首页 > 最新文献

Language Learning最新文献

英文 中文
Syntactic Adaptation and Word Learning in 3‐ to 4‐Year‐Olds 3 至 4 岁儿童的句法适应和单词学习
IF 4.4 1区 文学 Q1 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH Pub Date : 2024-07-24 DOI: 10.1111/lang.12661
Yukun Yu, Naomi Havron, Cynthia Fisher
In a recent study, preschoolers adapted their syntactic expectations about a familiar phrase in French; this adaptation affected later word learning. In two experiments, we probed the generality of this finding by replicating the experiment and extending it to a different expression in English. We examined the ambiguous phrase the baby, which can be followed by nouns (the baby monkeys) or verbs (the baby sleeps). In induction trials, the baby consistently preceded either familiar nouns (noun condition) or verbs (verb condition). In later novel‐word trials, children in the verb condition were more likely to interpret novel words following the baby (The baby gorps!) as verbs than were children in the noun condition. In Experiment 2, a modified design isolated the effect of experience with the critical phrase from possible effects of task structure, and an added baseline condition showed the adaptation effect to be asymmetrical, suggesting frequency or surprisal effects on adaptation.
在最近的一项研究中,学龄前儿童调整了他们对一个熟悉的法语短语的句法预期;这种调整影响了他们后来的单词学习。在两个实验中,我们通过复制实验并将其扩展到不同的英语表达来探究这一发现的普遍性。我们研究了the baby这个模棱两可的短语,它的后面可以是名词(the baby monkeys)或动词(the baby sleeps)。在诱导试验中,baby 总是出现在熟悉的名词(名词条件)或动词(动词条件)之前。在后来的新词试验中,与名词条件下的儿童相比,动词条件下的儿童更有可能将婴儿后面的新词(The baby gorps!)解释为动词。在实验 2 中,经过修改的设计将关键短语经验的影响与任务结构可能产生的影响隔离开来,并增加了一个基线条件,结果显示适应效应是不对称的,这表明频率或意外对适应有影响。
{"title":"Syntactic Adaptation and Word Learning in 3‐ to 4‐Year‐Olds","authors":"Yukun Yu, Naomi Havron, Cynthia Fisher","doi":"10.1111/lang.12661","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12661","url":null,"abstract":"In a recent study, preschoolers adapted their syntactic expectations about a familiar phrase in French; this adaptation affected later word learning. In two experiments, we probed the generality of this finding by replicating the experiment and extending it to a different expression in English. We examined the ambiguous phrase <jats:italic>the baby</jats:italic>, which can be followed by nouns (<jats:italic>the baby monkeys</jats:italic>) or verbs (<jats:italic>the baby sleeps</jats:italic>). In induction trials, <jats:italic>the baby</jats:italic> consistently preceded either familiar nouns (noun condition) or verbs (verb condition). In later novel‐word trials, children in the verb condition were more likely to interpret novel words following <jats:italic>the baby</jats:italic> (<jats:italic>The baby gorps!</jats:italic>) as verbs than were children in the noun condition. In Experiment 2, a modified design isolated the effect of experience with the critical phrase from possible effects of task structure, and an added baseline condition showed the adaptation effect to be asymmetrical, suggesting frequency or surprisal effects on adaptation.","PeriodicalId":51371,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning","volume":"06 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.4,"publicationDate":"2024-07-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141755153","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Learning Unacceptability: Repeated Exposure to Acceptable Sentences Improves Adult Learners’ Recognition of Unacceptable Sentences 学习不可接受性:反复接触可接受句子可提高成人学习者对不可接受句子的识别能力
IF 4.4 1区 文学 Q1 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH Pub Date : 2024-06-21 DOI: 10.1111/lang.12660
Karina Tachihara, Adele E. Goldberg
Adults learning a new language tend to judge unconventional utterances more leniently than fluent speakers do; ratings on acceptable utterances, however, tend to align more closely with fluent speakers. This asymmetry raises a question as to whether unconventional utterances can be statistically preempted by conventional utterances for adult learners. We report a preregistered study that provided undergraduates in Spanish classes with three days of exposure to conventional Spanish sentences without feedback. Judgment data reveal a significant effect of statistical preemption, particularly on intermediate learners, as predicted: Repeatedly witnessing conventional sentences led learners to subsequently judge as significantly lower the corresponding unconventional formulations in comparison to unrelated unconventional sentences. Current findings indicate that adult learners can take advantage of statistical preemption to learn the unacceptability of unconventional sentences from repeated exposure to acceptable alternatives, without explicit instruction or feedback.
