首页 > 最新文献

Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale最新文献

英文 中文
An event-related potential investigation of the influence of bilingualism on disambiguating homonyms in older adults. 双语对老年人同义词辨析影响的事件相关电位研究。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-18 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000331
Shanna Kousaie, Vanessa Taler

Ambiguity is ubiquitous in language; lexical ambiguity refers to instances where a single word has multiple meanings. The current investigation examined homonyms, words that have the same orthography and pronunciation in English but multiple meanings (e.g., BANK, meaning "financial institution" or "river's edge"). The processing of homonyms requires the engagement of executive control processes, for example, to select the appropriate meaning of the homonym while reducing interference from other meanings. Executive function processes are known to change over the lifespan and may be impacted by experiential factors such as bilingualism. The present study uses event-related brain potentials as an index of lexical access to examine whether bilingualism influences homonym processing in older adults. The results indicate that patterns of lexical access differ as a function of bilingual status in older adults and compared to young adults, suggesting that language experience may moderate language processing in both young and older adults, at least in situations where language processing is demanding on executive function, such as in the processing of homonyms. Importantly, we show that older bilinguals show a somewhat similar pattern of ambiguity processing as their younger counterparts, while age differences were observed in monolinguals when comparing across studies. This suggests that bilingual language experience may have a mitigating impact on age-related changes in ambiguity processing. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

歧义在语言中无处不在;词汇歧义是指一个词有多种含义的情况。本次调查研究的是同形异义词,即在英语中具有相同正字法和发音但有多种含义的词(例如,BANK,意为 "金融机构 "或 "河边")。处理同音异义词需要执行控制过程的参与,例如,选择同音异义词的适当含义,同时减少其他含义的干扰。众所周知,执行功能过程在人的一生中会发生变化,并可能受到双语等经验因素的影响。本研究利用事件相关脑电位作为词汇访问的指标,研究双语是否会影响老年人的同音词处理。研究结果表明,与年轻人相比,老年人的词汇访问模式因其双语状态而不同,这表明语言经验可能会缓和年轻人和老年人的语言处理过程,至少在语言处理对执行功能要求较高的情况下是这样,比如同音词的处理。重要的是,我们发现老年双语者的歧义处理模式与年轻双语者有些相似,而在比较不同的研究时,单语者则出现了年龄差异。这表明,双语语言经验可能对年龄相关的歧义处理变化有缓解作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"An event-related potential investigation of the influence of bilingualism on disambiguating homonyms in older adults.","authors":"Shanna Kousaie, Vanessa Taler","doi":"10.1037/cep0000331","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000331","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Ambiguity is ubiquitous in language; lexical ambiguity refers to instances where a single word has multiple meanings. The current investigation examined homonyms, words that have the same orthography and pronunciation in English but multiple meanings (e.g., BANK, meaning \"financial institution\" or \"river's edge\"). The processing of homonyms requires the engagement of executive control processes, for example, to select the appropriate meaning of the homonym while reducing interference from other meanings. Executive function processes are known to change over the lifespan and may be impacted by experiential factors such as bilingualism. The present study uses event-related brain potentials as an index of lexical access to examine whether bilingualism influences homonym processing in older adults. The results indicate that patterns of lexical access differ as a function of bilingual status in older adults and compared to young adults, suggesting that language experience may moderate language processing in both young and older adults, at least in situations where language processing is demanding on executive function, such as in the processing of homonyms. Importantly, we show that older bilinguals show a somewhat similar pattern of ambiguity processing as their younger counterparts, while age differences were observed in monolinguals when comparing across studies. This suggests that bilingual language experience may have a mitigating impact on age-related changes in ambiguity processing. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":" ","pages":"67-80"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140873586","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On the association between intention and visual word identification. 意向与视觉单词识别之间的关联。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-11 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000326
Derek Besner, Colin M MacLeod

One of the most fundamental distinctions in cognitive psychology is between processing that is "controlled" and processing that is "automatic." The widely held automatic processing account of visual word identification asserts that, among other characteristics, the presentation of a well-formed letter string triggers sublexical, lexical, and semantic activation in the absence of any intention to do so. Instead, the role of intention is seen as independent of stimulus identification and as restricted to selection for action using the products of identification (e.g., braking in response to a sign saying "BRIDGE OUT"). We consider four paradigms with respect to the role of an intention-defined here as a "task set" indicating how to perform in the current situation-when identifying single well-formed letter strings. Contrary to the received automaticity view, the literature regarding each of these paradigms demonstrates that the relation between an intention and stimulus identification is constrained in multiple ways, many of which are not well understood at present. One thing is clear: There is no simple relation between an intention, in the form of a task set, and stimulus identification. Automatic processing of words, if this indeed ever occurs, certainly is not a system default. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

