首页 > 最新文献

Animal Behaviour最新文献

英文 中文
Problem-solving ability in wild raccoons, Procyon lotor, in relation to external factors and individual traits 野生浣熊解决问题的能力与外部因素和个体特征的关系
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-14 DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.015
Louis Lazure , Robert B. Weladji

The cognitive process of innovation in animals produces new or modified behaviours in response to new challenges. Common raccoons rely on their problem-solving ability to exploit anthropogenic resources that are not freely available. As a result, they are often involved in human–wildlife conflict. We used two food extraction tasks of varying difficulty levels to measure problem-solving ability in wild raccoons living in three Canadian protected areas. We conducted experiments in two distinct locations within each park based on human footprint: recreation and preservation zones. We also looked at the effect of the presence of conspecifics and of two behavioural traits (exploratory diversity and persistence) on performance. Performance differed between the puzzles, with one of them being easier to solve based on success rate and time to success. The zone (presence of humans) did not affect problem-solving performance, while there was a tendency for solving time to increase with the presence of conspecifics. Exploratory diversity was positively related to success rate and time taken to solve. Contrary to predictions, persistence did not improve performance. There were also individual differences in performance in term of success rate and time to completion. We encourage using multiple concurrent tests to evaluate problem solving with wild individuals. Overall, we provide additional evidence that raccoons are apt problem-solvers, with the potential to adapt to new foraging opportunities, and a relevant species to study innovation in mammals.

动物的创新认知过程会产生新的或改变的行为,以应对新的挑战。普通浣熊依靠其解决问题的能力来利用人类无法免费提供的资源。因此,它们经常卷入人类与野生动物的冲突。我们使用两种难度不同的食物提取任务来测量生活在加拿大三个保护区的野生浣熊解决问题的能力。我们根据人类足迹在每个公园内的两个不同地点进行了实验:休闲区和保护区。我们还研究了同类的存在和两种行为特征(探索多样性和持久性)对成绩的影响。从成功率和成功时间来看,其中一个谜题更容易解决。区域(人类的存在)并不影响解题成绩,而解题时间则随着同类的存在而增加。探索多样性与成功率和解题时间呈正相关。与预测相反,坚持并不能提高成绩。在成功率和完成时间方面也存在个体差异。我们鼓励使用多个并发测试来评估野生个体的问题解决能力。总之,我们提供了更多证据,证明浣熊是解决问题的能手,具有适应新觅食机会的潜力,是研究哺乳动物创新的相关物种。
{"title":"Problem-solving ability in wild raccoons, Procyon lotor, in relation to external factors and individual traits","authors":"Louis Lazure ,&nbsp;Robert B. Weladji","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.015","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.015","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The cognitive process of innovation in animals produces new or modified behaviours in response to new challenges. Common raccoons rely on their problem-solving ability to exploit anthropogenic resources that are not freely available. As a result, they are often involved in human–wildlife conflict. We used two food extraction tasks of varying difficulty levels to measure problem-solving ability in wild raccoons living in three Canadian protected areas. We conducted experiments in two distinct locations within each park based on human footprint: recreation and preservation zones. We also looked at the effect of the presence of conspecifics and of two behavioural traits (exploratory diversity and persistence) on performance. Performance differed between the puzzles, with one of them being easier to solve based on success rate and time to success. The zone (presence of humans) did not affect problem-solving performance, while there was a tendency for solving time to increase with the presence of conspecifics. Exploratory diversity was positively related to success rate and time taken to solve. Contrary to predictions, persistence did not improve performance. There were also individual differences in performance in term of success rate and time to completion. We encourage using multiple concurrent tests to evaluate problem solving with wild individuals. Overall, we provide additional evidence that raccoons are apt problem-solvers, with the potential to adapt to new foraging opportunities, and a relevant species to study innovation in mammals.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"215 ","pages":"Pages 1-9"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0003347224001787/pdfft?md5=81a54a864f16f60d5e13d8b3587e2958&pid=1-s2.0-S0003347224001787-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141629983","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Woodpeckers are more cryptic against tree bark on which they forage 啄木鸟对其觅食的树皮更加隐蔽
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-12 DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.006
Monica L. Carlson, Joseph Kawalec , Mary Caswell Stoddard

Crypsis is a common strategy used by animals to avoid detection. In many bird taxa, plumage colour patterns are assumed to play a role in crypsis, but few studies have directly measured plumage crypsis on ecologically relevant substrates from the perspective of relevant signal receivers. To address this gap, we tested whether the plumage colour patterns of three North American woodpeckers commonly found in New Jersey (U.S.A.) are more cryptic to avian predators on trees utilized for foraging than a random subset of trees from their habitat. We focused on the red-bellied woodpecker, Melanerpes carolinus, downy woodpecker, Dryobates pubescens, and hairy woodpecker, Leuconotopicus villosus. First, we took standardized photographs of the bark of trees on which we observed woodpeckers foraging, as well as photographs of the bark of trees from transects in the same area. Next, we superimposed standardized photographs of woodpecker specimens (dorsal view) onto the bark samples and adjusted the resulting images to estimate how they would appear to the visual system of an avian predator. We then estimated four types of achromatic crypsis from each bird/bark combination image: granularity matching, contrast matching, luminance matching and disruptive coloration. We also estimated how well woodpeckers matched the bark substrates in terms of colour by quantifying the degree of colour similarity in a colour space representing a typical avian predator. We found that woodpeckers were more cryptic against trees they utilized for foraging in terms of disruptive coloration, luminance matching and colour matching, suggesting that the need to be cryptic while foraging is likely to have shaped the evolution of woodpecker plumage patterning.

