首页 > 最新文献

Linguistic Variation最新文献

英文 中文
Crosslinguistic variation in partitives 分词的跨语言变异
IF 0.3 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-02-10 DOI: 10.1075/lv.21020.lur
P. Sleeman, S. Luraghi
{"title":"Crosslinguistic variation in partitives","authors":"P. Sleeman, S. Luraghi","doi":"10.1075/lv.21020.lur","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/lv.21020.lur","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":53947,"journal":{"name":"Linguistic Variation","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.3,"publicationDate":"2022-02-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42406678","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Partitive sharing – How to help in Kinande 参与分享-如何在基南德提供帮助
IF 0.3 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-02-07 DOI: 10.1075/lv.21001.iri
M. Irimia, Patricia Schneider-Zioga
This paper examines a structure in the Bantu language Kinande, namely the so-called sociative causative, where partitive morphology occurs on a nominal without giving the nominal an NP-related partitive interpretation. We argue that the source of the partitivity in this construction lies in the co-extensiveness relation (incrementality relation) between the causing and the caused subevent. Partitive morphology on the nominal signals obligatory licensing of the theme so that it can be properly mapped to the co-extensive subevents.
本文研究了班图语Kinande中的一个结构,即所谓的关联使使句,其中分词形态发生在名义上,而不给名义一个np相关的分词解释。我们认为,这一建构的可参与性的根源在于起因与被起因之间的共广泛性关系(递增关系)。分词法对名义信号的强制许可,使其可以适当地映射到共扩展子事件。
{"title":"Partitive sharing – How to help in Kinande","authors":"M. Irimia, Patricia Schneider-Zioga","doi":"10.1075/lv.21001.iri","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/lv.21001.iri","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000This paper examines a structure in the Bantu language Kinande, namely the so-called sociative causative, where partitive morphology occurs on a nominal without giving the nominal an NP-related partitive interpretation. We argue that the source of the partitivity in this construction lies in the co-extensiveness relation (incrementality relation) between the causing and the caused subevent. Partitive morphology on the nominal signals obligatory licensing of the theme so that it can be properly mapped to the co-extensive subevents.","PeriodicalId":53947,"journal":{"name":"Linguistic Variation","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.3,"publicationDate":"2022-02-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47906713","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Partitive genitive constructions and agreement variations in Latvian 拉脱维亚语的分格属格结构和协议变化
IF 0.3 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-01-31 DOI: 10.1075/lv.20019.kal
Andra Kalnača, Ilze Lokmane
The article takes a closer look at the partitive genitive constructions in Latvian, their structure, semantics, and syntactic functions. With the help of a corpus analysis, an attempt has been made to find out what determines gender and number agreement variations between the partitive genitive constructions in subject position and the predicate, if it comprises a declinable participle. Attention was paid to such features as subject animacy, voice of the predicate, word order (SV / VS), quantifier lexeme and grammatical number of the genitive. 320 examples with partitive genitive constructions were selected from The Balanced Corpus of Modern Latvian LVK2018, including such quantifier lexemes as daļa ‘part’, skaits ‘number’, vairums ‘quantity’, vairākums ‘majority’, puse ‘half’, daudzums ‘amount / quantity’, lērums ‘bagful’, simts ‘hundred’, tūkstotis ‘thousand’, daudz ‘much/many’, maz ‘little / few’. The best results are given by the combination of values ‘animate’, ‘active’ and ‘SV’, in which 93.24% of the examples are partitive genitive agreement. Data analyzed so far suggest that quantifiers involving the semantic element of partitivity (e.g., daļa ‘part’) favor partitive genitive agreement, whereas quantifiers lacking this semantic element favor quantifier agreement. This is an exploratory quantitative study on agreement tendencies in Latvian.
