首页 > 最新文献

Human Factors最新文献

英文 中文
Do Augmented Reality Cues Aid Pedestrians in Crossing Multiple Lanes of Traffic? A Virtual Reality Study.
IF 2.9 3区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-02-24 DOI: 10.1177/00187208251320907
Jeehan Malik, Elizabeth O'Neal, Megan Noonan, Iman Noferesti, Nam-Yoon Kim, William Pixley, Jodie M Plumert, Joseph K Kearney

Objective: This study evaluated whether pedestrians can use augmented reality (AR) overlays to guide their road-crossing decisions when crossing two lanes of opposing traffic.

Background: Emerging technologies for enhancing traffic safety often focus on alerting drivers to hazards. Less attention has been given to understanding how pedestrians respond to technology designed to aid their road-crossing decisions, particularly in more complex traffic.

Method: Participants repeatedly crossed two lanes of opposing traffic displayed in a virtual reality system. Participants in the AR condition viewed matching-colored bars (AR overlays) suspended just above the gaps between cars where there was sufficient time to safely cross a pair of near and far lane gaps. Participants in the control condition performed the same road-crossing task but saw no AR overlays.

Results: Participants who viewed AR cues were more likely than participants who did not view AR cues to accept gap pairs classified as crossable and less likely to accept gap pairs classified as uncrossable. However, there was no difference between the AR and control conditions in time to spare when exiting the roadway. NASA Task Load Index (2020) responses indicated that perceived performance was higher and perceived frustration was lower in the AR than control condition, but perceived workload was higher in the AR condition.

Conclusion: The AR cues helped participants identify crossable gap pairs but did not lead to greater time to spare when exiting the roadway.

Application: These results show both the promise and risks of assistive technologies designed to increase pedestrian safety in more complex traffic situations.

{"title":"Do Augmented Reality Cues Aid Pedestrians in Crossing Multiple Lanes of Traffic? A Virtual Reality Study.","authors":"Jeehan Malik, Elizabeth O'Neal, Megan Noonan, Iman Noferesti, Nam-Yoon Kim, William Pixley, Jodie M Plumert, Joseph K Kearney","doi":"10.1177/00187208251320907","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00187208251320907","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>This study evaluated whether pedestrians can use augmented reality (AR) overlays to guide their road-crossing decisions when crossing two lanes of opposing traffic.</p><p><strong>Background: </strong>Emerging technologies for enhancing traffic safety often focus on alerting drivers to hazards. Less attention has been given to understanding how pedestrians respond to technology designed to aid their road-crossing decisions, particularly in more complex traffic.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Participants repeatedly crossed two lanes of opposing traffic displayed in a virtual reality system. Participants in the AR condition viewed matching-colored bars (AR overlays) suspended just above the gaps between cars where there was sufficient time to safely cross a pair of near and far lane gaps. Participants in the control condition performed the same road-crossing task but saw no AR overlays.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Participants who viewed AR cues were more likely than participants who did not view AR cues to accept gap pairs classified as crossable and less likely to accept gap pairs classified as uncrossable. However, there was no difference between the AR and control conditions in time to spare when exiting the roadway. NASA Task Load Index (2020) responses indicated that perceived performance was higher and perceived frustration was lower in the AR than control condition, but perceived workload was higher in the AR condition.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The AR cues helped participants identify crossable gap pairs but did not lead to greater time to spare when exiting the roadway.</p><p><strong>Application: </strong>These results show both the promise and risks of assistive technologies designed to increase pedestrian safety in more complex traffic situations.</p>","PeriodicalId":56333,"journal":{"name":"Human Factors","volume":" ","pages":"187208251320907"},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2025-02-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143484971","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of Auditory Anticipatory Cues and Lead Time on Visually Induced Motion Sickness.
IF 2.9 3区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-02-19 DOI: 10.1177/00187208251320179
Xin Xin, Xinyuan Chen, Wei Liu

Objective: This study aims to investigate the ability of auditory cues for predicting motion and lead times to mitigate visually induced motion sickness (VIMS).

Background: The vehicle information systems predominantly utilize visual displays, which can introduce conflicts between visual and vestibular motion cues, potentially resulting in VIMS. In these scenarios, auditory cues may provide a viable solution, especially when visual cues are diminished by fatigue or distractions.

