首页 > 最新文献

Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale最新文献

英文 中文
Automaticity and cognitive control in bilingual and translation expertise. 双语和翻译专业知识中的自动性和认知控制。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-03-01 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000268
Giulia Togato, P. Macizo, T. Bajo
It has been observed that different linguistic experiences might exert a differential effect on general cognitive processes. For example, research has shown that language control in professional translation differs from language control applied to other types of bilingual activities. The present study focuses on the construct of automaticity and aims at determining whether different linguistic experiences might modulate the balance between automaticity and cognitive control at the general cognitive level. Hence, monolinguals, bilinguals, and professional translators performed a memory search task that has extensively been employed to observe how automaticity is acquired through consistent practice. Comparisons between the groups showed overall differences in the ease with which the task was performed and, importantly, differences in both automaticity and cognitive control. Specifically, monolinguals showed higher levels of automaticity in the learning phase of the task, while bilinguals and professional translations carried out the task in a more controlled fashion. This pattern might have implied higher cognitive costs for the monolingual group when a switched learning condition was presented. Possibly due to previous control over the initial learning phase, bilinguals and translators were less affected by the cognitive costs associated to the reversal of the learning condition. Differences are explained in terms of professional translation and everyday bilingual practice. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
不同的语言经验可能对一般认知过程产生不同的影响。例如,研究表明,专业翻译中的语言控制不同于其他类型的双语活动中的语言控制。本研究着眼于自动性的结构,旨在确定不同的语言经验是否会在一般认知水平上调节自动性和认知控制之间的平衡。因此,单语者、双语者和专业翻译人员执行了一项记忆搜索任务,该任务被广泛用于观察如何通过持续的练习获得自动性。两组之间的比较显示了完成任务的难易程度的总体差异,更重要的是,在自动性和认知控制方面的差异。具体来说,单语者在任务的学习阶段表现出更高的自动性,而双语者和专业翻译者在任务的执行过程中更受控制。这种模式可能意味着,当出现切换学习条件时,单语组的认知成本更高。可能由于先前对初始学习阶段的控制,双语者和翻译者受到与学习条件逆转相关的认知成本的影响较小。从专业翻译和日常双语实践的角度来解释差异。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Automaticity and cognitive control in bilingual and translation expertise.","authors":"Giulia Togato, P. Macizo, T. Bajo","doi":"10.1037/cep0000268","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000268","url":null,"abstract":"It has been observed that different linguistic experiences might exert a differential effect on general cognitive processes. For example, research has shown that language control in professional translation differs from language control applied to other types of bilingual activities. The present study focuses on the construct of automaticity and aims at determining whether different linguistic experiences might modulate the balance between automaticity and cognitive control at the general cognitive level. Hence, monolinguals, bilinguals, and professional translators performed a memory search task that has extensively been employed to observe how automaticity is acquired through consistent practice. Comparisons between the groups showed overall differences in the ease with which the task was performed and, importantly, differences in both automaticity and cognitive control. Specifically, monolinguals showed higher levels of automaticity in the learning phase of the task, while bilinguals and professional translations carried out the task in a more controlled fashion. This pattern might have implied higher cognitive costs for the monolingual group when a switched learning condition was presented. Possibly due to previous control over the initial learning phase, bilinguals and translators were less affected by the cognitive costs associated to the reversal of the learning condition. Differences are explained in terms of professional translation and everyday bilingual practice. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"12 1","pages":"29-43"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2022-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"78370110","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Picking up the pieces: Sex differences in mechanisms of curve tracing. 拾起碎片:曲线追踪机制中的性别差异。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2021-12-01 Epub Date: 2021-10-28 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000265
Willem Millett, Daniel Voyer

