首页 > 最新文献

Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale最新文献

英文 中文
A motor task, not working memory, causes the revelation effect. 运动任务,而不是工作记忆,导致了启示效应。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-02 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000317
Hiroshi Miura, Yuji Itoh

Performing a cognitive task prior to making a recognition judgment increases the probability of old responses, which is known as the revelation effect. The criterion shift account (Niewiadomski & Hockley, 2001) proposes that occupation of working memory causes the revelation effect. However, we proposed that working memory does not cause it. Two experiments were conducted to disconfirm the relationship between working memory and the revelation effect and to consider an alternative explanation that metacognition causes the effect. In Experiment 1, the revelation effect was caused by a finger movement task, which puts little or no load on working memory. In Experiment 2, a metacognitive instruction that a cognitive task would make subsequent recognition easier induced a conservative criterion shift. The finding that a simple motor task caused the revelation effect in Experiment 1 disconfirms the relationship between working memory and the revelation effect and extends the boundaries of the occurrence of the effect. The findings in Experiment 2 suggest that metacognition may be related to the occurrence of the revelation effect. This study implies a paradoxical aspect of human cognition in that metacognition, which usually makes cognition more effective and rational, may also cause an irrational phenomenon, the revelation effect. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在做出识别判断之前执行认知任务会增加旧反应的概率,这被称为启示效应。标准转换解释(Niewiadomski和Hockley,2001)提出,工作记忆的占用导致了启示效应。然而,我们提出工作记忆并不是导致这种现象的原因。我们进行了两个实验来解开工作记忆与启示效应之间的关系,并考虑元认知导致启示效应的另一种解释。在实验1中,启示效应是由手指运动任务引起的,该任务对工作记忆几乎没有负荷。在实验2中,认知任务会使后续识别更容易的元认知指令导致了保守标准的转变。实验1发现,一个简单的运动任务引起了启示效应,这打破了工作记忆与启示效应之间的关系,拓展了启示效应发生的边界。实验2的研究结果表明,元认知可能与启示效应的发生有关。这项研究暗示了人类认知的一个悖论,即元认知通常使认知更加有效和理性,但也可能导致一种非理性现象,即启示效应。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"A motor task, not working memory, causes the revelation effect.","authors":"Hiroshi Miura, Yuji Itoh","doi":"10.1037/cep0000317","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000317","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Performing a cognitive task prior to making a recognition judgment increases the probability of old responses, which is known as the revelation effect. The criterion shift account (Niewiadomski & Hockley, 2001) proposes that occupation of working memory causes the revelation effect. However, we proposed that working memory does not cause it. Two experiments were conducted to disconfirm the relationship between working memory and the revelation effect and to consider an alternative explanation that metacognition causes the effect. In Experiment 1, the revelation effect was caused by a finger movement task, which puts little or no load on working memory. In Experiment 2, a metacognitive instruction that a cognitive task would make subsequent recognition easier induced a conservative criterion shift. The finding that a simple motor task caused the revelation effect in Experiment 1 disconfirms the relationship between working memory and the revelation effect and extends the boundaries of the occurrence of the effect. The findings in Experiment 2 suggest that metacognition may be related to the occurrence of the revelation effect. This study implies a paradoxical aspect of human cognition in that metacognition, which usually makes cognition more effective and rational, may also cause an irrational phenomenon, the revelation effect. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":" ","pages":"81-87"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71428984","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Looks can be deceiving: Investigating change blindness in an online setting. 外表是会骗人的:调查在线环境中的变化盲区。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-25 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000323
Saeeda Saeed, Arianna Cook, Victoria Mackie, Dana A Hayward

