首页 > 最新文献

Aggressive Behavior最新文献

英文 中文
Evaluations of aggressive chasing interactions by 7-month-old infants 评估 7 个月大婴儿的攻击性追逐互动。
IF 2.7 2区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-04 DOI: 10.1002/ab.22174
Alessandra Geraci, Silvia Benavides-Varela, Chiara Nascimben, Francesca Simion, Elisa Di Giorgio

Recent theories of socio-moral development assume that humans evolved a capacity to evaluate others' social actions in different kinds of interactions. Prior infant studies found both reaching and visual preferences for the prosocial over the antisocial agents. However, whether the attribution of either positive or negative valence to agents' actions involved in an aggressive chasing interaction can be inferred by both reaching behaviors and visual attention deployment (i.e., disengagement of visual attention) is still an open question. Here we presented 7-month-old infants (N = 92) with events displaying an aggressive chasing interaction. By using preferential reaching and an attentional task (i.e., overlap paradigm), we assessed whether and how infants evaluate aggressive chasing interactions. The results demonstrated that young infants prefer to reach the victim over the aggressor, but neither agent affects visual attention. Moreover, such reaching preferences emerged only when dynamic cues and emotional face-like features were congruent with agents' social roles. Overall, these findings suggested that infants' evaluations of aggressive interactions are based on infants' sensitivity to some kinematic cues that characterized agents' actions and, especially, to the congruency between such motions and the face-like emotional expressions of the agents.

最近的社会道德发展理论认为,人类进化出了在不同类型的互动中评估他人社会行为的能力。先前的婴儿研究发现,相对于反社会行为主体而言,婴儿在触觉和视觉上都更喜欢亲社会行为主体。然而,在攻击性追逐互动中,是否可以通过伸手行为和视觉注意力调配(即脱离视觉注意力)来推断行为主体行为的正面或负面价值归属,这仍然是一个未决问题。在此,我们向 7 个月大的婴儿(92 人)展示了攻击性追逐互动事件。通过使用优先伸手和注意任务(即重叠范式),我们评估了婴儿是否以及如何评估攻击性追逐互动。结果表明,幼婴倾向于伸手去够受害者而不是施暴者,但这两种行为都不会影响视觉注意力。此外,只有当动态线索和情绪面孔特征与行为主体的社会角色一致时,才会出现这种伸手偏好。总之,这些研究结果表明,婴儿对攻击性互动的评价是基于婴儿对一些运动线索的敏感性,这些线索是行为主体行动的特征,尤其是对这些运动与行为主体的脸部情绪表达之间的一致性的敏感性。
{"title":"Evaluations of aggressive chasing interactions by 7-month-old infants","authors":"Alessandra Geraci,&nbsp;Silvia Benavides-Varela,&nbsp;Chiara Nascimben,&nbsp;Francesca Simion,&nbsp;Elisa Di Giorgio","doi":"10.1002/ab.22174","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ab.22174","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Recent theories of socio-moral development assume that humans evolved a capacity to evaluate others' social actions in different kinds of interactions. Prior infant studies found both reaching and visual preferences for the prosocial over the antisocial agents. However, whether the attribution of either positive or negative valence to agents' actions involved in an aggressive chasing interaction can be inferred by both reaching behaviors and visual attention deployment (i.e., disengagement of visual attention) is still an open question. Here we presented 7-month-old infants (<i>N</i> = 92) with events displaying an aggressive chasing interaction. By using preferential reaching and an attentional task (i.e., overlap paradigm), we assessed whether and how infants evaluate aggressive chasing interactions. The results demonstrated that young infants prefer to reach the victim over the aggressor, but neither agent affects visual attention. Moreover, such reaching preferences emerged only when dynamic cues and emotional face-like features were congruent with agents' social roles. Overall, these findings suggested that infants' evaluations of aggressive interactions are based on infants' sensitivity to some kinematic cues that characterized agents' actions and, especially, to the congruency between such motions and the face-like emotional expressions of the agents.</p>","PeriodicalId":50842,"journal":{"name":"Aggressive Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142127243","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Can classroom seating arrangements help establish a safe environment for victims? A randomized controlled trial 教室座位安排有助于为受害者营造安全环境吗?随机对照试验。
IF 2.7 2区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-22 DOI: 10.1002/ab.22173
Nathalie A. H. Hoekstra, Yvonne H. M. van den Berg, Tessa A. M. Lansu, Hannah K. Peetz, M. Tim Mainhard, Antonius H. N. Cillessen

Students around the globe still experience bullying daily. Teachers play a key role in supporting victimized students and they could do so using their classroom seating arrangement. Common teacher strategies are to separate victims and bullies and to seat victims close to supportive others, but research has not examined whether these strategies support victims' wellbeing. Therefore, the current study tested an intervention in which victims in experimental classrooms were seated far away from their bullies and next to their best friends, whereas a random seating arrangement was implemented in control classrooms. The underlying reasoning was that victims would experience a sense of safety next to their best friend and to limit bullies' opportunities to harass the victim. The outcomes were classroom comfort, internalizing problems, academic engagement, and victimization frequency. We used a sample of 1746 Dutch upper elementary school students (Mage = 10.21) of whom 250 students reported to be chronically and frequently victimized (Mage = 9.96 years). Ethical and practical reasons rendered the conditions similar regarding victims' distances to their bullies. Consequently, the intervention in the end tested the effect of victims sitting next to their best friend. Several mixed-effects models showed that no support was found for the effectiveness of this intervention. Additional exploratory analyses testing the effect of victims' continuous distances to their bullies on their wellbeing also found no effects. These findings suggest that changing victims', bullies', and best friends' seats do not improve victims' classroom wellbeing. Alternative explanations, directions for future research, and practical implications are discussed.