This study examined potential sex differences in the application of models of curve tracing, namely the pixel-by-pixel model, the bipartite model, and the zoom lens model. The purpose of this study was therefore to determine whether sex differences existed in terms of reliance on a particular model or whether the results of each sex could be best explained by one model. This was done by examining the combined data obtained by Voyer and MacPherson (2020), consisting of 420 participants, with 194 men and 226 women. We examined only the curve-tracing task data from that study and compared the fit of the different models as well as a possible interaction with sex of participants on the proportion of correct responses and response time. Overall, sex was a significant factor, with men showing better average accuracy and faster performance than women. On accuracy, we found that the pixel-by-pixel model provided the best fit for women, whereas the zoom lens model produced the best fit for men. On response time, the zoom model was the best predictor of response time for both sexes. The discussion elaborates on an account of these findings and on how our results might generalize to other visual-spatial tasks where a performance advantage for men is found. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究探讨了在曲线追踪模型的应用中潜在的性别差异,即逐像素模型、二部模型和变焦镜头模型。因此,这项研究的目的是确定性别差异是否存在于对特定模型的依赖上,或者是否每个性别的结果都可以用一个模型来最好地解释。这是通过检查Voyer和MacPherson(2020)获得的综合数据来完成的,该数据由420名参与者组成,其中194名男性和226名女性。我们只检查了来自该研究的曲线追踪任务数据,并比较了不同模型的拟合以及参与者性别对正确反应比例和反应时间的可能相互作用。总的来说,性别是一个重要因素,男性比女性表现出更好的平均准确率和更快的表现。在准确性方面,我们发现逐像素模型最适合女性,而变焦镜头模型最适合男性。在反应时间上,缩放模型是男女反应时间的最佳预测器。讨论详细阐述了这些发现,以及我们的结果如何可以推广到其他视觉空间任务中,在这些任务中发现了男性的表现优势。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Picking up the pieces: Sex differences in mechanisms of curve tracing.","authors":"Willem Millett,&nbsp;Daniel Voyer","doi":"10.1037/cep0000265","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000265","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study examined potential sex differences in the application of models of curve tracing, namely the pixel-by-pixel model, the bipartite model, and the zoom lens model. The purpose of this study was therefore to determine whether sex differences existed in terms of reliance on a particular model or whether the results of each sex could be best explained by one model. This was done by examining the combined data obtained by Voyer and MacPherson (2020), consisting of 420 participants, with 194 men and 226 women. We examined only the curve-tracing task data from that study and compared the fit of the different models as well as a possible interaction with sex of participants on the proportion of correct responses and response time. Overall, sex was a significant factor, with men showing better average accuracy and faster performance than women. On accuracy, we found that the pixel-by-pixel model provided the best fit for women, whereas the zoom lens model produced the best fit for men. On response time, the zoom model was the best predictor of response time for both sexes. The discussion elaborates on an account of these findings and on how our results might generalize to other visual-spatial tasks where a performance advantage for men is found. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 4","pages":"387-392"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39582958","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A pompous snack: On the unreasonable complexity of the world's third-worst jokes. 一种浮夸的零食:关于世界第三糟糕笑话的不合理复杂性。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2021-12-01 Epub Date: 2021-03-25 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000234
Chris Westbury, Geoff Hollis

Although studies of humour are as old as the Western academic tradition, most theories are too vague to allow for modelling and prediction of humour judgments. Previous work in modelling humour judgments has succeeded by focusing on the world's worst jokes: the slight humour of single nonwords (Westbury, Shaoul, Moroschan, & Ramscar, 2016) and single words (Westbury & Hollis, 2019). Here that work is extended to the world's third-worst jokes, adjective-noun pairs such as dancing dildo, flabby goldfish, and pompous snack. Participants used best-worst scaling to rate the humour of random word pairs. Those judgments were modelled using both linear regression and genetic programming, which is not constrained by assumptions of linearity. The linear regression models were as successful as the nonlinear models at predicting humour judgments, accounting for 27% of the variance in a 540-item validation set. Predictors associated only with the noun and with the relationship between the adjective and noun accounted for much more variance (over 14% each) than predictors associated only with the adjective (6.3%). Greater cosine distance of the adjective word2vec vector from the vectors of the shared neighbors of the noun and adjective is associated with higher humour ratings, whereas the opposite relationship is true for the noun. This captures a form of incongruity not seen in single items, by which neighbours of the adjective become unexpectedly relevant only when the noun brings them into focus. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).