In the real world, we often fail to notice changes in our environment. In some cases, such as not noticing a car moving into our lane, the results can be catastrophic. This so-called change blindness has been seen experimentally both through failing to notice changes to images on-screen as well as failing to notice a change in other people's identity. However, less is known regarding how change blindness manifests in virtual settings varying in visual clutter or with varying types of interaction with someone prior to the change. Across two studies (n = 134), participants engaged in an online video chat with a confederate, with two levels of visual clutter (none, a lot) and three levels of interaction (none, light conversations about weather/TV, deeper conversations about goals/greatest regrets). We found no modulation of change blindness rates across perceptual clutter. Curiously, we found a large discrepancy in change blindness rates in Experiment 1 (79%; 52/66) versus Experiment 2 (16%; 11/68) that we explored, leading to some evidence that increasing the level of interaction led to greater change blindness rates, but only for pairs who identified as belonging to different ethnicities. Taken together, our work suggests that we may pay attention to people differently in virtual settings compared to in-person, that in-group and out-group biases may have an effect on change blindness rates, and that while clutter does not seem to affect change blindness rates, one's level of interaction just might. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在现实生活中,我们经常无法注意到周围环境的变化。在某些情况下,比如没有注意到一辆汽车驶入我们的车道,结果可能是灾难性的。这种所谓的 "变化盲 "已经在实验中被证实,既包括没有注意到屏幕上图像的变化,也包括没有注意到他人身份的变化。然而,人们对变化盲症在视觉杂乱的虚拟环境中或在变化前与他人进行不同类型的互动时的表现却知之甚少。在两项研究中(n = 134),参与者与一名同谋进行了在线视频聊天,聊天有两种视觉杂乱程度(无、大量)和三种互动程度(无、关于天气/电视的简单对话、关于目标/最大遗憾的深入对话)。我们没有发现盲变率会随着知觉杂乱程度的变化而变化。奇怪的是,我们发现实验 1(79%;52/66)与实验 2(16%;11/68)中的变化盲视率存在很大差异,这在一定程度上证明了互动程度的增加会导致变化盲视率的增加,但仅限于那些被认定属于不同种族的配对。综上所述,我们的研究表明,在虚拟环境中,我们对人的关注程度可能与面对面时不同;群体内和群体外的偏见可能会对变化盲视率产生影响;虽然杂乱无章似乎不会影响变化盲视率,但一个人的互动程度可能会产生影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Looks can be deceiving: Investigating change blindness in an online setting.","authors":"Saeeda Saeed, Arianna Cook, Victoria Mackie, Dana A Hayward","doi":"10.1037/cep0000323","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000323","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In the real world, we often fail to notice changes in our environment. In some cases, such as not noticing a car moving into our lane, the results can be catastrophic. This so-called change blindness has been seen experimentally both through failing to notice changes to images on-screen as well as failing to notice a change in other people's identity. However, less is known regarding how change blindness manifests in virtual settings varying in visual clutter or with varying types of interaction with someone prior to the change. Across two studies (<i>n</i> = 134), participants engaged in an online video chat with a confederate, with two levels of visual clutter (none, a lot) and three levels of interaction (none, light conversations about weather/TV, deeper conversations about goals/greatest regrets). We found no modulation of change blindness rates across perceptual clutter. Curiously, we found a large discrepancy in change blindness rates in Experiment 1 (79%; 52/66) versus Experiment 2 (16%; 11/68) that we explored, leading to some evidence that increasing the level of interaction led to greater change blindness rates, but only for pairs who identified as belonging to different ethnicities. Taken together, our work suggests that we may pay attention to people differently in virtual settings compared to in-person, that in-group and out-group biases may have an effect on change blindness rates, and that while clutter does not seem to affect change blindness rates, one's level of interaction just might. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":" ","pages":"1-8"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139565225","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Set size and the orthographic/phonological neighbourhood size effect in serial recognition: The importance of randomization. 序列识别中的集合大小和拼写/语音邻域大小效应:随机化的重要性。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-02 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000320
Dominic Guitard, Leonie M Miller, Ian Neath, Steven Roodenrys

The neighbourhood size effect refers to the finding of better memory for words with more orthographic/phonological neighbours than otherwise comparable words with fewer neighbours. Although many studies have replicated this result with serial recall, only one has used serial recognition. Greeno et al. (2022) found no neighbourhood size effect when a large stimulus pool was used and a reverse effect-better performance for small neighbourhood words-when a small stimulus pool was used. We reexamined these results but made two methodological changes. First, for the large pool, we randomly generated lists for each subject rather than creating one set of lists that all subjects experienced. Second, for the small pool, we randomly generated a small pool for each subject rather than using one small pool for all subjects. In both cases, we observed a neighbourhood size effect consistent with results from the serial recall literature. Implications for methodology and theoretical accounts of both the neighbourhood size effect and serial recognition are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