认知心理学中最基本的区别之一是 "受控 "处理和 "自动 "处理之间的区别。广为流传的视觉单词识别的自动加工理论认为,除其他特征外,一个格式良好的字母串的呈现会在没有任何意图的情况下触发副词汇、词性和语义的激活。相反,意向的作用被视为独立于刺激识别,仅限于利用识别的产物选择行动(例如,看到 "桥出 "的标志而刹车)。在识别单个格式正确的字母串时,我们考虑了四种范式,它们都将意图定义为 "任务集",表示在当前情况下如何执行任务。与公认的自动性观点相反,有关这些范例的文献都表明,意向与刺激识别之间的关系受到多方面的制约,其中许多制约因素目前还没有得到很好的理解。有一点是明确的:任务集形式的意图与刺激识别之间没有简单的关系。自动处理单词,如果真的发生过的话,肯定不是系统默认情况。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"On the association between intention and visual word identification.","authors":"Derek Besner, Colin M MacLeod","doi":"10.1037/cep0000326","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000326","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>One of the most fundamental distinctions in cognitive psychology is between processing that is \"controlled\" and processing that is \"automatic.\" The widely held automatic processing account of visual word identification asserts that, among other characteristics, the presentation of a well-formed letter string triggers sublexical, lexical, and semantic activation in the absence of any intention to do so. Instead, the role of intention is seen as independent of stimulus identification and as restricted to selection for action using the products of identification (e.g., braking in response to a sign saying \"BRIDGE OUT\"). We consider four paradigms with respect to the role of an intention-defined here as a \"task set\" indicating how to perform in the current situation-when identifying single well-formed letter strings. Contrary to the received automaticity view, the literature regarding each of these paradigms demonstrates that the relation between an intention and stimulus identification is constrained in multiple ways, many of which are not well understood at present. One thing is clear: There is no simple relation between an intention, in the form of a task set, and stimulus identification. Automatic processing of words, if this indeed ever occurs, certainly is not a system default. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":" ","pages":"114-128"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140867539","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Differences in politeness perception of irony and prosocial lies: Exploring the role of age, gender, and geographic location. 对反讽和亲社会谎言的礼貌感知差异:探索年龄、性别和地理位置的作用。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-07 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000324
Pavitra Rao Makarla, Gitte Henssel Joergensen, Kendal Brice Tyner, Caroline Sprinkle, Kathrin Rothermich