学习新语言的成人对非常规语篇的评判往往比讲流利语言的人更宽松;而对可接受语篇的评判往往与讲流利语言的人更接近。这种不对称现象提出了一个问题:对于成人学习者来说,非常规语篇是否能在统计学上被常规语篇所取代。我们报告了一项预先登记的研究,该研究让西班牙语班的本科生在没有反馈的情况下接触了三天常规西班牙语句子。判断数据显示,统计抢先效应效果显著,尤其是对中级学习者,正如所预测的那样:与不相关的非常规句子相比,反复看到常规句子会导致学习者随后判断相应的非常规句子的表达能力明显降低。目前的研究结果表明,成人学习者可以利用统计抢占的优势,在没有明确指导或反馈的情况下,通过反复接触可接受的替代句子来学习非常规句子的不可接受性。
{"title":"Learning Unacceptability: Repeated Exposure to Acceptable Sentences Improves Adult Learners’ Recognition of Unacceptable Sentences","authors":"Karina Tachihara, Adele E. Goldberg","doi":"10.1111/lang.12660","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12660","url":null,"abstract":"Adults learning a new language tend to judge unconventional utterances more leniently than fluent speakers do; ratings on acceptable utterances, however, tend to align more closely with fluent speakers. This asymmetry raises a question as to whether unconventional utterances can be statistically preempted by conventional utterances for adult learners. We report a preregistered study that provided undergraduates in Spanish classes with three days of exposure to conventional Spanish sentences without feedback. Judgment data reveal a significant effect of statistical preemption, particularly on intermediate learners, as predicted: Repeatedly witnessing conventional sentences led learners to subsequently judge as significantly lower the corresponding unconventional formulations in comparison to unrelated unconventional sentences. Current findings indicate that adult learners can take advantage of statistical preemption to learn the unacceptability of unconventional sentences from repeated exposure to acceptable alternatives, without explicit instruction or feedback.","PeriodicalId":51371,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning","volume":"20 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.4,"publicationDate":"2024-06-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141448155","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Verbal Symbols Support Concrete but Enable Abstract Concept Formation: Evidence From Brain-Constrained Deep Neural Networks 语言符号支持具体概念的形成,但也能促成抽象概念的形成:来自大脑约束深度神经网络的证据
IF 4.4 1区 文学 Q1 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH Pub Date : 2024-05-19 DOI: 10.1111/lang.12646
Fynn R. Dobler, Malte R. Henningsen-Schomers, Friedemann Pulvermüller

Concrete symbols (e.g., sun, run) can be learned in the context of objects and actions, thereby grounding their meaning in the world. However, it is controversial whether a comparable avenue to semantic learning exists for abstract symbols (e.g., democracy). When we simulated the putative brain mechanisms of conceptual/semantic grounding using brain-constrained deep neural networks, the learning of instances of concrete concepts outside of language contexts led to robust neural circuits generating substantial and prolonged activations. In contrast, the learning of instances of abstract concepts yielded much reduced and only short-lived activity. Crucially, when conceptual instances were learned in the context of wordforms, circuit activations became robust and long-lasting for both concrete and abstract meanings. These results indicate that, although the neural correlates of concrete conceptual representations can be built from grounding experiences alone, abstract concept formation at the neurobiological level is enabled by and requires the correlated presence of linguistic forms.