邻域大小效应指的是,与邻居较少的可比单词相比,拼写/语音邻居较多的单词记忆力更好。尽管许多研究已经用序列回忆复制了这一结果,但只有一项研究使用了序列识别。Greeno等人(2022)发现,当使用大的刺激池时,没有邻域大小效应,而当使用小的刺激库时,反向效应对小的邻域词表现更好。我们重新检查了这些结果,但在方法上做了两次改变。首先,对于大池,我们为每个主题随机生成列表,而不是创建一组所有主题都经历过的列表。其次,对于小水池,我们为每个受试者随机生成一个小水池,而不是为所有受试者使用一个小池。在这两种情况下,我们都观察到了邻域大小效应,这与系列回忆文献的结果一致。讨论了邻域大小效应和序列识别的方法论和理论解释的意义。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Set size and the orthographic/phonological neighbourhood size effect in serial recognition: The importance of randomization.","authors":"Dominic Guitard, Leonie M Miller, Ian Neath, Steven Roodenrys","doi":"10.1037/cep0000320","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000320","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The neighbourhood size effect refers to the finding of better memory for words with more orthographic/phonological neighbours than otherwise comparable words with fewer neighbours. Although many studies have replicated this result with serial recall, only one has used serial recognition. Greeno et al. (2022) found no neighbourhood size effect when a large stimulus pool was used and a reverse effect-better performance for small neighbourhood words-when a small stimulus pool was used. We reexamined these results but made two methodological changes. First, for the large pool, we randomly generated lists for each subject rather than creating one set of lists that all subjects experienced. Second, for the small pool, we randomly generated a small pool for each subject rather than using one small pool for all subjects. In both cases, we observed a neighbourhood size effect consistent with results from the serial recall literature. Implications for methodology and theoretical accounts of both the neighbourhood size effect and serial recognition are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":" ","pages":"9-16"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71428987","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Face processing in ADHD: A review of the N170 event-related potential. ADHD的面部加工:N170事件相关电位的回顾。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-30 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000321
Brady R T Roberts, Rebecca Trossman

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is associated with deficits in social functioning, including peer difficulties and poor relationship quality. Little is known, however, about the integrity of foundational sociocognitive abilities that support interpersonal interactions in ADHD. Face processing-a fundamental component of social cognition-has been a popular topic of recent investigations in this area. Researchers have attempted to delineate face processing mechanisms in ADHD to elucidate social deficits often seen in the disorder. Investigating the N170 event-related potential, a neural marker of face processing, has been a popular approach in this endeavour. Here, we present two accounts that offer competing views of how social deficits might arise in those with ADHD. Next, we systematically review and synthesise the literature on the N170 in ADHD to identify whether atypicalities in sociocognitive domains like face processing occur in this patient population. Gaps in the literature are identified and concrete solutions are offered to improve future research in this area. We end by discussing immediate implications for treatment approaches designed to address widely observed social deficits in individuals with ADHD. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

注意缺陷多动障碍(ADHD)与社会功能缺陷有关,包括同伴困难和关系质量差。然而,对于支持ADHD患者人际交往的基本社会认知能力的完整性,我们所知甚少。人脸处理是社会认知的一个基本组成部分,是近年来该领域研究的热门话题。研究人员试图描述多动症的面部加工机制,以阐明这种疾病中常见的社会缺陷。研究N170事件相关电位是人脸处理的一个神经标记,在这方面一直是一种流行的方法。在这里,我们提出了两种不同的观点来解释多动症患者的社会缺陷是如何产生的。接下来,我们系统地回顾和综合了N170在ADHD中的文献,以确定在社会认知领域,如面部处理是否发生在这一患者群体中。确定了文献中的差距,并提供了具体的解决方案,以改进该领域的未来研究。最后,我们讨论了治疗方法的直接意义,这些方法旨在解决多动症患者广泛观察到的社会缺陷。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Face processing in ADHD: A review of the N170 event-related potential.","authors":"Brady R T Roberts, Rebecca Trossman","doi":"10.1037/cep0000321","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000321","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is associated with deficits in social functioning, including peer difficulties and poor relationship quality. Little is known, however, about the integrity of foundational sociocognitive abilities that support interpersonal interactions in ADHD. Face processing-a fundamental component of social cognition-has been a popular topic of recent investigations in this area. Researchers have attempted to delineate face processing mechanisms in ADHD to elucidate social deficits often seen in the disorder. Investigating the N170 event-related potential, a neural marker of face processing, has been a popular approach in this endeavour. Here, we present two accounts that offer competing views of how social deficits might arise in those with ADHD. Next, we systematically review and synthesise the literature on the N170 in ADHD to identify whether atypicalities in sociocognitive domains like face processing occur in this patient population. Gaps in the literature are identified and concrete solutions are offered to improve future research in this area. We end by discussing immediate implications for treatment approaches designed to address widely observed social deficits in individuals with ADHD. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":" ","pages":"36-49"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138464218","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Active or passive? Investigating different types of cognitive fatigue. 主动还是被动?调查不同类型的认知疲劳
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-10 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000312
Todd Pickering, Bradley Wright, Linda Schücker, Clare MacMahon