隐身是动物避免被发现的一种常用策略。在许多鸟类类群中,羽色图案被认为在隐身中起一定作用,但很少有研究从相关信号接收者的角度直接测量生态相关基质上的羽色隐身。为了填补这一空白,我们测试了三种常见于美国新泽西州的北美啄木鸟的羽色图案在用于觅食的树木上对鸟类捕食者的隐蔽性是否比在其栖息地的随机树木子集上更强。我们重点研究了红腹啄木鸟 Melanerpes carolinus、绒毛啄木鸟 Dryobates pubescens 和毛啄木鸟 Leuconotopicus villosus。首先,我们拍摄了观察到啄木鸟觅食的树皮的标准化照片,以及同一地区横断面上树皮的照片。然后,我们将啄木鸟标本的标准化照片(背视图)叠加到树皮样本上,并调整所得到的图像,以估计它们在鸟类捕食者的视觉系统中会是什么样子。然后,我们从每张鸟类/树皮组合图像中估算出四种类型的消色差:粒度匹配、对比度匹配、亮度匹配和干扰色差。我们还通过量化代表典型鸟类捕食者的色彩空间中的色彩相似程度,估计了啄木鸟在色彩方面与树皮基质的匹配程度。我们发现,在破坏性色彩、亮度匹配和色彩匹配方面,啄木鸟对它们用来觅食的树木更加隐蔽,这表明在觅食时隐蔽的需要很可能决定了啄木鸟羽色图案的进化。
{"title":"Woodpeckers are more cryptic against tree bark on which they forage","authors":"Monica L. Carlson,&nbsp;Joseph Kawalec ,&nbsp;Mary Caswell Stoddard","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.006","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.006","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p><span><span>Crypsis is a common strategy used by animals to avoid detection. In many bird taxa, </span>plumage colour patterns are assumed to play a role in crypsis, but few studies have directly measured plumage crypsis on ecologically relevant substrates from the perspective of relevant signal receivers. To address this gap, we tested whether the plumage colour patterns of three North American woodpeckers commonly found in New Jersey (U.S.A.) are more cryptic to avian predators on trees utilized for foraging than a random subset of trees from their habitat. We focused on the red-bellied woodpecker, </span><em>Melanerpes carolinus</em><span>, downy woodpecker, </span><em>Dryobates pubescens</em>, and hairy woodpecker, <em>Leuconotopicus villosus</em>. First, we took standardized photographs of the bark of trees on which we observed woodpeckers foraging, as well as photographs of the bark of trees from transects in the same area. Next, we superimposed standardized photographs of woodpecker specimens (dorsal view) onto the bark samples and adjusted the resulting images to estimate how they would appear to the visual system of an avian predator. We then estimated four types of achromatic crypsis from each bird/bark combination image: granularity matching, contrast matching, luminance matching and disruptive coloration. We also estimated how well woodpeckers matched the bark substrates in terms of colour by quantifying the degree of colour similarity in a colour space representing a typical avian predator. We found that woodpeckers were more cryptic against trees they utilized for foraging in terms of disruptive coloration, luminance matching and colour matching, suggesting that the need to be cryptic while foraging is likely to have shaped the evolution of woodpecker plumage patterning.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"215 ","pages":"Pages 191-209"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141690505","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Lighting and behaviour in captivity: butterflies prefer light environments containing UV wavelengths 人工饲养中的光照和行为:蝴蝶更喜欢含有紫外线波长的光照环境
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-10 DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.03.014
Rowan K. Thomas , Alan P. Gay , Dylan Gwynn-Jones , Natasha de Vere , Roger D. Santer

The behaviour and welfare of animals in captivity is of great importance to zoological collections, captive breeding programmes, food production and keepers of companion animals. Artificial lighting is commonly deficient in UV wavelengths, and use of such lighting for indoor animal enclosures could have significant impacts on the behaviour and welfare of animals to which UV wavelengths are visible. This includes birds, reptiles and fish, but also insects. Here we investigated the effect of UV-present and UV-absent light environments on the behaviour of Vanessa cardui, a butterfly that possesses a trichromatic visual system typical of many insects. We conducted behavioural experiments using a free-flight arena divided in half, where each half could be subjected to UV+ or UV− illumination. When lighting conditions for the two arena halves were the same, we found no significant differences in activity between UV+ and UV− light environments. However when lighting conditions for the two arena halves were different, butterflies showed a significant preference for the UV+ over the UV− half. This remained the case even when the overall intensity of UV+ illumination was less than that of UV− illumination. Our results suggest that UV-deficient artificial lighting conditions do not themselves affect the activity of butterflies, but that given a choice, butterflies prefer lighting that contains UV. Based on these findings, captive light environments can be designed that use supplementary lighting or filters to improve the welfare of captive insects, and the visitor experience.