全球各地的学生每天都在遭受欺凌。教师在支持受害学生方面发挥着关键作用,他们可以通过安排教室座位来做到这一点。常见的教师策略是将受害者和欺凌者分开,让受害者坐在靠近支持者的座位上,但研究并未考察这些策略是否有助于受害者的健康。因此,本研究测试了一种干预措施,即在实验班级中,受害者的座位远离欺凌者,与他们的好朋友相邻,而在对照班级中则采用随机座位安排。这样做的根本原因是,受害者可以在好友身边体验到安全感,并限制欺凌者骚扰受害者的机会。研究结果包括课堂舒适度、内化问题、学业参与度和受害频率。我们使用了 1746 名荷兰高年级小学生(年龄=10.21)作为样本,其中 250 名学生表示长期和经常受到伤害(年龄=9.96 岁)。由于伦理和实际原因,受害者与欺凌者的距离条件相似。因此,干预最终测试了受害者坐在其最好朋友旁边的效果。几个混合效应模型显示,这一干预措施的有效性没有得到支持。其他探索性分析测试了受害者与欺凌者的持续距离对其幸福感的影响,也没有发现任何效果。这些结果表明,改变受害者、欺凌者和好朋友的座位并不能改善受害者的课堂幸福感。本文讨论了其他解释、未来研究方向和实际意义。
{"title":"Can classroom seating arrangements help establish a safe environment for victims? A randomized controlled trial","authors":"Nathalie A. H. Hoekstra,&nbsp;Yvonne H. M. van den Berg,&nbsp;Tessa A. M. Lansu,&nbsp;Hannah K. Peetz,&nbsp;M. Tim Mainhard,&nbsp;Antonius H. N. Cillessen","doi":"10.1002/ab.22173","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ab.22173","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Students around the globe still experience bullying daily. Teachers play a key role in supporting victimized students and they could do so using their classroom seating arrangement. Common teacher strategies are to separate victims and bullies and to seat victims close to supportive others, but research has not examined whether these strategies support victims' wellbeing. Therefore, the current study tested an intervention in which victims in experimental classrooms were seated far away from their bullies and next to their best friends, whereas a random seating arrangement was implemented in control classrooms. The underlying reasoning was that victims would experience a sense of safety next to their best friend and to limit bullies' opportunities to harass the victim. The outcomes were classroom comfort, internalizing problems, academic engagement, and victimization frequency. We used a sample of 1746 Dutch upper elementary school students (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 10.21) of whom 250 students reported to be chronically and frequently victimized (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 9.96 years). Ethical and practical reasons rendered the conditions similar regarding victims' distances to their bullies. Consequently, the intervention in the end tested the effect of victims sitting next to their best friend. Several mixed-effects models showed that no support was found for the effectiveness of this intervention. Additional exploratory analyses testing the effect of victims' continuous distances to their bullies on their wellbeing also found no effects. These findings suggest that changing victims', bullies', and best friends' seats do not improve victims' classroom wellbeing. Alternative explanations, directions for future research, and practical implications are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":50842,"journal":{"name":"Aggressive Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ab.22173","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142037645","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Brief report: Social comparison, hypercompetitiveness, and indirect aggression: Associations with loneliness and mental health 简要报告:社会比较、过度竞争和间接攻击:与孤独和心理健康的关系。
IF 2.7 2区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-13 DOI: 10.1002/ab.22171
Ann H. Farrell, Mollie Eriksson, Tracy Vaillancourt

Indirect aggression is commonly used in adulthood, but most researchers have focused on this behavior in romantic relationships or from an intrasexual competition perspective. Therefore, we aimed to understand the social characteristics and mental health correlates of indirect aggression by combining perspectives from developmental psychopathology and evolutionary psychology. We examined: (1) whether social characteristics (social comparison, hypercompetitiveness) contributed to indirect aggression (perpetration, victimization) and (2) whether there were indirect effects from indirect aggression (perpetration, victimization) to mental health difficulties through loneliness. In a cross-sectional sample of 475 young adults (57.7% women, 51.6% White, Mage = 20.2, SDage = 2.18), path analyses revealed that social comparison predicted indirect aggression victimization, which indirectly predicted mental health difficulties (depression, anxiety, somatic symptoms) through loneliness. In contrast, indirect aggression perpetration was only predicted by hypercompetitiveness. The findings highlight that reframing cognitions associated with social comparison could help prevent indirect aggression and mental health difficulties among young people.