尽管对幽默的研究与西方学术传统一样古老,但大多数理论都太模糊,无法对幽默的判断进行建模和预测。之前的幽默判断建模工作成功地关注了世界上最糟糕的笑话:单个非单词的轻微幽默(Westbury, Shaoul, Moroschan, & Ramscar, 2016)和单个单词(Westbury & Hollis, 2019)。在这里,这项工作扩展到世界上第三糟糕的笑话,形容词-名词组合,如跳舞的假阳具,松弛的金鱼和浮夸的小吃。参与者使用最佳-最差尺度来评价随机单词对的幽默程度。这些判断使用线性回归和遗传规划建模,不受线性假设的约束。线性回归模型在预测幽默判断方面与非线性模型一样成功,在540个项目的验证集中占27%的方差。与仅与形容词相关的预测因子(6.3%)相比,仅与名词和形容词与名词之间的关系相关的预测因子占比更大(分别超过14%)。形容词word2vec向量与名词和形容词的共同邻居向量的余弦距离越大,幽默评分越高,而名词的关系则相反。这抓住了一种不协调的形式,这种不协调在单个项目中是看不到的,通过这种不协调,形容词的邻居只有在名词使它们成为焦点时才会出乎意料地相关。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"A pompous snack: On the unreasonable complexity of the world's third-worst jokes.","authors":"Chris Westbury,&nbsp;Geoff Hollis","doi":"10.1037/cep0000234","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000234","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Although studies of humour are as old as the Western academic tradition, most theories are too vague to allow for modelling and prediction of humour judgments. Previous work in modelling humour judgments has succeeded by focusing on the world's worst jokes: the slight humour of single nonwords (Westbury, Shaoul, Moroschan, & Ramscar, 2016) and single words (Westbury & Hollis, 2019). Here that work is extended to the world's third-worst jokes, adjective-noun pairs such as <i>dancing dildo, flabby goldfish, and pompous snack</i>. Participants used best-worst scaling to rate the humour of random word pairs. Those judgments were modelled using both linear regression and genetic programming, which is not constrained by assumptions of linearity. The linear regression models were as successful as the nonlinear models at predicting humour judgments, accounting for 27% of the variance in a 540-item validation set. Predictors associated only with the noun and with the relationship between the adjective and noun accounted for much more variance (over 14% each) than predictors associated only with the adjective (6.3%). Greater cosine distance of the adjective word2vec vector from the vectors of the shared neighbors of the noun and adjective is associated with higher humour ratings, whereas the opposite relationship is true for the noun. This captures a form of incongruity not seen in single items, by which neighbours of the adjective become unexpectedly relevant only when the noun brings them into focus. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 4","pages":"327-347"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25514598","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Canadian Journal of (Experimental) Psychology: The first 70 years. 加拿大(实验)心理学杂志:前70年。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2021-12-01 Epub Date: 2021-06-07 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000253
Colin M MacLeod

This article presents a survey of the first 70 years of this journal, covering (a) the origin and subsequent history of the journal, (b) who the Editors have been, (c) how the Editors have influenced the journal, (d) the most highly cited articles, and (e) consideration of the journal's content. After shifts in its purpose over its first two decades, the journal settled into being an outlet that is well respected around the world for research in the field of human experimental psychology. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).

本文对该期刊的前70年进行了调查,包括(a)期刊的起源和后来的历史,(b)编辑都是谁,(c)编辑如何影响期刊,(d)被引用次数最多的文章,以及(e)对期刊内容的考虑。在最初的二十年里,该杂志的宗旨发生了变化,后来成为了一个在人类实验心理学领域受到世界尊重的研究渠道。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Canadian Journal of (Experimental) Psychology: The first 70 years.","authors":"Colin M MacLeod","doi":"10.1037/cep0000253","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000253","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This article presents a survey of the first 70 years of this journal, covering (a) the origin and subsequent history of the journal, (b) who the Editors have been, (c) how the Editors have influenced the journal, (d) the most highly cited articles, and (e) consideration of the journal's content. After shifts in its purpose over its first two decades, the journal settled into being an outlet that is well respected around the world for research in the field of human experimental psychology. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 4","pages":"393-402"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39068152","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The influence of location, ownership, and the presence of a coactor on the processing of objects. 位置、所有权和合作者的存在对对象处理的影响。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2021-12-01 Epub Date: 2021-03-25 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000232
Anna Michelle McPhee, Merryn D Constable, Elizabeth J Saccone, Timothy N Welsh

Humans operate in complex environments where social interactions require individuals to constantly attend to people and objects around them. Despite the complexity of these interactions from a visuomotor perspective, humans can engage and thrive in social settings. The purpose of the current study was to examine the simultaneous influence of multiple social cues (i.e., ownership and the presence of a coactor) on the processing of objects. Participants performed an object-based compatibility task in the presence and absence of a coacting confederate. Participants indicated whether pictures of mugs (that were either self-owned or unowned) were upright or inverted. The pictures appeared at one of 2 locations (a near or far location relative to the participant) on a computer screen laid flat on (parallel to) the tabletop. When present, the coactor stood on the opposite side of the screen/table. Analysis of response times (RTs) indicated that the processing of objects was influenced by the object's ownership status, the presence of the coactor, and where the object was located on the screen. Specifically, RTs for pictures of self-owned mugs were shorter than unowned mugs, but only when the pictures were located at the near location. Further, the presence of a confederate resulted in shorter RTs for pictures located at the near but not the far location. These findings suggest that when objects were placed at the far location, the additional social cues of ownership and social context did not influence visuomotor processing of the objects. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).