Methods: In two distinct studies, a total of 180 participants were involved in investigating the impact of auditory cues on VIMS. In Study 1, participants were categorized based on the type of auditory cue they received (speech, nonspeech, or no-cue). Study 2 examined the effects of three different lead times (1 s, 2 s, and 3 s) between the activation of the auditory cue and the occurrence of car braking or turning in nonspeech conditions. VIMS severity was assessed with the Simulator Sickness Questionnaire (SSQ) before and after the simulation phase.

Results: Nonspeech cues significantly reduced VIMS compared to speech or no-cue. VIMS was notably lower with a 2 s lead time than with 1 s or 3 s lead times, and females reported higher levels of VIMS than males.

Conclusion: Results across two studies suggest using nonspeech cues with a 2-second lead time to reduce VIMS. It also recommends investigating the effects of duration, tone, and voice frequency. Furthermore, the study proposes extensive research into lead time settings for various scenarios such as driving fatigue, hillside roads, and traffic congestion.

Application: These findings offer potential value in designing auditory cues to reduce VIMS in autonomous driving, simulators, VR games, and films.

{"title":"Effects of Auditory Anticipatory Cues and Lead Time on Visually Induced Motion Sickness.","authors":"Xin Xin, Xinyuan Chen, Wei Liu","doi":"10.1177/00187208251320179","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00187208251320179","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>This study aims to investigate the ability of auditory cues for predicting motion and lead times to mitigate visually induced motion sickness (VIMS).</p><p><strong>Background: </strong>The vehicle information systems predominantly utilize visual displays, which can introduce conflicts between visual and vestibular motion cues, potentially resulting in VIMS. In these scenarios, auditory cues may provide a viable solution, especially when visual cues are diminished by fatigue or distractions.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>In two distinct studies, a total of 180 participants were involved in investigating the impact of auditory cues on VIMS. In Study 1, participants were categorized based on the type of auditory cue they received (speech, nonspeech, or no-cue). Study 2 examined the effects of three different lead times (1 s, 2 s, and 3 s) between the activation of the auditory cue and the occurrence of car braking or turning in nonspeech conditions. VIMS severity was assessed with the Simulator Sickness Questionnaire (SSQ) before and after the simulation phase.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Nonspeech cues significantly reduced VIMS compared to speech or no-cue. VIMS was notably lower with a 2 s lead time than with 1 s or 3 s lead times, and females reported higher levels of VIMS than males.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Results across two studies suggest using nonspeech cues with a 2-second lead time to reduce VIMS. It also recommends investigating the effects of duration, tone, and voice frequency. Furthermore, the study proposes extensive research into lead time settings for various scenarios such as driving fatigue, hillside roads, and traffic congestion.</p><p><strong>Application: </strong>These findings offer potential value in designing auditory cues to reduce VIMS in autonomous driving, simulators, VR games, and films.</p>","PeriodicalId":56333,"journal":{"name":"Human Factors","volume":" ","pages":"187208251320179"},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2025-02-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143451140","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Likelihood Systems Can Improve Hit Rates in Low-Prevalence Visual Search Over Binary Systems.
IF 2.9 3区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-02-12 DOI: 10.1177/00187208251320589
Tobias Rieger, Benita Marx, Dietrich Manzey

Objective: To study the performance consequences of binary versus likelihood decision support systems in low-prevalence visual search.

Background: Hit rates in visual search are often low if target prevalence is low, an issue that is relevant for numerous real-world visual search tasks (e.g., luggage screening and medical imaging). Given that binary decision support systems produce many false alarms at low prevalence, they have often been discounted as a solution to this low-prevalence problem. By offering additional information about the certainty of target-present indications through splitting these into warnings and alarms, likelihood-based systems could potentially boost hit rates without raising the number of false alarms.

Method: We used a simulated medical search task with low target prevalence in a paradigm where participants sequentially uncovered parts of the stimulus with their mouse. In two sessions, participants completed the task either while being supported by a binary or a likelihood system.

Results: Hit rates were higher when interacting with the likelihood systems than with the binary system, at no cost of higher false alarms.

Conclusion: Likelihood systems are a promising way to tackle the low-prevalence problem, and might further be an effective means to make systems more transparent.

Application: Simple-to-process information about system certainty for each case might be a solution to low hit rates in domains with low target prevalence, such as radiology.

