首页 > 最新文献

Research on Language and Social Interaction最新文献

英文 中文
Affiliating in Second Position: Response Tokens with Rising Pitch in Danish 处于第二位的附属关系:丹麦语中音调上升的响应标记
IF 2.7 1区 文学 Q1 COMMUNICATION Pub Date : 2021-01-02 DOI: 10.1080/08351813.2020.1864159
S. S. Sørensen
ABSTRACT This article examines the use of the Danish response tokens ja (“yes”) and nej (“no”) with rising pitch in everyday interaction in Danish. Ja and nej do more than (dis)confirmation, and the analysis shows that the tokens with rising pitch achieve affiliation in second position in sequences containing displays of affective stance, which is shown to be contrastive with the tokens with level pitch that instead disaffiliate in the same sequences. Turns eliciting the tokens are also often marked with a wide pitch span, but sometimes other prosodic features than pitch are employed to perform a display of affective stance. Eliciting turns often request reconfirmation but can also implement other actions that make ja or nej a relevant response. The affiliation achieved is shown to be similar across both ja and nej when doing a range of actions, such as (dis)confirmation, acceptance, or agreement. Data are in Danish.
摘要:本文考察了丹麦语应答标记ja(“是”)和nej(“否”)在丹麦语日常互动中的使用情况。Ja和nej所做的不仅仅是(dis)证实,分析表明,在包含情感立场显示的序列中,音调上升的标记在第二位置实现了附属,这与音调水平的标记相反,后者在相同序列中脱离了附属。引发表征的转折通常也标有宽的音高跨度,但有时会使用音高以外的其他韵律特征来表现情感立场。Eliciting回合通常要求再次确认,但也可以实施其他行动,使ja或nej成为相关的回应。在进行一系列行动时,如(dis)确认、接受或同意时,ja和nej的隶属关系是相似的。数据为丹麦语。
{"title":"Affiliating in Second Position: Response Tokens with Rising Pitch in Danish","authors":"S. S. Sørensen","doi":"10.1080/08351813.2020.1864159","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2020.1864159","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT This article examines the use of the Danish response tokens ja (“yes”) and nej (“no”) with rising pitch in everyday interaction in Danish. Ja and nej do more than (dis)confirmation, and the analysis shows that the tokens with rising pitch achieve affiliation in second position in sequences containing displays of affective stance, which is shown to be contrastive with the tokens with level pitch that instead disaffiliate in the same sequences. Turns eliciting the tokens are also often marked with a wide pitch span, but sometimes other prosodic features than pitch are employed to perform a display of affective stance. Eliciting turns often request reconfirmation but can also implement other actions that make ja or nej a relevant response. The affiliation achieved is shown to be similar across both ja and nej when doing a range of actions, such as (dis)confirmation, acceptance, or agreement. Data are in Danish.","PeriodicalId":51484,"journal":{"name":"Research on Language and Social Interaction","volume":"54 1","pages":"101 - 125"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2021-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/08351813.2020.1864159","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49350597","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Preference and Polarity: Epistemic Stance in Question Design 偏好与极性:问题设计中的认识立场
IF 2.7 1区 文学 Q1 COMMUNICATION Pub Date : 2021-01-02 DOI: 10.1080/08351813.2020.1864155
J. Heritage, C. W. Raymond
ABSTRACT This article considers the use of negative polarization in polar (yes/no) questions. It argues that question polarity is used to take an epistemic stance toward the probability or improbability of the state of affairs referenced in the question and that taking such a stance is effectively unavoidable. Focusing on negatively polarized questions (NPQs), four main kinds of evidence are adduced that NPQs are associated with the questioner’s stance that the question’s underlying proposition is unlikely: (a) self-repair to reverse or otherwise adjust polarity; (b) evidence from the prior talk from which the question is occasioned; (c) contexts in which a particular state of affairs is relevant but has remained unstated; (d) overall structural organizational features of talk (e.g., conversational closings) that militate against the likelihood of affirmative responses. Finally, the article proposes that question design represents a distinct organizational layer vis-à-vis the preference-organizational characteristics of actions, and it appears to function in distinctive ways in relation to recruitment- and affiliation-relevant questions (e.g., requests, offers, etc.) by comparison with information-seeking questions. Data are drawn from corpora of British and American English conversations.