圈养动物的行为和福利对动物学收藏、圈养繁殖计划、食品生产和伴侣动物饲养者都非常重要。人工照明通常缺乏紫外线波长,在室内动物围栏中使用这种照明可能会对可见紫外线波长的动物的行为和福利产生重大影响。这些动物包括鸟类、爬行动物和鱼类,也包括昆虫。在这里,我们研究了紫外线存在和紫外线不存在的光环境对 Vanessa cardui 行为的影响,Vanessa cardui 是一种蝴蝶,具有许多昆虫典型的三基色视觉系统。我们使用一个分成两半的自由飞行竞技场进行了行为实验,每半竞技场都可以接受紫外线+或紫外线-的光照。当两半竞技场的光照条件相同时,我们发现紫外线+和紫外线-光照环境下的活动没有显著差异。然而,当两个半场的光照条件不同时,蝴蝶对紫外线+半场的偏好明显高于紫外线-半场。即使紫外线+的总体光照强度低于紫外线-的光照强度,情况也是如此。我们的研究结果表明,缺乏紫外线的人工照明条件本身并不会影响蝴蝶的活动,但在有选择的情况下,蝴蝶更喜欢含有紫外线的照明。基于这些发现,可以设计使用辅助照明或滤光器的人工饲养光环境,以改善人工饲养昆虫的福利和游客体验。
{"title":"Lighting and behaviour in captivity: butterflies prefer light environments containing UV wavelengths","authors":"Rowan K. Thomas ,&nbsp;Alan P. Gay ,&nbsp;Dylan Gwynn-Jones ,&nbsp;Natasha de Vere ,&nbsp;Roger D. Santer","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.03.014","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.03.014","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The behaviour and welfare of animals in captivity is of great importance to zoological collections, captive breeding programmes, food production and keepers of companion animals. Artificial lighting is commonly deficient in UV wavelengths, and use of such lighting for indoor animal enclosures could have significant impacts on the behaviour and welfare of animals to which UV wavelengths are visible. This includes birds, reptiles and fish, but also insects. Here we investigated the effect of UV-present and UV-absent light environments on the behaviour of <em>Vanessa cardui</em>, a butterfly that possesses a trichromatic visual system typical of many insects. We conducted behavioural experiments using a free-flight arena divided in half, where each half could be subjected to UV+ or UV− illumination. When lighting conditions for the two arena halves were the same, we found no significant differences in activity between UV+ and UV− light environments. However when lighting conditions for the two arena halves were different, butterflies showed a significant preference for the UV+ over the UV− half. This remained the case even when the overall intensity of UV+ illumination was less than that of UV− illumination. Our results suggest that UV-deficient artificial lighting conditions do not themselves affect the activity of butterflies, but that given a choice, butterflies prefer lighting that contains UV. Based on these findings, captive light environments can be designed that use supplementary lighting or filters to improve the welfare of captive insects, and the visitor experience.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"214 ","pages":"Pages 165-172"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0003347224000940/pdfft?md5=30a9999d5d60d7c620b6af12c6457127&pid=1-s2.0-S0003347224000940-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141637371","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Male-mediated early maturation unlikely to evolve via adaptive evolution 男性介导的早熟不太可能通过适应性进化演变而来
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-09 DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.002
Brian A. Lerch , Matthew N. Zipple , Laurence R. Gesquiere , Evan T. Sloan , Jacinta C. Beehner , Susan C. Alberts

The Vandenbergh effect, or male-mediated maturation, occurs when females reach sexual maturation upon exposure to a novel male. Male-mediated maturation is found across mammals, including in geladas, Theropithecus gelada, where it may be an adaptive counterstrategy to infanticide that follows the immigration of a new male; maturing after male immigration maximizes a female's chances of weaning her first offspring before the next infanticidal male immigrates (the ‘optimal timing hypothesis’). Alternatively, the nonadaptive ‘Bruce effect by-product hypothesis’ posits that male-mediated maturation in geladas (and possibly other mammals) is triggered by the same physiological changes that, in pregnant females, produce spontaneous abortion (the Bruce effect). We test both hypotheses using theory and observational data. We show that neither male-mediated maturation nor its associated hormonal changes occur in baboons (Papio cynocephalus × Papio anubis), a primate without the Bruce effect. An individual-based model suggests that male-mediated maturation should not evolve via adaptive evolution in either geladas or baboons. Finally, we derive the selection coefficient for male-mediated maturation and show it is likely to be very small because male-mediated maturation yields only marginal potential benefits unless the system is extremely fine-tuned. We conclude that male-mediated maturation in geladas is a by-product of the Bruce effect and more broadly that the Vandenbergh effect may be nonadaptive.