In daily interactions, individuals use irony and prosocial lies for various reasons, for example, to be humorous, to criticize, or to be polite. While some studies have examined individual differences in perceiving the politeness of such language, research using naturalistic, context-rich materials is lacking. To address this gap, we utilized short videos to assess politeness perception in literal, ironic, and prosocial lie scenarios while also exploring differences based on age, gender, and geographical location. Our sample included 288 participants from the United States and the United Kingdom. We focused on five different types of language: literal positive, blunt, sarcastic, teasing, and prosocial lies. Participants rated the politeness of these statements and completed surveys on communication preferences (Self-Reported Sarcasm Questionnaire and Conversational Indirectness Scale Questionnaire). While the demographic groups showed similarities, individual factors also shaped politeness perception. Older adults perceived teasing as less polite than middle-aged and younger adults, and male participants rated blunt and sarcastic statements as more polite. Geographical variations were found for prosocial lies, with U.K. participants rating them more polite than their U.S. counterparts. These findings underscore the importance of considering context-rich materials and individual factors in understanding the social functions of irony and prosocial lies. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在日常互动中,个人出于各种原因使用反讽和亲社会谎言,例如,为了幽默、批评或礼貌。虽然有些研究已经考察了个体在感知此类语言的礼貌性方面的差异,但还缺乏使用自然、语境丰富的材料进行的研究。为了弥补这一不足,我们利用短视频来评估在字面、讽刺和亲社会谎言场景中的礼貌感知,同时还探讨了基于年龄、性别和地理位置的差异。我们的样本包括来自美国和英国的 288 名参与者。我们重点研究了五种不同类型的语言:字面肯定、直率、讽刺、戏弄和亲社会谎言。参与者对这些语句的礼貌性进行了评分,并完成了有关交流偏好的调查(自述讽刺问卷和会话间接性量表问卷)。虽然各人口学群体显示出相似性,但个人因素也影响着对礼貌的认知。与中年人和年轻人相比,老年人认为戏弄他人不那么有礼貌,而男性参与者则认为直率和讽刺性的陈述更有礼貌。亲社会谎言也存在地域差异,英国受试者比美国受试者认为亲社会谎言更有礼貌。这些发现强调了在理解反讽和亲社会谎言的社会功能时考虑丰富的背景材料和个人因素的重要性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Differences in politeness perception of irony and prosocial lies: Exploring the role of age, gender, and geographic location.","authors":"Pavitra Rao Makarla, Gitte Henssel Joergensen, Kendal Brice Tyner, Caroline Sprinkle, Kathrin Rothermich","doi":"10.1037/cep0000324","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000324","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In daily interactions, individuals use irony and prosocial lies for various reasons, for example, to be humorous, to criticize, or to be polite. While some studies have examined individual differences in perceiving the politeness of such language, research using naturalistic, context-rich materials is lacking. To address this gap, we utilized short videos to assess politeness perception in literal, ironic, and prosocial lie scenarios while also exploring differences based on age, gender, and geographical location. Our sample included 288 participants from the United States and the United Kingdom. We focused on five different types of language: literal positive, blunt, sarcastic, teasing, and prosocial lies. Participants rated the politeness of these statements and completed surveys on communication preferences (Self-Reported Sarcasm Questionnaire and Conversational Indirectness Scale Questionnaire). While the demographic groups showed similarities, individual factors also shaped politeness perception. Older adults perceived teasing as less polite than middle-aged and younger adults, and male participants rated blunt and sarcastic statements as more polite. Geographical variations were found for prosocial lies, with U.K. participants rating them more polite than their U.S. counterparts. These findings underscore the importance of considering context-rich materials and individual factors in understanding the social functions of irony and prosocial lies. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":" ","pages":"100-113"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140061229","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A motor task, not working memory, causes the revelation effect. 运动任务,而不是工作记忆,导致了启示效应。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-02 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000317
Hiroshi Miura, Yuji Itoh

Performing a cognitive task prior to making a recognition judgment increases the probability of old responses, which is known as the revelation effect. The criterion shift account (Niewiadomski & Hockley, 2001) proposes that occupation of working memory causes the revelation effect. However, we proposed that working memory does not cause it. Two experiments were conducted to disconfirm the relationship between working memory and the revelation effect and to consider an alternative explanation that metacognition causes the effect. In Experiment 1, the revelation effect was caused by a finger movement task, which puts little or no load on working memory. In Experiment 2, a metacognitive instruction that a cognitive task would make subsequent recognition easier induced a conservative criterion shift. The finding that a simple motor task caused the revelation effect in Experiment 1 disconfirms the relationship between working memory and the revelation effect and extends the boundaries of the occurrence of the effect. The findings in Experiment 2 suggest that metacognition may be related to the occurrence of the revelation effect. This study implies a paradoxical aspect of human cognition in that metacognition, which usually makes cognition more effective and rational, may also cause an irrational phenomenon, the revelation effect. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在做出识别判断之前执行认知任务会增加旧反应的概率,这被称为启示效应。标准转换解释(Niewiadomski和Hockley,2001)提出,工作记忆的占用导致了启示效应。然而,我们提出工作记忆并不是导致这种现象的原因。我们进行了两个实验来解开工作记忆与启示效应之间的关系,并考虑元认知导致启示效应的另一种解释。在实验1中,启示效应是由手指运动任务引起的,该任务对工作记忆几乎没有负荷。在实验2中,认知任务会使后续识别更容易的元认知指令导致了保守标准的转变。实验1发现,一个简单的运动任务引起了启示效应,这打破了工作记忆与启示效应之间的关系,拓展了启示效应发生的边界。实验2的研究结果表明,元认知可能与启示效应的发生有关。这项研究暗示了人类认知的一个悖论,即元认知通常使认知更加有效和理性,但也可能导致一种非理性现象,即启示效应。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"A motor task, not working memory, causes the revelation effect.","authors":"Hiroshi Miura, Yuji Itoh","doi":"10.1037/cep0000317","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000317","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Performing a cognitive task prior to making a recognition judgment increases the probability of old responses, which is known as the revelation effect. The criterion shift account (Niewiadomski & Hockley, 2001) proposes that occupation of working memory causes the revelation effect. However, we proposed that working memory does not cause it. Two experiments were conducted to disconfirm the relationship between working memory and the revelation effect and to consider an alternative explanation that metacognition causes the effect. In Experiment 1, the revelation effect was caused by a finger movement task, which puts little or no load on working memory. In Experiment 2, a metacognitive instruction that a cognitive task would make subsequent recognition easier induced a conservative criterion shift. The finding that a simple motor task caused the revelation effect in Experiment 1 disconfirms the relationship between working memory and the revelation effect and extends the boundaries of the occurrence of the effect. The findings in Experiment 2 suggest that metacognition may be related to the occurrence of the revelation effect. This study implies a paradoxical aspect of human cognition in that metacognition, which usually makes cognition more effective and rational, may also cause an irrational phenomenon, the revelation effect. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":" ","pages":"81-87"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71428984","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Looks can be deceiving: Investigating change blindness in an online setting. 外表是会骗人的:调查在线环境中的变化盲区。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-25 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000323
Saeeda Saeed, Arianna Cook, Victoria Mackie, Dana A Hayward