摘要:本文研究了负极化在极性(是/否)问题中的应用。它认为,问题极性是用来对问题中提及的事态的概率或非概率采取一种认识立场,采取这种立场实际上是不可避免的。针对负极性问题(NPQs),本文引证了四种主要证据,表明NPQs与提问者认为问题的基本命题不可能存在的立场有关:(a)自我修复以逆转或以其他方式调整极性;(b)引起问题的先前谈话的证据;(c)与某一特定事态有关但尚未说明的情况;(d)谈话的整体结构组织特征(例如,谈话结束)不利于肯定回应的可能性。最后,本文提出,问题设计相对于-à-vis行为的偏好-组织特征代表了一个独特的组织层,并且通过与信息寻求问题相比,它似乎以不同的方式与招聘和从属关系相关的问题(例如,请求,要约等)有关。数据来源于英式英语和美式英语会话的语料库。
{"title":"Preference and Polarity: Epistemic Stance in Question Design","authors":"J. Heritage, C. W. Raymond","doi":"10.1080/08351813.2020.1864155","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2020.1864155","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT This article considers the use of negative polarization in polar (yes/no) questions. It argues that question polarity is used to take an epistemic stance toward the probability or improbability of the state of affairs referenced in the question and that taking such a stance is effectively unavoidable. Focusing on negatively polarized questions (NPQs), four main kinds of evidence are adduced that NPQs are associated with the questioner’s stance that the question’s underlying proposition is unlikely: (a) self-repair to reverse or otherwise adjust polarity; (b) evidence from the prior talk from which the question is occasioned; (c) contexts in which a particular state of affairs is relevant but has remained unstated; (d) overall structural organizational features of talk (e.g., conversational closings) that militate against the likelihood of affirmative responses. Finally, the article proposes that question design represents a distinct organizational layer vis-à-vis the preference-organizational characteristics of actions, and it appears to function in distinctive ways in relation to recruitment- and affiliation-relevant questions (e.g., requests, offers, etc.) by comparison with information-seeking questions. Data are drawn from corpora of British and American English conversations.","PeriodicalId":51484,"journal":{"name":"Research on Language and Social Interaction","volume":"54 1","pages":"39 - 59"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2021-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/08351813.2020.1864155","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48563640","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 25
An Adjunct to Repair: You Know in Speech Production and Understanding Difficulties 修饰语:你知道在言语产生和理解困难
IF 2.7 1区 文学 Q1 COMMUNICATION Pub Date : 2021-01-02 DOI: 10.1080/08351813.2020.1864157
S. Clayman, C. W. Raymond
ABSTRACT The English-language particle you know is frequently associated with speech production and understanding difficulties. The present study combines sequential and distributional analyses to explicate the particle’s relationship to the conversational repair system. It demonstrates that you know functions as an adjunct to repair, addressing secondary difficulties associated with implementing self-repair in practice, while also promoting the avoidance of transformative repair operations. This repair adjunct viewpoint trades off the particle’s general import as an alignment token and is supported by examining its specialized role in: (a) self-repair operations, (b) suboptimal formulations, and (c) understanding pursuits. This article elaborates our understanding of the repair system by identifying an ancillary practice that smooths over recurrent shortcomings of natural speech. Data in American English.