{"title":"Likelihood Systems Can Improve Hit Rates in Low-Prevalence Visual Search Over Binary Systems.","authors":"Tobias Rieger, Benita Marx, Dietrich Manzey","doi":"10.1177/00187208251320589","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00187208251320589","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>To study the performance consequences of binary versus likelihood decision support systems in low-prevalence visual search.</p><p><strong>Background: </strong>Hit rates in visual search are often low if target prevalence is low, an issue that is relevant for numerous real-world visual search tasks (e.g., luggage screening and medical imaging). Given that binary decision support systems produce many false alarms at low prevalence, they have often been discounted as a solution to this low-prevalence problem. By offering additional information about the certainty of target-present indications through splitting these into warnings and alarms, likelihood-based systems could potentially boost hit rates without raising the number of false alarms.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We used a simulated medical search task with low target prevalence in a paradigm where participants sequentially uncovered parts of the stimulus with their mouse. In two sessions, participants completed the task either while being supported by a binary or a likelihood system.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Hit rates were higher when interacting with the likelihood systems than with the binary system, at no cost of higher false alarms.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Likelihood systems are a promising way to tackle the low-prevalence problem, and might further be an effective means to make systems more transparent.</p><p><strong>Application: </strong>Simple-to-process information about system certainty for each case might be a solution to low hit rates in domains with low target prevalence, such as radiology.</p>","PeriodicalId":56333,"journal":{"name":"Human Factors","volume":" ","pages":"187208251320589"},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2025-02-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143399880","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Context-Based Human Influence and Causal Responsibility for Assisted Decision-Making.
IF 2.9 3区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-02-03 DOI: 10.1177/00187208251317470
Yossef Saad, Joachim Meyer

Objective: The impact of the context in which automation is introduced to a decision-making system was analyzed theoretically and empirically.

Background: Previous work dealt with causality and responsibility in human-automation systems without considering the effects of how the automation's role is presented to users.

Methods: An existing analytical model for predicting the human contribution to outcomes was adapted to accommodate the context of automation. An aided signal detection experiment with 400 participants was conducted to assess the correspondence of observed behavior to model predictions.

Results: The context in which the automation's role is presented affected users' tendency to follow its advice. When automation made decisions, and users only supervised it, they tended to contribute less to the outcome than in systems where the automation had an advisory capacity. The adapted theoretical model for human contribution was generally aligned with participants' behavior.

Conclusion: The specific way automation is integrated into a system affects its use and the perceptions of user involvement, possibly altering overall system performance.

Application: The research can help design systems with automation-assisted decision-making and provide information on regulatory requirements and operational processes for such systems.

{"title":"Context-Based Human Influence and Causal Responsibility for Assisted Decision-Making.","authors":"Yossef Saad, Joachim Meyer","doi":"10.1177/00187208251317470","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00187208251317470","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>The impact of the context in which automation is introduced to a decision-making system was analyzed theoretically and empirically.</p><p><strong>Background: </strong>Previous work dealt with causality and responsibility in human-automation systems without considering the effects of how the automation's role is presented to users.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>An existing analytical model for predicting the human contribution to outcomes was adapted to accommodate the context of automation. An aided signal detection experiment with 400 participants was conducted to assess the correspondence of observed behavior to model predictions.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The context in which the automation's role is presented affected users' tendency to follow its advice. When automation made decisions, and users only supervised it, they tended to contribute less to the outcome than in systems where the automation had an advisory capacity. The adapted theoretical model for human contribution was generally aligned with participants' behavior.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The specific way automation is integrated into a system affects its use and the perceptions of user involvement, possibly altering overall system performance.</p><p><strong>Application: </strong>The research can help design systems with automation-assisted decision-making and provide information on regulatory requirements and operational processes for such systems.</p>","PeriodicalId":56333,"journal":{"name":"Human Factors","volume":" ","pages":"187208251317470"},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2025-02-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143124276","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Deciphering Automation Transparency: Do the Benefits of Transparency Differ Based on Whether Decision Recommendations Are Provided?
IF 2.9 3区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-02-03 DOI: 10.1177/00187208251318465
Isabella Gegoff, Monica Tatasciore, Vanessa K Bowden, Shayne Loft

Objective: To better understand automation transparency, we experimentally isolated the effects of additional information and decision recommendations on decision accuracy, decision time, perceived workload, trust, and system usability.