间接攻击是成年期的常见行为,但大多数研究人员都侧重于研究恋爱关系中的这种行为或从性内竞争的角度来研究这种行为。因此,我们旨在通过结合发展精神病理学和进化心理学的观点,了解间接攻击行为的社会特征和心理健康相关因素。我们研究了:(1) 社会特征(社会比较、过度竞争)是否会导致间接攻击行为(实施、受害);(2) 间接攻击行为(实施、受害)是否会通过孤独感对心理健康困难产生间接影响。在一个由 475 名年轻成年人(57.7% 为女性,51.6% 为白人,平均年龄 = 20.2 岁,平均年龄 = 2.18 岁)组成的横截面样本中,路径分析显示,社会比较会预测间接攻击行为的受害情况,而间接攻击行为会通过孤独感间接预测心理健康问题(抑郁、焦虑、躯体症状)。与此相反,只有超竞争性才能预测间接攻击行为。研究结果强调,重塑与社会比较相关的认知有助于预防青少年的间接攻击行为和心理健康问题。
{"title":"Brief report: Social comparison, hypercompetitiveness, and indirect aggression: Associations with loneliness and mental health","authors":"Ann H. Farrell,&nbsp;Mollie Eriksson,&nbsp;Tracy Vaillancourt","doi":"10.1002/ab.22171","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ab.22171","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Indirect aggression is commonly used in adulthood, but most researchers have focused on this behavior in romantic relationships or from an intrasexual competition perspective. Therefore, we aimed to understand the social characteristics and mental health correlates of indirect aggression by combining perspectives from developmental psychopathology and evolutionary psychology. We examined: (1) whether social characteristics (social comparison, hypercompetitiveness) contributed to indirect aggression (perpetration, victimization) and (2) whether there were indirect effects from indirect aggression (perpetration, victimization) to mental health difficulties through loneliness. In a cross-sectional sample of 475 young adults (57.7% women, 51.6% White, <i>M</i><sub><i>age</i></sub> = 20.2, SD<sub><i>age</i></sub> = 2.18), path analyses revealed that social comparison predicted indirect aggression victimization, which indirectly predicted mental health difficulties (depression, anxiety, somatic symptoms) through loneliness. In contrast, indirect aggression perpetration was only predicted by hypercompetitiveness. The findings highlight that reframing cognitions associated with social comparison could help prevent indirect aggression and mental health difficulties among young people.</p>","PeriodicalId":50842,"journal":{"name":"Aggressive Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-08-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ab.22171","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141977140","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Comparing behavioral measures of aggression in the laboratory: Taylor Aggression Paradigm versus Point-Subtraction Aggression Paradigm 比较实验室中的攻击行为测量方法:泰勒攻击范式与点牵引攻击范式。
IF 2.7 2区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-28 DOI: 10.1002/ab.22167
Matthew A. Timmins, Mitchell E. Berman, Emil F. Coccaro

Aggression refers to a wide range of behaviors with lasting individual and societal consequences. Recurrent, unplanned aggressive behavior is the core diagnostic criterion for intermittent explosive disorder (IED). In this study, we compared two behavioral measures of aggression in the laboratory: the Taylor Aggression Paradigm (TAP) and the Point-Subtraction Aggression Paradigm (PSAP). This sample (n = 528) included community participants who met DSM-5 criteria for IED (n = 156), met DSM-5 criteria for a nonaggressive psychiatric disorder (n = 205), or did not meet DSM-5 criteria for any psychiatric disorder (n = 167). All participants completed the TAP, a single-session PSAP, and relevant self-report measures. MANOVA analyses demonstrated differences between IED participants and nonaggressive participants; however, these group differences were no longer significant for the PSAP after including demographic variables. Correlation analyses found that the TAP and PSAP were positively related to one another and the composite variables associated with aggressive behavior (i.e., history of aggression, impulsivity, and propensity to experience anger) and; dependent correlations revealed that past aggression and trait anger were more strongly related to the TAP. Differences in TAP and PSAP outcomes may be partially attributed to operationalizations of aggression and methods of aggression and provocation. Further, as aggressive and nonaggressive participants differed on the PSAP somewhat mirroring the TAP, our results add to growing evidence of the validity of a single-session PSAP; further research is needed to fully establish single-session PSAP as a laboratory aggression task compared to the multi-session PSAP.

