Pub Date : 2021-09-01Epub Date: 2021-02-04DOI: 10.1037/cep0000210
Lixia Yang, Linda Truong, Lingqian Li
Past research demonstrated enhanced memory for information encoded with relevance to a survival scenario compared to a control scenario, an effect referred to as the survival processing effect in memory. This effect has been explained by a proximate mechanism hypothesis (i.e., survival processing enables deep elaborative processing that promotes memory). In support of this hypothesis, past research found that, during encoding, the survival processing effect was largely intact under a perceptual or low-load secondary task condition but eliminated under a high-load secondary task condition. To test semantic encoding as a possible proximate mechanism, the current study assesses the impact of high-load and low-load divided attention tasks that require semantic processing of digits on the survival processing effect. Seventy-two young adults rated words for their relevance to two survival scenarios (i.e., grassland and mountain) and one non-survival control scenario (i.e., cruise), while completing a concurrent high-load or low-load semantic digit-monitoring task. No survival processing effect was found in either condition. The results suggest that semantic encoding probably serves as a proximate mechanism for the survival processing effect in memory. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
过去的研究表明,与控制情景相比,对与生存情景相关的信息编码的记忆增强,这种效应被称为记忆中的生存处理效应。这种效应可以用一种近似的机制假说来解释(即,生存加工能够促进深度精细加工,从而促进记忆)。为了支持这一假设,过去的研究发现,在编码过程中,在感知或低负荷的次要任务条件下,生存加工效应基本完整,而在高负荷的次要任务条件下,生存加工效应被消除。为了验证语义编码作为一种可能的近似机制,本研究评估了需要数字语义加工的高负荷和低负荷分散注意任务对生存加工效应的影响。72名年轻人在完成高负荷或低负荷语义数字监测任务的同时,对单词与两个生存情景(即草原和山区)和一个非生存控制情景(即巡航)的相关性进行评分。两种条件下均未发现生存加工效应。结果表明,语义编码可能是记忆中生存加工效应的近似机制。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Survival processing effect in memory under semantic divided attention.","authors":"Lixia Yang, Linda Truong, Lingqian Li","doi":"10.1037/cep0000210","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000210","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Past research demonstrated enhanced memory for information encoded with relevance to a survival scenario compared to a control scenario, an effect referred to as the survival processing effect in memory. This effect has been explained by a proximate mechanism hypothesis (i.e., survival processing enables deep elaborative processing that promotes memory). In support of this hypothesis, past research found that, during encoding, the survival processing effect was largely intact under a perceptual or low-load secondary task condition but eliminated under a high-load secondary task condition. To test semantic encoding as a possible proximate mechanism, the current study assesses the impact of high-load and low-load divided attention tasks that require semantic processing of digits on the survival processing effect. Seventy-two young adults rated words for their relevance to two survival scenarios (i.e., grassland and mountain) and one non-survival control scenario (i.e., cruise), while completing a concurrent high-load or low-load semantic digit-monitoring task. No survival processing effect was found in either condition. The results suggest that semantic encoding probably serves as a proximate mechanism for the survival processing effect in memory. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 3","pages":"299-306"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25331845","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2021-09-01Epub Date: 2021-06-17DOI: 10.1037/cep0000257
Jacqueline Cummine, Angela Cullum, Daniel Aalto, Tyson Sereda, Cassidy Fleming, Alesha Reed, Amberley Ostevik, Sienna Cashion-Dextrase, Caroline C Jeffery, William E Hodgetts
Objective: There is a strong relationship between reading and articulation (Lervåg & Hulme, 2009; Pan et al., 2011). Given the tight coupling of these processes, innovative approaches are needed to understand the intricacies associated with print-speech connections. Here we ran a series of tightly controlled experiments to examine the impact of mouth perturbations on silent reading.
Method: We altered the mouth, via somatosensory feedback, in several ways: (a) a large lollipop in the mouth (E1), (b) a candy stick (bite bar) held horizontally between the teeth (E2), and (c) lidocaine that served to numb the mouth (E3). Three tasks were completed: (a) picture categorization, (b) "spell" lexical decision (Spell-LDT; "does the letter string spell a real word, yes or no?"), and (c) "sound" lexical decision (Sound-LDT; "does the letter string sound like a real word, yes or no?"). Participants (N = 97; E1 = 27; E2 = 32; E3 = 38) completed each of the tasks two times: once with a somatosensory perturbation (lollipop, bite bar, or lidocaine) and once without.