Research in cognitive fatigue has identified the negative impact that cognitive exertion can have on subsequent task performance. An underexamined question is whether there are different types of fatigue, particularly: active fatigue, similar to cognitive fatigue, and passive fatigue, similar to boredom. This online study examined whether active and passive fatigue can be elicited and differentiated using computerized cognitive tasks. We compared subjective and behavioural outcomes to look for distinctions between fatigue types in response to different cognitive tasks. A sample of 122 participants (53% male; age 30.04 ± 3.50 years) rated their subjective state before and after one of three 8-min cognitive task conditions (TloadDback, Mackworth Clock, Documentary/Control). Next, participants also completed a second cognitive task (Flanker task). The task expected to be actively fatiguing (TloadDback) was rated the most difficult, effortful, and mentally and temporally demanding. The task expected to be passively fatiguing (Mackworth Clock) had the greatest increases in subjective fatigue, boredom, and sleepiness, and the greatest decrease in "want-to" motivation. There were no differences between conditions for Flanker performance. We showed that different fatigue types could be elicited using different computerized cognitive tasks. The passively fatiguing task had the most negative influence on subjective fatigue and motivation, suggesting a nonengaging or "boringly fatiguing" task induces a more detrimental type of fatigue. A key next step is to examine longer cognitive tasks to determine whether effects from different fatigue types become more prominent with time-on-task. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

对认知疲劳的研究发现,认知消耗会对随后的任务表现产生负面影响。一个尚未得到充分研究的问题是,是否存在不同类型的疲劳,特别是:与认知疲劳类似的主动疲劳和与无聊类似的被动疲劳。这项在线研究探讨了主动疲劳和被动疲劳是否可以通过计算机化认知任务来激发和区分。我们比较了主观和行为结果,以寻找不同认知任务下疲劳类型的区别。122 名参与者(53% 为男性;年龄为 30.04 ± 3.50 岁)在完成三项 8 分钟认知任务(TloadDback、Mackworth Clock、纪录片/对照)之一之前和之后对自己的主观状态进行了评分。接下来,参与者还完成了第二项认知任务(侧翼任务)。预期会使人主动疲劳的任务(TloadDback)被评为最难、最费力、对精神和时间要求最高的任务。被动疲劳任务(Mackworth Clock)的主观疲劳感、无聊感和困倦感增加最多,"想要 "的动机减少最多。不同条件下的 Flanker 成绩没有差异。我们的研究表明,使用不同的计算机化认知任务可以引起不同类型的疲劳。被动疲劳任务对主观疲劳和动机的负面影响最大,这表明非参与性或 "无聊疲劳 "任务会诱发更有害的疲劳类型。下一步的关键是研究时间更长的认知任务,以确定不同疲劳类型的影响是否会随着任务时间的延长而变得更加突出。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Active or passive? Investigating different types of cognitive fatigue.","authors":"Todd Pickering, Bradley Wright, Linda Schücker, Clare MacMahon","doi":"10.1037/cep0000312","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000312","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research in cognitive fatigue has identified the negative impact that cognitive exertion can have on subsequent task performance. An underexamined question is whether there are different types of fatigue, particularly: active fatigue, similar to cognitive fatigue, and passive fatigue, similar to boredom. This online study examined whether active and passive fatigue can be elicited and differentiated using computerized cognitive tasks. We compared subjective and behavioural outcomes to look for distinctions between fatigue types in response to different cognitive tasks. A sample of 122 participants (53% male; age 30.04 ± 3.50 years) rated their subjective state before and after one of three 8-min cognitive task conditions (TloadDback, Mackworth Clock, Documentary/Control). Next, participants also completed a second cognitive task (Flanker task). The task expected to be actively fatiguing (TloadDback) was rated the most difficult, effortful, and mentally and temporally demanding. The task expected to be passively fatiguing (Mackworth Clock) had the greatest increases in subjective fatigue, boredom, and sleepiness, and the greatest decrease in \"want-to\" motivation. There were no differences between conditions for Flanker performance. We showed that different fatigue types could be elicited using different computerized cognitive tasks. The passively fatiguing task had the most negative influence on subjective fatigue and motivation, suggesting a nonengaging or \"boringly fatiguing\" task induces a more detrimental type of fatigue. A key next step is to examine longer cognitive tasks to determine whether effects from different fatigue types become more prominent with time-on-task. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":" ","pages":"50-65"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9967727","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Facial emotional congruence in healthy adults and patients suffering from a psychiatric or neurological disorder. 健康成年人和精神或神经失调患者的面部情绪一致性。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-12-21 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000318
Pauline Gury, Pauline Narme, Alessandra Tommasi, Nathalie Ehrlé