具体符号(如太阳、奔跑)可以在物体和行动的背景下学习,从而使其意义立足于世界。然而,对于抽象符号(如民主)是否存在类似的语义学习途径还存在争议。当我们使用脑约束深度神经网络模拟概念/语义基础的假定大脑机制时,在语言环境之外学习具体概念的实例会导致神经回路产生大量和长时间的激活。与此相反,学习抽象概念实例时,神经活动大大减少,而且只有短暂的活动。最重要的是,当在词形语境中学习概念实例时,无论是具体含义还是抽象含义,神经回路的激活都变得稳健而持久。这些结果表明,虽然具体概念表征的神经相关性可以仅从基础经验中建立,但抽象概念在神经生物学层面的形成是由语言形式促成的,并且需要语言形式的相关存在。
{"title":"Verbal Symbols Support Concrete but Enable Abstract Concept Formation: Evidence From Brain-Constrained Deep Neural Networks","authors":"Fynn R. Dobler,&nbsp;Malte R. Henningsen-Schomers,&nbsp;Friedemann Pulvermüller","doi":"10.1111/lang.12646","DOIUrl":"10.1111/lang.12646","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Concrete symbols (e.g., <i>sun</i>, <i>run</i>) can be learned in the context of objects and actions, thereby grounding their meaning in the world. However, it is controversial whether a comparable avenue to semantic learning exists for abstract symbols (e.g., <i>democracy</i>). When we simulated the putative brain mechanisms of conceptual/semantic grounding using brain-constrained deep neural networks, the learning of instances of concrete concepts outside of language contexts led to robust neural circuits generating substantial and prolonged activations. In contrast, the learning of instances of abstract concepts yielded much reduced and only short-lived activity. Crucially, when conceptual instances were learned in the context of wordforms, circuit activations became robust and long-lasting for both concrete and abstract meanings. These results indicate that, although the neural correlates of concrete conceptual representations can be built from grounding experiences alone, abstract concept formation at the neurobiological level is enabled by and requires the correlated presence of linguistic forms.</p>","PeriodicalId":51371,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning","volume":"74 S1","pages":"258-295"},"PeriodicalIF":4.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/lang.12646","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141074289","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do Implicit Learning Deficit and Dyslexia Go Together? An fMRI and Behavioral Study 内隐学习缺陷与阅读障碍是否同时存在?一项 fMRI 和行为研究
IF 4.4 1区 文学 Q1 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH Pub Date : 2024-05-13 DOI: 10.1111/lang.12652
Marta Wójcik, Joanna Beck, Katarzyna Chyl, Agnieszka Dynak, Gabriela Dzięgiel‐Fivet, Magdalena Łuniewska, Anna Grabowska, Katarzyna Jednoróg, Agnieszka Dębska
What is the relationship between literacy skills and implicit learning? To address previous mixed findings, we compared school‐aged readers, typical (CON, n = 54) and with dyslexia (DYS, n = 53), in relation to their performance on a serial reaction time task. For the first time, we also included an isolated spelling deficit group (ISD, n = 30) to control for distinctive effects of reading and spelling deficits. A linear reaction times analysis did not reveal between‐group differences in implicit learning. However, further examination revealed that most CON (65%) and ISD (63%) were implicit learners, whereas most DYS were nonlearners (64%). Brain activity showed differences in early learning phases: CON learners and DYS nonlearners activated the left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and left insula more than other groups. Our findings imply that implicit learning is more frequently disrupted in children with dyslexia than in typical readers, and that activation of the left IFG and insula contributes to effective learning in the latter group but it does not in the former.