Background: The benefits of automation transparency are well documented. Previously, however, transparency (in the form of additional information) has been coupled with the provision of decision recommendations, potentially decreasing decision-maker agency and promoting automation bias. It may instead be more beneficial to provide additional information without decision recommendations to inform operators' unaided decision making.

Methods: Participants selected the optimal uninhabited vehicle (UV) to complete missions. Additional display information and decision recommendations were provided but were not always accurate. The level of additional information (no, medium, high) was manipulated between-subjects, and the provision of recommendations (absent, present) within-subjects.

Results: When decision recommendations were provided, participants made more accurate and faster decisions, and rated the UV system as more usable. However, recommendation provision reduced participants' ability to discriminate UV system information accuracy. Increased additional information led to faster decisions, lower perceived workload, and higher trust and usability ratings but only significantly improved decision (UV selection) accuracy when recommendations were provided.

Conclusion: Individuals scrutinized additional information more when not provided decision recommendations, potentially indicating a higher expected value of processing that information. However, additional information only improved performance when accompanied by recommendations to support decisions.

Application: It is critical to understand the potential differential impact of, and interaction between, additional display information and decision recommendations to design effective transparent automated systems in the modern workplace.

{"title":"Deciphering Automation Transparency: Do the Benefits of Transparency Differ Based on Whether Decision Recommendations Are Provided?","authors":"Isabella Gegoff, Monica Tatasciore, Vanessa K Bowden, Shayne Loft","doi":"10.1177/00187208251318465","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00187208251318465","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>To better understand automation transparency, we experimentally isolated the effects of additional information and decision recommendations on decision accuracy, decision time, perceived workload, trust, and system usability.</p><p><strong>Background: </strong>The benefits of automation transparency are well documented. Previously, however, transparency (in the form of additional information) has been coupled with the provision of decision recommendations, potentially decreasing decision-maker agency and promoting automation bias. It may instead be more beneficial to provide additional information without decision recommendations to inform operators' unaided decision making.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Participants selected the optimal uninhabited vehicle (UV) to complete missions. Additional display information and decision recommendations were provided but were not always accurate. The level of additional information (no, medium, high) was manipulated between-subjects, and the provision of recommendations (absent, present) within-subjects.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>When decision recommendations were provided, participants made more accurate and faster decisions, and rated the UV system as more usable. However, recommendation provision reduced participants' ability to discriminate UV system information accuracy. Increased additional information led to faster decisions, lower perceived workload, and higher trust and usability ratings but only significantly improved decision (UV selection) accuracy when recommendations were provided.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Individuals scrutinized additional information more when not provided decision recommendations, potentially indicating a higher expected value of processing that information. However, additional information only improved performance when accompanied by recommendations to support decisions.</p><p><strong>Application: </strong>It is critical to understand the potential differential impact of, and interaction between, additional display information and decision recommendations to design effective transparent automated systems in the modern workplace.</p>","PeriodicalId":56333,"journal":{"name":"Human Factors","volume":" ","pages":"187208251318465"},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2025-02-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143082103","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evaluation of Biomechanical and Mental Workload During Human-Robot Collaborative Pollination Task. 评估人机协作授粉任务中的生物力学和脑力劳动负荷
IF 2.9 3区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-28 DOI: 10.1177/00187208241254696
Mustafa Ozkan Yerebakan, Yu Gu, Jason Gross, Boyi Hu

Objective: The purpose of this study is to identify the potential biomechanical and cognitive workload effects induced by human robot collaborative pollination task, how additional cues and reliability of the robot influence these effects and whether interacting with the robot influences the participant's anxiety and attitude towards robots.

Background: Human-Robot Collaboration (HRC) could be used to alleviate pollinator shortages and robot performance issues. However, the effects of HRC for this setting have not been investigated.

Methods: Sixteen participants were recruited. Four HRC modes, no cue, with cue, unreliable, and manual control were included. Three categories of dependent variables were measured: (1) spine kinematics (L5/S1, L1/T12, and T1/C7), (2) pupillary activation data, and (3) subjective measures such as perceived workload, robot-related anxiety, and negative attitudes towards robotics.

Results: HRC reduced anxiety towards the cobot, decreased joint angles and angular velocity for the L5/S1 and L1/T12 joints, and reduced pupil dilation, with the "with cue" mode producing the lowest values. However, unreliability was detrimental to these gains. In addition, HRC resulted in a higher flexion angle for the neck (i.e., T1/C7).