The production of facial emotions is an important conveyor of social communication. The present review of the literature concerns the congruence of facial emotions, that is the facial muscular activation that takes place in response to the emotional facial expression perceived in others. Although scientific interest in facial emotions has increased exponentially in the last few years, the production of facial emotions is still underexplored as compared to emotional perception. Several studies, mainly conducted with electromyography, have shown that facial emotional congruence exists in a robust way, largely for anger and happiness. While facial emotional congruence was long considered as innate and automatic, recent work has demonstrated that several sociocultural factors may influence or reduce this ability, challenging its automaticity. From a neuroanatomical point of view, studies have clearly highlighted the implication of mirror neurons but our knowledge is still limited because of the few methodologies assessing this system and the lack of homogeneity between the protocols used. Many explanatory, and probably not mutually exclusive, theories of emotional facial congruence have been put forward. In experimental neuropsychology, emotional facial congruence has seldom been investigated but the few available results suggest an impairment in psychiatric and neurological patients. In view of the important role of emotional facial productions in human relations and social interactions, new methods for easy clinical assessment need to be designed for the diagnosis and the cognitive care of these abilities. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

面部情绪的产生是社会交流的重要传达者。本文献综述涉及面部情绪的一致性,即面部肌肉激活对他人感知到的情绪面部表情的反应。尽管在过去几年中,科学界对面部情绪的兴趣急剧增加,但与情绪感知相比,对面部情绪的产生仍未进行充分探索。主要通过肌电图进行的几项研究表明,面部情绪一致性以一种强有力的方式存在,主要针对愤怒和快乐。虽然面部情绪一致性长期以来被认为是与生俱来的自动能力,但最近的研究表明,一些社会文化因素可能会影响或削弱这种能力,从而对其自动性提出挑战。从神经解剖学的角度来看,研究明确强调了镜像神经元的影响,但由于评估该系统的方法很少,而且所使用的方案之间缺乏一致性,我们的知识仍然有限。人们提出了许多解释情绪面部一致性的理论,这些理论可能并不相互排斥。在实验神经心理学中,很少对情绪面部一致性进行研究,但现有的少数研究结果表明,精神病和神经病患者的情绪面部一致性存在障碍。鉴于情绪化面部表情在人际关系和社会交往中的重要作用,需要设计新的临床评估方法,以便对这些能力进行诊断和认知护理。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Facial emotional congruence in healthy adults and patients suffering from a psychiatric or neurological disorder.","authors":"Pauline Gury, Pauline Narme, Alessandra Tommasi, Nathalie Ehrlé","doi":"10.1037/cep0000318","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000318","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The production of facial emotions is an important conveyor of social communication. The present review of the literature concerns the congruence of facial emotions, that is the facial muscular activation that takes place in response to the emotional facial expression perceived in others. Although scientific interest in facial emotions has increased exponentially in the last few years, the production of facial emotions is still underexplored as compared to emotional perception. Several studies, mainly conducted with electromyography, have shown that facial emotional congruence exists in a robust way, largely for anger and happiness. While facial emotional congruence was long considered as innate and automatic, recent work has demonstrated that several sociocultural factors may influence or reduce this ability, challenging its automaticity. From a neuroanatomical point of view, studies have clearly highlighted the implication of mirror neurons but our knowledge is still limited because of the few methodologies assessing this system and the lack of homogeneity between the protocols used. Many explanatory, and probably not mutually exclusive, theories of emotional facial congruence have been put forward. In experimental neuropsychology, emotional facial congruence has seldom been investigated but the few available results suggest an impairment in psychiatric and neurological patients. In view of the important role of emotional facial productions in human relations and social interactions, new methods for easy clinical assessment need to be designed for the diagnosis and the cognitive care of these abilities. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":" ","pages":"17-35"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138832951","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceptions of (in)sincerity in satirical discourse: A study of word reading times using minimally different texts. 讽刺话语中对真诚的感知:使用最小差异文本的单词阅读时间研究。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-10 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000314
Stephen Skalicky