摘要你所知道的英语助词经常与言语产生和理解困难有关。本研究结合序列分析和分布分析来解释助词与会话修复系统的关系。它表明,你知道功能是修复的辅助,解决了在实践中实施自我修复的次要困难,同时也促进了避免变革性修复操作。这种修复辅助观点权衡了粒子作为对齐标记的一般重要性,并通过检查其在以下方面的特殊作用来支持:(a)自我修复操作,(b)次优公式,以及(c)理解追求。这篇文章通过确定一种辅助实践来阐述我们对修复系统的理解,这种辅助实践可以消除自然语言反复出现的缺点。美国英语数据。
{"title":"An Adjunct to Repair: You Know in Speech Production and Understanding Difficulties","authors":"S. Clayman, C. W. Raymond","doi":"10.1080/08351813.2020.1864157","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2020.1864157","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT The English-language particle you know is frequently associated with speech production and understanding difficulties. The present study combines sequential and distributional analyses to explicate the particle’s relationship to the conversational repair system. It demonstrates that you know functions as an adjunct to repair, addressing secondary difficulties associated with implementing self-repair in practice, while also promoting the avoidance of transformative repair operations. This repair adjunct viewpoint trades off the particle’s general import as an alignment token and is supported by examining its specialized role in: (a) self-repair operations, (b) suboptimal formulations, and (c) understanding pursuits. This article elaborates our understanding of the repair system by identifying an ancillary practice that smooths over recurrent shortcomings of natural speech. Data in American English.","PeriodicalId":51484,"journal":{"name":"Research on Language and Social Interaction","volume":"54 1","pages":"80 - 100"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2021-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/08351813.2020.1864157","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44595190","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Thanks to Reviewers 感谢审稿人
IF 2.7 1区 文学 Q1 COMMUNICATION Pub Date : 2020-12-14 DOI: 10.1080/08351813.2020.1863104
Charles Antaki
(2020). Thanks to Reviewers. Research on Language and Social Interaction: Vol. 53, No. 4, pp. i-i.
(2020)。感谢评论者。《语言与社会互动研究》,第53卷,第4期,第1 - 4页。
{"title":"Thanks to Reviewers","authors":"Charles Antaki","doi":"10.1080/08351813.2020.1863104","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2020.1863104","url":null,"abstract":"(2020). Thanks to Reviewers. Research on Language and Social Interaction: Vol. 53, No. 4, pp. i-i.","PeriodicalId":51484,"journal":{"name":"Research on Language and Social Interaction","volume":"100 10","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2020-12-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138495565","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mock Aggression: Navigating Affiliation and Disaffiliation in Interaction 模拟攻击:在互动中导航隶属与脱离
IF 2.7 1区 文学 Q1 COMMUNICATION Pub Date : 2020-12-09 DOI: 10.1080/08351813.2020.1833590
Reihaneh Afshari Saleh

ABSTRACT

One way people have of managing interpersonal conflict is what I call “mock aggression.” So far unexplored in interactional detail, mock aggression refers to the embodiments which, in one way or another, appear aggressive (punching, pinching, slapping, etc.) but are not designed to be, or oriented to as, serious physical threats. Mock aggression occurs between intimate interactants, and in this interactional situation, it sanctions transgressions and at the same time provides systematic opportunities for participants to engage in more affiliative interaction. The findings show that despite its aggressive appearance, mock aggression facilitates participants’ exit from a disaffiliative interaction, owing to its detailed design features, and thereby contributes to maintaining their social bonds. It is argued that a categorical affiliative versus disaffiliative perspective does not work for some interactional practices like mock aggression. Data are in Persian and collected in Iran.