Conclusion: HRC reduced the physical and mental workload during the simulated pollination task. Benefits of the additional cue were minimal compared to no cues. The increased joint angle in the neck and unreliability affecting lower and mid back joint angles and workload requires further investigation.

Application: These findings could be used to inform design decisions for HRC frameworks for agricultural applications that are cognizant of the different effects induced by HRC.

研究目的本研究旨在确定人机协作授粉任务可能引起的生物力学和认知工作量效应,机器人的额外提示和可靠性如何影响这些效应,以及与机器人互动是否会影响参与者的焦虑和对机器人的态度:背景:人机协作(HRC)可用于缓解授粉员短缺和机器人性能问题。背景:人机协作可用于缓解授粉员短缺和机器人性能问题,但人机协作在这种情况下的效果尚未得到研究:招募了 16 名参与者。包括四种 HRC 模式:无提示、有提示、不可靠和手动控制。测量了三类因变量:(1)脊柱运动学(L5/S1、L1/T12 和 T1/C7);(2)瞳孔激活数据;(3)主观测量,如感知工作量、与机器人相关的焦虑以及对机器人的负面态度:HRC降低了对机器人的焦虑,减少了L5/S1和L1/T12关节的关节角度和角速度,并减少了瞳孔放大,其中 "有提示 "模式产生的数值最低。然而,不可靠因素影响了这些收益。此外,HRC 导致颈部(即 T1/C7)的弯曲角度增大:HRC减少了模拟授粉任务中的体力和脑力工作量。与无提示相比,附加提示的益处微乎其微。颈部关节角度的增加以及影响中下背部关节角度和工作量的不可靠因素需要进一步研究:这些研究结果可用于为农业应用中的热轧卷框架设计决策提供信息,使其认识到热轧卷引起的不同影响。
{"title":"Evaluation of Biomechanical and Mental Workload During Human-Robot Collaborative Pollination Task.","authors":"Mustafa Ozkan Yerebakan, Yu Gu, Jason Gross, Boyi Hu","doi":"10.1177/00187208241254696","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00187208241254696","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>The purpose of this study is to identify the potential biomechanical and cognitive workload effects induced by human robot collaborative pollination task, how additional cues and reliability of the robot influence these effects and whether interacting with the robot influences the participant's anxiety and attitude towards robots.</p><p><strong>Background: </strong>Human-Robot Collaboration (HRC) could be used to alleviate pollinator shortages and robot performance issues. However, the effects of HRC for this setting have not been investigated.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Sixteen participants were recruited. Four HRC modes, no cue, with cue, unreliable, and manual control were included. Three categories of dependent variables were measured: (1) spine kinematics (L5/S1, L1/T12, and T1/C7), (2) pupillary activation data, and (3) subjective measures such as perceived workload, robot-related anxiety, and negative attitudes towards robotics.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>HRC reduced anxiety towards the cobot, decreased joint angles and angular velocity for the L5/S1 and L1/T12 joints, and reduced pupil dilation, with the \"with cue\" mode producing the lowest values. However, unreliability was detrimental to these gains. In addition, HRC resulted in a higher flexion angle for the neck (i.e., T1/C7).</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>HRC reduced the physical and mental workload during the simulated pollination task. Benefits of the additional cue were minimal compared to no cues. The increased joint angle in the neck and unreliability affecting lower and mid back joint angles and workload requires further investigation.</p><p><strong>Application: </strong>These findings could be used to inform design decisions for HRC frameworks for agricultural applications that are cognizant of the different effects induced by HRC.</p>","PeriodicalId":56333,"journal":{"name":"Human Factors","volume":" ","pages":"100-114"},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141162827","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bound by Experience: Updating the Body Representation When Using Virtual Objects. 被经验束缚:使用虚拟物体时更新身体表征
IF 2.9 3区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-14 DOI: 10.1177/00187208241258315
Julia Eck, Roland Pfister

Objective: Four web-based experiments investigated flexibility of disembodiment of a virtual object that is no longer actively controlled. Emphasis was on possibilities to modify the timescale of this process.

Background: Interactions with virtual objects are commonplace in settings like teleoperation, rehabilitation, and computer-aided design. These objects are quickly integrated into the operator's body schema (embodiment). Less is known about how long such embodiment lasts. Understanding the dynamics of this process is crucial because different applied settings either profit from fast or slow disembodiment.