In the real world, we often fail to notice changes in our environment. In some cases, such as not noticing a car moving into our lane, the results can be catastrophic. This so-called change blindness has been seen experimentally both through failing to notice changes to images on-screen as well as failing to notice a change in other people's identity. However, less is known regarding how change blindness manifests in virtual settings varying in visual clutter or with varying types of interaction with someone prior to the change. Across two studies (n = 134), participants engaged in an online video chat with a confederate, with two levels of visual clutter (none, a lot) and three levels of interaction (none, light conversations about weather/TV, deeper conversations about goals/greatest regrets). We found no modulation of change blindness rates across perceptual clutter. Curiously, we found a large discrepancy in change blindness rates in Experiment 1 (79%; 52/66) versus Experiment 2 (16%; 11/68) that we explored, leading to some evidence that increasing the level of interaction led to greater change blindness rates, but only for pairs who identified as belonging to different ethnicities. Taken together, our work suggests that we may pay attention to people differently in virtual settings compared to in-person, that in-group and out-group biases may have an effect on change blindness rates, and that while clutter does not seem to affect change blindness rates, one's level of interaction just might. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在现实生活中,我们经常无法注意到周围环境的变化。在某些情况下,比如没有注意到一辆汽车驶入我们的车道,结果可能是灾难性的。这种所谓的 "变化盲 "已经在实验中被证实,既包括没有注意到屏幕上图像的变化,也包括没有注意到他人身份的变化。然而,人们对变化盲症在视觉杂乱的虚拟环境中或在变化前与他人进行不同类型的互动时的表现却知之甚少。在两项研究中(n = 134),参与者与一名同谋进行了在线视频聊天,聊天有两种视觉杂乱程度(无、大量)和三种互动程度(无、关于天气/电视的简单对话、关于目标/最大遗憾的深入对话)。我们没有发现盲变率会随着知觉杂乱程度的变化而变化。奇怪的是,我们发现实验 1(79%;52/66)与实验 2(16%;11/68)中的变化盲视率存在很大差异,这在一定程度上证明了互动程度的增加会导致变化盲视率的增加,但仅限于那些被认定属于不同种族的配对。综上所述,我们的研究表明,在虚拟环境中,我们对人的关注程度可能与面对面时不同;群体内和群体外的偏见可能会对变化盲视率产生影响;虽然杂乱无章似乎不会影响变化盲视率,但一个人的互动程度可能会产生影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Looks can be deceiving: Investigating change blindness in an online setting.","authors":"Saeeda Saeed, Arianna Cook, Victoria Mackie, Dana A Hayward","doi":"10.1037/cep0000323","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000323","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In the real world, we often fail to notice changes in our environment. In some cases, such as not noticing a car moving into our lane, the results can be catastrophic. This so-called change blindness has been seen experimentally both through failing to notice changes to images on-screen as well as failing to notice a change in other people's identity. However, less is known regarding how change blindness manifests in virtual settings varying in visual clutter or with varying types of interaction with someone prior to the change. Across two studies (<i>n</i> = 134), participants engaged in an online video chat with a confederate, with two levels of visual clutter (none, a lot) and three levels of interaction (none, light conversations about weather/TV, deeper conversations about goals/greatest regrets). We found no modulation of change blindness rates across perceptual clutter. Curiously, we found a large discrepancy in change blindness rates in Experiment 1 (79%; 52/66) versus Experiment 2 (16%; 11/68) that we explored, leading to some evidence that increasing the level of interaction led to greater change blindness rates, but only for pairs who identified as belonging to different ethnicities. Taken together, our work suggests that we may pay attention to people differently in virtual settings compared to in-person, that in-group and out-group biases may have an effect on change blindness rates, and that while clutter does not seem to affect change blindness rates, one's level of interaction just might. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":" ","pages":"1-8"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139565225","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Set size and the orthographic/phonological neighbourhood size effect in serial recognition: The importance of randomization. 序列识别中的集合大小和拼写/语音邻域大小效应:随机化的重要性。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-02 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000320
Dominic Guitard, Leonie M Miller, Ian Neath, Steven Roodenrys