读写能力与内隐学习之间有什么关系?为了解决以往研究结果参差不齐的问题,我们比较了学龄期典型阅读者(CON,n = 54)和阅读障碍者(DYS,n = 53)在连续反应时间任务中的表现。我们还首次加入了一个单独的拼写缺陷组(ISD,n = 30),以控制阅读和拼写缺陷的不同影响。通过线性反应时间分析,我们并未发现组间内隐学习的差异。然而,进一步的检查发现,大多数 CON 组(65%)和 ISD 组(63%)是内隐学习者,而大多数 DYS 组则是非学习者(64%)。大脑活动显示出早期学习阶段的差异:与其他组别相比,CON 学习者和 DYS 非学习者激活左侧额叶下回(IFG)和左侧岛叶的程度更高。我们的研究结果表明,阅读障碍儿童的内隐学习比典型阅读障碍儿童更容易受到干扰,左侧额下回和脑岛的激活有助于后者的有效学习,但对前者却没有帮助。
{"title":"Do Implicit Learning Deficit and Dyslexia Go Together? An fMRI and Behavioral Study","authors":"Marta Wójcik, Joanna Beck, Katarzyna Chyl, Agnieszka Dynak, Gabriela Dzięgiel‐Fivet, Magdalena Łuniewska, Anna Grabowska, Katarzyna Jednoróg, Agnieszka Dębska","doi":"10.1111/lang.12652","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12652","url":null,"abstract":"What is the relationship between literacy skills and implicit learning? To address previous mixed findings, we compared school‐aged readers, typical (CON, <jats:italic>n</jats:italic> = 54) and with dyslexia (DYS, <jats:italic>n</jats:italic> = 53), in relation to their performance on a serial reaction time task. For the first time, we also included an isolated spelling deficit group (ISD, <jats:italic>n</jats:italic> = 30) to control for distinctive effects of reading and spelling deficits. A linear reaction times analysis did not reveal between‐group differences in implicit learning. However, further examination revealed that most CON (65%) and ISD (63%) were implicit learners, whereas most DYS were nonlearners (64%). Brain activity showed differences in early learning phases: CON learners and DYS nonlearners activated the left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and left insula more than other groups. Our findings imply that implicit learning is more frequently disrupted in children with dyslexia than in typical readers, and that activation of the left IFG and insula contributes to effective learning in the latter group but it does not in the former.","PeriodicalId":51371,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning","volume":"60 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140919859","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Electrophysiological Evidence for a Whorfian Double Dissociation of Categorical Perception Across Two Languages 两种语言中分类感知的沃尔夫双重分离的电生理学证据
IF 4.4 1区 文学 Q1 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH Pub Date : 2024-05-10 DOI: 10.1111/lang.12648
Aina Casaponsa, M. Acebo García-Guerrero, Alejandro Martínez, Natalia Ojeda, Guillaume Thierry, Panos Athanasopoulos

Taza in Spanish refers to cups and mugs in English, whereas glass refers to different glass types in Spanish: copa and vaso. It is still unclear whether such categorical distinctions induce early perceptual differences in speakers of different languages. In this study, for the first time, we report symmetrical effects of terminology on preattentive indices of categorical perception across languages. Native speakers of English or Spanish saw arrays of cups, mugs, copas, and vasos flashed in streams. Visual mismatch negativity, an implicit electrophysiological correlate of perceptual change in the peripheral visual field, was modulated for categorical contrasts marked in the participants’ native language but not for objects designated by the same label. Conversely, P3a, an index of attentional orienting, was modulated only for missing contrasts in the participants’ native language. Thus, whereas native labels influenced participants’ preattentive perceptual encoding of objects, nonverbally encoded dissociations reoriented their attention at a later processing stage.