本文对拉脱维亚语中的部分属格结构及其结构、语义和句法功能进行了较为深入的研究。在语料库分析的帮助下,试图找出是什么决定了主词位置上的部分属格结构和谓语之间的性别和数量一致性变化,如果它包含可降分词的话。注意主词动物性、谓语语态、语序、量词词位和属格语法数等特点。从《现代拉脱维亚语平衡语料库》LVK2018中选取了320个具有部分属格结构的例子,包括daļa‘part’、skaits‘number’、vairrums‘quantity’、vairākums‘domity’、puse‘half’、daudzums‘amount/quantity”、lērums‘bagful’、simts‘centry’、túkstotis‘Thousa000’、dauz‘many/many’等量词,maz“很少/很少”。“animate”、“active”和“SV”值的组合给出了最佳结果,其中93.24%的例子是部分-属格一致的。迄今为止分析的数据表明,涉及部分性语义元素(例如,daļa“part”)的量词有利于部分-属格一致性,而缺乏该语义元素的量词则有利于量词一致性。这是一项关于拉脱维亚语中一致性倾向的探索性定量研究。
{"title":"Partitive genitive constructions and agreement variations in Latvian","authors":"Andra Kalnača, Ilze Lokmane","doi":"10.1075/lv.20019.kal","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/lv.20019.kal","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 The article takes a closer look at the partitive genitive constructions in Latvian, their structure, semantics, and syntactic functions. With the help of a corpus analysis, an attempt has been made to find out what determines gender and number agreement variations between the partitive genitive constructions in subject position and the predicate, if it comprises a declinable participle. Attention was paid to such features as subject animacy, voice of the predicate, word order (SV / VS), quantifier lexeme and grammatical number of the genitive. 320 examples with partitive genitive constructions were selected from The Balanced Corpus of Modern Latvian LVK2018, including such quantifier lexemes as daļa ‘part’, skaits ‘number’, vairums ‘quantity’, vairākums ‘majority’, puse ‘half’, daudzums ‘amount / quantity’, lērums ‘bagful’, simts ‘hundred’, tūkstotis ‘thousand’, daudz ‘much/many’, maz ‘little / few’. The best results are given by the combination of values ‘animate’, ‘active’ and ‘SV’, in which 93.24% of the examples are partitive genitive agreement. Data analyzed so far suggest that quantifiers involving the semantic element of partitivity (e.g., daļa ‘part’) favor partitive genitive agreement, whereas quantifiers lacking this semantic element favor quantifier agreement. This is an exploratory quantitative study on agreement tendencies in Latvian.","PeriodicalId":53947,"journal":{"name":"Linguistic Variation","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42670231","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The second genitive in the history of Russian and across its dialects 俄语历史上的第二个属格
IF 0.3 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-01-31 DOI: 10.1075/lv.21004.ter
Alexandra Ter-Avanesova, Michael Daniel
In this paper, we provide a survey of the diachronic development of the Russian second genitive (Gen2). As endpoints of this development, we consider data from Russian dialects representing different dialect groups. Assumedly, the expansion of Gen2 started off as ‘recycling’ of the genitive of a declension type that became obsolete already in the pre-written period. Nouns of this declension type were adopted by another declension, carrying their old genitive over as a variant form. This alternative ending started spreading, always as a variant, to other nouns in the adoptive declension. As the survey of the literature shows, in the course of this expansion new constraints evolved, including phonological, morphophonological, phonotactic, syntactic and semantic conditioning. While there is no declension class or even individual nouns where Gen2 became the only option, it expanded to different extents in different dialects. We believe that the diversity of functions associated with the form, the range of language-internal factors driving its expansion, as well as the current geographic distribution of constraints on its formation weaken the claim that emergence of Gen2 as a morphological category dedicated to partitive was due to contact with the languages of the Circumbaltic area, a suggestion made on a macro-areal basis and also based on comparison with the northern dialects alone. While we cannot argue that the data we present disproves the contact factor, we would at least expect that the increased granularity of dialectal data would provide some data to support it. This is not what happens, which we consider to be an argument against contact-induced change.The aim of the paper is two-fold: to present a synopsis of the discussions of the history of Gen2 and a survey of the data on the use of Gen2 in the dialects, both firsthand and available from the literature; and to question the role of contact in the emergence of the new category of Gen2 in Russian.