The present study investigated the relationship between satirical discourse processing and a theoretical model of satire comprehension known as satirical uptake. Word reading times and participant perceptions of sincerity for a set of minimally different satirical and nonsatirical texts were modelled considering individual differences such as need for cognition (NFC) and genre familiarity. Across two experiments, participants read either a mixture of satirical and nonsatirical texts (Experiment 1) or only satirical/nonsatirical texts (Experiment 2), indicating the degree to which they felt the meaning of the text was sincere. Results from both experiments demonstrated satirical texts were read slower than nonsatirical texts. Moreover, longer word reading times were associated with lower sincerity ratings for satirical texts, but only after participants encountered one or more satirical texts. NFC interacted with reading times in Experiment 1 but not Experiment 2, and there were no strong effects for genre familiarity in either experiment. The main conclusion drawn from these results is that successful satirical uptake may require greater processing effort, a result which aligns with theoretical models of satirical discourse as well as the related construct of verbal irony. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究探讨了讽刺话语加工与讽刺理解理论模型——讽刺吸收之间的关系。考虑到个体差异,如认知需求(NFC)和体裁熟悉度,对一组最小差异的讽刺和非讽刺文本的单词阅读时间和参与者对诚意的感知进行了建模。在两个实验中,参与者要么阅读讽刺和非讽刺的混合文本(实验1),要么只阅读讽刺/非讽刺的文本(实验2),这表明他们认为文本的意思是真诚的程度。两个实验的结果都表明,讽刺文本的阅读速度比非讽刺文本慢。此外,较长的单词阅读时间与较低的讽刺文本的诚意评级有关,但只有在参与者遇到一个或多个讽刺文本之后。在实验1中,NFC与阅读时间存在交互作用,而在实验2中不存在交互作用;在实验2中,NFC对体裁熟悉度的影响均不明显。从这些结果中得出的主要结论是,成功的讽刺吸收可能需要更大的加工努力,这一结果与讽刺话语的理论模型以及言语讽刺的相关结构相一致。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Perceptions of (in)sincerity in satirical discourse: A study of word reading times using minimally different texts.","authors":"Stephen Skalicky","doi":"10.1037/cep0000314","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000314","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present study investigated the relationship between satirical discourse processing and a theoretical model of satire comprehension known as satirical uptake. Word reading times and participant perceptions of sincerity for a set of minimally different satirical and nonsatirical texts were modelled considering individual differences such as need for cognition (NFC) and genre familiarity. Across two experiments, participants read either a mixture of satirical and nonsatirical texts (Experiment 1) or only satirical/nonsatirical texts (Experiment 2), indicating the degree to which they felt the meaning of the text was sincere. Results from both experiments demonstrated satirical texts were read slower than nonsatirical texts. Moreover, longer word reading times were associated with lower sincerity ratings for satirical texts, but only after participants encountered one or more satirical texts. NFC interacted with reading times in Experiment 1 but not Experiment 2, and there were no strong effects for genre familiarity in either experiment. The main conclusion drawn from these results is that successful satirical uptake may require greater processing effort, a result which aligns with theoretical models of satirical discourse as well as the related construct of verbal irony. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":" ","pages":"247-261"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9967724","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Drawing enhances memory for emotional words. 绘画可以增强对情感词汇的记忆。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-02 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000319
Sophia H N Tran, Myra A Fernandes

Past work has demonstrated that drawing a sketch, compared to writing during encoding, improves memory of to-be-remembered words, pictures, and academic terms. We examined whether this benefit extended to emotional materials. In Experiment 1, negative, positive, and neutral words were presented in an encoding phase, with intermixed prompts to either write out or draw a picture representing the word. Participants later freely recalled words by writing them out. Recall was higher for words drawn than for words written at encoding, and the magnitude of the benefit was differentially enhanced for emotional compared to neutral words. In Experiment 2, negative, positive, and neutral words were again presented but encoding type was compared using pure lists between participants. The pattern of memory performance replicated that observed in Experiment 1. Further, the use of drawing as an encoding technique interacted with emotionality, whereby emotional words that were drawn were best remembered. Our results demonstrate that the memory benefit conferred by drawing at encoding extends to emotional materials. Our findings suggest that the use of drawing as an encoding strategy, and the emotionality of the stimulus itself, contributes independently to enhance retention. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