人类在复杂的环境中活动,社会互动需要个体不断地关注周围的人和物。尽管从视觉运动的角度来看,这些互动是复杂的,但人类可以在社会环境中参与并茁壮成长。本研究的目的是考察多种社会线索(即所有权和合作者的存在)对物体加工的同时影响。参与者在有或没有合作伙伴的情况下执行基于对象的兼容性任务。参与者指出马克杯的图片(无论是自己的还是没有自己的)是直立的还是倒置的。图片出现在与桌面平行的电脑屏幕上的两个位置之一(相对于参与者的近或远位置)。在场时,摄影师站在屏幕/桌子的对面。对响应时间(RTs)的分析表明,对象的处理受到对象的所有权状态、协同器的存在以及对象在屏幕上的位置的影响。具体来说,自持马克杯照片的时间比无主马克杯短,但只有当照片位于附近位置时才如此。此外,同盟者的存在会导致位于近而非远位置的图片的RTs更短。这些发现表明,当物体被放置在较远的位置时,额外的社会线索(所有权和社会背景)并不影响物体的视觉运动加工。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"The influence of location, ownership, and the presence of a coactor on the processing of objects.","authors":"Anna Michelle McPhee,&nbsp;Merryn D Constable,&nbsp;Elizabeth J Saccone,&nbsp;Timothy N Welsh","doi":"10.1037/cep0000232","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000232","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Humans operate in complex environments where social interactions require individuals to constantly attend to people and objects around them. Despite the complexity of these interactions from a visuomotor perspective, humans can engage and thrive in social settings. The purpose of the current study was to examine the simultaneous influence of multiple social cues (i.e., ownership and the presence of a coactor) on the processing of objects. Participants performed an object-based compatibility task in the presence and absence of a coacting confederate. Participants indicated whether pictures of mugs (that were either self-owned or unowned) were upright or inverted. The pictures appeared at one of 2 locations (a near or far location relative to the participant) on a computer screen laid flat on (parallel to) the tabletop. When present, the coactor stood on the opposite side of the screen/table. Analysis of response times (RTs) indicated that the processing of objects was influenced by the object's ownership status, the presence of the coactor, and where the object was located on the screen. Specifically, RTs for pictures of self-owned mugs were shorter than unowned mugs, but only when the pictures were located at the near location. Further, the presence of a confederate resulted in shorter RTs for pictures located at the near but not the far location. These findings suggest that when objects were placed at the far location, the additional social cues of ownership and social context did not influence visuomotor processing of the objects. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 4","pages":"362-373"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25514151","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Cross-script phonological activation in Chinese-English bilinguals: The effect of SOA from masked priming. 中英双语者跨文字语音激活:隐性启动对SOA的影响。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2021-12-01 Epub Date: 2021-06-07 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000262
Ge Xu, Jiexuan Lin, Yanping Dong

The issue of bilingual phonological access remains unclear for bilinguals with cross-script language systems, which is especially true when the time course of phonological activation is involved. To investigate the time course of cross-script phonological activation, the present study asked Chinese-English bilinguals to complete a word naming task that was conducted in a forward-masked phonological priming paradigm in three stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) conditions. By comparing the interlingual and intralingual phonological priming effects in a within-subjects design, we found that (a) target naming in Chinese and English was facilitated by a phonologically similar English or Chinese prime in the three SOA conditions (43 ms, 75 ms, and 150 ms) and the facilitation effect of the prime reached the peak when the pronunciation of the prime-target pair most resembled each other and (b) manipulation of the SOAs affected both the naming latencies of target words and the sizes of the phonological priming effect. In particular, naming latencies in each prime-target type displayed an increasing tendency as the SOA prolonged. Moreover, despite the varied sizes of the priming effect in the three SOA conditions, we found a consistent pattern that the priming effects in two interlingual conditions resembled their respective intralingual conditions along the time course. Taken together, these findings provide strong support for an integrated phonological representation of bilinguals and further extend the language nonselective access hypothesis to language pairs with very different orthographic systems. Implications for the manipulation of the SOAs in the masked priming paradigm are also discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).