本文对俄语第二属格(Gen2)的历时发展进行了考察。作为这一发展的终点,我们考虑了来自代表不同方言组的俄语方言的数据。据推测,Gen2的扩展一开始是对一种在预编时期就已经过时的变体的生殖器的“回收”。这种变格类型的名词被另一个变格所采用,将其旧属格作为变体形式延续。这种替代词尾开始传播,始终作为变体,传播到采用变格的其他名词。正如文献调查所表明的那样,在这一扩展过程中,新的制约因素不断演变,包括语音、形态语音、表音策略、句法和语义条件反射。虽然没有变格类,甚至没有单独的名词,Gen2成为唯一的选择,但它在不同的方言中扩展到了不同的程度。我们认为,与形式相关的功能的多样性、驱动其扩展的语言内部因素的范围,以及目前对其形成的限制的地理分布,削弱了Gen2作为一个专门用于分隔的形态类别的出现是由于与环波罗的海地区的语言接触,这一建议是在宏观地域的基础上提出的,也是在与北方方言单独比较的基础上得出的。虽然我们不能争辩说我们提供的数据否定了接触因素,但我们至少可以预期,方言数据粒度的增加将提供一些数据来支持它。事实并非如此,我们认为这是反对接触引起的变化的论点。本文的目的有两个:简要介绍第二代的历史讨论,并调查第二代在方言中的使用数据,无论是第一手资料还是文献资料;并质疑接触在俄语第2代新类别出现中的作用。
{"title":"The second genitive in the history of Russian and across its dialects","authors":"Alexandra Ter-Avanesova, Michael Daniel","doi":"10.1075/lv.21004.ter","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/lv.21004.ter","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000In this paper, we provide a survey of the diachronic development of the Russian second genitive (Gen2). As endpoints of this development, we consider data from Russian dialects representing different dialect groups. Assumedly, the expansion of Gen2 started off as ‘recycling’ of the genitive of a declension type that became obsolete already in the pre-written period. Nouns of this declension type were adopted by another declension, carrying their old genitive over as a variant form. This alternative ending started spreading, always as a variant, to other nouns in the adoptive declension. As the survey of the literature shows, in the course of this expansion new constraints evolved, including phonological, morphophonological, phonotactic, syntactic and semantic conditioning. While there is no declension class or even individual nouns where Gen2 became the only option, it expanded to different extents in different dialects. We believe that the diversity of functions associated with the form, the range of language-internal factors driving its expansion, as well as the current geographic distribution of constraints on its formation weaken the claim that emergence of Gen2 as a morphological category dedicated to partitive was due to contact with the languages of the Circumbaltic area, a suggestion made on a macro-areal basis and also based on comparison with the northern dialects alone. While we cannot argue that the data we present disproves the contact factor, we would at least expect that the increased granularity of dialectal data would provide some data to support it. This is not what happens, which we consider to be an argument against contact-induced change.\u0000The aim of the paper is two-fold: to present a synopsis of the discussions of the history of Gen2 and a survey of the data on the use of Gen2 in the dialects, both firsthand and available from the literature; and to question the role of contact in the emergence of the new category of Gen2 in Russian.","PeriodicalId":53947,"journal":{"name":"Linguistic Variation","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46045629","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Diachronic bottlenecks of the Uralic (ablative-)partitive 乌拉尔(消融)分区的长期瓶颈
IF 0.3 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-01-27 DOI: 10.1075/lv.21003.gru
R. Grünthal
This article discusses the emergence of the partitive case in the three western-most branches of the Uralic language family, which are Saamic and Finnic in North Europe, and Mordvinic in Central Russia. The Finnic languages represent the outer edge of development in the partitive from an earlier ablative case, which used to manifest ‘source’, a specific property of spatial relations. In Finnic the partitive case is a multifunctional and conceptually distinct case, an inflectional category which has developed highly specific functions in object marking, negative phrase and as a case of non-canonical subject. Traces of this development are found in Saamic and Mordvinic as well, whereas other Uralic languages don’t share this kind of secondary development and functional extension. The development of this particular affix consists of several stages, special bottlenecks, enhancing functional properties and triggering the reanalysis of an inherited affix *-ta/-tä. This article focuses on the diachrony of this particular affix with special emphasis on western Uralic.