过去的研究表明,与编码期间的写作相比,绘制草图可以提高对待记忆单词、图片和学术术语的记忆。我们研究了这种益处是否延伸到情感材料。在实验1中,阴性、阳性和中性词在编码阶段出现,并混合提示写出或绘制代表该词的图片。参与者随后通过书写单词来自由回忆单词。提取的单词的回忆比编码时写的单词更高,与中性单词相比,情感单词的益处程度也有所提高。在实验2中,再次呈现否定词、肯定词和中性词,但使用参与者之间的纯列表来比较编码类型。记忆性能的模式与实验1中观察到的模式相同。此外,绘画作为一种编码技术的使用与情感性相互作用,从而使所画的情感词汇被最好地记住。我们的研究结果表明,在编码时画画所带来的记忆益处延伸到了情感材料。我们的研究结果表明,绘画作为一种编码策略的使用,以及刺激本身的情感性,独立地有助于提高记忆力。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Drawing enhances memory for emotional words.","authors":"Sophia H N Tran, Myra A Fernandes","doi":"10.1037/cep0000319","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000319","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Past work has demonstrated that drawing a sketch, compared to writing during encoding, improves memory of to-be-remembered words, pictures, and academic terms. We examined whether this benefit extended to emotional materials. In Experiment 1, negative, positive, and neutral words were presented in an encoding phase, with intermixed prompts to either write out or draw a picture representing the word. Participants later freely recalled words by writing them out. Recall was higher for words drawn than for words written at encoding, and the magnitude of the benefit was differentially enhanced for emotional compared to neutral words. In Experiment 2, negative, positive, and neutral words were again presented but encoding type was compared using pure lists between participants. The pattern of memory performance replicated that observed in Experiment 1. Further, the use of drawing as an encoding technique interacted with emotionality, whereby emotional words that were drawn were best remembered. Our results demonstrate that the memory benefit conferred by drawing at encoding extends to emotional materials. Our findings suggest that the use of drawing as an encoding strategy, and the emotionality of the stimulus itself, contributes independently to enhance retention. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":" ","pages":"296-307"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71428985","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Is gaze cuing more like endogenous or exogenous orienting? 凝视更像是内生定向还是外生定向?
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-02 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000315
Nicholas E Murray, Richard S Drake, Raymond M Klein

People shift their attention in the direction of another person's gaze. This phenomenon, called gaze cuing, shares properties with purely endogenous (i.e., "deliberate") and purely exogenous (i.e., "reflexive") control of spatial attention. For example, as with purely endogenous orienting, gaze cues appear at visual fixation; yet, as with purely exogenous orienting, gaze cues elicit shifts of attention rapidly after their appearance. Prior experiments have shown that when controlled endogenously versus exogenously, the effects of attention upon the processing of targets are dramatically different. Briand and Klein (1987; see also Briand, 1998) showed that endogenous orienting is additive with opportunities for illusory conjunctions, whereas exogenous orienting is interactive. Klein (1994) showed that endogenous orienting is interactive with nonspatial expectancies, whereas exogenous orienting is additive. In the present project, we applied this double-dissociation strategy to attention controlled by gaze cues. In Experiment 1, gaze cuing effects (in accuracy) were additive with opportunities for illusory conjunctions (akin to endogenous control), whereas in Experiment 2, gaze cuing was additive with the nonspatial expectancy effect (akin to exogenous orienting). Therefore, in the nature of its effects upon performance, gaze cuing functions like a hybrid of endogenous and exogenous orienting. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

人们将注意力转移到另一个人的注视方向上。这种现象被称为凝视提示,与空间注意力的纯内生(即“刻意”)和纯外生(即,“反射”)控制具有共同的特性。例如,与纯粹的内生定向一样,凝视线索出现在视觉注视上;然而,与纯粹的外源性定向一样,凝视线索在出现后会迅速引起注意力的转移。先前的实验表明,当内源性和外源性控制时,注意力对目标处理的影响显著不同。Briand和Klein(1987;另见Briand,1998)表明,内生定向是与虚幻连词的机会相加的,而外生定向是相互作用的。Klein(1994)指出,内生定向与非空间预期是相互作用的,而外源定向是加性的。在本项目中,我们将这种双重分离策略应用于由凝视线索控制的注意力。在实验1中,凝视提示效应(准确性)与虚幻连接的机会相加(类似于内生控制),而在实验2中,凝视提示与非空间预期效应相加(类似于外生定向)。因此,就其对表现的影响而言,凝视引导的功能就像内源性和外源性定向的混合体。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Is gaze cuing more like endogenous or exogenous orienting?","authors":"Nicholas E Murray, Richard S Drake, Raymond M Klein","doi":"10.1037/cep0000315","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000315","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People shift their attention in the direction of another person's gaze. This phenomenon, called gaze cuing, shares properties with purely endogenous (i.e., \"deliberate\") and purely exogenous (i.e., \"reflexive\") control of spatial attention. For example, as with purely endogenous orienting, gaze cues appear at visual fixation; yet, as with purely exogenous orienting, gaze cues elicit shifts of attention rapidly after their appearance. Prior experiments have shown that when controlled endogenously versus exogenously, the effects of attention upon the processing of targets are dramatically different. Briand and Klein (1987; see also Briand, 1998) showed that endogenous orienting is additive with opportunities for illusory conjunctions, whereas exogenous orienting is interactive. Klein (1994) showed that endogenous orienting is interactive with nonspatial expectancies, whereas exogenous orienting is additive. In the present project, we applied this double-dissociation strategy to attention controlled by gaze cues. In Experiment 1, gaze cuing effects (in accuracy) were additive with opportunities for illusory conjunctions (akin to endogenous control), whereas in Experiment 2, gaze cuing was additive with the nonspatial expectancy effect (akin to exogenous orienting). Therefore, in the nature of its effects upon performance, gaze cuing functions like a hybrid of endogenous and exogenous orienting. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":" ","pages":"262-270"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71428986","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of instructions and response format on smile judgement. 指示和回应形式对微笑判断的影响。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-02 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000313
Annie Roy-Charland, Victoria Foglia, Karolyn Cloutier, Emalie Hendel, Marie-Pier Mazerolle