人们处理人际冲突的一种方式是我所说的“模拟攻击”。到目前为止,还没有在互动细节上进行探索,模拟攻击是指以这样或那样的方式表现出攻击性(拳打脚踢、捏捏、拍打等),但不是被设计为或导向为严重的身体威胁的实施方式。模拟攻击发生在亲密互动之间,在这种互动情境中,它制裁了越轨行为,同时为参与者提供了系统的机会,使其参与更多的从属互动。研究结果表明,尽管模拟攻击具有攻击性,但由于其详细的设计特征,模拟攻击有助于参与者退出分离的互动,从而有助于维持他们的社会联系。有人认为,分类隶属关系与非隶属关系的观点并不适用于一些相互作用的实践,如模拟攻击。数据是波斯语的,在伊朗收集。
{"title":"Mock Aggression: Navigating Affiliation and Disaffiliation in Interaction","authors":"Reihaneh Afshari Saleh","doi":"10.1080/08351813.2020.1833590","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2020.1833590","url":null,"abstract":"<p><b>ABSTRACT</b></p> <p>One way people have of managing interpersonal conflict is what I call “mock aggression.” So far unexplored in interactional detail, mock aggression refers to the embodiments which, in one way or another, appear aggressive (punching, pinching, slapping, etc.) but are not designed to be, or oriented to as, serious physical threats. Mock aggression occurs between intimate interactants, and in this interactional situation, it sanctions transgressions and at the same time provides systematic opportunities for participants to engage in more affiliative interaction. The findings show that despite its aggressive appearance, mock aggression facilitates participants’ exit from a disaffiliative interaction, owing to its detailed design features, and thereby contributes to maintaining their social bonds. It is argued that a categorical affiliative versus disaffiliative perspective does not work for some interactional practices like mock aggression. Data are in Persian and collected in Iran.</p>","PeriodicalId":51484,"journal":{"name":"Research on Language and Social Interaction","volume":"1 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2020-12-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138517070","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The Comparative Study of Social Action: What You Must and What You Can Do to Align with a Prior Speaker 社会行动的比较研究:你必须做什么和你能做什么才能与先前的演讲者保持一致
IF 2.7 1区 文学 Q1 COMMUNICATION Pub Date : 2020-10-01 DOI: 10.1080/08351813.2020.1826764
J. Zinken
ABSTRACT This article makes an empirical and a methodological contribution to the comparative study of action. The empirical contribution is a comparative study of three distinct types of action regularly accomplished with the turn format du meinst x (“you mean/think x”) in German: candidate understandings, formulations of the other’s mind, and requests for a judgment. These empirical materials are the basis for a methodological exploration of different levels of researcher abstraction in the comparative study of action. Two levels are examined: the (coarser) level of conditionally relevant responses (what a response speaker must do to align with the action of the prior turn) and the (finer) level of “full alignment” (what a response speaker can do to align with the action of a prior turn). Both levels of abstraction provide empirically viable and analytically interesting descriptive concepts for the comparative study of action. Data are in German.
本文对行动的比较研究做出了实证和方法论的贡献。实证贡献是对三种不同类型的行动进行比较研究,这三种行动通常采用德语中的转向形式du meinst x(“你的意思是/思考x”)来完成:候选人的理解、对他人思想的表述和对判断的请求。这些经验材料是在行动比较研究中对不同层次的研究者抽象进行方法论探索的基础。考察了两个级别:条件相关响应的(较粗)级别(响应说话者必须做什么才能与前一回合的动作保持一致)和“完全一致”的(较细)级别(回应说话者可以做什么来与上一回合的行动保持一致)。这两个抽象层次都为行动的比较研究提供了经验上可行和分析上有趣的描述性概念。数据为德语。
{"title":"The Comparative Study of Social Action: What You Must and What You Can Do to Align with a Prior Speaker","authors":"J. Zinken","doi":"10.1080/08351813.2020.1826764","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2020.1826764","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT This article makes an empirical and a methodological contribution to the comparative study of action. The empirical contribution is a comparative study of three distinct types of action regularly accomplished with the turn format du meinst x (“you mean/think x”) in German: candidate understandings, formulations of the other’s mind, and requests for a judgment. These empirical materials are the basis for a methodological exploration of different levels of researcher abstraction in the comparative study of action. Two levels are examined: the (coarser) level of conditionally relevant responses (what a response speaker must do to align with the action of the prior turn) and the (finer) level of “full alignment” (what a response speaker can do to align with the action of a prior turn). Both levels of abstraction provide empirically viable and analytically interesting descriptive concepts for the comparative study of action. Data are in German.","PeriodicalId":51484,"journal":{"name":"Research on Language and Social Interaction","volume":"53 1","pages":"443 - 462"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2020-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/08351813.2020.1826764","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46571384","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Turning the Tables: Objecting to Conduct in Conflict Talk 扭转局势:反对冲突谈话中的行为
IF 2.7 1区 文学 Q1 COMMUNICATION Pub Date : 2020-10-01 DOI: 10.1080/08351813.2020.1826765
Rebecca Clift, M. Pino
ABSTRACT This article is a Conversation Analytic study of occasions where a speaker formulates what a recipient is doing as something objectionable, thereby delivering an accusation, e.g., “Why you shouting” or “I dunno why you’re being so aggressive.” We call these lexical formulations of what someone has just done conduct formulations. These are: (a) responsive to an ongoing imputation of misconduct or misdemeanor, and (b) produced in response to an upgrade on prior attempts by the recipient to engage the producer of the conduct formulation in aligning with their project. The speaker thereby “turns the tables” on the recipient, challenging the legitimacy of, and thus rendering accountable, his line of action. The response by the recipient involves a downgrade of her prior action and so proposes resetting the terms of engagement on a more conciliatory basis. Data are in English and Italian.