The neighbourhood size effect refers to the finding of better memory for words with more orthographic/phonological neighbours than otherwise comparable words with fewer neighbours. Although many studies have replicated this result with serial recall, only one has used serial recognition. Greeno et al. (2022) found no neighbourhood size effect when a large stimulus pool was used and a reverse effect-better performance for small neighbourhood words-when a small stimulus pool was used. We reexamined these results but made two methodological changes. First, for the large pool, we randomly generated lists for each subject rather than creating one set of lists that all subjects experienced. Second, for the small pool, we randomly generated a small pool for each subject rather than using one small pool for all subjects. In both cases, we observed a neighbourhood size effect consistent with results from the serial recall literature. Implications for methodology and theoretical accounts of both the neighbourhood size effect and serial recognition are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

邻域大小效应指的是,与邻居较少的可比单词相比,拼写/语音邻居较多的单词记忆力更好。尽管许多研究已经用序列回忆复制了这一结果,但只有一项研究使用了序列识别。Greeno等人(2022)发现,当使用大的刺激池时,没有邻域大小效应,而当使用小的刺激库时,反向效应对小的邻域词表现更好。我们重新检查了这些结果,但在方法上做了两次改变。首先,对于大池,我们为每个主题随机生成列表,而不是创建一组所有主题都经历过的列表。其次,对于小水池,我们为每个受试者随机生成一个小水池,而不是为所有受试者使用一个小池。在这两种情况下,我们都观察到了邻域大小效应,这与系列回忆文献的结果一致。讨论了邻域大小效应和序列识别的方法论和理论解释的意义。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Set size and the orthographic/phonological neighbourhood size effect in serial recognition: The importance of randomization.","authors":"Dominic Guitard, Leonie M Miller, Ian Neath, Steven Roodenrys","doi":"10.1037/cep0000320","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000320","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The neighbourhood size effect refers to the finding of better memory for words with more orthographic/phonological neighbours than otherwise comparable words with fewer neighbours. Although many studies have replicated this result with serial recall, only one has used serial recognition. Greeno et al. (2022) found no neighbourhood size effect when a large stimulus pool was used and a reverse effect-better performance for small neighbourhood words-when a small stimulus pool was used. We reexamined these results but made two methodological changes. First, for the large pool, we randomly generated lists for each subject rather than creating one set of lists that all subjects experienced. Second, for the small pool, we randomly generated a small pool for each subject rather than using one small pool for all subjects. In both cases, we observed a neighbourhood size effect consistent with results from the serial recall literature. Implications for methodology and theoretical accounts of both the neighbourhood size effect and serial recognition are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":" ","pages":"9-16"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71428987","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Face processing in ADHD: A review of the N170 event-related potential. ADHD的面部加工:N170事件相关电位的回顾。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-30 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000321
Brady R T Roberts, Rebecca Trossman

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is associated with deficits in social functioning, including peer difficulties and poor relationship quality. Little is known, however, about the integrity of foundational sociocognitive abilities that support interpersonal interactions in ADHD. Face processing-a fundamental component of social cognition-has been a popular topic of recent investigations in this area. Researchers have attempted to delineate face processing mechanisms in ADHD to elucidate social deficits often seen in the disorder. Investigating the N170 event-related potential, a neural marker of face processing, has been a popular approach in this endeavour. Here, we present two accounts that offer competing views of how social deficits might arise in those with ADHD. Next, we systematically review and synthesise the literature on the N170 in ADHD to identify whether atypicalities in sociocognitive domains like face processing occur in this patient population. Gaps in the literature are identified and concrete solutions are offered to improve future research in this area. We end by discussing immediate implications for treatment approaches designed to address widely observed social deficits in individuals with ADHD. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