西班牙语中的 Taza 指的是英语中的杯子和马克杯,而 glass 指的是西班牙语中的不同玻璃类型:copa 和 vaso。目前还不清楚这种分类区别是否会引起不同语言使用者的早期知觉差异。在本研究中,我们首次报告了术语对不同语言分类感知前注意指数的对称影响。以英语或西班牙语为母语的人看到的是在流中闪现的杯子、马克杯、copas 和 vasos 阵列。视觉错配负性是外围视野中知觉变化的一种隐性电生理相关性,它对用参与者母语标记的分类对比有调节作用,但对用相同标签指定的物体则没有。相反,作为注意定向指标的 P3a 仅对参与者母语中的缺失对比有调节作用。因此,母语标签会影响受试者对物体的前注意知觉编码,而非语言编码的差异则会在较晚的处理阶段调整受试者的注意方向。
{"title":"Electrophysiological Evidence for a Whorfian Double Dissociation of Categorical Perception Across Two Languages","authors":"Aina Casaponsa,&nbsp;M. Acebo García-Guerrero,&nbsp;Alejandro Martínez,&nbsp;Natalia Ojeda,&nbsp;Guillaume Thierry,&nbsp;Panos Athanasopoulos","doi":"10.1111/lang.12648","DOIUrl":"10.1111/lang.12648","url":null,"abstract":"<p><i>Taza</i> in Spanish refers to cups and mugs in English, whereas glass refers to different glass types in Spanish: <i>copa</i> and <i>vaso</i>. It is still unclear whether such categorical distinctions induce early perceptual differences in speakers of different languages. In this study, for the first time, we report symmetrical effects of terminology on preattentive indices of categorical perception across languages. Native speakers of English or Spanish saw arrays of cups, mugs, <i>copas</i>, and <i>vasos</i> flashed in streams. Visual mismatch negativity, an implicit electrophysiological correlate of perceptual change in the peripheral visual field, was modulated for categorical contrasts marked in the participants’ native language but not for objects designated by the same label. Conversely, P3a, an index of attentional orienting, was modulated only for missing contrasts in the participants’ native language. Thus, whereas native labels influenced participants’ preattentive perceptual encoding of objects, nonverbally encoded dissociations reoriented their attention at a later processing stage.</p>","PeriodicalId":51371,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning","volume":"74 S1","pages":"136-156"},"PeriodicalIF":4.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/lang.12648","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140910651","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sensitivity to Subphonemic Differences in First Language Predicts Vocabulary Size in a Foreign Language 对母语副音位差异的敏感性可预测外语词汇量的大小
IF 4.4 1区 文学 Q1 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH Pub Date : 2024-05-07 DOI: 10.1111/lang.12650
Efthymia C. Kapnoula, Arthur G. Samuel
Some listeners exhibit higher sensitivity to subphonemic acoustic differences (i.e., higher speech gradiency). Here, we asked whether higher gradiency in a listener's first language (L1) facilitates foreign language learning and explored the possible sources of individual differences in L1 gradiency. To address these questions, we tested 164 native Spanish speakers with different linguistic profiles. Speech gradiency was assessed via a Visual Analogue Scale task, and foreign language proficiency was assessed via an English vocabulary test. Possible sources of gradiency included domain‐general auditory acuity, overall exposure to spoken language (indexed by age), and exposure to phonological diversity. Control measures were collected to account for variables such as phoneme categorization consistency, working memory, and musical training. The results revealed a positive link between L1 speech gradiency and vocabulary acquisition in a foreign language over and above all other variables. L1 speech gradiency itself was predicted by domain‐general auditory acuity and overall exposure to spoken language.