对于具有跨文字语言系统的双语者来说,双语语音通路的问题仍然不清楚,特别是当涉及语音激活的时间过程时。为了研究跨文字语音激活的时间过程,本研究要求中英双语者在三种刺激启动异步(SOA)条件下,在前向掩蔽语音启动范式下完成单词命名任务。通过比较被试内部设计中的语际和语内语音启动效应,我们发现(a)在3种SOA条件下(43 ms、75 ms和75 ms),语音相似的英语或汉语启动效应促进了汉语和英语目标的命名。当启动词和目标词对的发音最相似时,启动词的促进效应达到峰值。(b)对目标词的命名潜伏期和语音启动效应的大小,都受到对目标词的命名潜伏期的影响。特别是,随着SOA的延长,每种主要目标类型中的命名延迟呈现出增加的趋势。此外,尽管三种SOA条件下的启动效应大小不同,但我们发现了一个一致的模式,即两种语际条件下的启动效应在时间过程中与其各自的语内条件相似。综上所述,这些发现为双语者的综合语音表征提供了强有力的支持,并进一步将语言非选择获取假说扩展到具有不同正字法系统的语言对。还讨论了在掩蔽启动范式中对soa进行操作的含义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Cross-script phonological activation in Chinese-English bilinguals: The effect of SOA from masked priming.","authors":"Ge Xu,&nbsp;Jiexuan Lin,&nbsp;Yanping Dong","doi":"10.1037/cep0000262","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000262","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The issue of bilingual phonological access remains unclear for bilinguals with cross-script language systems, which is especially true when the time course of phonological activation is involved. To investigate the time course of cross-script phonological activation, the present study asked Chinese-English bilinguals to complete a word naming task that was conducted in a forward-masked phonological priming paradigm in three stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) conditions. By comparing the interlingual and intralingual phonological priming effects in a within-subjects design, we found that (a) target naming in Chinese and English was facilitated by a phonologically similar English or Chinese prime in the three SOA conditions (43 ms, 75 ms, and 150 ms) and the facilitation effect of the prime reached the peak when the pronunciation of the prime-target pair most resembled each other and (b) manipulation of the SOAs affected both the naming latencies of target words and the sizes of the phonological priming effect. In particular, naming latencies in each prime-target type displayed an increasing tendency as the SOA prolonged. Moreover, despite the varied sizes of the priming effect in the three SOA conditions, we found a consistent pattern that the priming effects in two interlingual conditions resembled their respective intralingual conditions along the time course. Taken together, these findings provide strong support for an integrated phonological representation of bilinguals and further extend the language nonselective access hypothesis to language pairs with very different orthographic systems. Implications for the manipulation of the SOAs in the masked priming paradigm are also discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 4","pages":"374-386"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39068153","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effect of self-reported internal memory strategy use on age-related episodic and working memory decline: Contribution of control processes. 自我报告的内部记忆策略的使用对与年龄相关的外显记忆和工作记忆衰退的影响:控制过程的贡献
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2021-12-01 Epub Date: 2021-07-22 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000240
Lina Guerrero, Michel Isingrini, Lucie Angel, Séverine Fay, Laurence Taconnat, Badiâa Bouazzaoui

We explored whether control processes could account for age-related differences in internal strategy use, which in turn would contribute to episodic and working memory decline in aging. Young and older adults completed the internal strategy subscale of the Metamemory in Adulthood (MIA) questionnaire, a free-recall task (FRT), a reading span task (RST), and 3 executive control tasks (the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, the Initial Letter Fluency Test, and the Digit Symbol Substitution Test) allowing us to calculate a composite index of control processes. Results indicated that both self-reported internal strategy use and control processes index accounted for a significant proportion of the age-related variance in the FRT and the RST. However, once the control processes index was controlled for, variance in both the FRT and RST explained by internal strategy use were significantly reduced. Additionally, age-related variance in internal strategy use was mediated by the control processes index. These results suggest a cascade model in which individual control level would mediate age-related differences in internal strategy use, which in turn would mediate age-related differences in episodic and working memory performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).