本文讨论了分裂格在乌拉尔语系最西部的三个分支中的出现,这三个分支是北欧的萨米语和芬尼语,以及俄罗斯中部的莫尔德维尼语。芬尼语代表了早期消融格中分裂格发展的外部边缘,该格用于表示“来源”,这是空间关系的一个特定性质。在芬尼语中,分部格是一个多功能的、概念上不同的格,是一个屈折范畴,在宾语标记、否定短语和非规范主语格中发展出了高度特定的功能。这种发展的痕迹在萨米语和莫尔德维尼语中也有发现,而其他乌拉尔语则没有这种二次发展和功能扩展。这种特殊词缀的发展包括几个阶段、特殊的瓶颈、增强功能特性和触发对遗传词缀*-ta/-tä的重新分析。本文着重研究了这一特殊词缀的历时性,特别强调了西乌拉尔语。
{"title":"Diachronic bottlenecks of the Uralic (ablative-)partitive","authors":"R. Grünthal","doi":"10.1075/lv.21003.gru","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/lv.21003.gru","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000This article discusses the emergence of the partitive case in the three western-most branches of the Uralic language family, which are Saamic and Finnic in North Europe, and Mordvinic in Central Russia. The Finnic languages represent the outer edge of development in the partitive from an earlier ablative case, which used to manifest ‘source’, a specific property of spatial relations. In Finnic the partitive case is a multifunctional and conceptually distinct case, an inflectional category which has developed highly specific functions in object marking, negative phrase and as a case of non-canonical subject. Traces of this development are found in Saamic and Mordvinic as well, whereas other Uralic languages don’t share this kind of secondary development and functional extension. The development of this particular affix consists of several stages, special bottlenecks, enhancing functional properties and triggering the reanalysis of an inherited affix *-ta/-tä. This article focuses on the diachrony of this particular affix with special emphasis on western Uralic.","PeriodicalId":53947,"journal":{"name":"Linguistic Variation","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49593769","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Beyond affectedness – partitive objects and degrees of agenthood in Ancient Greek 超越情感——古希腊语中可分割的对象和代理的程度
IF 0.3 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-01-27 DOI: 10.1075/lv.21002.lur
S. Luraghi
In several ancient and modern Indo-European languages, the partitive-genitive may be used in place of the accusative to encode the second argument of two-place verbs. In Ancient Greek the two types of object encoding can alternate with change-of-state verbs, alternation being viewed as connected with degrees of patient affectedness: the partitive-genitive encodes partially affected objects. Alternation also extends to experiential verbs, which are typically characterized by a low degree of transitivity and do not imply any change of state of the object-stimulus. Rather than concentrating on the implications of case alternation on the construal of the object, I consider the effects of variation on the whole construction, and argue that genitive vs. accusative marking of the object (NomGen vs. NomAcc constructions) reflects the construal of the subject-experiencer. While the different construal of the experiencer in terms of degrees of control cross-linguistically often results in non-nominative encoding of the experiencer, in Ancient Greek it is object encoding that affects the construal of the experiencer and reflects a scale based on possible control. The distribution of constructions with experiential verbs shows that NomAcc is typical of verbs of sight, thought, intellectual knowledge and emotions connected to sight and awareness, such as wonder and fear. NomGen is connected with touch, smell, taste, memory, forgetfulness, care and desire. In the in-between area, verbs of hearing, learning and verbs of affection may feature both accusative and genitive encoding, thus constituting a fuzzy transition area. The connection between sight and other experiential verbs that feature accusative encoding reflects an embodied conceptualization of experiential situations.