攻击行为是指对个人和社会产生持久影响的一系列行为。反复出现、无计划的攻击行为是间歇性爆发障碍(IED)的核心诊断标准。在这项研究中,我们比较了实验室中的两种攻击行为测量方法:泰勒攻击范式(TAP)和点-牵引攻击范式(PSAP)。该样本(n = 528)包括符合 DSM-5 IED 标准(n = 156)、符合 DSM-5 非攻击性精神障碍标准(n = 205)或不符合 DSM-5 任何精神障碍标准(n = 167)的社区参与者。所有参与者都完成了 TAP、单次 PSAP 和相关的自我报告测量。MANOVA 分析表明,IED 参与者与非攻击性参与者之间存在差异;但在纳入人口统计学变量后,这些群体差异在 PSAP 中不再显著。相关性分析发现,TAP 和 PSAP 与彼此以及与攻击行为相关的综合变量(即攻击史、冲动性和愤怒倾向)呈正相关;因果相关性显示,过去的攻击行为和特质愤怒与 TAP 的相关性更强。TAP 和 PSAP 结果的差异可能部分归因于攻击性的操作方法以及攻击和挑衅的方法。此外,由于攻击性参与者和非攻击性参与者在 PSAP 上的差异在某种程度上反映了 TAP 的差异,我们的结果为单次 PSAP 的有效性提供了更多证据;与多次 PSAP 相比,还需要进一步的研究来全面确定单次 PSAP 作为实验室攻击任务的有效性。
{"title":"Comparing behavioral measures of aggression in the laboratory: Taylor Aggression Paradigm versus Point-Subtraction Aggression Paradigm","authors":"Matthew A. Timmins,&nbsp;Mitchell E. Berman,&nbsp;Emil F. Coccaro","doi":"10.1002/ab.22167","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ab.22167","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Aggression refers to a wide range of behaviors with lasting individual and societal consequences. Recurrent, unplanned aggressive behavior is the core diagnostic criterion for intermittent explosive disorder (IED). In this study, we compared two behavioral measures of aggression in the laboratory: the Taylor Aggression Paradigm (TAP) and the Point-Subtraction Aggression Paradigm (PSAP). This sample (<i>n</i> = 528) included community participants who met DSM-5 criteria for IED (<i>n</i> = 156), met DSM-5 criteria for a nonaggressive psychiatric disorder (<i>n</i> = 205), or did not meet DSM-5 criteria for any psychiatric disorder (<i>n</i> = 167). All participants completed the TAP, a single-session PSAP, and relevant self-report measures. MANOVA analyses demonstrated differences between IED participants and nonaggressive participants; however, these group differences were no longer significant for the PSAP after including demographic variables. Correlation analyses found that the TAP and PSAP were positively related to one another and the composite variables associated with aggressive behavior (i.e., history of aggression, impulsivity, and propensity to experience anger) and; dependent correlations revealed that past aggression and trait anger were more strongly related to the TAP. Differences in TAP and PSAP outcomes may be partially attributed to operationalizations of aggression and methods of aggression and provocation. Further, as aggressive and nonaggressive participants differed on the PSAP somewhat mirroring the TAP, our results add to growing evidence of the validity of a single-session PSAP; further research is needed to fully establish single-session PSAP as a laboratory aggression task compared to the multi-session PSAP.</p>","PeriodicalId":50842,"journal":{"name":"Aggressive Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141789679","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Using reaction time procedures to assess implicit attitudes toward violence in a nonconvicted male sample 利用反应时间程序评估未定罪男性样本对暴力的内隐态度。
IF 2.7 2区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-28 DOI: 10.1002/ab.22168
Sacha Maimone, Michael C. Seto, Adekunle G. Ahmed, Kevin L. Nunes

In this study, we sought to capture implicit attitudes toward violence by administering response latency measures. We then examined their associations with explicit (e.g., assessed with self-report) attitudes toward violence and self-reported violent behavior in a combined sample of males from a Canadian university and males from the general community (N = 251; 156 students and 95 community members). To date, there have been mixed findings regarding these associations; some of this inconsistency may be due to the difficulty in accurately conceptualizing and assessing implicit attitudes toward violence. Therefore, we administered three response latency measures to assess this construct: a violence evaluation implicit association test (VE-IAT), a personalized VE-IAT (P-VE-IAT), and a violence evaluation relational responding task, along with three self-report measures of explicit attitudes toward violence and three self-report measures of violent behavior. More positive implicit attitudes toward violence were related to more positive explicit attitudes toward violence (for VE-IAT and P-VE-IAT; r = 0.18 to 0.22), greater likelihood of violence (for VE-IAT; r = 0.18 and for P-VE-IAT; r = 0.16), and greater propensity for violence (for the VE-IAT; r = 0.16). All measures of explicit attitudes toward violence and violent behavior were moderately to strongly associated with one another (r = 0.42 to 0.81). Furthermore, implicit attitudes toward violence explained additional variance in some violent outcomes above explicit attitudes alone. Our findings suggest that scores on certain reaction time measures are important for understanding likelihood and propensity for violence, especially when combined with explicit attitude measures.