有些听者对次音位声学差异表现出更高的敏感度(即更高的语音渐变)。在此,我们询问听者第一语言(L1)中较高的渐变性是否有助于外语学习,并探讨了 L1 渐变性个体差异的可能来源。为了解决这些问题,我们对 164 名母语为西班牙语的不同语言背景的人进行了测试。语音渐变通过视觉模拟量表任务进行评估,外语水平通过英语词汇测试进行评估。梯度的可能来源包括领域总体听觉敏锐度、对口语的总体接触(以年龄为指标)以及对语音多样性的接触。此外,还收集了控制测量,以考虑音素分类一致性、工作记忆和音乐训练等变量。研究结果表明,在所有其他变量之上,L1语音渐变与外语词汇掌握之间存在正向联系。L1 语音渐变性本身受领域总体听觉敏锐度和口语总体接触程度的影响。
{"title":"Sensitivity to Subphonemic Differences in First Language Predicts Vocabulary Size in a Foreign Language","authors":"Efthymia C. Kapnoula, Arthur G. Samuel","doi":"10.1111/lang.12650","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12650","url":null,"abstract":"Some listeners exhibit higher sensitivity to subphonemic acoustic differences (i.e., higher speech gradiency). Here, we asked whether higher gradiency in a listener's first language (L1) facilitates foreign language learning and explored the possible sources of individual differences in L1 gradiency. To address these questions, we tested 164 native Spanish speakers with different linguistic profiles. Speech gradiency was assessed via a Visual Analogue Scale task, and foreign language proficiency was assessed via an English vocabulary test. Possible sources of gradiency included domain‐general auditory acuity, overall exposure to spoken language (indexed by age), and exposure to phonological diversity. Control measures were collected to account for variables such as phoneme categorization consistency, working memory, and musical training. The results revealed a positive link between L1 speech gradiency and vocabulary acquisition in a foreign language over and above all other variables. L1 speech gradiency itself was predicted by domain‐general auditory acuity and overall exposure to spoken language.","PeriodicalId":51371,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning","volume":"16 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140890406","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Effect of COVID-Related Quarantine and Attitudes on Time Conceptualization: Evidence From Temporal Focus and Implicit Space-Time Mappings 与 COVID 相关的隔离和态度对时间概念化的影响:时空聚焦和内隐时空映射的证据
IF 4.4 1区 文学 Q1 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH Pub Date : 2024-05-06 DOI: 10.1111/lang.12649
Panos Athanasopoulos, Rui Su

The temporal focus hypothesis (TFH) entails that individuals who value the past tend to conceptualize it in front, whereas individuals who value the future tend to map the future in front instead (de la Fuente et al., 2014). This varies as a function of culture, individual differences, and context. Here, we extend this line of inquiry by testing a contextual variable, namely COVID-19 quarantine status, and an individual differences variable, namely future precautionary behavior towards COVID-19. Contrary to what the TFH would predict, we show that participants map the future to a frontal position, regardless of individual attitudes and quarantine status. However, participants who displayed more future precautionary behavior were also more future-focused than participants who displayed less such behaviour, but this did not predict their front–back mappings of the future. These findings suggest that individual differences may be stronger determinants of temporal focus than contextual variables.

时间焦点假说(TFH)认为,重视过去的个体倾向于将过去概念化在前面,而重视未来的个体则倾向于将未来映射在前面(de la Fuente 等人,2014 年)。这种情况因文化、个体差异和环境而异。在此,我们通过测试背景变量(即 COVID-19 检疫状态)和个体差异变量(即对 COVID-19 的未来预防行为)来扩展这一研究思路。与 TFH 预测的结果相反,我们发现,无论个人态度和检疫状况如何,参与者都会将未来映射到正面位置。然而,表现出更多未来预防行为的参与者也比表现出较少此类行为的参与者更关注未来,但这并不能预测他们对未来的前后映射。这些发现表明,个体差异可能比环境变量更能决定时间焦点。
{"title":"The Effect of COVID-Related Quarantine and Attitudes on Time Conceptualization: Evidence From Temporal Focus and Implicit Space-Time Mappings","authors":"Panos Athanasopoulos,&nbsp;Rui Su","doi":"10.1111/lang.12649","DOIUrl":"10.1111/lang.12649","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The temporal focus hypothesis (TFH) entails that individuals who value the past tend to conceptualize it in front, whereas individuals who value the future tend to map the future in front instead (de la Fuente et al., 2014). This varies as a function of culture, individual differences, and context. Here, we extend this line of inquiry by testing a contextual variable, namely COVID-19 quarantine status, and an individual differences variable, namely future precautionary behavior towards COVID-19. Contrary to what the TFH would predict, we show that participants map the future to a frontal position, regardless of individual attitudes and quarantine status. However, participants who displayed more future precautionary behavior were also more future-focused than participants who displayed less such behaviour, but this did not predict their front–back mappings of the future. These findings suggest that individual differences may be stronger determinants of temporal focus than contextual variables.</p>","PeriodicalId":51371,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning","volume":"74 S1","pages":"72-103"},"PeriodicalIF":4.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/lang.12649","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140890410","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Proactive Language Learning Theory 主动语言学习理论