在一些古代和现代印欧语言中,分部属格可以用来代替宾格来编码两地动词的第二自变量。在古希腊语中,两种类型的宾语编码可以与状态变化动词交替,交替被视为与患者受影响的程度有关:部分属格编码部分受影响的宾语。交替也延伸到经验动词,这些动词通常具有较低的及物性,并不意味着对象刺激状态的任何变化。我没有把注意力集中在格交替对宾语的构念上,而是考虑了变化对整个构念的影响,并认为宾语的属格标记与宾格标记(NomGen与NomAcc构念)反映了主体体验者的构念。虽然在跨语言的控制程度方面对体验者的不同理解通常导致体验者的非主格编码,但在古希腊语中,是宾语编码影响了体验者的理解,并反映了基于可能控制的尺度。经验动词结构的分布表明,NomAcc是与视觉和意识相关的视觉、思维、智力知识和情感动词的典型代表,如惊奇和恐惧。NomGen与触觉、嗅觉、味觉、记忆、健忘、关怀和欲望联系在一起。在中间区,听觉动词、学习动词和情感动词可能同时具有宾格和属格编码,从而构成一个模糊过渡区。视觉和其他具有宾格编码特征的经验动词之间的联系反映了经验情境的具体概念化。
{"title":"Beyond affectedness – partitive objects and degrees of agenthood in Ancient Greek","authors":"S. Luraghi","doi":"10.1075/lv.21002.lur","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/lv.21002.lur","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000In several ancient and modern Indo-European languages, the partitive-genitive may be used in place of the accusative to encode the second argument of two-place verbs. In Ancient Greek the two types of object encoding can alternate with change-of-state verbs, alternation being viewed as connected with degrees of patient affectedness: the partitive-genitive encodes partially affected objects. Alternation also extends to experiential verbs, which are typically characterized by a low degree of transitivity and do not imply any change of state of the object-stimulus. Rather than concentrating on the implications of case alternation on the construal of the object, I consider the effects of variation on the whole construction, and argue that genitive vs. accusative marking of the object (NomGen vs. NomAcc constructions) reflects the construal of the subject-experiencer. While the different construal of the experiencer in terms of degrees of control cross-linguistically often results in non-nominative encoding of the experiencer, in Ancient Greek it is object encoding that affects the construal of the experiencer and reflects a scale based on possible control. The distribution of constructions with experiential verbs shows that NomAcc is typical of verbs of sight, thought, intellectual knowledge and emotions connected to sight and awareness, such as wonder and fear. NomGen is connected with touch, smell, taste, memory, forgetfulness, care and desire. In the in-between area, verbs of hearing, learning and verbs of affection may feature both accusative and genitive encoding, thus constituting a fuzzy transition area. The connection between sight and other experiential verbs that feature accusative encoding reflects an embodied conceptualization of experiential situations.","PeriodicalId":53947,"journal":{"name":"Linguistic Variation","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42861798","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Partitive, genitive or nominative? 分格,属格还是主格?
IF 0.3 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-01-27 DOI: 10.1075/lv.20016.met
H. Metslang, Külli Habicht
The object has two variants in Estonian: partial object (in partitive, the functionally unmarked variant) and total object (in genitive or nominative). We examine the variation in object case in Estonian texts from the 17th to the 20th century, focusing on the 19th century and regarding this variation as an indicator in assessing the sociolinguistic variation of Estonian in texts. The texts of Old Literary Estonian were written by German scholars for whom Estonian was a collective interlanguage. In the 19th century the development of written Estonian came gradually into the hands of native speakers, who were surrounded by a predominantly German-language cultural space. In the Estonian of Germans the total object was overused. The 19th-century texts written by native Estonians represent an amalgam of native language and earlier interlanguage, they show fluctuations, and overuse of the partial object. By the turn of the 20th century, object case usage has stabilized.