Results: For each experiment, a linear mixed effects analysis was run. Overall, we found that the lollipop (E1) and lidocaine (E3) had some specific effects on word recognition (e.g., for "no" responses), particularly in the Spell-LDT, whereas the bite bar (E2) had no effect on word recognition. The picture categorization task was not impacted by any perturbations.
Conclusion: These findings provide evidence that sensorimotor information is connected to reading. We discuss how these findings advance our understanding of a print-to-speech framework. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
{"title":"From lollipops to lidocaine: The need for a universal print-to-speech framework.","authors":"Jacqueline Cummine, Angela Cullum, Daniel Aalto, Tyson Sereda, Cassidy Fleming, Alesha Reed, Amberley Ostevik, Sienna Cashion-Dextrase, Caroline C Jeffery, William E Hodgetts","doi":"10.1037/cep0000257","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000257","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>There is a strong relationship between reading and articulation (Lervåg & Hulme, 2009; Pan et al., 2011). Given the tight coupling of these processes, innovative approaches are needed to understand the intricacies associated with print-speech connections. Here we ran a series of tightly controlled experiments to examine the impact of mouth perturbations on silent reading.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We altered the mouth, via somatosensory feedback, in several ways: (a) a large lollipop in the mouth (E1), (b) a candy stick (bite bar) held horizontally between the teeth (E2), and (c) lidocaine that served to numb the mouth (E3). Three tasks were completed: (a) picture categorization, (b) \"spell\" lexical decision (Spell-LDT; \"does the letter string spell a real word, yes or no?\"), and (c) \"sound\" lexical decision (Sound-LDT; \"does the letter string sound like a real word, yes or no?\"). Participants (<i>N</i> = 97; E1 = 27; E2 = 32; E3 = 38) completed each of the tasks two times: once with a somatosensory perturbation (lollipop, bite bar, or lidocaine) and once without.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>For each experiment, a linear mixed effects analysis was run. Overall, we found that the lollipop (E1) and lidocaine (E3) had some specific effects on word recognition (e.g., for \"no\" responses), particularly in the Spell-LDT, whereas the bite bar (E2) had no effect on word recognition. The picture categorization task was not impacted by any perturbations.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>These findings provide evidence that sensorimotor information is connected to reading. We discuss how these findings advance our understanding of a print-to-speech framework. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 3","pages":"279-298"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39240198","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2021-09-01Epub Date: 2021-05-06DOI: 10.1037/cep0000255
Harinder Aujla
Computational models of semantic memory have been successful in accounting for a wide range of cognitive phenomena, including word categorization, semantic priming, and release from proactive interference. Conventionally, the texts input to these models have been curated to represent the average individual's language experience. While this approach has proven successful for making predictions that generalize across individuals, it prevents consideration of situations in which individuals have divergent semantic representations. The use of a representative corpus prevents the generation of predictions specific to the language experience of an individual. While this limitation has been discussed in the literature, previous investigations have not yet validated such corpus-specific predictions. I present an approach to generate corpus-specific semantic representations using internet news sites as corpora. I then validate the semantic representations against subjects that read specific news sites. Results demonstrate that similarities between news sites are specific to the words under consideration and that news site-specific representations successfully predict differential priming effects in lexical decision as a function of news readership. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