摘要这篇文章是一篇对话分析研究,研究了说话者将接受者所做的事情表述为令人反感的事情,从而提出指控的情况,例如,“你为什么大喊大叫”或“我不知道你为什么这么咄咄逼人”。我们将这些词汇表述称为行为表述。它们是:(a)对正在进行的不当行为或轻罪指控作出回应,以及(b)对接受者先前试图让行为制定者参与其项目的升级作出回应。因此,发言人对接受者“翻盘”,质疑其行动路线的合法性,从而追究其责任。接收方的回应涉及对其先前行动的降级,因此建议在更具和解性的基础上重新设定参与条款。数据有英文和意大利文两种。
{"title":"Turning the Tables: Objecting to Conduct in Conflict Talk","authors":"Rebecca Clift, M. Pino","doi":"10.1080/08351813.2020.1826765","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2020.1826765","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT This article is a Conversation Analytic study of occasions where a speaker formulates what a recipient is doing as something objectionable, thereby delivering an accusation, e.g., “Why you shouting” or “I dunno why you’re being so aggressive.” We call these lexical formulations of what someone has just done conduct formulations. These are: (a) responsive to an ongoing imputation of misconduct or misdemeanor, and (b) produced in response to an upgrade on prior attempts by the recipient to engage the producer of the conduct formulation in aligning with their project. The speaker thereby “turns the tables” on the recipient, challenging the legitimacy of, and thus rendering accountable, his line of action. The response by the recipient involves a downgrade of her prior action and so proposes resetting the terms of engagement on a more conciliatory basis. Data are in English and Italian.","PeriodicalId":51484,"journal":{"name":"Research on Language and Social Interaction","volume":"53 1","pages":"463 - 480"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2020-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/08351813.2020.1826765","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42607656","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
One Type of Polar, Information-Seeking Question and Its Stance of Probability: Implications for the Preference for Agreement 一类极性信息寻求问题及其概率立场——对协议偏好的启示
IF 2.7 1区 文学 Q1 COMMUNICATION Pub Date : 2020-10-01 DOI: 10.1080/08351813.2020.1826759
Jeffrey D. Robinson
ABSTRACT There is little doubt that Sacks’s notion of the “preference for agreement” is generally valid. However, that it is valid does not tell us how it is valid. This article further unpacks the preference for agreement by conversation-analytically grounding one of its many underlying mechanisms. Specifically, this article examines the practice of formatting an action—in this case, a type of information seeking—as a positively formatted polar interrogative without polarity items (e.g., Did you go fishing?). This article demonstrates that doing so enacts a speaker stance that the question’s proposed state of affairs (e.g., that the recipient went fishing) is probable and thus that a response is more likely to constitute affirmation than disaffirmation. Additionally, this article describes the preference-organizational effects of such formatting on some aspects of response construction. Data are gathered from videotapes of unstructured, face-to-face conversations, included 289 interrogatives, and are in American English.