注意缺陷多动障碍(ADHD)与社会功能缺陷有关,包括同伴困难和关系质量差。然而,对于支持ADHD患者人际交往的基本社会认知能力的完整性,我们所知甚少。人脸处理是社会认知的一个基本组成部分,是近年来该领域研究的热门话题。研究人员试图描述多动症的面部加工机制,以阐明这种疾病中常见的社会缺陷。研究N170事件相关电位是人脸处理的一个神经标记,在这方面一直是一种流行的方法。在这里,我们提出了两种不同的观点来解释多动症患者的社会缺陷是如何产生的。接下来,我们系统地回顾和综合了N170在ADHD中的文献,以确定在社会认知领域,如面部处理是否发生在这一患者群体中。确定了文献中的差距,并提供了具体的解决方案,以改进该领域的未来研究。最后,我们讨论了治疗方法的直接意义,这些方法旨在解决多动症患者广泛观察到的社会缺陷。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Face processing in ADHD: A review of the N170 event-related potential.","authors":"Brady R T Roberts, Rebecca Trossman","doi":"10.1037/cep0000321","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000321","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is associated with deficits in social functioning, including peer difficulties and poor relationship quality. Little is known, however, about the integrity of foundational sociocognitive abilities that support interpersonal interactions in ADHD. Face processing-a fundamental component of social cognition-has been a popular topic of recent investigations in this area. Researchers have attempted to delineate face processing mechanisms in ADHD to elucidate social deficits often seen in the disorder. Investigating the N170 event-related potential, a neural marker of face processing, has been a popular approach in this endeavour. Here, we present two accounts that offer competing views of how social deficits might arise in those with ADHD. Next, we systematically review and synthesise the literature on the N170 in ADHD to identify whether atypicalities in sociocognitive domains like face processing occur in this patient population. Gaps in the literature are identified and concrete solutions are offered to improve future research in this area. We end by discussing immediate implications for treatment approaches designed to address widely observed social deficits in individuals with ADHD. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":" ","pages":"36-49"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138464218","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Active or passive? Investigating different types of cognitive fatigue. 主动还是被动?调查不同类型的认知疲劳
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-10 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000312
Todd Pickering, Bradley Wright, Linda Schücker, Clare MacMahon

Research in cognitive fatigue has identified the negative impact that cognitive exertion can have on subsequent task performance. An underexamined question is whether there are different types of fatigue, particularly: active fatigue, similar to cognitive fatigue, and passive fatigue, similar to boredom. This online study examined whether active and passive fatigue can be elicited and differentiated using computerized cognitive tasks. We compared subjective and behavioural outcomes to look for distinctions between fatigue types in response to different cognitive tasks. A sample of 122 participants (53% male; age 30.04 ± 3.50 years) rated their subjective state before and after one of three 8-min cognitive task conditions (TloadDback, Mackworth Clock, Documentary/Control). Next, participants also completed a second cognitive task (Flanker task). The task expected to be actively fatiguing (TloadDback) was rated the most difficult, effortful, and mentally and temporally demanding. The task expected to be passively fatiguing (Mackworth Clock) had the greatest increases in subjective fatigue, boredom, and sleepiness, and the greatest decrease in "want-to" motivation. There were no differences between conditions for Flanker performance. We showed that different fatigue types could be elicited using different computerized cognitive tasks. The passively fatiguing task had the most negative influence on subjective fatigue and motivation, suggesting a nonengaging or "boringly fatiguing" task induces a more detrimental type of fatigue. A key next step is to examine longer cognitive tasks to determine whether effects from different fatigue types become more prominent with time-on-task. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