宾语在爱沙尼亚语中有两种变体:部分宾语(在分部格中,功能上没有标记的变体)和全部宾语(在属格或主格中)。我们研究了17世纪至20世纪爱沙尼亚语文本中宾语的变化,重点关注19世纪,并将这种变化视为评估爱沙尼亚语文本社会语言学变化的指标。《古爱沙尼亚文学》的文本是由德国学者撰写的,对他们来说,爱沙尼亚语是一种集体中介语。在19世纪,书面爱沙尼亚语的发展逐渐掌握在母语人士手中,他们被以德语为主的文化空间所包围。在德国人的爱沙尼亚语中,这个词被过度使用了。19世纪由爱沙尼亚原住民撰写的文本代表了母语和早期中介语的混合体,它们显示出波动和对部分宾语的过度使用。到20世纪之交,宾语从句的用法已经稳定下来。
{"title":"Partitive, genitive or nominative?","authors":"H. Metslang, Külli Habicht","doi":"10.1075/lv.20016.met","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/lv.20016.met","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 The object has two variants in Estonian: partial object (in partitive, the functionally unmarked variant) and\u0000 total object (in genitive or nominative). We examine the variation in object case in Estonian texts from the 17th to the 20th\u0000 century, focusing on the 19th century and regarding this variation as an indicator in assessing the sociolinguistic variation of\u0000 Estonian in texts. The texts of Old Literary Estonian were written by German scholars for whom Estonian was a collective\u0000 interlanguage. In the 19th century the development of written Estonian came gradually into the hands of native speakers, who were\u0000 surrounded by a predominantly German-language cultural space. In the Estonian of Germans the total object was overused. The\u0000 19th-century texts written by native Estonians represent an amalgam of native language and earlier interlanguage, they show\u0000 fluctuations, and overuse of the partial object. By the turn of the 20th century, object case usage has stabilized.","PeriodicalId":53947,"journal":{"name":"Linguistic Variation","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42288356","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Partitive pronouns in intransitive contexts in Italian and Dutch 意大利语和荷兰语不及物语境中的分格代词
IF 0.3 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-01-27 DOI: 10.1075/lv.21018.sle
P. Sleeman
In the literature it is often assumed that partitive pronouns can only be used in combination with elliptical objects of transitive or unaccusative verbs. Some counterevidence has been provided as well, however, showing that partitive pronouns may also occur with intransitive verbs. In this paper it is investigated, by means of a Grammaticality Judgment Task, if native speakers of Italian and Dutch accept the use of the partitive pronoun with three types of intransitive verbs, in combination with an elliptical quantified adverbial NP. It is shown that both groups of participants were quite ready to accept the partitive pronoun in these cases, in some contexts more than in others. Various explanations for the results are considered and one more specific suggestion is made to account for the data, also based on a comparison with other constructions and other languages.