摘要毫无疑问,萨克斯的“协议优先权”概念是普遍有效的。然而,它是有效的并不能告诉我们它是如何有效的。这篇文章通过对话分析其众多潜在机制之一,进一步揭示了对协议的偏好。具体来说,这篇文章研究了将一个动作(在这种情况下,是一种信息寻求)格式化为没有极性项目的正格式极性疑问句的做法(例如,你去钓鱼了吗?)。这篇文章表明,这样做表明了发言人的立场,即问题的拟议事态(例如,接受者去钓鱼)是可能的,因此回答更有可能构成肯定而非否定。此外,本文还描述了这种格式对响应构建的某些方面的偏好组织影响。数据是从非结构化面对面对话的录像带中收集的,包括289个问句,都是美式英语。
{"title":"One Type of Polar, Information-Seeking Question and Its Stance of Probability: Implications for the Preference for Agreement","authors":"Jeffrey D. Robinson","doi":"10.1080/08351813.2020.1826759","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2020.1826759","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT There is little doubt that Sacks’s notion of the “preference for agreement” is generally valid. However, that it is valid does not tell us how it is valid. This article further unpacks the preference for agreement by conversation-analytically grounding one of its many underlying mechanisms. Specifically, this article examines the practice of formatting an action—in this case, a type of information seeking—as a positively formatted polar interrogative without polarity items (e.g., Did you go fishing?). This article demonstrates that doing so enacts a speaker stance that the question’s proposed state of affairs (e.g., that the recipient went fishing) is probable and thus that a response is more likely to constitute affirmation than disaffirmation. Additionally, this article describes the preference-organizational effects of such formatting on some aspects of response construction. Data are gathered from videotapes of unstructured, face-to-face conversations, included 289 interrogatives, and are in American English.","PeriodicalId":51484,"journal":{"name":"Research on Language and Social Interaction","volume":"53 1","pages":"425 - 442"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2020-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/08351813.2020.1826759","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47019359","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
The Normativity of Medical Tests: Test Ordering as a Routine Activity in “New Problem” Consultations in Secondary Care 医学检查的规范性:二级医疗“新问题”咨询中的常规检查排序
IF 2.7 1区 文学 Q1 COMMUNICATION Pub Date : 2020-08-14 DOI: 10.1080/08351813.2020.1785768
Merran Toerien, C. Jackson, M. Reuber
ABSTRACT How does ordering a test fit into “new problem” medical consultations? Responding to calls for studies of the overall structural organization of consultations beyond primary care, this article depicts the organization of new problem consultations observed in two large neuroscience centers in the UK. This shows that—in addition to Robinson’s widely cited four main activities (establishing the reason for the visit, gathering information, delivering a diagnosis, recommending treatment)—test ordering is oriented to as an additional, normative activity. We show this numerically (tests were ordered in over 60% of our 65 new problem consultations) and by analyzing how participants orient to the activity of test ordering even when neurologists decide against testing. We argue that test ordering is a distinct activity, which, despite being treatment-oriented, displaces treatment in the here and now. Test ordering is thus consequential for progressivity, serving as both bridge and barrier to accomplishing the overarching medical project. Data are in British English.