Our study examined the role of instructions, response type, and definition on the judgement of enjoyment and nonenjoyment smiles. Participants viewed symmetric Duchenne, non-Duchenne, and asymmetric smiles. They were instructed to judge the happiness, authenticity, and sincerity of the smiles using either Likert scales or a dichotomous response type. Participants were also either given a definition of the instruction words "happy," "authentic," and "sincere" or not. Results showed that the probability of saying "really (happy/sincere/authentic)" was higher for the symmetric Duchenne than the asymmetric smiles and higher for the asymmetric than non-Duchenne smiles. Changing the instructions given to participants did not override the effect of smile type with the use of Likert scale or dichotomous response. However, with the use of Likert scale, we observed subtilities that were not observed with the use of dichotomous response. When given a definition, in the case of symmetric non-Duchenne smiles, Likert ratings were significantly lower, and participants were more accurate in their judgement on the dichotomous scale. However, no differences were observed for the asymmetric Duchenne and symmetric Duchenne smiles whether a definition was given or not. Symmetric non-Duchenne and asymmetric Duchenne smiles were also viewed longer when a definition was given than when one was not. Nevertheless, considering methodological variations of our study failed to explain the variations in the pattern of results of previous studies, other avenues should be explored, such as the use of dynamic stimuli and a greater variety of encoders. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

我们的研究考察了指示、反应类型和定义对判断快乐和非快乐微笑的作用。参与者看到对称的杜兴、非杜兴和不对称的微笑。他们被要求使用Likert量表或二分法反应类型来判断微笑的幸福感、真实性和真诚性。参与者还被给出了“快乐”、“真实”和“真诚”等指示词的定义。结果表明,对称的杜兴微笑比非对称的微笑说“真的(快乐/真诚/真实)”的概率更高,非对称的笑比非杜兴微笑说“真正(快乐/诚挚/真实”的概率也更高。改变给参与者的指示并没有通过使用Likert量表或二分法反应来克服微笑类型的影响。然而,通过使用Likert量表,我们观察到了使用二分反应所没有观察到的微妙性。当给出定义时,在对称非杜兴微笑的情况下,Likert评分明显较低,参与者在二分量表上的判断更准确。然而,无论是否给出定义,都没有观察到不对称杜兴和对称杜兴微笑的差异。对称的非杜兴微笑和不对称的杜兴微笑在给出定义时也比没有给出定义时更长。然而,考虑到我们研究的方法学变化未能解释先前研究结果模式的变化,应该探索其他途径,例如使用动态刺激和更多种类的编码器。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"The effect of instructions and response format on smile judgement.","authors":"Annie Roy-Charland, Victoria Foglia, Karolyn Cloutier, Emalie Hendel, Marie-Pier Mazerolle","doi":"10.1037/cep0000313","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000313","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Our study examined the role of instructions, response type, and definition on the judgement of enjoyment and nonenjoyment smiles. Participants viewed symmetric Duchenne, non-Duchenne, and asymmetric smiles. They were instructed to judge the happiness, authenticity, and sincerity of the smiles using either Likert scales or a dichotomous response type. Participants were also either given a definition of the instruction words \"happy,\" \"authentic,\" and \"sincere\" or not. Results showed that the probability of saying \"really (happy/sincere/authentic)\" was higher for the symmetric Duchenne than the asymmetric smiles and higher for the asymmetric than non-Duchenne smiles. Changing the instructions given to participants did not override the effect of smile type with the use of Likert scale or dichotomous response. However, with the use of Likert scale, we observed subtilities that were not observed with the use of dichotomous response. When given a definition, in the case of symmetric non-Duchenne smiles, Likert ratings were significantly lower, and participants were more accurate in their judgement on the dichotomous scale. However, no differences were observed for the asymmetric Duchenne and symmetric Duchenne smiles whether a definition was given or not. Symmetric non-Duchenne and asymmetric Duchenne smiles were also viewed longer when a definition was given than when one was not. Nevertheless, considering methodological variations of our study failed to explain the variations in the pattern of results of previous studies, other avenues should be explored, such as the use of dynamic stimuli and a greater variety of encoders. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":" ","pages":"308-318"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71428988","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1