凡登伯格效应(Vandenbergh effect),或称雄性介导的成熟,是指雌性在接触到新的雄性后达到性成熟。雄性介导的成熟在哺乳动物中都有发现,包括在狮尾狒(Theropithecus gelada)中,这可能是在新雄性迁入后对杀婴行为的一种适应性反击策略;在雄性迁入后成熟可使雌性在下一个杀婴的雄性迁入前断奶其第一个后代的机会最大化("最佳时机假说")。另一种非适应性的 "布鲁斯效应副产品假说 "则认为,由雄性动物(也可能是其他哺乳动物)促成的明胶蛙的成熟是由怀孕雌性动物产生自发性流产(布鲁斯效应)的相同生理变化引发的。我们利用理论和观察数据检验了这两个假设。我们的研究表明,在狒狒(Papio cynocephalus × Papio anubis)这种没有布鲁斯效应的灵长类动物身上,既没有雄性介导的成熟,也没有与之相关的激素变化。一个基于个体的模型表明,雄性介导的成熟在狮尾狒和狒狒中都不应该通过适应性进化来实现。最后,我们推导出了以雄性为媒介的成熟的选择系数,并表明该系数可能非常小,因为以雄性为媒介的成熟只能产生微不足道的潜在益处,除非该系统经过了极其精细的调整。我们的结论是,明胶中以雄性为媒介的成熟是布鲁斯效应的副产品,更广泛地说,范登堡效应可能是非适应性的。
{"title":"Male-mediated early maturation unlikely to evolve via adaptive evolution","authors":"Brian A. Lerch ,&nbsp;Matthew N. Zipple ,&nbsp;Laurence R. Gesquiere ,&nbsp;Evan T. Sloan ,&nbsp;Jacinta C. Beehner ,&nbsp;Susan C. Alberts","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.002","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.002","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The Vandenbergh effect, or male-mediated maturation, occurs when females reach sexual maturation upon exposure to a novel male. Male-mediated maturation is found across mammals, including in geladas, <em>Theropithecus gelada</em>, where it may be an adaptive counterstrategy to infanticide that follows the immigration of a new male; maturing after male immigration maximizes a female's chances of weaning her first offspring before the next infanticidal male immigrates (the ‘optimal timing hypothesis’). Alternatively, the nonadaptive ‘Bruce effect by-product hypothesis’ posits that male-mediated maturation in geladas (and possibly other mammals) is triggered by the same physiological changes that, in pregnant females, produce spontaneous abortion (the Bruce effect). We test both hypotheses using theory and observational data. We show that neither male-mediated maturation nor its associated hormonal changes occur in baboons (<em>Papio cynocephalus</em> <!-->×<!--> <em>Papio anubis</em>), a primate without the Bruce effect. An individual-based model suggests that male-mediated maturation should not evolve via adaptive evolution in either geladas or baboons. Finally, we derive the selection coefficient for male-mediated maturation and show it is likely to be very small because male-mediated maturation yields only marginal potential benefits unless the system is extremely fine-tuned. We conclude that male-mediated maturation in geladas is a by-product of the Bruce effect and more broadly that the Vandenbergh effect may be nonadaptive.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"214 ","pages":"Pages 219-240"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0003347224001532/pdfft?md5=16b60fbb1db592688314e1dbaaf5c250&pid=1-s2.0-S0003347224001532-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141637374","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Corrigendum to “The ontogeny of nest-building behaviour in Sumatran orang-utans, Pongo abelii” [Anim Behav 211 (2024) 53–67] 苏门答腊猩猩筑巢行为的本体发育》更正 [Anim Behav 211 (2024) 53-67]
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.012
Andrea L. Permana , Junaidi Jaka Permana , Lara Nellissen , Didik Prasetyo , Serge A. Wich , Carel P. van Schaik , Caroline Schuppli
{"title":"Corrigendum to “The ontogeny of nest-building behaviour in Sumatran orang-utans, Pongo abelii” [Anim Behav 211 (2024) 53–67]","authors":"Andrea L. Permana ,&nbsp;Junaidi Jaka Permana ,&nbsp;Lara Nellissen ,&nbsp;Didik Prasetyo ,&nbsp;Serge A. Wich ,&nbsp;Carel P. van Schaik ,&nbsp;Caroline Schuppli","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.012","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.012","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"214 ","pages":"Page 257"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0003347224001635/pdfft?md5=1874366c01d1a23c28229e824ca6f103&pid=1-s2.0-S0003347224001635-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141638287","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does parental experience with visual and olfactory predator cues have consequences for offspring in guppies? 亲鸟在视觉和嗅觉捕食者线索方面的经验是否会对后代产生影响?
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.014
Faith Leri, Laura R. Stein

Parental effects, or parental phenotypes affecting offspring phenotypes, are widespread across taxa, yet there is significant variation within species regarding which offspring traits are affected. One reason for this observed variation could be the type of sensory cues present in the parental environment. By exposing parents to sensory cues containing different information about the same ecological stressor, we can determine whether information is integrated differently by parents based on cue type, leading to differential trait development in offspring. In this study, we utilized predator cues, which can be found in isolation and in combination in natural settings, to test whether cue type plays a role in differential phenotype expression in Trinidadian guppies, Poecilia reticulata. Parents were exposed to predator cues (visual, olfactory or both combined) over 14 days, after which we assessed life history traits, morphology and activity. Offspring were then raised with no predator cues and tested for morphology and activity in adulthood. No differences in life history traits were observed across 10 weeks. In line with previous findings, behaviour differed in both the parent and F1 generations in response to predator cues; however, effects were dependent on cue type and sex. Our results suggest that exposure to even a single sensory cue is strong enough to initiate a cascade of responses both in parent and F1 generations, and that interacting factors such as cue type and sex lend importance to understanding consequences of parent risk perception for offspring.