订购检查如何适应“新问题”医学咨询?响应对咨询的整体结构组织超越初级保健的研究呼吁,这篇文章描述了在英国两个大型神经科学中心观察到的新问题咨询的组织。这表明,除了罗宾逊被广泛引用的四项主要活动(确定就诊原因、收集信息、提供诊断、推荐治疗)之外,测试排序也被视为一项额外的、规范的活动。我们用数字来证明这一点(在我们的65个新问题咨询中,超过60%的人安排了测试),并通过分析参与者如何适应测试安排的活动,即使神经学家决定不进行测试。我们认为,测试排序是一种独特的活动,尽管是面向治疗的,但在此时此地取代了治疗。因此,测试顺序对进步性至关重要,既是完成总体医疗项目的桥梁,也是障碍。数据是英式英语。
{"title":"The Normativity of Medical Tests: Test Ordering as a Routine Activity in “New Problem” Consultations in Secondary Care","authors":"Merran Toerien, C. Jackson, M. Reuber","doi":"10.1080/08351813.2020.1785768","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2020.1785768","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT How does ordering a test fit into “new problem” medical consultations? Responding to calls for studies of the overall structural organization of consultations beyond primary care, this article depicts the organization of new problem consultations observed in two large neuroscience centers in the UK. This shows that—in addition to Robinson’s widely cited four main activities (establishing the reason for the visit, gathering information, delivering a diagnosis, recommending treatment)—test ordering is oriented to as an additional, normative activity. We show this numerically (tests were ordered in over 60% of our 65 new problem consultations) and by analyzing how participants orient to the activity of test ordering even when neurologists decide against testing. We argue that test ordering is a distinct activity, which, despite being treatment-oriented, displaces treatment in the here and now. Test ordering is thus consequential for progressivity, serving as both bridge and barrier to accomplishing the overarching medical project. Data are in British English.","PeriodicalId":51484,"journal":{"name":"Research on Language and Social Interaction","volume":"53 1","pages":"405 - 424"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2020-08-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/08351813.2020.1785768","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42681612","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
A Bip, a Beeeep, and a Beep Beep: How Horns Are Sounded in Chennai Traffic 哔哔声,哔哔声,哔哔声:在金奈的交通中喇叭是如何响起的
IF 2.7 1区 文学 Q1 COMMUNICATION Pub Date : 2020-07-02 DOI: 10.1080/08351813.2020.1785775
E. Laurier, Daniel Muñoz, Rebekah Miller, Barry A. T. Brown
ABSTRACT Although the vehicle horn is a minimal audible unit for communication, we will show that its uses are impressively varied. Drawing upon a corpus of video recordings from dashcams, we show how drivers use the horn for creating awareness; how they target particular vehicles; and how they use it for warnings, for complaints, and in instructing the seeing of an aspect of an ambiguous traffic object. Drivers’ use of the horn involves, first, their sounding it in recognizable relations to past, current, and projected configurations of traffic on the road. Second, it involves drivers manipulating the vehicle horn to create sounds of shorter and longer durations that can then produce hearably distinct actions. Third, and finally, the driver can use the horn as an initiating or responsive action in relation to the actions of other members of traffic. The data are from road users in Chennai, India.
摘要尽管汽车喇叭是一种用于通信的最小可听单元,但我们将展示其用途的多样性。根据行车记录仪的视频记录,我们展示了驾驶员如何使用喇叭来提高意识;他们如何瞄准特定车辆;以及他们如何将其用于警告、投诉和指示看到模糊交通物体的某个方面。驾驶员对喇叭的使用首先涉及到他们将喇叭按与过去、当前和预计道路交通配置的可识别关系发出。其次,它涉及驾驶员操纵车辆喇叭,产生持续时间更短和更长的声音,然后可以产生明显的听觉动作。第三,也是最后一点,驾驶员可以使用喇叭作为相对于其他交通成员的行动的启动或响应行动。这些数据来自印度金奈的道路使用者。
{"title":"A Bip, a Beeeep, and a Beep Beep: How Horns Are Sounded in Chennai Traffic","authors":"E. Laurier, Daniel Muñoz, Rebekah Miller, Barry A. T. Brown","doi":"10.1080/08351813.2020.1785775","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2020.1785775","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Although the vehicle horn is a minimal audible unit for communication, we will show that its uses are impressively varied. Drawing upon a corpus of video recordings from dashcams, we show how drivers use the horn for creating awareness; how they target particular vehicles; and how they use it for warnings, for complaints, and in instructing the seeing of an aspect of an ambiguous traffic object. Drivers’ use of the horn involves, first, their sounding it in recognizable relations to past, current, and projected configurations of traffic on the road. Second, it involves drivers manipulating the vehicle horn to create sounds of shorter and longer durations that can then produce hearably distinct actions. Third, and finally, the driver can use the horn as an initiating or responsive action in relation to the actions of other members of traffic. The data are from road users in Chennai, India.","PeriodicalId":51484,"journal":{"name":"Research on Language and Social Interaction","volume":"53 1","pages":"341 - 356"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2020-07-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1080/08351813.2020.1785775","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44577574","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8
期刊
Research on Language and Social Interaction
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1