语义记忆的计算模型已经成功地解释了广泛的认知现象,包括词分类、语义启动和从主动干扰中释放。按照惯例,输入到这些模型中的文本都是经过整理的,以代表一般人的语言经验。虽然这种方法已被证明可以成功地预测个体,但它阻止了个体具有不同语义表示的情况。使用代表性语料库可以防止生成特定于个人语言经验的预测。虽然这一限制已经在文献中讨论过,但以前的调查尚未证实这种针对语料库的预测。我提出了一种使用互联网新闻网站作为语料库来生成语料库特定语义表示的方法。然后,我针对阅读特定新闻站点的主题验证语义表示。结果表明,新闻网站之间的相似性是特定于所考虑的单词的,新闻网站特定表征成功地预测了词汇决策中作为新闻读者的函数的差异启动效应。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Language experience predicts semantic priming of lexical decision.","authors":"Harinder Aujla","doi":"10.1037/cep0000255","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000255","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Computational models of semantic memory have been successful in accounting for a wide range of cognitive phenomena, including word categorization, semantic priming, and release from proactive interference. Conventionally, the texts input to these models have been curated to represent the average individual's language experience. While this approach has proven successful for making predictions that generalize across individuals, it prevents consideration of situations in which individuals have divergent semantic representations. The use of a representative corpus prevents the generation of predictions specific to the language experience of an individual. While this limitation has been discussed in the literature, previous investigations have not yet validated such corpus-specific predictions. I present an approach to generate corpus-specific semantic representations using internet news sites as corpora. I then validate the semantic representations against subjects that read specific news sites. Results demonstrate that similarities between news sites are specific to the words under consideration and that news site-specific representations successfully predict differential priming effects in lexical decision as a function of news readership. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 3","pages":"235-244"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"38956090","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2021-09-01Epub Date: 2021-02-04DOI: 10.1037/cep0000238
Hélène Maire
The self is a crucial component of the psychic life and plays a central role for the adaptation to the environment. In daily life, this adaptative function is ensured, inter alia, by numerous biases filtering information and favoring those which are self-related. After succinctly reviewing the most documented among them which are affective and mnesic biases, the current paper provides a critical review of literature about a bias which is supposed to be perceptive, the self-prioritization effect (SPE). That has been revealed by Sui et al. (2012, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(5), 1105) with an astute matching task and consists in the fact that arbitrarily tagging shapes to a word referring to the participant (e.g., you-square) leads to faster and more accurate responses as compared to shapes tagged to a word referring to another identity (e.g., strange-circle). The methodological variations of this task and the SPE's both extension and putative origins will be presented, as well as the restrictions which border it, related to the individuals, to the experimental situation and to some more general properties of the self. Finally, some avenues for future research will be proposed, drawing some promising paths: beyond being a robust and intriguing phenomenon, SPE can indeed be considered as a convenient tool to assess some mechanisms underlying social cognition, in various fields using an experimental approach such as developmental psychology and social psychology. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
自我是心理生活的重要组成部分,对心理适应环境起着核心作用。在日常生活中,这种适应功能的保证,除其他外,是通过许多偏见过滤信息并偏爱那些与自身有关的信息。在简要回顾了其中文献记载最多的情感偏差和记忆偏差之后,本文对一种被认为是感知的偏差——自我优先化效应(SPE)的文献进行了批判性的回顾。Sui等人(2012,Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(5), 1105)通过一项机敏的匹配任务揭示了这一点,其中包括这样一个事实,即任意标记与参与者相关的单词的形状(例如,u-square)比标记与另一个身份相关的单词的形状(例如,strange-circle)更快,更准确的反应。本文将介绍这项任务的方法变化,SPE的扩展和假定的起源,以及与个体、实验情况和自我的一些更一般的属性相关的边界限制。最后,提出了未来研究的一些途径,并提出了一些有希望的途径:除了是一个强大而有趣的现象之外,SPE确实可以被认为是一种方便的工具,可以使用发展心理学和社会心理学等实验方法在各个领域评估社会认知的一些机制。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"« Moi d'abord ! » ou l'égocentrisme ordinaire : Une revue critique de l'effet de priorité au Soi.","authors":"Hélène Maire","doi":"10.1037/cep0000238","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000238","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The self is a crucial component of the psychic life and plays a central role for the adaptation to the environment. In daily life, this adaptative function is ensured, inter alia, by numerous biases filtering information and favoring those which are self-related. After succinctly reviewing the most documented among them which are affective and mnesic biases, the current paper provides a critical review of literature about a bias which is supposed to be perceptive, the self-prioritization effect (SPE). That has been revealed by Sui et al. (2012, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(5), 1105) with an astute matching task and consists in the fact that arbitrarily tagging shapes to a word referring to the participant (e.g., you-square) leads to faster and more accurate responses as compared to shapes tagged to a word referring to another identity (e.g., strange-circle). The methodological variations of this task and the SPE's both extension and putative origins will be presented, as well as the restrictions which border it, related to the individuals, to the experimental situation and to some more general properties of the self. Finally, some avenues for future research will be proposed, drawing some promising paths: beyond being a robust and intriguing phenomenon, SPE can indeed be considered as a convenient tool to assess some mechanisms underlying social cognition, in various fields using an experimental approach such as developmental psychology and social psychology. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 3","pages":"307-325"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25331788","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2021-09-01Epub Date: 2021-03-29DOI: 10.1037/cep0000248
Dominic Guitard, Jean Saint-Aubin
In backward immediate serial recall, participants recall lists of items immediately after their presentation by beginning with the last presented item and ending with the first presented one. Despite the similarities with forward recall in which participants recall the items from the first to the last presented, benchmark memory phenomena reliably found in forward recall are not constantly observed in backward recall. Here, we proposed a new framework called the encoding-retrieval matching (ERM) hypothesis to account for backward recall. The ERM retains the main features of the visuospatial hypothesis and the item-order trade-off hypothesis, the two dominant accounts of backward recall. According to the ERM, output modality and foreknowledge of recall direction influence the availability of visuospatial representations and the weight devoted to item and order processing. We tested the ERM with irrelevant speech, a well-known working memory factor disrupting forward recall. In two experiments, we manipulated recall direction (forward vs. backward), irrelevant speech (control vs. irrelevant speech), and response modality (manual vs. oral). As predicted by the ERM, when recall direction was unpredictable in Experiment 1, the magnitude of the irrelevant speech effect was larger in backward manual recall than in backward oral recall. In Experiment 2, recall direction was predictable. As predicted by the ERM, in backward recall, the irrelevant speech effect was reduced with a manual response and absent with an oral response. We concluded that ERM effectively accounts for the complex interplay between response modality, foreknowledge of recall direction, and benchmark memory effects in backward recall. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
在逆向直接连续回忆中,参与者在展示完后立即回忆项目列表,从最后展示的项目开始,以第一个展示的项目结束。尽管在前向回忆中,参与者回忆从第一个到最后一个出现的项目,这与前向回忆有相似之处,但在后向回忆中可靠发现的基准记忆现象在后向回忆中并不经常观察到。在此,我们提出了一个新的框架,称为编码检索匹配(ERM)假设来解释向后回忆。ERM保留了视觉空间假说和项目顺序权衡假说的主要特征,这是向后回忆的两个主要解释。根据ERM,输出方式和回忆方向的预知性影响视觉空间表征的可用性以及用于项目和订单处理的权重。我们用不相关的言语来测试ERM,这是一个众所周知的工作记忆因素,会干扰前向回忆。在两个实验中,我们操纵了回忆方向(向前与向后)、无关言语(对照与无关言语)和反应方式(手动与口头)。正如ERM预测的那样,当实验1中回忆方向不可预测时,不相关言语效应的大小在向后手动回忆中大于向后口头回忆。在实验2中,回忆方向是可预测的。正如ERM预测的那样,在向后回忆中,手工反应减少了无关言语效应,而口头反应则没有。结果表明,ERM有效地解释了后向回忆中反应方式、回忆方向预知和基准记忆效应之间复杂的相互作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"The irrelevant speech effect in backward recall is modulated by foreknowledge of recall direction and response modality.","authors":"Dominic Guitard, Jean Saint-Aubin","doi":"10.1037/cep0000248","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000248","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In backward immediate serial recall, participants recall lists of items immediately after their presentation by beginning with the last presented item and ending with the first presented one. Despite the similarities with forward recall in which participants recall the items from the first to the last presented, benchmark memory phenomena reliably found in forward recall are not constantly observed in backward recall. Here, we proposed a new framework called the encoding-retrieval matching (ERM) hypothesis to account for backward recall. The ERM retains the main features of the visuospatial hypothesis and the item-order trade-off hypothesis, the two dominant accounts of backward recall. According to the ERM, output modality and foreknowledge of recall direction influence the availability of visuospatial representations and the weight devoted to item and order processing. We tested the ERM with irrelevant speech, a well-known working memory factor disrupting forward recall. In two experiments, we manipulated recall direction (forward vs. backward), irrelevant speech (control vs. irrelevant speech), and response modality (manual vs. oral). As predicted by the ERM, when recall direction was unpredictable in Experiment 1, the magnitude of the irrelevant speech effect was larger in backward manual recall than in backward oral recall. In Experiment 2, recall direction was predictable. As predicted by the ERM, in backward recall, the irrelevant speech effect was reduced with a manual response and absent with an oral response. We concluded that ERM effectively accounts for the complex interplay between response modality, foreknowledge of recall direction, and benchmark memory effects in backward recall. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 3","pages":"245-260"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25527037","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2021-09-01Epub Date: 2021-06-07DOI: 10.1037/cep0000261