亲代效应或亲代表型对子代表型的影响在各类群中普遍存在,但在物种内部,哪些子代性状会受到影响却存在显著差异。造成这种观察到的差异的原因之一可能是亲代环境中存在的感官线索类型。通过让亲本接触包含相同生态压力源不同信息的感官线索,我们可以确定亲本是否会根据线索类型对信息进行不同的整合,从而导致后代性状发展的差异。在本研究中,我们利用在自然环境中可以单独或组合发现的捕食者线索,来检验线索类型是否在特立尼达河豚鱼(Poecilia reticulata)的差异表型表达中发挥作用。在14天内,亲鱼暴露于捕食者线索(视觉、嗅觉或两者结合),之后我们对其生活史特征、形态和活动进行评估。然后在没有捕食者线索的情况下饲养后代,并在成年后对其形态和活动进行测试。在10周的时间里,我们没有观察到生活史特征的差异。与之前的研究结果一致,亲代和 F1 代对捕食者线索的行为均有差异;但是,影响取决于线索类型和性别。我们的研究结果表明,即使暴露于一个单一的感官线索,也足以在亲代和F1代中引发一连串的反应,而线索类型和性别等相互作用的因素对于了解亲代风险认知对后代的影响具有重要意义。
{"title":"Does parental experience with visual and olfactory predator cues have consequences for offspring in guppies?","authors":"Faith Leri,&nbsp;Laura R. Stein","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.014","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.014","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Parental effects, or parental phenotypes affecting offspring phenotypes, are widespread across taxa, yet there is significant variation within species regarding which offspring traits are affected. One reason for this observed variation could be the type of sensory cues present in the parental environment. By exposing parents to sensory cues containing different information about the same ecological stressor, we can determine whether information is integrated differently by parents based on cue type, leading to differential trait development in offspring. In this study, we utilized predator cues, which can be found in isolation and in combination in natural settings, to test whether cue type plays a role in differential phenotype expression in Trinidadian guppies, <em>Poecilia reticulata</em>. Parents were exposed to predator cues (visual, olfactory or both combined) over 14 days, after which we assessed life history traits, morphology and activity. Offspring were then raised with no predator cues and tested for morphology and activity in adulthood. No differences in life history traits were observed across 10 weeks. In line with previous findings, behaviour differed in both the parent and F1 generations in response to predator cues; however, effects were dependent on cue type and sex. Our results suggest that exposure to even a single sensory cue is strong enough to initiate a cascade of responses both in parent and F1 generations, and that interacting factors such as cue type and sex lend importance to understanding consequences of parent risk perception for offspring.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"214 ","pages":"Pages 241-255"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0003347224001775/pdfft?md5=9d6f895565b70c4922018b1aff624d5c&pid=1-s2.0-S0003347224001775-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141638286","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Honey bee foraging behaviour can be influenced by preferencesfor oscillating flowers 蜜蜂的觅食行为可能受到对振荡花朵偏好的影响
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.005
Rishabh Desai , Matthew A. Garratt , Mandyam V. Srinivasan , Sridhar Ravi

Foraging honey bees, Apis mellifera, need to interact with a range of moving objects, including flowers during windy conditions. Their ability to land on moving flowers, which they demonstrate regularly in nature, would require them to be able to detect, identify and compensate for the flowers' movements. We sought to investigate whether honey bees can distinguish between a stationary and an oscillating flower and whether they display a preference for one or the other. Different sets of individual free-flying honey bees were trained by presenting them with either a stationary or an oscillating flower-like stimulus, which were identical in shape and colour. Subsequently, when prompted to spontaneously choose between two identical flowers, one moving and the other stationary, honey bees exhibited a preference for the moving flower, regardless of whether they were previously trained on the stationary or the moving flower. In a further experiment, a separate set of bees were presented, after being trained, with a choice between stationary or oscillating flowers whose shape differed from the training flower. Here too, bees displayed a significant preference to land on the moving novel-shaped flower. These findings highlight the significance of flower movement to honey bee foraging behaviour. Moving objects like flowers could contribute additional visual salience which would enable easier detection, highlighting motion as an important descriptor used by insects to identify and interact with relevant environmental stimuli.

蜜蜂(Apis mellifera)觅食时需要与一系列移动物体互动,包括在有风的情况下与花朵互动。蜜蜂能在运动的花朵上着陆(它们在自然界中经常表现出这种能力),这就要求它们能够探测、识别花朵的运动并对其进行补偿。我们试图研究蜜蜂能否区分静止的花朵和摆动的花朵,以及它们是否对其中一种花朵有偏好。我们对不同组的自由飞行蜜蜂进行了训练,向它们展示了形状和颜色完全相同的静止花朵或振荡花朵。随后,当提示蜜蜂自发地在一动一静的两朵相同花朵中做出选择时,蜜蜂表现出了对运动花朵的偏爱,无论它们之前接受的是静止花朵还是运动花朵的训练。在另一项实验中,另一组蜜蜂在接受训练后,在形状与训练花不同的静止花或摆动花之间进行选择。在此实验中,蜜蜂也表现出明显的偏好,更愿意落在形状新颖的运动花朵上。这些发现凸显了花朵运动对蜜蜂觅食行为的重要性。像花朵这样的运动物体可能会带来额外的视觉显著性,从而使蜜蜂更容易发现,这突出表明运动是昆虫用来识别相关环境刺激并与之相互作用的重要描述符。
{"title":"Honey bee foraging behaviour can be influenced by preferencesfor oscillating flowers","authors":"Rishabh Desai ,&nbsp;Matthew A. Garratt ,&nbsp;Mandyam V. Srinivasan ,&nbsp;Sridhar Ravi","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.005","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.005","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Foraging honey bees, <em>Apis mellifera</em>, need to interact with a range of moving objects, including flowers during windy conditions. Their ability to land on moving flowers, which they demonstrate regularly in nature, would require them to be able to detect, identify and compensate for the flowers' movements. We sought to investigate whether honey bees can distinguish between a stationary and an oscillating flower and whether they display a preference for one or the other. Different sets of individual free-flying honey bees were trained by presenting them with either a stationary or an oscillating flower-like stimulus, which were identical in shape and colour. Subsequently, when prompted to spontaneously choose between two identical flowers, one moving and the other stationary, honey bees exhibited a preference for the moving flower, regardless of whether they were previously trained on the stationary or the moving flower. In a further experiment, a separate set of bees were presented, after being trained, with a choice between stationary or oscillating flowers whose shape differed from the training flower. Here too, bees displayed a significant preference to land on the moving novel-shaped flower. These findings highlight the significance of flower movement to honey bee foraging behaviour. Moving objects like flowers could contribute additional visual salience which would enable easier detection, highlighting motion as an important descriptor used by insects to identify and interact with relevant environmental stimuli.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"214 ","pages":"Pages 147-156"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0003347224001568/pdfft?md5=bf4b35ec131e9b2e039556ee8e13b38a&pid=1-s2.0-S0003347224001568-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141593285","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Homing pigeon navigational ontogeny: no evidence that exposure to a novel release site is sufficient for learning 归巢鸽的导航本体:没有证据表明接触新的放飞地点足以促进学习
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.009
Joe Morford , Anna Gagliardo , Enrica Pollonara , Tim Guilford