在这项研究中,我们试图通过实施反应潜伏期测量来捕捉内隐的暴力态度。然后,我们以加拿大一所大学的男生和普通社区的男生为样本(样本数 = 251;156 名学生和 95 名社区成员),研究了他们对暴力的显性态度(例如,通过自我报告进行评估)与自我报告的暴力行为之间的关联。迄今为止,有关这些关联的研究结果不一;其中一些不一致的原因可能是难以准确概念化和评估对暴力的内隐态度。因此,我们采用了三种反应潜伏期测量方法来评估这一概念:暴力评估内隐联想测验(VE-IAT)、个性化暴力评估内隐联想测验(P-VE-IAT)和暴力评估关系反应任务,以及三种自我报告的显性暴力态度测量方法和三种自我报告的暴力行为测量方法。更积极的内隐暴力态度与更积极的外显暴力态度相关(VE-IAT 和 P-VE-IAT;r = 0.18 至 0.22),更有可能使用暴力(VE-IAT;r = 0.18,P-VE-IAT;r = 0.16),以及更有可能使用暴力(VE-IAT;r = 0.16)。所有显性暴力态度和暴力行为的测量值之间都存在中度到高度的关联(r = 0.42 到 0.81)。此外,对暴力的内隐态度还能解释某些暴力结果的额外差异,而不仅仅是显性态度。我们的研究结果表明,某些反应时间测量指标的得分对于了解暴力发生的可能性和倾向性非常重要,尤其是在与显性态度测量指标相结合的情况下。
{"title":"Using reaction time procedures to assess implicit attitudes toward violence in a nonconvicted male sample","authors":"Sacha Maimone,&nbsp;Michael C. Seto,&nbsp;Adekunle G. Ahmed,&nbsp;Kevin L. Nunes","doi":"10.1002/ab.22168","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ab.22168","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In this study, we sought to capture implicit attitudes toward violence by administering response latency measures. We then examined their associations with explicit (e.g., assessed with self-report) attitudes toward violence and self-reported violent behavior in a combined sample of males from a Canadian university and males from the general community (<i>N</i> = 251; 156 students and 95 community members). To date, there have been mixed findings regarding these associations; some of this inconsistency may be due to the difficulty in accurately conceptualizing and assessing implicit attitudes toward violence. Therefore, we administered three response latency measures to assess this construct: a violence evaluation implicit association test (VE-IAT), a personalized VE-IAT (P-VE-IAT), and a violence evaluation relational responding task, along with three self-report measures of explicit attitudes toward violence and three self-report measures of violent behavior. More positive implicit attitudes toward violence were related to more positive explicit attitudes toward violence (for VE-IAT and P-VE-IAT; <i>r</i> = 0.18 to 0.22), greater likelihood of violence (for VE-IAT; <i>r</i> = 0.18 and for P-VE-IAT; <i>r</i> = 0.16), and greater propensity for violence (for the VE-IAT; <i>r</i> = 0.16). All measures of explicit attitudes toward violence and violent behavior were moderately to strongly associated with one another (<i>r</i> = 0.42 to 0.81). Furthermore, implicit attitudes toward violence explained additional variance in some violent outcomes above explicit attitudes alone. Our findings suggest that scores on certain reaction time measures are important for understanding likelihood and propensity for violence, especially when combined with explicit attitude measures.</p>","PeriodicalId":50842,"journal":{"name":"Aggressive Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ab.22168","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141789680","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The mediating role of hostile attribution bias in social exclusion affecting aggressive behavior 敌意归因偏差在社会排斥影响攻击行为中的中介作用。
IF 2.7 2区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-24 DOI: 10.1002/ab.22169
Fangying Quan, Jiayu Zhou, Yan Gou, Mengqiong Gui, Lu Wang, Shuyue Zhang

Aggression is one of the public social issues affecting campus harmony and stability, and social exclusion is an important interpersonal contextual factor among many factors affecting aggression. However, studies examining the influence of social exclusion on aggression and its mediating mechanism are not systematic enough. Based on the general aggression model (GAM), we intend to explore the role of hostile attribution bias (HAB) in both trait and state levels of social exclusion, which leads to aggression through a combination of questionnaire and experimental methods. Study 1 surveyed 388 current high school students (Mage = 16.09, SD = 1.01) and found that HAB mediates the relationship between long-term social exclusion (trait level) and aggression tendency. Study 2 experimented with 181 high school students (Mage = 16.95, SD = 1.13) to examine whether short-term social exclusion (state level) after initiating the Cyberball paradigm could still influence aggressive behavior through the mediating role of HAB. Results found that the mediating role of HAB still holds. The findings of the study further enrich the GAM and have important implications for a more targeted approach to aggression prevention and intervention.

攻击行为是影响校园和谐稳定的社会公共问题之一,而在影响攻击行为的诸多因素中,社会排斥是一个重要的人际环境因素。然而,有关社会排斥对攻击行为的影响及其中介机制的研究还不够系统。基于一般攻击模型(GAM),我们拟通过问卷调查和实验相结合的方法,探讨敌意归因偏差(HAB)在社会排斥的特质和状态水平上的作用,从而探讨社会排斥导致的攻击行为。研究 1 调查了 388 名在校高中生(Mage = 16.09,SD = 1.01),发现 HAB 在长期社会排斥(特质水平)与攻击倾向之间起中介作用。研究 2 以 181 名高中生(Mage = 16.95,SD = 1.13)为实验对象,考察了启动网络球范式后的短期社会排斥(状态水平)是否仍能通过 HAB 的中介作用影响攻击行为。结果发现,HAB 的中介作用仍然有效。研究结果进一步丰富了 GAM 的内容,对更有针对性地预防和干预攻击行为具有重要意义。
{"title":"The mediating role of hostile attribution bias in social exclusion affecting aggressive behavior","authors":"Fangying Quan,&nbsp;Jiayu Zhou,&nbsp;Yan Gou,&nbsp;Mengqiong Gui,&nbsp;Lu Wang,&nbsp;Shuyue Zhang","doi":"10.1002/ab.22169","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ab.22169","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Aggression is one of the public social issues affecting campus harmony and stability, and social exclusion is an important interpersonal contextual factor among many factors affecting aggression. However, studies examining the influence of social exclusion on aggression and its mediating mechanism are not systematic enough. Based on the general aggression model (GAM), we intend to explore the role of hostile attribution bias (HAB) in both trait and state levels of social exclusion, which leads to aggression through a combination of questionnaire and experimental methods. Study 1 surveyed 388 current high school students (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 16.09, SD = 1.01) and found that HAB mediates the relationship between long-term social exclusion (trait level) and aggression tendency. Study 2 experimented with 181 high school students (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 16.95, SD = 1.13) to examine whether short-term social exclusion (state level) after initiating the Cyberball paradigm could still influence aggressive behavior through the mediating role of HAB. Results found that the mediating role of HAB still holds. The findings of the study further enrich the GAM and have important implications for a more targeted approach to aggression prevention and intervention.</p>","PeriodicalId":50842,"journal":{"name":"Aggressive Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141762398","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bullying victimization and bullying perpetration, social anxiety, and social withdrawal in Portuguese adolescents: A reciprocal association model 葡萄牙青少年中的欺凌受害者和欺凌行为、社交焦虑和社交退缩:相互关联模型
IF 2.7 2区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-21 DOI: 10.1002/ab.22170
Mariana Sousa, Sara Cruz, Richard Inman, Marta Marchante, Vítor Alexandre Coelho