我们探讨了控制过程是否能解释内部策略使用中与年龄相关的差异,而这种差异反过来又会导致衰老过程中的外显记忆和工作记忆衰退。年轻人和老年人分别完成了成人元记忆(MIA)问卷的内部策略分量表、自由回忆任务(FRT)、阅读跨度任务(RST)和 3 项执行控制任务(威斯康星卡片分类测试、首字母流畅性测试和数字符号替换测试),从而计算出了控制过程的综合指数。结果表明,自我报告的内部策略使用情况和控制过程指数在 FRT 和 RST 与年龄有关的差异中占了很大比例。然而,一旦控制过程指数得到控制,内部策略使用所解释的 FRT 和 RST 变异就会显著减少。此外,内部策略使用中与年龄相关的变异是由控制过程指数介导的。这些结果表明,在一个级联模型中,个体控制水平将介导内部策略使用中与年龄相关的差异,而内部策略使用中与年龄相关的差异反过来又将介导外显记忆和工作记忆表现中与年龄相关的差异。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Effect of self-reported internal memory strategy use on age-related episodic and working memory decline: Contribution of control processes.","authors":"Lina Guerrero, Michel Isingrini, Lucie Angel, Séverine Fay, Laurence Taconnat, Badiâa Bouazzaoui","doi":"10.1037/cep0000240","DOIUrl":"10.1037/cep0000240","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We explored whether control processes could account for age-related differences in internal strategy use, which in turn would contribute to episodic and working memory decline in aging. Young and older adults completed the internal strategy subscale of the Metamemory in Adulthood (MIA) questionnaire, a free-recall task (FRT), a reading span task (RST), and 3 executive control tasks (the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, the Initial Letter Fluency Test, and the Digit Symbol Substitution Test) allowing us to calculate a composite index of control processes. Results indicated that both self-reported internal strategy use and control processes index accounted for a significant proportion of the age-related variance in the FRT and the RST. However, once the control processes index was controlled for, variance in both the FRT and RST explained by internal strategy use were significantly reduced. Additionally, age-related variance in internal strategy use was mediated by the control processes index. These results suggest a cascade model in which individual control level would mediate age-related differences in internal strategy use, which in turn would mediate age-related differences in episodic and working memory performance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 4","pages":"348-361"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39209290","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Survival processing effect in memory under semantic divided attention. 语义分裂注意下记忆的生存加工效应。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2021-09-01 Epub Date: 2021-02-04 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000210
Lixia Yang, Linda Truong, Lingqian Li

Past research demonstrated enhanced memory for information encoded with relevance to a survival scenario compared to a control scenario, an effect referred to as the survival processing effect in memory. This effect has been explained by a proximate mechanism hypothesis (i.e., survival processing enables deep elaborative processing that promotes memory). In support of this hypothesis, past research found that, during encoding, the survival processing effect was largely intact under a perceptual or low-load secondary task condition but eliminated under a high-load secondary task condition. To test semantic encoding as a possible proximate mechanism, the current study assesses the impact of high-load and low-load divided attention tasks that require semantic processing of digits on the survival processing effect. Seventy-two young adults rated words for their relevance to two survival scenarios (i.e., grassland and mountain) and one non-survival control scenario (i.e., cruise), while completing a concurrent high-load or low-load semantic digit-monitoring task. No survival processing effect was found in either condition. The results suggest that semantic encoding probably serves as a proximate mechanism for the survival processing effect in memory. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).

过去的研究表明,与控制情景相比,对与生存情景相关的信息编码的记忆增强,这种效应被称为记忆中的生存处理效应。这种效应可以用一种近似的机制假说来解释(即,生存加工能够促进深度精细加工,从而促进记忆)。为了支持这一假设,过去的研究发现,在编码过程中,在感知或低负荷的次要任务条件下,生存加工效应基本完整,而在高负荷的次要任务条件下,生存加工效应被消除。为了验证语义编码作为一种可能的近似机制,本研究评估了需要数字语义加工的高负荷和低负荷分散注意任务对生存加工效应的影响。72名年轻人在完成高负荷或低负荷语义数字监测任务的同时,对单词与两个生存情景(即草原和山区)和一个非生存控制情景(即巡航)的相关性进行评分。两种条件下均未发现生存加工效应。结果表明,语义编码可能是记忆中生存加工效应的近似机制。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Survival processing effect in memory under semantic divided attention.","authors":"Lixia Yang,&nbsp;Linda Truong,&nbsp;Lingqian Li","doi":"10.1037/cep0000210","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000210","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Past research demonstrated enhanced memory for information encoded with relevance to a survival scenario compared to a control scenario, an effect referred to as the survival processing effect in memory. This effect has been explained by a proximate mechanism hypothesis (i.e., survival processing enables deep elaborative processing that promotes memory). In support of this hypothesis, past research found that, during encoding, the survival processing effect was largely intact under a perceptual or low-load secondary task condition but eliminated under a high-load secondary task condition. To test semantic encoding as a possible proximate mechanism, the current study assesses the impact of high-load and low-load divided attention tasks that require semantic processing of digits on the survival processing effect. Seventy-two young adults rated words for their relevance to two survival scenarios (i.e., grassland and mountain) and one non-survival control scenario (i.e., cruise), while completing a concurrent high-load or low-load semantic digit-monitoring task. No survival processing effect was found in either condition. The results suggest that semantic encoding probably serves as a proximate mechanism for the survival processing effect in memory. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 3","pages":"299-306"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"25331845","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
From lollipops to lidocaine: The need for a universal print-to-speech framework. 从棒棒糖到利多卡因:需要一个通用的从印刷到语言的框架。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2021-09-01 Epub Date: 2021-06-17 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000257
Jacqueline Cummine, Angela Cullum, Daniel Aalto, Tyson Sereda, Cassidy Fleming, Alesha Reed, Amberley Ostevik, Sienna Cashion-Dextrase, Caroline C Jeffery, William E Hodgetts