The navigational mechanisms of homing pigeons, Columba livia, have been extensively studied and represent a useful model for the navigation of birds and other animals. Pigeons navigate with an olfactory map and sun compass from unfamiliar areas and, in familiar areas, are largely guided by visual landscape cues, following stereotyped and idiosyncratic routes. However, the mechanisms by which they gain familiarity, improve their navigation and transition between navigational strategies during learning are not fully understood. Addressing these outstanding questions in this navigational model will help to improve our understanding of navigational ontogeny. We sought to investigate whether passive exposure to the cues at a site, without release, was sufficient for navigational learning, given that pigeons can determine the home direction before taking off. We exposed pigeons to cues at a novel site before returning them to the site the next day and releasing them alongside controls. We found no differences in the directional distributions, mean vector lengths, virtual vanishing times, efficiency indices or homing efficiency indices between birds that had and had not previously visited the site. We therefore found no evidence to suggest that passive exposure to the cues at a novel site was sufficient to facilitate a detectable improvement in navigational performance. There are three possible explanations for this result: first, a larger sample size would have detected a weak effect of learning; second, passive exposure to a release site is insufficient to generate navigational learning; and third, pigeons learn from passive exposure but do not rely upon this information, showing no difference in performance, despite learning. We discuss these three explanations with reference to previous findings on navigational learning in homing pigeons. We suggest that experiments should continue to examine navigational ontogeny in homing pigeons to help address this major problem for the field of navigation.

人们对归巢鸽(Columba livia)的导航机制进行了广泛的研究,它是鸟类和其他动物导航的一个有用模型。鸽子在不熟悉的地区利用嗅觉地图和太阳罗盘导航,而在熟悉的地区则主要依靠视觉景观线索的指引,遵循定型和特异的路线。然而,它们在学习过程中获得熟悉感、改善导航能力以及在不同导航策略之间转换的机制尚未完全明了。在这一导航模型中解决这些悬而未决的问题将有助于提高我们对导航本体的理解。考虑到鸽子在起飞前就能确定归巢方向,我们试图研究在不放飞的情况下被动地暴露于某一地点的线索是否足以促进导航学习。我们先让鸽子在一个新地点接触提示物,第二天再将它们送回该地点,然后将它们与对照组一起放飞。我们发现,在方向分布、平均矢量长度、虚拟消失时间、效率指数或归巢效率指数方面,到访过和未到访过该地点的鸽子没有差异。因此,我们没有发现任何证据表明,被动接触新地点的提示足以促进导航性能的明显改善。对这一结果有三种可能的解释:第一,更大的样本量会检测到微弱的学习效果;第二,被动接触放飞地点不足以产生导航学习;第三,鸽子从被动接触中学到了信息,但并不依赖于这些信息,尽管鸽子进行了学习,但它们的表现并无差异。我们结合以前对归巢鸽导航学习的研究结果,对这三种解释进行了讨论。我们建议应继续通过实验研究归巢鸽的导航本体,以帮助解决导航领域的这一重大问题。
{"title":"Homing pigeon navigational ontogeny: no evidence that exposure to a novel release site is sufficient for learning","authors":"Joe Morford ,&nbsp;Anna Gagliardo ,&nbsp;Enrica Pollonara ,&nbsp;Tim Guilford","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.009","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.009","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The navigational mechanisms of homing pigeons, <em>Columba livia</em>, have been extensively studied and represent a useful model for the navigation of birds and other animals. Pigeons navigate with an olfactory map and sun compass from unfamiliar areas and, in familiar areas, are largely guided by visual landscape cues, following stereotyped and idiosyncratic routes. However, the mechanisms by which they gain familiarity, improve their navigation and transition between navigational strategies during learning are not fully understood. Addressing these outstanding questions in this navigational model will help to improve our understanding of navigational ontogeny. We sought to investigate whether passive exposure to the cues at a site, without release, was sufficient for navigational learning, given that pigeons can determine the home direction before taking off. We exposed pigeons to cues at a novel site before returning them to the site the next day and releasing them alongside controls. We found no differences in the directional distributions, mean vector lengths, virtual vanishing times, efficiency indices or homing efficiency indices between birds that had and had not previously visited the site. We therefore found no evidence to suggest that passive exposure to the cues at a novel site was sufficient to facilitate a detectable improvement in navigational performance. There are three possible explanations for this result: first, a larger sample size would have detected a weak effect of learning; second, passive exposure to a release site is insufficient to generate navigational learning; and third, pigeons learn from passive exposure but do not rely upon this information, showing no difference in performance, despite learning. We discuss these three explanations with reference to previous findings on navigational learning in homing pigeons. We suggest that experiments should continue to examine navigational ontogeny in homing pigeons to help address this major problem for the field of navigation.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"214 ","pages":"Pages 157-164"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S000334722400160X/pdfft?md5=8420224f870439070d5b7e6b86c05e6b&pid=1-s2.0-S000334722400160X-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141593284","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Call-timing plasticity of a treefrog in response to noise 树蛙对噪音的呼叫时间可塑性
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-08 DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.013
Chunwen Chang , Feng-Chun Lin , Jhan-Wei Lin , Si-Min Lin

