memory, combining the psychological refractory period (PRP) procedure from attention research with cued recall of two items from brief six-item lists. We report six experiments showing robust PRP effects in cued recall from memory (1–4) and cued report from perceptual displays (5–6), which suggest that memory retrieval requires the same attentional bottleneck as “retrieval” from perception. There were strong direction effects in each memory experiment. Response time (RT) was shorter and accuracy was higher when the cues occurred in the forward direction (left-to-right, top-to-bottom, first-to-last), replicating differences between forward and backward serial recall. Cue positions had strong effects on RT and accuracy in the memory experiments (1–4). The pattern suggested that subjects find cued items in memory by stepping through the list from the beginning or the end, with a preference for starting at the beginning. The perceptual experiments (5–6) showed weak effects of position that were more consistent with direct access. In all experiments, the distance between the cues in the list (lag) had weak effects, suggesting that subjects searched for each cue from the beginning or end of the list more often than they moved through the list from the first cue to the second. Direction, distance, and lag effects on RT and inter-response interval changed with SOA in a manner that suggested they affect bottleneck or pre-bottleneck processes that create and execute a plan for successive retrievals. We conclude that sequential retrieval from memory and sequential attention to perception engage the same computations and we show how computational models of memory can be interpreted as models of attention focused on memory.

在记忆检索是注意力向内转向的猜想的指导下,我们将注意力研究中的心理不应期(PRP)过程与从简短的六个项目列表中提示回忆两个项目相结合,检验了连续记忆中的连续注意。我们报告了六个实验,显示了来自记忆的提示回忆(1-4)和来自感知显示的提示报告(5-6)中强大的PRP效应,这表明记忆检索需要与来自感知的“检索”相同的注意瓶颈。每个记忆实验都有很强的方向效应。当线索发生在正向(从左到右、从上到下、从前到后)时,反应时间(RT)更短,准确率更高,复制了正向和反向序列回忆之间的差异。提示位置对记忆实验中的RT和准确性有很强的影响(1-4)。该模式表明,受试者通过从头或尾遍历列表来找到记忆中的提示项目,并倾向于从头开始。感知实验(5-6)显示,位置的影响较弱,更符合直接访问。在所有实验中,列表中线索之间的距离(滞后)影响较弱,这表明受试者从列表的开头或结尾搜索每个线索的频率高于他们从第一个线索到第二个线索的搜索频率。方向、距离和滞后对RT和响应间隔的影响随着SOA的变化而变化,表明它们会影响创建和执行连续检索计划的瓶颈或瓶颈前流程。我们得出结论,从记忆的顺序检索和从注意力到感知的顺序检索涉及相同的计算,我们展示了记忆的计算模型如何被解释为专注于记忆的注意力模型。
{"title":"Serial attention to serial memory: The psychological refractory period in forward and backward cued recall","authors":"Gordon D. Logan,&nbsp;Simon D. Lilburn,&nbsp;Jana E. Ulrich","doi":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101583","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cogpsych.2023.101583","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Guided by the conjecture that memory retrieval is attention turned inward, we examined serial attention in serial memory, combining the psychological refractory period (PRP) procedure from attention research with cued recall of two items from brief six-item lists. We report six experiments showing robust PRP effects in cued recall from memory (1–4) and cued report from perceptual displays (5–6), which suggest that memory retrieval requires the same attentional bottleneck as “retrieval” from perception. There were strong direction effects in each memory experiment. Response time (RT) was shorter and accuracy was higher when the cues occurred in the forward direction (left-to-right, top-to-bottom, first-to-last), replicating differences between forward and backward serial recall. Cue positions had strong effects on RT and accuracy in the memory experiments (1–4). The pattern suggested that subjects find cued items in memory by stepping through the list from the beginning or the end, with a preference for starting at the beginning. The perceptual experiments (5–6) showed weak effects of position that were more consistent with direct access. In all experiments, the distance between the cues in the list (lag) had weak effects, suggesting that subjects searched for each cue from the beginning or end of the list more often than they moved through the list from the first cue to the second. Direction, distance, and lag effects on RT and inter-response interval changed with SOA in a manner that suggested they affect bottleneck or pre-bottleneck processes that create and execute a plan for successive retrievals. We conclude that sequential retrieval from memory and sequential attention to perception engage the same computations and we show how computational models of memory can be interpreted as models of attention focused on memory.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50669,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Psychology","volume":"145 ","pages":"Article 101583"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10574688","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1