Method: To induce embodiment, participants moved a 2D virtual hand through operating a computer mouse or touchpad. After initial embodiment, participants either stopped or continued moving for a fixed period of time. Embodiment ratings were collected continuously during each trial.

Results: Results across all experiments indicated that embodiment for the virtual hand gradually increased during active use and gradually decreased after stopping to use it. Disembodiment unfolded nearly twice as fast as embodiment and showed a curved decay pattern. These dynamics remained unaffected by anticipation of active control that would be required in an upcoming task.

Conclusion: The results highlight the importance of continuously experiencing active control in virtual interactions if aiming at inducing stable embodiment of a virtual object.

Application: Our findings suggest that applications of virtual disembodiment such as virtual tools or interventions to affect a person's body representation critically depend on continuous updating of sensorimotor experience. However, if switching between virtual objects, for example, during teleoperation or video gaming, after-effects are unlikely to affect performance.

目的:四项基于网络的实验研究了不再受主动控制的虚拟物体脱离实体的灵活性。重点是改变这一过程的时间尺度的可能性:背景:在远程操作、康复和计算机辅助设计等环境中,与虚拟物体的交互已司空见惯。这些物体会迅速融入操作者的身体图式(体现)中。至于这种融入能持续多长时间,人们知之甚少。了解这一过程的动态是至关重要的,因为不同的应用环境会从快速或缓慢的嵌入中获益:为了诱导体现,参与者通过操作电脑鼠标或触摸板来移动一只二维虚拟手。在最初的体现之后,参与者要么停止移动,要么继续移动一段时间。在每次试验过程中都会持续收集体现评分:所有实验的结果都表明,虚拟手的体现在主动使用过程中逐渐增加,在停止使用后逐渐减少。非体现的展开速度几乎是体现速度的两倍,并呈现出曲线衰减模式。这些动态变化不受对即将到来的任务中所需的主动控制的预期影响:结论:研究结果强调了在虚拟互动中持续体验主动控制的重要性,这样才能诱导虚拟对象的稳定体现:我们的研究结果表明,诸如虚拟工具或干预措施等影响人的身体表征的虚拟非实体化应用,关键在于持续更新感觉运动体验。不过,如果在远程操作或视频游戏等过程中切换虚拟对象,后遗症不太可能影响表现。
{"title":"Bound by Experience: Updating the Body Representation When Using Virtual Objects.","authors":"Julia Eck, Roland Pfister","doi":"10.1177/00187208241258315","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00187208241258315","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Four web-based experiments investigated flexibility of disembodiment of a virtual object that is no longer actively controlled. Emphasis was on possibilities to modify the timescale of this process.</p><p><strong>Background: </strong>Interactions with virtual objects are commonplace in settings like teleoperation, rehabilitation, and computer-aided design. These objects are quickly integrated into the operator's body schema (embodiment). Less is known about how long such embodiment lasts. Understanding the dynamics of this process is crucial because different applied settings either profit from fast or slow disembodiment.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>To induce embodiment, participants moved a 2D virtual hand through operating a computer mouse or touchpad. After initial embodiment, participants either stopped or continued moving for a fixed period of time. Embodiment ratings were collected continuously during each trial.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Results across all experiments indicated that embodiment for the virtual hand gradually increased during active use and gradually decreased after stopping to use it. Disembodiment unfolded nearly twice as fast as embodiment and showed a curved decay pattern. These dynamics remained unaffected by anticipation of active control that would be required in an upcoming task.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The results highlight the importance of continuously experiencing active control in virtual interactions if aiming at inducing stable embodiment of a virtual object.</p><p><strong>Application: </strong>Our findings suggest that applications of virtual disembodiment such as virtual tools or interventions to affect a person's body representation critically depend on continuous updating of sensorimotor experience. However, if switching between virtual objects, for example, during teleoperation or video gaming, after-effects are unlikely to affect performance.</p>","PeriodicalId":56333,"journal":{"name":"Human Factors","volume":" ","pages":"115-140"},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11626855/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141322138","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Multilevel Confirmatory Factor Analysis Reveals Two Distinct Human-Automation Trust Constructs. 多层次确认因素分析揭示了两种不同的人机信任结构。
IF 2.9 3区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-23 DOI: 10.1177/00187208241263774
Yusuke Yamani, Shelby K Long, Tetsuya Sato, Abby L Braitman, Michael S Politowicz, Eric T Chancey

Objective: This work examined the relationship of the constructs measured by the trust scales developed by Chancey et al. (2017) and Jian et al. (2000) using a multilevel confirmatory factor analysis (CFA).