Derek Besner, David McLean, Torin Young
It is a widely held view that the determination of eye gaze direction is "automatic" in various senses (e.g., innate; informationally encapsulated; triggered without intent). The determination of arrow direction is also held to be automatic (following a certain amount of learning) despite not being innate. The present experiments evaluate the automaticity assumption of both eyes and arrows in terms of an interference criterion. The results of 10 experiments support the inference that explicit judgements of eye gaze direction, when participants respond with a lateralized key press, are (a) neither automatic in the strong sense (they are interfered with by an uninformative, incongruent arrow in the display) and (b) nor are they are automatic in a weaker sense (uninformative, incongruent arrows interfere more strongly with the determination of eye gaze direction than uninformative, incongruent eyes interfere with the arrow direction task). However, the determination of arrow direction is also not strongly automatic, given that it is interfered with by irrelevant eyes. At least with respect to an interference criterion, the determination of eye gaze direction appears less prepotent than the determination of arrow direction, which itself is only weakly automatic. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
人们普遍认为,眼睛注视方向的决定在各种意义上是“自动的”(例如,天生的;信息封装;触发无意图)。箭头方向的决定也被认为是自动的(遵循一定数量的学习),尽管不是天生的。本实验根据干涉准则对眼睛和箭头的自动性假设进行了评价。10个实验的结果支持这样的推断,即当参与者以侧边按键作出反应时,对眼睛注视方向的明确判断(a)在强意义上既不是自动的(它们受到显示器中不提供信息的、不一致的箭头的干扰),(b)在弱意义上也不是自动的(不提供信息的、不一致的箭头比不提供信息的箭头更强烈地干扰眼睛注视方向的确定)。不一致的眼睛干扰箭头方向任务)。然而,箭头方向的确定也不是很强的自动,因为它被不相干的眼睛干扰。至少就干扰标准而言,眼睛凝视方向的确定似乎不如箭头方向的确定更有优势,箭头方向本身只是弱自动的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"On the determination of eye gaze and arrow direction: Automaticity reconsidered.","authors":"Derek Besner, David McLean, Torin Young","doi":"10.1037/cep0000261","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000261","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is a widely held view that the determination of eye gaze direction is \"automatic\" in various senses (e.g., innate; informationally encapsulated; triggered without intent). The determination of arrow direction is also held to be automatic (following a certain amount of learning) despite not being innate. The present experiments evaluate the automaticity assumption of both eyes and arrows in terms of an interference criterion. The results of 10 experiments support the inference that explicit judgements of eye gaze direction, when participants respond with a lateralized key press, are (a) neither automatic in the strong sense (they are interfered with by an uninformative, incongruent arrow in the display) and (b) nor are they are automatic in a weaker sense (uninformative, incongruent arrows interfere more strongly with the determination of eye gaze direction than uninformative, incongruent eyes interfere with the arrow direction task). However, the determination of arrow direction is also not strongly automatic, given that it is interfered with by irrelevant eyes. At least with respect to an interference criterion, the determination of eye gaze direction appears less prepotent than the determination of arrow direction, which itself is only weakly automatic. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 3","pages":"261-278"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39068151","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2021-06-01Epub Date: 2021-02-04DOI: 10.1037/cep0000242
Felix S Pambuccian, Gary E Raney