在文献中,通常认为分格代词只能与及物动词或非宾格动词的省略宾语结合使用。然而,也有人提出了一些反证据,表明分格代词也可能出现在不及物动词中。本文通过语法判断任务,考察了以意大利语和荷兰语为母语的人是否接受分格代词与三种不及物动词结合使用的省略式量词状语。结果表明,在这些情况下,两组参与者都很容易接受分格代词,在某些情况下比在其他情况下更容易接受。考虑了对结果的各种解释,并提出了一个更具体的建议来解释数据,这也是基于与其他结构和其他语言的比较。
{"title":"Partitive pronouns in intransitive contexts in Italian and Dutch","authors":"P. Sleeman","doi":"10.1075/lv.21018.sle","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/lv.21018.sle","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000In the literature it is often assumed that partitive pronouns can only be used in combination with elliptical objects of transitive or unaccusative verbs. Some counterevidence has been provided as well, however, showing that partitive pronouns may also occur with intransitive verbs. In this paper it is investigated, by means of a Grammaticality Judgment Task, if native speakers of Italian and Dutch accept the use of the partitive pronoun with three types of intransitive verbs, in combination with an elliptical quantified adverbial NP. It is shown that both groups of participants were quite ready to accept the partitive pronoun in these cases, in some contexts more than in others. Various explanations for the results are considered and one more specific suggestion is made to account for the data, also based on a comparison with other constructions and other languages.","PeriodicalId":53947,"journal":{"name":"Linguistic Variation","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.3,"publicationDate":"2022-01-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46353331","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Optional agreement as successful/failed Agree 作为成功/失败的可选协议同意
IF 0.3 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-10-26 DOI: 10.1075/lv.20013.lys
Paulina Lyskawa, Rodrigo Ranero
We investigate optional predicate agreement in Santiago Tz’utujil (Mayan). Several generalizations emerge: (i) inanimate arguments base-generated as complements control agreement optionally; (ii) some animate arguments base-generated as complements control agreement optionally; (iii) all arguments base-generated as specifiers control full agreement obligatorily. We propose that two conditions must be met for the operation Agree to succeed, resulting in the exponence of all the features of the agreement controller. First, a goal must be visible (bear the right feature). Second, a goal must be accessible (be in the right structural position). If one or both conditions are not met, Agree fails, but the derivation converges and 3sg agreement is exponed. While Agree is deterministic, surface optionality arises when the operation fails. We use optional agreement to diagnose the syntactic structure of understudied constructions in Mayan (nominalizations, Agent Focus). We discuss microvariation, highlighting methodological considerations that arise when assuming an I-language approach.
我们研究了Santiago T'utujil(玛雅语)中的可选谓语一致性。出现了几个概括:(i)作为补充控制协议生成的无生命论证基础;(ii)可选择地生成一些作为补充控制协议的动画论据库;(iii)作为指定人产生的所有论据基础都有义务控制完全协议。我们提出,“同意”操作必须满足两个条件才能成功,从而使“协议控制器”的所有特征都失效。首先,目标必须是可见的(具有正确的特征)。其次,目标必须是可实现的(处于正确的结构位置)。如果一个或两个条件都不满足,则Agree失败,但推导收敛,3sg一致性失效。虽然同意是决定性的,但当操作失败时,会产生表面选择性。我们使用可选协议来诊断玛雅语中研究不足的结构的句法结构(名词化,代理焦点)。我们讨论了微观变量,强调了在假设I语言方法时出现的方法论考虑。
{"title":"Optional agreement as successful/failed Agree","authors":"Paulina Lyskawa, Rodrigo Ranero","doi":"10.1075/lv.20013.lys","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/lv.20013.lys","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 We investigate optional predicate agreement in Santiago Tz’utujil (Mayan). Several generalizations emerge: (i) inanimate\u0000 arguments base-generated as complements control agreement optionally; (ii) some animate arguments base-generated as complements control\u0000 agreement optionally; (iii) all arguments base-generated as specifiers control full agreement obligatorily. We propose that two conditions\u0000 must be met for the operation Agree to succeed, resulting in the exponence of all the features of the agreement controller. First,\u0000 a goal must be visible (bear the right feature). Second, a goal must be accessible (be in the right structural position). If one or both\u0000 conditions are not met, Agree fails, but the derivation converges and 3sg agreement is exponed. While Agree is\u0000 deterministic, surface optionality arises when the operation fails. We use optional agreement to diagnose the syntactic structure of\u0000 understudied constructions in Mayan (nominalizations, Agent Focus). We discuss microvariation, highlighting methodological considerations\u0000 that arise when assuming an I-language approach.","PeriodicalId":53947,"journal":{"name":"Linguistic Variation","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.3,"publicationDate":"2021-10-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44708686","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Differential object marking in Catalan 加泰罗尼亚语中的差异对象标记