Background: Modern theories of automation trust have been proposed based on data collected using trust scales. Chancey et al. (2017) adapted Madsen and Gregor's (2000) trust scale to align with Lee and See's (2004) human-automation trust framework. In contrast, Jian et al. (2000) developed a scale empirically with trust and distrust as factors. However, it remains unclear whether these two scales measure the same construct.

Method: We analyzed data collected from previous experiments to investigate the relationship between the two trust scales using a multilevel CFA.

Results: Data provided evidence that Jian et al. (2000) and Chancey et al. (2017) automation trust scales are only weakly related. Trust and distrust are found to be distinct factors in Jian et al.'s (2000) scale, whereas performance, process, and purpose are distinct factors in Chancey et al.'s (2017) trust scale.

Conclusion: The analysis suggested that the two scales purporting to measure human-automation trust are only weakly related.

Application: Trust researchers and automation designers may consider using Chancey et al. (2017) and Jian et al. (2000) scales to capture different characteristics of human-automation trust.

研究目的本研究采用多层次确证因子分析(CFA)方法,研究了 Chancey 等人(2017 年)和 Jian 等人(2000 年)开发的信任量表所测量的构造之间的关系:现代自动化信任理论是基于信任量表收集的数据提出的。Chancey 等人(2017 年)改编了 Madsen 和 Gregor(2000 年)的信任量表,使其与 Lee 和 See(2004 年)的人类-自动化信任框架相一致。相反,Jian 等人(2000 年)则根据经验开发了一个以信任和不信任为因子的量表。然而,这两个量表是否测量的是同一概念仍不清楚:方法:我们分析了从以前的实验中收集到的数据,使用多层次 CFA 方法研究了两个信任量表之间的关系:数据提供的证据表明,Jian 等人(2000 年)和 Chancey 等人(2017 年)的自动化信任量表只是弱相关。在 Jian 等人(2000 年)的量表中,信任和不信任是不同的因素,而在 Chancey 等人(2017 年)的信任量表中,性能、过程和目的是不同的因素:分析表明,这两个旨在衡量人与自动化信任度的量表只有微弱的关联性:信任研究人员和自动化设计人员可以考虑使用 Chancey 等人(2017 年)和 Jian 等人(2000 年)的量表来捕捉人机信任的不同特征。
{"title":"Multilevel Confirmatory Factor Analysis Reveals Two Distinct Human-Automation Trust Constructs.","authors":"Yusuke Yamani, Shelby K Long, Tetsuya Sato, Abby L Braitman, Michael S Politowicz, Eric T Chancey","doi":"10.1177/00187208241263774","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00187208241263774","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>This work examined the relationship of the constructs measured by the trust scales developed by Chancey et al. (2017) and Jian et al. (2000) using a multilevel confirmatory factor analysis (CFA).</p><p><strong>Background: </strong>Modern theories of automation trust have been proposed based on data collected using trust scales. Chancey et al. (2017) adapted Madsen and Gregor's (2000) trust scale to align with Lee and See's (2004) human-automation trust framework. In contrast, Jian et al. (2000) developed a scale empirically with trust and distrust as factors. However, it remains unclear whether these two scales measure the same construct.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We analyzed data collected from previous experiments to investigate the relationship between the two trust scales using a multilevel CFA.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Data provided evidence that Jian et al. (2000) and Chancey et al. (2017) automation trust scales are only weakly related. Trust and distrust are found to be distinct factors in Jian et al.'s (2000) scale, whereas performance, process, and purpose are distinct factors in Chancey et al.'s (2017) trust scale.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The analysis suggested that the two scales purporting to measure human-automation trust are only weakly related.</p><p><strong>Application: </strong>Trust researchers and automation designers may consider using Chancey et al. (2017) and Jian et al. (2000) scales to capture different characteristics of human-automation trust.</p>","PeriodicalId":56333,"journal":{"name":"Human Factors","volume":" ","pages":"166-180"},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141753504","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Lateral Pelvis and Lumbar Motion in Seated and Standing Office Work and Their Association With Transient Low Back Pain. 坐姿和站姿办公室工作中的骨盆外侧和腰部运动及其与短暂性腰痛的关系。
IF 2.9 3区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-07 DOI: 10.1177/00187208241249423
Jessa M Davidson, Jackie D Zehr, Mamiko Noguchi, Donna J Fok, Liana M Tennant, Jack P Callaghan

Objective: To assess frontal plane motion of the pelvis and lumbar spine during 2 h of seated and standing office work and evaluate associations with transient low back pain.