One of Katz's significant contributions to the study of figurative language is his work highlighting the importance of familiarity in metaphor processing. In this study, we examined how metaphor and simile comprehension change as a function of familiarity. The Categorization model (Glucksberg, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2003, 7, 92) proposes that metaphor comprehension relies on an automatic process (categorization) regardless of familiarity. By contrast, the Career of Metaphor model (Bowdle & Gentner, Psychological Review, 2005, 112, 193) proposes that as conventionality or familiarity declines, comprehension shifts from categorization to comparison, a controlled, effortful process. Both models assume that similes, regardless of familiarity, are understood through controlled, comparison processes. The present study used a resource depletion manipulation to investigate the processes recruited in metaphor and simile comprehension. Because resource depletion negatively impacts controlled, effortful processes but does not affect automatic processes (Schmeichel et al., Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2003, 85, 33), comparing the effects of resource depletion on comprehension of familiar and unfamiliar metaphors and similes may shed light on the comprehension processes (controlled or automatic) being used. Across two experiments, we induced resource depletion using a Stroop task and tested the impact of depletion on metaphor and simile comprehension. Metaphor stimuli were drawn from Katz et al. (Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 1988, 3, 191) normed database; similes were constructed by adding the word like to each metaphor (e.g., love is (like) a flower). For both tropes, resource depletion slowed comprehension of unfamiliar expressions but had no little-or-no impact on highly familiar expressions. Our results suggest that comprehension of both similes and metaphors shifts from automatic to controlled processing as familiarity decreases. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
卡茨对比喻语言研究的重要贡献之一是他强调了隐喻处理中熟悉度的重要性。在本研究中,我们考察了隐喻和明喻理解是如何随着熟悉度的变化而变化的。分类模型(Glucksberg, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2003,7,92)提出,隐喻理解依赖于一个自动过程(分类),而与熟悉程度无关。相比之下,隐喻生涯模型(Bowdle & genner, Psychological Review, 2005,112,193)提出,随着传统或熟悉度的下降,理解从分类转向比较,这是一个受控的、费力的过程。这两种模型都假设,无论熟悉程度如何,比喻都是通过受控的比较过程来理解的。本研究采用资源耗竭操作来研究隐喻和明喻理解过程。由于资源枯竭会对可控的、费力的过程产生负面影响,但不会对自动过程产生影响(Schmeichel et al., Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2003,85,33),比较资源枯竭对熟悉和不熟悉的隐喻和明语理解的影响,可能有助于了解正在使用的理解过程(受控或自动)。在两个实验中,我们使用Stroop任务诱导资源枯竭,并测试了资源枯竭对隐喻和明喻理解的影响。隐喻刺激来源于Katz et al. (Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 1988,3,191)规范数据库;明喻是通过在每个隐喻后面加上like这个词来构建的(例如,love is (like) a flower)。对于这两个比喻,资源枯竭减缓了对不熟悉的表达的理解,但对高度熟悉的表达没有影响。我们的研究结果表明,随着熟悉程度的降低,对明喻和隐喻的理解从自动处理转变为控制处理。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"A simile is (like) a metaphor: Comparing metaphor and simile processing across the familiarity spectrum.","authors":"Felix S Pambuccian, Gary E Raney","doi":"10.1037/cep0000242","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000242","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>One of Katz's significant contributions to the study of figurative language is his work highlighting the importance of familiarity in metaphor processing. In this study, we examined how metaphor and simile comprehension change as a function of familiarity. The Categorization model (Glucksberg, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2003, 7, 92) proposes that metaphor comprehension relies on an automatic process (categorization) regardless of familiarity. By contrast, the Career of Metaphor model (Bowdle & Gentner, Psychological Review, 2005, 112, 193) proposes that as conventionality or familiarity declines, comprehension shifts from categorization to comparison, a controlled, effortful process. Both models assume that similes, regardless of familiarity, are understood through controlled, comparison processes. The present study used a resource depletion manipulation to investigate the processes recruited in metaphor and simile comprehension. Because resource depletion negatively impacts controlled, effortful processes but does not affect automatic processes (Schmeichel et al., Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2003, 85, 33), comparing the effects of resource depletion on comprehension of familiar and unfamiliar metaphors and similes may shed light on the comprehension processes (controlled or automatic) being used. Across two experiments, we induced resource depletion using a Stroop task and tested the impact of depletion on metaphor and simile comprehension. Metaphor stimuli were drawn from Katz et al. (Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 1988, 3, 191) normed database; similes were constructed by adding the word like to each metaphor (e.g., love is (like) a flower). For both tropes, resource depletion slowed comprehension of unfamiliar expressions but had no little-or-no impact on highly familiar expressions. Our results suggest that comprehension of both similes and metaphors shifts from automatic to controlled processing as familiarity decreases. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 2","pages":"182-188"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25331844","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2021-06-01Epub Date: 2021-03-29DOI: 10.1037/cep0000251