对认知疲劳的研究发现,认知消耗会对随后的任务表现产生负面影响。一个尚未得到充分研究的问题是,是否存在不同类型的疲劳,特别是:与认知疲劳类似的主动疲劳和与无聊类似的被动疲劳。这项在线研究探讨了主动疲劳和被动疲劳是否可以通过计算机化认知任务来激发和区分。我们比较了主观和行为结果,以寻找不同认知任务下疲劳类型的区别。122 名参与者(53% 为男性;年龄为 30.04 ± 3.50 岁)在完成三项 8 分钟认知任务(TloadDback、Mackworth Clock、纪录片/对照)之一之前和之后对自己的主观状态进行了评分。接下来,参与者还完成了第二项认知任务(侧翼任务)。预期会使人主动疲劳的任务(TloadDback)被评为最难、最费力、对精神和时间要求最高的任务。被动疲劳任务(Mackworth Clock)的主观疲劳感、无聊感和困倦感增加最多,"想要 "的动机减少最多。不同条件下的 Flanker 成绩没有差异。我们的研究表明,使用不同的计算机化认知任务可以引起不同类型的疲劳。被动疲劳任务对主观疲劳和动机的负面影响最大,这表明非参与性或 "无聊疲劳 "任务会诱发更有害的疲劳类型。下一步的关键是研究时间更长的认知任务,以确定不同疲劳类型的影响是否会随着任务时间的延长而变得更加突出。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Active or passive? Investigating different types of cognitive fatigue.","authors":"Todd Pickering, Bradley Wright, Linda Schücker, Clare MacMahon","doi":"10.1037/cep0000312","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000312","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research in cognitive fatigue has identified the negative impact that cognitive exertion can have on subsequent task performance. An underexamined question is whether there are different types of fatigue, particularly: active fatigue, similar to cognitive fatigue, and passive fatigue, similar to boredom. This online study examined whether active and passive fatigue can be elicited and differentiated using computerized cognitive tasks. We compared subjective and behavioural outcomes to look for distinctions between fatigue types in response to different cognitive tasks. A sample of 122 participants (53% male; age 30.04 ± 3.50 years) rated their subjective state before and after one of three 8-min cognitive task conditions (TloadDback, Mackworth Clock, Documentary/Control). Next, participants also completed a second cognitive task (Flanker task). The task expected to be actively fatiguing (TloadDback) was rated the most difficult, effortful, and mentally and temporally demanding. The task expected to be passively fatiguing (Mackworth Clock) had the greatest increases in subjective fatigue, boredom, and sleepiness, and the greatest decrease in \"want-to\" motivation. There were no differences between conditions for Flanker performance. We showed that different fatigue types could be elicited using different computerized cognitive tasks. The passively fatiguing task had the most negative influence on subjective fatigue and motivation, suggesting a nonengaging or \"boringly fatiguing\" task induces a more detrimental type of fatigue. A key next step is to examine longer cognitive tasks to determine whether effects from different fatigue types become more prominent with time-on-task. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":" ","pages":"50-65"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9967727","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Facial emotional congruence in healthy adults and patients suffering from a psychiatric or neurological disorder. 健康成年人和精神或神经失调患者的面部情绪一致性。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-12-21 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000318
Pauline Gury, Pauline Narme, Alessandra Tommasi, Nathalie Ehrlé

The production of facial emotions is an important conveyor of social communication. The present review of the literature concerns the congruence of facial emotions, that is the facial muscular activation that takes place in response to the emotional facial expression perceived in others. Although scientific interest in facial emotions has increased exponentially in the last few years, the production of facial emotions is still underexplored as compared to emotional perception. Several studies, mainly conducted with electromyography, have shown that facial emotional congruence exists in a robust way, largely for anger and happiness. While facial emotional congruence was long considered as innate and automatic, recent work has demonstrated that several sociocultural factors may influence or reduce this ability, challenging its automaticity. From a neuroanatomical point of view, studies have clearly highlighted the implication of mirror neurons but our knowledge is still limited because of the few methodologies assessing this system and the lack of homogeneity between the protocols used. Many explanatory, and probably not mutually exclusive, theories of emotional facial congruence have been put forward. In experimental neuropsychology, emotional facial congruence has seldom been investigated but the few available results suggest an impairment in psychiatric and neurological patients. In view of the important role of emotional facial productions in human relations and social interactions, new methods for easy clinical assessment need to be designed for the diagnosis and the cognitive care of these abilities. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

面部情绪的产生是社会交流的重要传达者。本文献综述涉及面部情绪的一致性,即面部肌肉激活对他人感知到的情绪面部表情的反应。尽管在过去几年中,科学界对面部情绪的兴趣急剧增加,但与情绪感知相比,对面部情绪的产生仍未进行充分探索。主要通过肌电图进行的几项研究表明,面部情绪一致性以一种强有力的方式存在,主要针对愤怒和快乐。虽然面部情绪一致性长期以来被认为是与生俱来的自动能力,但最近的研究表明,一些社会文化因素可能会影响或削弱这种能力,从而对其自动性提出挑战。从神经解剖学的角度来看,研究明确强调了镜像神经元的影响,但由于评估该系统的方法很少,而且所使用的方案之间缺乏一致性,我们的知识仍然有限。人们提出了许多解释情绪面部一致性的理论,这些理论可能并不相互排斥。在实验神经心理学中,很少对情绪面部一致性进行研究,但现有的少数研究结果表明,精神病和神经病患者的情绪面部一致性存在障碍。鉴于情绪化面部表情在人际关系和社会交往中的重要作用,需要设计新的临床评估方法,以便对这些能力进行诊断和认知护理。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Facial emotional congruence in healthy adults and patients suffering from a psychiatric or neurological disorder.","authors":"Pauline Gury, Pauline Narme, Alessandra Tommasi, Nathalie Ehrlé","doi":"10.1037/cep0000318","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000318","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The production of facial emotions is an important conveyor of social communication. The present review of the literature concerns the congruence of facial emotions, that is the facial muscular activation that takes place in response to the emotional facial expression perceived in others. Although scientific interest in facial emotions has increased exponentially in the last few years, the production of facial emotions is still underexplored as compared to emotional perception. Several studies, mainly conducted with electromyography, have shown that facial emotional congruence exists in a robust way, largely for anger and happiness. While facial emotional congruence was long considered as innate and automatic, recent work has demonstrated that several sociocultural factors may influence or reduce this ability, challenging its automaticity. From a neuroanatomical point of view, studies have clearly highlighted the implication of mirror neurons but our knowledge is still limited because of the few methodologies assessing this system and the lack of homogeneity between the protocols used. Many explanatory, and probably not mutually exclusive, theories of emotional facial congruence have been put forward. In experimental neuropsychology, emotional facial congruence has seldom been investigated but the few available results suggest an impairment in psychiatric and neurological patients. In view of the important role of emotional facial productions in human relations and social interactions, new methods for easy clinical assessment need to be designed for the diagnosis and the cognitive care of these abilities. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":" ","pages":"17-35"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138832951","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceptions of (in)sincerity in satirical discourse: A study of word reading times using minimally different texts. 讽刺话语中对真诚的感知:使用最小差异文本的单词阅读时间研究。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-10 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000314
Stephen Skalicky