Objective: There is a strong relationship between reading and articulation (Lervåg & Hulme, 2009; Pan et al., 2011). Given the tight coupling of these processes, innovative approaches are needed to understand the intricacies associated with print-speech connections. Here we ran a series of tightly controlled experiments to examine the impact of mouth perturbations on silent reading.

Method: We altered the mouth, via somatosensory feedback, in several ways: (a) a large lollipop in the mouth (E1), (b) a candy stick (bite bar) held horizontally between the teeth (E2), and (c) lidocaine that served to numb the mouth (E3). Three tasks were completed: (a) picture categorization, (b) "spell" lexical decision (Spell-LDT; "does the letter string spell a real word, yes or no?"), and (c) "sound" lexical decision (Sound-LDT; "does the letter string sound like a real word, yes or no?"). Participants (N = 97; E1 = 27; E2 = 32; E3 = 38) completed each of the tasks two times: once with a somatosensory perturbation (lollipop, bite bar, or lidocaine) and once without.

Results: For each experiment, a linear mixed effects analysis was run. Overall, we found that the lollipop (E1) and lidocaine (E3) had some specific effects on word recognition (e.g., for "no" responses), particularly in the Spell-LDT, whereas the bite bar (E2) had no effect on word recognition. The picture categorization task was not impacted by any perturbations.

Conclusion: These findings provide evidence that sensorimotor information is connected to reading. We discuss how these findings advance our understanding of a print-to-speech framework. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).

目的:阅读和发音之间存在很强的关系(lerv & Hulme, 2009;Pan et al., 2011)。鉴于这些过程的紧密耦合,需要创新的方法来理解与印刷-言语连接相关的复杂性。在这里,我们进行了一系列严格控制的实验,以检验嘴部扰动对默读的影响。方法:我们通过体感反馈,以几种方式改变口腔:(a)在口腔中放入一根大棒棒糖(E1), (b)在牙齿之间水平放置一根糖果棒(咬条)(E2),以及(c)利多卡因用于麻痹口腔(E3)。完成三个任务:(a)图片分类;(b)“拼写”词法决定(spell - ldt;“这个字母串拼写的是一个真正的单词,是还是不是?”),以及(c)“声音”词汇决定(sound - ldt;“这个字母串听起来像一个真正的单词吗,是还是不是?”)。参与者(N = 97;E1 = 27;E2 = 32;E3 = 38)完成每项任务两次:一次有躯体感觉干扰(棒棒糖、咬棒或利多卡因),另一次没有。结果:对每个实验进行线性混合效应分析。总的来说,我们发现棒棒糖(E1)和利多卡因(E3)对单词识别有一些特定的影响(例如,对于“否”的反应),特别是在拼写- ldt中,而咬条(E2)对单词识别没有影响。图像分类任务不受任何干扰的影响。结论:这些发现为感觉运动信息与阅读有关提供了证据。我们讨论了这些发现如何促进我们对打印到语音框架的理解。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"From lollipops to lidocaine: The need for a universal print-to-speech framework.","authors":"Jacqueline Cummine,&nbsp;Angela Cullum,&nbsp;Daniel Aalto,&nbsp;Tyson Sereda,&nbsp;Cassidy Fleming,&nbsp;Alesha Reed,&nbsp;Amberley Ostevik,&nbsp;Sienna Cashion-Dextrase,&nbsp;Caroline C Jeffery,&nbsp;William E Hodgetts","doi":"10.1037/cep0000257","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000257","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>There is a strong relationship between reading and articulation (Lervåg & Hulme, 2009; Pan et al., 2011). Given the tight coupling of these processes, innovative approaches are needed to understand the intricacies associated with print-speech connections. Here we ran a series of tightly controlled experiments to examine the impact of mouth perturbations on silent reading.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We altered the mouth, via somatosensory feedback, in several ways: (a) a large lollipop in the mouth (E1), (b) a candy stick (bite bar) held horizontally between the teeth (E2), and (c) lidocaine that served to numb the mouth (E3). Three tasks were completed: (a) picture categorization, (b) \"spell\" lexical decision (Spell-LDT; \"does the letter string spell a real word, yes or no?\"), and (c) \"sound\" lexical decision (Sound-LDT; \"does the letter string sound like a real word, yes or no?\"). Participants (<i>N</i> = 97; E1 = 27; E2 = 32; E3 = 38) completed each of the tasks two times: once with a somatosensory perturbation (lollipop, bite bar, or lidocaine) and once without.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>For each experiment, a linear mixed effects analysis was run. Overall, we found that the lollipop (E1) and lidocaine (E3) had some specific effects on word recognition (e.g., for \"no\" responses), particularly in the Spell-LDT, whereas the bite bar (E2) had no effect on word recognition. The picture categorization task was not impacted by any perturbations.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>These findings provide evidence that sensorimotor information is connected to reading. We discuss how these findings advance our understanding of a print-to-speech framework. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 3","pages":"279-298"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39240198","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Language experience predicts semantic priming of lexical decision. 语言经验预测词汇决定的语义启动。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2021-09-01 Epub Date: 2021-05-06 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000255
Harinder Aujla