Further research is needed to clarify the association of the different forms of bullying with social anxiety and social withdrawal over time in adolescents. This two-wave panel study with a 1-year time lag (October 2021–October 2022) examined the cross-lagged relationships between bullying victimization and bullying perpetration, social anxiety (i.e., fear or distress in social situations), and social withdrawal (i.e., consistent, and deliberate social solitude). Participants were 485 middle school students (234 girls) attending the seventh or eighth grade at Time 1 (T1) (Mage = 12.67 years, SD = 1.14 years). Social anxiety and social withdrawal were assessed using subscales of the Social and Emotional Competencies Evaluation Questionnaire. Bullying perpetration and bullying victimization were assessed using the Bullying and Cyberbullying Behavior Questionnaire–Short Form. The within-wave associations between the study variables were similar at T1 and Time 2 (T2), with the exception that the association between bullying perpetration and social anxiety was much weaker at T1 than at T2. The results of the path analysis showed that T1 bullying perpetration predicted T2 social anxiety, and that T1 bullying victimization predicted T2 social withdrawal. We also found a reciprocal relationship between social anxiety and social withdrawal. These findings highlight the importance of preventive and remediation interventions to reduce social anxiety in adolescents who engage in and experience bullying behavior.

需要进一步开展研究,以明确不同形式的欺凌行为与青少年社交焦虑和社交退缩之间的关系。这项时滞为 1 年(2021 年 10 月至 2022 年 10 月)的两波面板研究考察了欺凌受害与欺凌实施、社交焦虑(即社交场合中的恐惧或痛苦)和社交退缩(即持续、刻意的社交孤独)之间的跨时滞关系。研究对象为时间1(T1)就读于七年级或八年级的485名初中生(234名女生)(年龄=12.67岁,平均年龄=1.14岁)。社交焦虑和社交退缩通过社会和情感能力评估问卷的分量表进行评估。欺凌和网络欺凌行为问卷简表对欺凌行为和欺凌受害情况进行了评估。研究变量之间的波内关联在时间 1 和时间 2(T2)相似,但欺凌行为与社交焦虑之间的关联在时间 1 比在时间 2 弱得多。路径分析结果显示,第一阶段的欺凌行为预示着第二阶段的社交焦虑,而第一阶段的欺凌受害行为预示着第二阶段的社交退缩。我们还发现,社交焦虑和社交退缩之间存在互为因果的关系。这些研究结果凸显了预防和补救干预措施的重要性,这些干预措施可以减少有欺凌行为和经历过欺凌行为的青少年的社交焦虑。
{"title":"Bullying victimization and bullying perpetration, social anxiety, and social withdrawal in Portuguese adolescents: A reciprocal association model","authors":"Mariana Sousa,&nbsp;Sara Cruz,&nbsp;Richard Inman,&nbsp;Marta Marchante,&nbsp;Vítor Alexandre Coelho","doi":"10.1002/ab.22170","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ab.22170","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Further research is needed to clarify the association of the different forms of bullying with social anxiety and social withdrawal over time in adolescents. This two-wave panel study with a 1-year time lag (October 2021–October 2022) examined the cross-lagged relationships between bullying victimization and bullying perpetration, social anxiety (i.e., fear or distress in social situations), and social withdrawal (i.e., consistent, and deliberate social solitude). Participants were 485 middle school students (234 girls) attending the seventh or eighth grade at Time 1 (T1) (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 12.67 years, SD = 1.14 years). Social anxiety and social withdrawal were assessed using subscales of the Social and Emotional Competencies Evaluation Questionnaire. Bullying perpetration and bullying victimization were assessed using the Bullying and Cyberbullying Behavior Questionnaire–Short Form. The within-wave associations between the study variables were similar at T1 and Time 2 (T2), with the exception that the association between bullying perpetration and social anxiety was much weaker at T1 than at T2. The results of the path analysis showed that T1 bullying perpetration predicted T2 social anxiety, and that T1 bullying victimization predicted T2 social withdrawal. We also found a reciprocal relationship between social anxiety and social withdrawal. These findings highlight the importance of preventive and remediation interventions to reduce social anxiety in adolescents who engage in and experience bullying behavior.</p>","PeriodicalId":50842,"journal":{"name":"Aggressive Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141735568","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Population variation in signaling behavior and contest outcome in the jacky dragon 杰克龙信号行为和竞赛结果的种群变异
IF 2.7 2区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.1002/ab.22166
Marco D. Barquero