The present study investigated the relationship between satirical discourse processing and a theoretical model of satire comprehension known as satirical uptake. Word reading times and participant perceptions of sincerity for a set of minimally different satirical and nonsatirical texts were modelled considering individual differences such as need for cognition (NFC) and genre familiarity. Across two experiments, participants read either a mixture of satirical and nonsatirical texts (Experiment 1) or only satirical/nonsatirical texts (Experiment 2), indicating the degree to which they felt the meaning of the text was sincere. Results from both experiments demonstrated satirical texts were read slower than nonsatirical texts. Moreover, longer word reading times were associated with lower sincerity ratings for satirical texts, but only after participants encountered one or more satirical texts. NFC interacted with reading times in Experiment 1 but not Experiment 2, and there were no strong effects for genre familiarity in either experiment. The main conclusion drawn from these results is that successful satirical uptake may require greater processing effort, a result which aligns with theoretical models of satirical discourse as well as the related construct of verbal irony. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究探讨了讽刺话语加工与讽刺理解理论模型——讽刺吸收之间的关系。考虑到个体差异,如认知需求(NFC)和体裁熟悉度,对一组最小差异的讽刺和非讽刺文本的单词阅读时间和参与者对诚意的感知进行了建模。在两个实验中,参与者要么阅读讽刺和非讽刺的混合文本(实验1),要么只阅读讽刺/非讽刺的文本(实验2),这表明他们认为文本的意思是真诚的程度。两个实验的结果都表明,讽刺文本的阅读速度比非讽刺文本慢。此外,较长的单词阅读时间与较低的讽刺文本的诚意评级有关,但只有在参与者遇到一个或多个讽刺文本之后。在实验1中,NFC与阅读时间存在交互作用,而在实验2中不存在交互作用;在实验2中,NFC对体裁熟悉度的影响均不明显。从这些结果中得出的主要结论是,成功的讽刺吸收可能需要更大的加工努力,这一结果与讽刺话语的理论模型以及言语讽刺的相关结构相一致。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Perceptions of (in)sincerity in satirical discourse: A study of word reading times using minimally different texts.","authors":"Stephen Skalicky","doi":"10.1037/cep0000314","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000314","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present study investigated the relationship between satirical discourse processing and a theoretical model of satire comprehension known as satirical uptake. Word reading times and participant perceptions of sincerity for a set of minimally different satirical and nonsatirical texts were modelled considering individual differences such as need for cognition (NFC) and genre familiarity. Across two experiments, participants read either a mixture of satirical and nonsatirical texts (Experiment 1) or only satirical/nonsatirical texts (Experiment 2), indicating the degree to which they felt the meaning of the text was sincere. Results from both experiments demonstrated satirical texts were read slower than nonsatirical texts. Moreover, longer word reading times were associated with lower sincerity ratings for satirical texts, but only after participants encountered one or more satirical texts. NFC interacted with reading times in Experiment 1 but not Experiment 2, and there were no strong effects for genre familiarity in either experiment. The main conclusion drawn from these results is that successful satirical uptake may require greater processing effort, a result which aligns with theoretical models of satirical discourse as well as the related construct of verbal irony. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":" ","pages":"247-261"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9967724","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1