Computational models of semantic memory have been successful in accounting for a wide range of cognitive phenomena, including word categorization, semantic priming, and release from proactive interference. Conventionally, the texts input to these models have been curated to represent the average individual's language experience. While this approach has proven successful for making predictions that generalize across individuals, it prevents consideration of situations in which individuals have divergent semantic representations. The use of a representative corpus prevents the generation of predictions specific to the language experience of an individual. While this limitation has been discussed in the literature, previous investigations have not yet validated such corpus-specific predictions. I present an approach to generate corpus-specific semantic representations using internet news sites as corpora. I then validate the semantic representations against subjects that read specific news sites. Results demonstrate that similarities between news sites are specific to the words under consideration and that news site-specific representations successfully predict differential priming effects in lexical decision as a function of news readership. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).

语义记忆的计算模型已经成功地解释了广泛的认知现象,包括词分类、语义启动和从主动干扰中释放。按照惯例,输入到这些模型中的文本都是经过整理的,以代表一般人的语言经验。虽然这种方法已被证明可以成功地预测个体,但它阻止了个体具有不同语义表示的情况。使用代表性语料库可以防止生成特定于个人语言经验的预测。虽然这一限制已经在文献中讨论过,但以前的调查尚未证实这种针对语料库的预测。我提出了一种使用互联网新闻网站作为语料库来生成语料库特定语义表示的方法。然后,我针对阅读特定新闻站点的主题验证语义表示。结果表明,新闻网站之间的相似性是特定于所考虑的单词的,新闻网站特定表征成功地预测了词汇决策中作为新闻读者的函数的差异启动效应。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Language experience predicts semantic priming of lexical decision.","authors":"Harinder Aujla","doi":"10.1037/cep0000255","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000255","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Computational models of semantic memory have been successful in accounting for a wide range of cognitive phenomena, including word categorization, semantic priming, and release from proactive interference. Conventionally, the texts input to these models have been curated to represent the average individual's language experience. While this approach has proven successful for making predictions that generalize across individuals, it prevents consideration of situations in which individuals have divergent semantic representations. The use of a representative corpus prevents the generation of predictions specific to the language experience of an individual. While this limitation has been discussed in the literature, previous investigations have not yet validated such corpus-specific predictions. I present an approach to generate corpus-specific semantic representations using internet news sites as corpora. I then validate the semantic representations against subjects that read specific news sites. Results demonstrate that similarities between news sites are specific to the words under consideration and that news site-specific representations successfully predict differential priming effects in lexical decision as a function of news readership. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":51529,"journal":{"name":"Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale","volume":"75 3","pages":"235-244"},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2021-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"38956090","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
期刊
Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1