Being aggressive and by extension, dominant, is an important mechanism for determining access to resources such as mates or territories. While predictors of contest outcome and dominance are increasingly studied, we have a poor understanding of how they vary across populations. Here, I use the widely distributed Australian agamid lizard, the Jacky dragon (Amphibolurus muricatus), to quantify variation in features predicting contest outcome among males of different populations. I measured physical attributes, maximal physiological performance capacity (sprint speed, endurance, bite force) and visual displays during staged encounters. I found that morphology, performance capacity and the type and frequency of visual displays used during agonistic interactions varied significantly across populations. Contest winners from the Cann River State Forest population favored tail-flicks and push-up/body-rocks, while those from Royal National Park were more likely to chase and individuals from Yarratt State Forest performed more bite-lunges than other populations. The losers of contests also differed in their displays. Individuals from the Cann River population were dominant over the others based on behavioral attributes (i.e., aggressive visual displays, chases and bite-lunges). I suggest that population differences in signal form and function could have implications for range dynamics as populations come into contact in an era of rapid environmental change.

具有攻击性,进而具有优势,是决定能否获得配偶或领地等资源的重要机制。尽管对竞争结果和支配地位的预测因素的研究越来越多,但我们对它们在不同种群间的差异却知之甚少。在这里,我利用分布广泛的澳大利亚巨蜥杰克龙(Amphibolurus muricatus)来量化不同种群雄性之间预测竞争结果特征的差异。我测量了雄性蜥蜴的身体特征、最大生理表现能力(冲刺速度、耐力、咬合力)以及在阶段性交锋中的视觉表现。我发现,不同种群的雄性在形态、表现能力以及在激战互动中使用的视觉表现类型和频率上存在显著差异。坎河州立森林种群中的竞赛获胜者更喜欢甩尾和俯卧撑/身体撞击,而皇家国家公园的获胜者更喜欢追逐,亚拉特州立森林的个体则比其他种群表现出更多的撕咬。比赛失败者的表现也各不相同。根据行为特征(即攻击性视觉表现、追逐和咬拖),坎河种群的个体比其他种群的个体占优势。我认为,在环境快速变化的时代,随着种群的接触,种群在信号形式和功能上的差异可能会对范围动态产生影响。
{"title":"Population variation in signaling behavior and contest outcome in the jacky dragon","authors":"Marco D. Barquero","doi":"10.1002/ab.22166","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.22166","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Being aggressive and by extension, dominant, is an important mechanism for determining access to resources such as mates or territories. While predictors of contest outcome and dominance are increasingly studied, we have a poor understanding of how they vary across populations. Here, I use the widely distributed Australian agamid lizard, the Jacky dragon (<i>Amphibolurus muricatus</i>), to quantify variation in features predicting contest outcome among males of different populations. I measured physical attributes, maximal physiological performance capacity (sprint speed, endurance, bite force) and visual displays during staged encounters. I found that morphology, performance capacity and the type and frequency of visual displays used during agonistic interactions varied significantly across populations. Contest winners from the Cann River State Forest population favored tail-flicks and push-up/body-rocks, while those from Royal National Park were more likely to chase and individuals from Yarratt State Forest performed more bite-lunges than other populations. The losers of contests also differed in their displays. Individuals from the Cann River population were dominant over the others based on behavioral attributes (i.e., aggressive visual displays, chases and bite-lunges). I suggest that population differences in signal form and function could have implications for range dynamics as populations come into contact in an era of rapid environmental change.</p>","PeriodicalId":50842,"journal":{"name":"Aggressive Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141730038","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Aggressiveness, inhibitory control, and emotional states: A provocation paradigm 攻击性、抑制控制和情绪状态:挑衅范式
IF 2.7 2区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-14 DOI: 10.1002/ab.22165
Ivan Sebalo, Melanie Bozzay, Edelyn Verona, Simon Chu, Jane L. Ireland

The current study examines the effects of trait aggressiveness, inhibitory control and emotional states on aggressive behavior in a laboratory paradigm. One hundred and fifty-one adult participants took part (73 men, 71 women, and 7 nondisclosed). Event Related Potentials (ERPs) during a Go/No-Go task were utilized to capture the extent of inhibitory processing, with a laboratory provocation paradigm used to assess aggression. Contrary to the expectations, negative affective responses to provocation were negatively associated only with short-lived aggression and only among those with high past aggressiveness. Furthermore, past aggressiveness was related to a continuous increase in laboratory aggressive behavior regardless of the level of inhibitory control (P3 difference amplitude). However, feeling hostile was associated with short-lived aggressive behavior, only in those with lower levels of inhibitory control. These findings demonstrate the effect of distinct mechanisms on different patterns of aggressive behavior.