Hamad Al-Azary, Christina L Gagné, Thomas L Spalding
People take longer to determine that metaphoric sentences (e.g., some birds are flutes) are literally false compared to anomalous sentences (e.g., some birds are pickles). This metaphor interference effect (MIE) shows that metaphorical interpretations are automatically computed even in contexts and tasks that only require literal interpretations. Although a well-replicated finding, the MIE has only been investigated in sentence stimuli in which the metaphoric composition is explicitly stated (such that birds are asserted to be flutes). This raises questions about the generalizability of the MIE because (a) A is B metaphors are rare in discourse and (b) other metaphor variants, such as flute bird, are unspecified in their metaphoric composition (i.e., do not specifically assert which concept, if any, is metaphorical). In this experiment, we investigated whether metaphoric modifier-noun phrases such as flute bird and creamy sky produce a MIE. In addition, we explored if word-level semantic variables (semantic neighborhood density and concreteness) play a role in the MIE. We asked participants to determine if modifier-noun phrases refer to things that literally exist or not. We found a MIE in which metaphoric phrases (e.g., flute bird, creamy sky) took longer to judge as literally false relative to scrambled counterparts (e.g., flute sky, creamy bird). Moreover, we found that word-level semantic variables affect the magnitude of the MIE only for adjective-noun phrases. Therefore, metaphoric meaning can be automatically extracted from metaphoric compounds, suggesting that the MIE is more robust than previously demonstrated. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
与异常句子(例如,有些鸟是咸菜)相比,人们需要更长的时间来确定隐喻句子(例如,有些鸟是笛子)在字面上是错误的。这种隐喻干扰效应表明,即使在只需要字面解释的语境和任务中,隐喻解释也是自动计算出来的。尽管这一发现得到了很好的重复,但MIE只在隐喻成分明确陈述的句子刺激中进行了研究(例如,鸟类被断言为笛子)。这就提出了关于MIE的可泛化性的问题,因为(a) ais B隐喻在语篇中很少见,(B)其他隐喻变体,如笛鸟,在其隐喻构成中未指明(即,没有具体断言哪个概念是隐喻的,如果有的话)。在本实验中,我们考察了长笛鸟和奶油天空等隐喻性修饰语-名词短语是否产生MIE。此外,我们还探讨了词级语义变量(语义邻域密度和具体性)是否在MIE中发挥作用。我们要求参与者判断修饰语-名词短语是否指的是字面上存在的事物。我们发现,在MIE中,隐喻短语(例如,长笛鸟,奶油色的天空)相对于杂乱无章的对应短语(例如,长笛天空,奶油色的鸟),需要更长的时间来判断字面上的错误。此外,我们发现词级语义变量仅对形容词-名词短语的MIE大小有影响。因此,隐喻意义可以自动从隐喻复合词中提取出来,这表明MIE比之前证明的更为稳健。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Flute birds and creamy skies: The metaphor interference effect in modifier-noun phrases.","authors":"Hamad Al-Azary, Christina L Gagné, Thomas L Spalding","doi":"10.1037/cep0000251","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000251","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People take longer to determine that metaphoric sentences (e.g., some birds are flutes) are literally false compared to anomalous sentences (e.g., some birds are pickles). This metaphor interference effect (MIE) shows that metaphorical interpretations are automatically computed even in contexts and tasks that only require literal interpretations. Although a well-replicated finding, the MIE has only been investigated in sentence stimuli in which the metaphoric composition is explicitly stated (such that birds are asserted to be flutes). This raises questions about the generalizability of the MIE because (a) A is B metaphors are rare in discourse and (b) other metaphor variants, such as flute bird, are unspecified in their metaphoric composition (i.e., do not specifically assert which concept, if any, is metaphorical). In this experiment, we investigated whether metaphoric modifier-noun phrases such as flute bird and creamy sky produce a MIE. In addition, we explored if word-level semantic variables (semantic neighborhood density and concreteness) play a role in the MIE. We asked participants to determine if modifier-noun phrases refer to things that literally exist or not. We found a MIE in which metaphoric phrases (e.g., flute bird, creamy sky) took longer to judge as literally false relative to scrambled counterparts (e.g., flute sky, creamy bird). Moreover, we found that word-level semantic variables affect the magnitude of the MIE only for adjective-noun phrases. Therefore, metaphoric meaning can be automatically extracted from metaphoric compounds, suggesting that the MIE is more robust than previously demonstrated. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 2","pages":"175-181"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25527038","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Throughout his 45-year career, Professor Albert Katz (Department of Psychology, Western University) has tackled challeng ing aspects of human communication in a way that creatively merges the theoretical insights and empirical rigor of cognitive linguistics, psycholinguistics, and cognitive neuroscience. In this personal reflection, Professor Katz writes a short biographical piece on the life journey that led to his research programs. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