Chorusing animals constantly face the challenge of overcoming background noises. To minimize the masking of their own calls by conspecific or heterospecific calls, plasticity in call timing serves as an alternative tactic for chorusing animals. We examined how male rhacophorid treefrogs, Zhangixalus prasinatus, adjust their call timing in response to various noisy environments. When exposed to periodic white noise disturbances with brief silent intervals, the frogs accurately avoided overlapping with the noise and delivered their calls in the silent gaps. Moreover, they delivered long calls earlier than short calls during the silent periods to accommodate the longer duration within a brief silent gap. When exposed to noises of varying frequencies, the frogs specifically avoided medium-frequency noise, which closely matched their own call frequency. This phenomenon was also observed in experiments testing the aggregating behaviour of male frogs; they ceased to aggregate towards conspecific calls when interfered with by medium-frequency noise, whereas high-frequency and low-frequency noise interferences did not elicit the same behaviour. Compared to high-quality males, frogs with low SMi (scaled mass index) exhibited greater plasticity in call timing, indicating a condition-dependent tactic for energy allocation. Such behavioural plasticity in call timing was reflected in natural soundscape recordings. Our recordings revealed that, as a result of these adaptive strategies, males tended to avoid overlapping their calls with background noises in their natural habitats. Our research provides evidence that call plasticity is an effective strategy for chorusing animals to respond to noise at both individual and population levels. Furthermore, our study suggests that plasticity in call timing is a complex decision-making behaviour involving the consideration of noise properties and an individual's body condition.

合群动物一直面临着克服背景噪声的挑战。为了最大限度地减少同种或异种叫声对自身叫声的掩蔽,叫声时间的可塑性成为合唱动物的另一种策略。我们研究了雄性犀角树蛙(Zhangixalus prasinatus)如何调整其鸣叫时间以应对各种噪声环境。当暴露于周期性的白噪声干扰和短暂的静默间歇时,蛙类会准确地避免与噪声重叠,并在静默间隙中发出叫声。此外,它们在静默期间发出长叫声的时间早于发出短叫声的时间,以适应短暂静默间隙中较长的持续时间。当暴露于不同频率的噪音时,蛙类会特别避开中频噪音,因为中频噪音与它们自身的鸣叫频率非常接近。在测试雄蛙聚集行为的实验中也观察到了这一现象;当受到中频噪声干扰时,雄蛙不再向同类的叫声聚集,而高频和低频噪声干扰则不会引起雄蛙的同样行为。与优质雄蛙相比,低SMi(按比例质量指数)雄蛙在鸣叫时间上表现出更大的可塑性,这表明它们的能量分配策略与条件有关。这种行为上的可塑性在自然声景录音中也有所反映。我们的记录显示,由于这些适应性策略,雄性往往会避免在自然栖息地与背景噪声重叠。我们的研究证明,在个体和种群水平上,叫声可塑性是和鸣动物应对噪声的一种有效策略。此外,我们的研究还表明,鸣叫时间的可塑性是一种复杂的决策行为,需要考虑噪声特性和个体的身体状况。
{"title":"Call-timing plasticity of a treefrog in response to noise","authors":"Chunwen Chang ,&nbsp;Feng-Chun Lin ,&nbsp;Jhan-Wei Lin ,&nbsp;Si-Min Lin","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.013","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.013","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Chorusing animals constantly face the challenge of overcoming background noises. To minimize the masking of their own calls by conspecific or heterospecific calls, plasticity in call timing serves as an alternative tactic for chorusing animals. We examined how male rhacophorid treefrogs, <em>Zhangixalus prasinatus</em>, adjust their call timing in response to various noisy environments. When exposed to periodic white noise disturbances with brief silent intervals, the frogs accurately avoided overlapping with the noise and delivered their calls in the silent gaps. Moreover, they delivered long calls earlier than short calls during the silent periods to accommodate the longer duration within a brief silent gap. When exposed to noises of varying frequencies, the frogs specifically avoided medium-frequency noise, which closely matched their own call frequency. This phenomenon was also observed in experiments testing the aggregating behaviour of male frogs; they ceased to aggregate towards conspecific calls when interfered with by medium-frequency noise, whereas high-frequency and low-frequency noise interferences did not elicit the same behaviour. Compared to high-quality males, frogs with low SM<sub>i</sub> (scaled mass index) exhibited greater plasticity in call timing, indicating a condition-dependent tactic for energy allocation. Such behavioural plasticity in call timing was reflected in natural soundscape recordings. Our recordings revealed that, as a result of these adaptive strategies, males tended to avoid overlapping their calls with background noises in their natural habitats. Our research provides evidence that call plasticity is an effective strategy for chorusing animals to respond to noise at both individual and population levels. Furthermore, our study suggests that plasticity in call timing is a complex decision-making behaviour involving the consideration of noise properties and an individual's body condition.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"214 ","pages":"Pages 137-145"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141593609","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Feeding rate reflects quality in both parents and offspring: a longitudinal study in common terns 喂食率反映亲代和子代的质量:燕鸥的纵向研究
IF 2.3 2区 生物学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-04 DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.010
Thomas Cansse , Oscar Vedder , Nathalie Kürten , Sandra Bouwhuis