本研究在实验室范例中考察了特质攻击性、抑制控制和情绪状态对攻击行为的影响。151 名成年参与者(73 名男性、71 名女性和 7 名未公开身份者)参加了研究。研究人员利用 "去/不去 "任务中的事件相关电位(ERPs)来捕捉抑制处理的程度,并利用实验室激怒范式来评估攻击行为。与预期相反,对挑衅的消极情绪反应只与短暂的攻击行为呈负相关,而且只与那些过去具有高度攻击性的人呈负相关。此外,无论抑制控制水平如何(P3 差幅),过去的攻击性都与实验室攻击行为的持续增加有关。然而,只有抑制控制水平较低的人,敌对感才与短暂的攻击行为有关。这些发现表明,不同的机制会对不同的攻击行为模式产生影响。
{"title":"Aggressiveness, inhibitory control, and emotional states: A provocation paradigm","authors":"Ivan Sebalo,&nbsp;Melanie Bozzay,&nbsp;Edelyn Verona,&nbsp;Simon Chu,&nbsp;Jane L. Ireland","doi":"10.1002/ab.22165","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ab.22165","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The current study examines the effects of trait aggressiveness, inhibitory control and emotional states on aggressive behavior in a laboratory paradigm. One hundred and fifty-one adult participants took part (73 men, 71 women, and 7 nondisclosed). Event Related Potentials (ERPs) during a Go/No-Go task were utilized to capture the extent of inhibitory processing, with a laboratory provocation paradigm used to assess aggression. Contrary to the expectations, negative affective responses to provocation were negatively associated only with short-lived aggression and only among those with high past aggressiveness. Furthermore, past aggressiveness was related to a continuous increase in laboratory aggressive behavior regardless of the level of inhibitory control (P3 difference amplitude). However, feeling hostile was associated with short-lived aggressive behavior, only in those with lower levels of inhibitory control. These findings demonstrate the effect of distinct mechanisms on different patterns of aggressive behavior.</p>","PeriodicalId":50842,"journal":{"name":"Aggressive Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/ab.22165","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141617515","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Understanding the moral cognition of aggression: Longitudinal serial effects of personal relative deprivation and hostility on within-person changes in civic moral disengagement 了解攻击行为的道德认知:个人相对贫困和敌意对公民道德疏离的个人内部变化的纵向序列效应。
IF 2.7 2区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-03 DOI: 10.1002/ab.22164
Xiong Li, Zuo-Shan Li, Ling-Xiang Xia

Moral disengagement is an important aggressive and moral cognition. The mechanisms of changes in moral disengagement remain unclear, especially at the within-person level. We attempted to clarify this by exploring the serial effects of personal relative deprivation and hostility on civic moral disengagement. We conducted a three-wave longitudinal survey with 1058 undergraduates (63.61% women; mean age = 20.97). The results of the random intercept cross-lagged panel model showed that personal relative deprivation at Wave 1 and hostility at Wave 2 formed a serial effect on the within-person changes in civic moral disengagement at Wave 3, and the longitudinal indirect effect test showed that the within-person dynamics in hostility at Wave 2 acted as a mediator. The results of multiple group analysis across genders further showed that the longitudinal indirect role of hostility at Wave 2 was only observed for men, but not for women, which indicates the moderating effect of gender. These findings facilitate an understanding of the mechanisms of aggressive cognitions at the within-person level and offer implications for the prevention and intervention of aggression from the perspective of moral cognition.

道德脱离是一种重要的攻击性道德认知。道德脱离的变化机制仍不清楚,尤其是在个人层面上。我们试图通过探讨个人相对贫困和敌意对公民道德脱离的序列效应来澄清这一点。我们对 1058 名大学生(63.61% 为女性,平均年龄为 20.97 岁)进行了三波纵向调查。随机截距交叉滞后面板模型的结果表明,第 1 波的个人相对贫困和第 2 波的敌意对第 3 波的公民道德脱离的人内变化形成了序列效应,纵向间接效应检验表明,第 2 波的敌意的人内动态变化起到了中介作用。跨性别多组分析的结果进一步表明,敌意在第 2 波的纵向间接作用只在男性中观察到,而在女性中没有观察到,这表明性别具有调节作用。这些研究结果有助于了解人体内攻击性认知的机制,并从道德认知的角度为预防和干预攻击行为提供了启示。
{"title":"Understanding the moral cognition of aggression: Longitudinal serial effects of personal relative deprivation and hostility on within-person changes in civic moral disengagement","authors":"Xiong Li,&nbsp;Zuo-Shan Li,&nbsp;Ling-Xiang Xia","doi":"10.1002/ab.22164","DOIUrl":"10.1002/ab.22164","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Moral disengagement is an important aggressive and moral cognition. The mechanisms of changes in moral disengagement remain unclear, especially at the within-person level. We attempted to clarify this by exploring the serial effects of personal relative deprivation and hostility on civic moral disengagement. We conducted a three-wave longitudinal survey with 1058 undergraduates (63.61% women; mean age = 20.97). The results of the random intercept cross-lagged panel model showed that personal relative deprivation at Wave 1 and hostility at Wave 2 formed a serial effect on the within-person changes in civic moral disengagement at Wave 3, and the longitudinal indirect effect test showed that the within-person dynamics in hostility at Wave 2 acted as a mediator. The results of multiple group analysis across genders further showed that the longitudinal indirect role of hostility at Wave 2 was only observed for men, but not for women, which indicates the moderating effect of gender. These findings facilitate an understanding of the mechanisms of aggressive cognitions at the within-person level and offer implications for the prevention and intervention of aggression from the perspective of moral cognition.</p>","PeriodicalId":50842,"journal":{"name":"Aggressive Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141494157","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Aggressive Behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1