IF 4.4 1区 文学 Q1 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH Pub Date : 2024-05-04 DOI: 10.1111/lang.12644
Mostafa Papi, Phil Hiver
Second language acquisition theory has traditionally focused on the cognitive and psycholinguistic processes involved in additional language (L2) learning. In addition, research on learner psychology has primarily centered on learners’ cognitive abilities (e.g., aptitude and working memory) and internal traits or states (e.g., dispositions, motivations, and affect). Language learning behavior, however, has remained largely neglected and under‐theorized. To address this gap, this paper proposes the proactive language learning theory, which delineates the agentic and strategic behaviors that learners employ to learn an additional language. These behaviors include input‐seeking behavior, interaction‐seeking behavior, information‐seeking behavior, and feedback‐seeking behavior. This paper presents theoretical arguments supporting the proposal, describes the four behavioral dimensions of the theory, and outlines general hypotheses concerning the contextual and learner‐related antecedents of these behaviors and their effects on L2 outcomes. Finally, the potential implications of this theory for advancing our understanding of L2 learning and instruction are discussed.
第二语言习得理论历来侧重于额外语言(L2)学习所涉及的认知和心理语言过程。此外,对学习者心理的研究主要集中在学习者的认知能力(如能力倾向和工作记忆)和内部特征或状态(如倾向、动机和情感)上。然而,语言学习行为在很大程度上仍被忽视,理论研究不足。为了弥补这一不足,本文提出了积极主动的语言学习理论,该理论描述了学习者在学习其他语言时所采用的代理和策略行为。这些行为包括寻求输入行为、寻求互动行为、寻求信息行为和寻求反馈行为。本文提出了支持该提议的理论论据,描述了该理论的四个行为维度,并概述了有关这些行为的语境和学习者相关前因及其对 L2 结果影响的一般假设。最后,讨论了这一理论对促进我们理解语言学习和教学的潜在影响。
{"title":"Proactive Language Learning Theory","authors":"Mostafa Papi, Phil Hiver","doi":"10.1111/lang.12644","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12644","url":null,"abstract":"Second language acquisition theory has traditionally focused on the cognitive and psycholinguistic processes involved in additional language (L2) learning. In addition, research on learner psychology has primarily centered on learners’ cognitive abilities (e.g., aptitude and working memory) and internal traits or states (e.g., dispositions, motivations, and affect). Language learning behavior, however, has remained largely neglected and under‐theorized. To address this gap, this paper proposes the proactive language learning theory, which delineates the agentic and strategic behaviors that learners employ to learn an additional language. These behaviors include input‐seeking behavior, interaction‐seeking behavior, information‐seeking behavior, and feedback‐seeking behavior. This paper presents theoretical arguments supporting the proposal, describes the four behavioral dimensions of the theory, and outlines general hypotheses concerning the contextual and learner‐related antecedents of these behaviors and their effects on L2 outcomes. Finally, the potential implications of this theory for advancing our understanding of L2 learning and instruction are discussed.","PeriodicalId":51371,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning","volume":"30 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140826376","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Active Language Modulates Color Perception in Bilinguals 主动语言调节双语者的色彩感知
IF 4.4 1区 文学 Q1 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH Pub Date : 2024-05-02 DOI: 10.1111/lang.12645
Akvile Sinkeviciute, Julien Mayor, Mila Dimitrova Vulchanova, Natalia Kartushina

Color terms divide the color spectrum differently across languages. Previous studies have reported that speakers of languages that have different words for light and dark blue (e.g., Russian siniy and goluboy) discriminate color chips sampled from these two linguistic categories faster than speakers of languages that use one basic color term for blue (e.g., English blue). This effect has been reported to be disrupted when participants engaged in a verbal interference task, suggesting that active language use can modulate the linguistic category effect. The current study provided a stringent test of this hypothesis by examining color discrimination under verbal interference in bilinguals speaking Lithuanian (two blue categories) and Norwegian (one blue category). The results revealed that the language activated during verbal interference had a significant impact on bilinguals’ color discrimination. Specifically, Lithuanian–Norwegian bilinguals exhibited a color category effect only when performing the task under verbal interference in Lithuanian but not in Norwegian. This demonstrated, within the same individuals, the momentary effect of active language processing on color perception.