Offspring provisioning can act as a proxy of resource acquisition and vary with parental sex and age. Age-related variation can arise from individual experience and senescence, but also from selective disappearance of poor-quality parents. Distinguishing between these processes and quantifying their effect on the resource acquisition and fate of individual chicks requires longitudinal monitoring of known-age individuals, which is still rare. In our longitudinal study, we observed offspring provisioning of common terns, Sterna hirundo, across a 6-year period and analysed provisioning behaviour from both a parental and offspring perspective. Using repeated measures of provisioning of individual parents, our analyses showed that provisioning did not increase with age, but that parents that were observed at older ages provisioned more, suggesting selective disappearance of parents that provisioned less. Parental provisioning was higher in males than females and increased with brood size in both sexes. For offspring, energetic acquisition declined with hatching order and increased with age. Acquisition from the mother increased faster with chick age than that from the father, and mothers distributed their provisioning more evenly across chicks of different hatching order. Parental age, however, did not affect the energetic acquisition of the offspring. The early energetic acquisition rate of chicks predicted their fledging success, but not fledging mass. When decomposing effects on energetic provisioning and acquisition rate into effects on feeding rate, prey energetic density and prey size, we found that all arose from variation in feeding rate. Overall, these results therefore show that both parents and offspring vary in quality, which is reflected in their feeding rate.

后代的供给可作为资源获取的替代物,并随父母的性别和年龄而变化。与年龄相关的变化可能源于个体经验和衰老,也可能源于劣质亲本的选择性消失。要区分这些过程并量化它们对个体雏鸟的资源获取和命运的影响,需要对已知年龄的个体进行纵向监测,而这种情况仍然很少见。在我们的纵向研究中,我们观察了普通燕鸥(Sterna hirundo)6年的后代供给情况,并从亲代和后代的角度分析了供给行为。通过重复测量单个亲鸟的供养行为,我们的分析表明,供养行为并不随着年龄的增长而增加,但观察到的年龄越大的亲鸟供养行为越多,这表明供养行为较少的亲鸟选择性地消失了。雄性亲鸟的供养率高于雌性亲鸟,而且雌雄亲鸟的供养率都随着育雏规模的增加而增加。对后代而言,能量获取随孵化顺序而减少,随年龄而增加。随着雏鸟年龄的增长,从母亲那里获得的能量比从父亲那里获得的能量增长得更快,而且母亲在不同孵化顺序的雏鸟身上的供给分配更均匀。然而,父母的年龄并不影响后代的能量获取。雏鸟的早期能量获取率可预测其羽化成功率,但不能预测羽化质量。当把对能量供给和获取率的影响分解为对摄食率、猎物能量密度和猎物大小的影响时,我们发现所有影响都来自摄食率的变化。总之,这些结果表明,亲代和子代的质量都有差异,这反映在它们的摄食率上。
{"title":"Feeding rate reflects quality in both parents and offspring: a longitudinal study in common terns","authors":"Thomas Cansse ,&nbsp;Oscar Vedder ,&nbsp;Nathalie Kürten ,&nbsp;Sandra Bouwhuis","doi":"10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.010","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2024.06.010","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Offspring provisioning can act as a proxy of resource acquisition and vary with parental sex and age. Age-related variation can arise from individual experience and senescence, but also from selective disappearance of poor-quality parents. Distinguishing between these processes and quantifying their effect on the resource acquisition and fate of individual chicks requires longitudinal monitoring of known-age individuals, which is still rare. In our longitudinal study, we observed offspring provisioning of common terns, <em>Sterna hirundo</em>, across a 6-year period and analysed provisioning behaviour from both a parental and offspring perspective. Using repeated measures of provisioning of individual parents, our analyses showed that provisioning did not increase with age, but that parents that were observed at older ages provisioned more, suggesting selective disappearance of parents that provisioned less. Parental provisioning was higher in males than females and increased with brood size in both sexes. For offspring, energetic acquisition declined with hatching order and increased with age. Acquisition from the mother increased faster with chick age than that from the father, and mothers distributed their provisioning more evenly across chicks of different hatching order. Parental age, however, did not affect the energetic acquisition of the offspring. The early energetic acquisition rate of chicks predicted their fledging success, but not fledging mass. When decomposing effects on energetic provisioning and acquisition rate into effects on feeding rate, prey energetic density and prey size, we found that all arose from variation in feeding rate. Overall, these results therefore show that both parents and offspring vary in quality, which is reflected in their feeding rate.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":50788,"journal":{"name":"Animal Behaviour","volume":"214 ","pages":"Pages 111-120"},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0003347224001611/pdfft?md5=41a2d54cc876ad7b4a4f1cffa09c18ff&pid=1-s2.0-S0003347224001611-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141540304","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Animal Behaviour
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1