在其 45 年的职业生涯中,阿尔伯特-卡茨教授(西区大学心理学系)以一种创造性地融合认知语言学、心理语言学和认知神经科学的理论见解和严谨实证的方式,解决了人类交流中具有挑战性的问题。在这篇个人反思中,卡茨教授写了一篇简短的自传,介绍了他从事研究项目的人生历程。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"The psychology of saying what you don't mean: Celebrating the research career of Professor Albert Katz-A personal reflection.","authors":"Albert N Katz","doi":"10.1037/cep0000260","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000260","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Throughout his 45-year career, Professor Albert Katz (Department of Psychology, Western University) has tackled challeng ing aspects of human communication in a way that creatively merges the theoretical insights and empirical rigor of cognitive linguistics, psycholinguistics, and cognitive neuroscience. In this personal reflection, Professor Katz writes a short biographical piece on the life journey that led to his research programs. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 2","pages":"96-98"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39091669","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2021-06-01Epub Date: 2021-02-08DOI: 10.1037/cep0000222
Debra Jared, Alyssa Pandolfo
We investigated a cue that readers may use in determining whether a remark such as "You are so helpful!" is intended as a compliment or as an ironic insult. The cue was the age of the speaker. Remarks were preceded by a sentence that either invited a literal or ironic interpretation of the remark. Data were collected on the familiarity of the remark as an ironic statement, and the incongruity of the remark with the prior context. In Experiment 1, participants were asked to rate the intent of the speaker as to how ironic, mocking, polite, and funny they intended their remark to be. In Experiment 2, participants read the scenarios as their eye movements were tracked. The results showed that age of the speaker had an impact on first pass reading times when statements were not familiar as ironic statements. Our younger adult participants did not appear to immediately activate a nonliteral interpretation of an ambiguous remark made by an older adult unless they had evidence from past experience that the remark is often used as an insult. However, ratings of the ironic intent of the statements were unaffected by speaker age; the age of the speaker affects the ease of interpretation but not the final outcome. The results are consistent with constraint-based theories of sentence comprehension. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
我们研究了一种线索,读者可以用它来判断像“你真有帮助!”这样的话是恭维还是讽刺的侮辱。线索是讲话者的年龄。评论之前有一句话,可以让人对评论进行字面或讽刺的解释。数据收集的熟悉程度的评论,作为一个讽刺的说法,和不协调的评论与先前的上下文。在实验1中,参与者被要求评价说话者的意图,即他们想要表达的讽刺、嘲弄、礼貌和有趣程度。在实验二中,参与者在阅读场景的同时,他们的眼球运动被跟踪。结果表明,说话者的年龄对第一次通过阅读的时间有影响,当陈述不被认为是讽刺陈述时。我们的年轻参与者似乎不会立即激活对老年人含糊不清的话的非字面解释,除非他们有过去经验的证据表明这句话经常被用作侮辱。然而,对这些陈述的讽刺意图的评分不受说话者年龄的影响;说话者的年龄会影响口译的难易程度,但不会影响最终的结果。结果与基于约束的句子理解理论一致。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"The effect of speaker age on the perception of ironic insults.","authors":"Debra Jared, Alyssa Pandolfo","doi":"10.1037/cep0000222","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000222","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We investigated a cue that readers may use in determining whether a remark such as \"You are so helpful!\" is intended as a compliment or as an ironic insult. The cue was the age of the speaker. Remarks were preceded by a sentence that either invited a literal or ironic interpretation of the remark. Data were collected on the familiarity of the remark as an ironic statement, and the incongruity of the remark with the prior context. In Experiment 1, participants were asked to rate the intent of the speaker as to how ironic, mocking, polite, and funny they intended their remark to be. In Experiment 2, participants read the scenarios as their eye movements were tracked. The results showed that age of the speaker had an impact on first pass reading times when statements were not familiar as ironic statements. Our younger adult participants did not appear to immediately activate a nonliteral interpretation of an ambiguous remark made by an older adult unless they had evidence from past experience that the remark is often used as an insult. However, ratings of the ironic intent of the statements were unaffected by speaker age; the age of the speaker affects the ease of interpretation but not the final outcome. The results are consistent with constraint-based theories of sentence comprehension. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 2","pages":"146-154"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25344911","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}