不同语言的颜色术语对色谱的划分是不同的。以往的研究表明,使用不同颜色词来表示浅蓝色和深蓝色的语言(如俄语中的 siniy 和 goluboy)的人比使用一个基本颜色词来表示蓝色的语言(如英语中的 blue)的人更快地分辨出从这两种语言类别中采样的颜色芯片。据报道,这种效应在受试者参与言语干扰任务时会受到干扰,这表明主动语言使用可以调节语言类别效应。本研究通过对立陶宛语(两个蓝色类别)和挪威语(一个蓝色类别)的双语受试者进行语言干扰下的颜色辨别测试,对这一假设进行了严格的检验。结果显示,语言干扰时激活的语言对双语者的颜色辨别能力有显著影响。具体而言,立陶宛-挪威双语者只有在用立陶宛语而不是挪威语进行语言干扰时,才会表现出颜色类别效应。这表明,在同一个人身上,主动语言处理对颜色感知有瞬间效应。
{"title":"Active Language Modulates Color Perception in Bilinguals","authors":"Akvile Sinkeviciute,&nbsp;Julien Mayor,&nbsp;Mila Dimitrova Vulchanova,&nbsp;Natalia Kartushina","doi":"10.1111/lang.12645","DOIUrl":"10.1111/lang.12645","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Color terms divide the color spectrum differently across languages. Previous studies have reported that speakers of languages that have different words for light and dark blue (e.g., Russian <i>siniy</i> and <i>goluboy</i>) discriminate color chips sampled from these two linguistic categories faster than speakers of languages that use one basic color term for blue (e.g., English <i>blue</i>). This effect has been reported to be disrupted when participants engaged in a verbal interference task, suggesting that active language use can modulate the linguistic category effect. The current study provided a stringent test of this hypothesis by examining color discrimination under verbal interference in bilinguals speaking Lithuanian (two blue categories) and Norwegian (one blue category). The results revealed that the language activated during verbal interference had a significant impact on bilinguals’ color discrimination. Specifically, Lithuanian–Norwegian bilinguals exhibited a color category effect only when performing the task under verbal interference in Lithuanian but not in Norwegian. This demonstrated, within the same individuals, the momentary effect of active language processing on color perception.</p>","PeriodicalId":51371,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning","volume":"74 S1","pages":"40-71"},"PeriodicalIF":4.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/lang.12645","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140826392","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction to ‘Community, Equity, and Cultural Change in Open Research: A Response to Open Peer Commentaries’ 对 "开放研究中的社区、公平和文化变革 "的更正:对开放式同行评论的回应
IF 4.4 1区 文学 Q1 EDUCATION & EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH Pub Date : 2024-05-02 DOI: 10.1111/lang.12651
{"title":"Correction to ‘Community, Equity, and Cultural Change in Open Research: A Response to Open Peer Commentaries’","authors":"","doi":"10.1111/lang.12651","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12651","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":51371,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning","volume":"17 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140826385","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Language Learning
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1