IF 0.3 Q2 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-08-02 DOI: 10.1075/lv.20009.iri
M. Irimia, Anna Pineda
In this paper we provide a comprehensive picture of differential object marking in Catalan, focusing on both the empirical facts and their theoretical contribution. We support some important conclusions. First, Catalan differential object marking is quite a robust and widespread phenomenon, contrary to what prescriptive grammars assume. Second, we show that, from a formal perspective, Catalan differential object marking cannot be completely subsumed under hierarchical generalizations known as scales. The contribution of narrow syntax mechanisms and nominal structure is fundamental, supporting recent views by López (2012) or Ormazabal and Romero (2007, 2010, 2013a, b), a.o. Building on these works as well as on observations initially made by Cornilescu (2000) and Rodríguez-Mondoñedo (2007), a.o., we adopt an analysis under which canonical, animacy-based differential marking results from the presence of an additional (PERSON) feature, beyond Case. This structural make-up is not only at the core of differences marked objects exhibit from unmarked objects with a Case feature, but also derives the prominence of differential marking on (animates) under information-structure processes, in the high left (and right) periphery, in contexts of the type discussed by Escandell-Vidal (2007a, b, 2009).
在本文中,我们提供了加泰罗尼亚语中微分对象标记的全面图像,重点关注经验事实及其理论贡献。我们支持一些重要结论。首先,加泰罗尼亚语差分宾语标记是一种相当稳健和普遍的现象,与规定语法的假设相反。其次,我们表明,从形式的角度来看,加泰罗尼亚语差分对象标记不能完全包含在称为尺度的层次概括下。狭义句法机制和名词结构的贡献是根本的,支持了López(2012)或Ormazabal和Romero(200720102013a,b)等人最近的观点。在这些作品以及Cornelescu(2000)和Rodríguez-Mondoñedo(2007)等人最初的观察结果的基础上,我们采用了一种分析方法,基于动画的差异标记是由Case之外的附加(PERSON)特征的存在产生的。这种结构构成不仅是标记对象与具有Case特征的未标记对象所表现出的差异的核心,而且在Escandell Vidal(2007a,b,2009)所讨论的类型的上下文中,在信息结构过程下,在左(和右)边缘,衍生出差异标记在(动画)上的突出性。
{"title":"Differential object marking in Catalan","authors":"M. Irimia, Anna Pineda","doi":"10.1075/lv.20009.iri","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1075/lv.20009.iri","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 In this paper we provide a comprehensive picture of differential object marking in Catalan, focusing on both the\u0000 empirical facts and their theoretical contribution. We support some important conclusions. First, Catalan differential object\u0000 marking is quite a robust and widespread phenomenon, contrary to what prescriptive grammars assume. Second, we show that, from a\u0000 formal perspective, Catalan differential object marking cannot be completely subsumed under hierarchical generalizations known as\u0000 scales. The contribution of narrow syntax mechanisms and nominal structure is fundamental, supporting recent\u0000 views by López (2012) or Ormazabal and Romero (2007, 2010, 2013a, b), a.o. Building on these works as well as on observations initially made by Cornilescu (2000) and Rodríguez-Mondoñedo\u0000 (2007), a.o., we adopt an analysis under which canonical, animacy-based differential marking results from the presence\u0000 of an additional (PERSON) feature, beyond Case. This structural make-up is not only at the core of differences marked objects\u0000 exhibit from unmarked objects with a Case feature, but also derives the prominence of differential marking on (animates) under\u0000 information-structure processes, in the high left (and right) periphery, in contexts of the type discussed by Escandell-Vidal\u0000 (2007a, b, 2009).","PeriodicalId":53947,"journal":{"name":"Linguistic Variation","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.3,"publicationDate":"2021-08-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42867641","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Linguistic Variation
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1