Background: Although bending and twisting motions are cited as risk factors for low back injuries in occupational tasks, few studies have assessed frontal plane motion during sedentary exposures.

Methods: Twenty-one participants completed 2 h of seated and standing office work while pelvic obliquity, lumbar lateral bending angles, and ratings of perceived low back pain were recorded. Mean absolute angles were compared across 15-min blocks, amplitude probability distribution functions were calculated, and associations between lateral postures and low back pain were evaluated.

Results: Mean pelvic obliquity (sit = 4.0 ± 2.8°, stand = 3.5 ± 1.7°) and lumbar lateral bending (sit = 4.5 ± 2.5°, stand = 4.1 ± 1.6°) were consistently asymmetrical. Pelvic obliquity range of motion was 4.7° larger in standing (13.6 ± 7.5°) than sitting (8.9 ± 8.7°). In sitting, 52% (pelvis) and 71% (lumbar) of participants, and in standing, 71% (pelvis and lumbar) of participants, were considered asymmetric for >90% of the protocol. Lateral postures displayed weak to low correlations with peak low back pain (R ≤ 0.388).

Conclusion: The majority of participants displayed lateral asymmetries for the pelvis and lumbar spine within 5° of their upright standing posture.

Application: In short-term sedentary exposures, associations between lateral postures and pain indicated that as the range in lateral postures increases there may be an increased possibility of pain.

摘要评估坐姿和站姿办公室工作 2 小时期间骨盆和腰椎的额面运动,并评估与短暂腰痛的关联:背景:虽然弯曲和扭转运动被认为是职业任务中腰部受伤的风险因素,但很少有研究对久坐时的额面运动进行评估:21名参与者完成了2小时的坐姿和站姿办公室工作,同时记录了骨盆倾斜度、腰部侧弯角度和腰痛感觉评分。比较各 15 分钟区块的平均绝对角度,计算振幅概率分布函数,并评估侧向姿势与腰痛之间的关联:平均骨盆倾斜度(坐姿 = 4.0 ± 2.8°,站姿 = 3.5 ± 1.7°)和腰椎侧弯(坐姿 = 4.5 ± 2.5°,站姿 = 4.1 ± 1.6°)始终不对称。站立时的骨盆斜度活动范围(13.6 ± 7.5°)比坐姿时(8.9 ± 8.7°)大 4.7°。在坐姿中,52% 的参与者(骨盆)和 71%的参与者(腰部)被认为在超过 90% 的规程中不对称;在站姿中,71% 的参与者(骨盆和腰部)被认为不对称。侧向姿势与腰痛峰值的相关性较弱或较低(R ≤ 0.388):结论:大多数参与者的骨盆和腰椎在直立姿势的 5° 范围内显示出侧向不对称:应用:在短期久坐情况下,横向姿势与疼痛之间的关联表明,随着横向姿势范围的增加,出现疼痛的可能性也会增加。
{"title":"Lateral Pelvis and Lumbar Motion in Seated and Standing Office Work and Their Association With Transient Low Back Pain.","authors":"Jessa M Davidson, Jackie D Zehr, Mamiko Noguchi, Donna J Fok, Liana M Tennant, Jack P Callaghan","doi":"10.1177/00187208241249423","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00187208241249423","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>To assess frontal plane motion of the pelvis and lumbar spine during 2 h of seated and standing office work and evaluate associations with transient low back pain.</p><p><strong>Background: </strong>Although bending and twisting motions are cited as risk factors for low back injuries in occupational tasks, few studies have assessed frontal plane motion during sedentary exposures.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Twenty-one participants completed 2 h of seated and standing office work while pelvic obliquity, lumbar lateral bending angles, and ratings of perceived low back pain were recorded. Mean absolute angles were compared across 15-min blocks, amplitude probability distribution functions were calculated, and associations between lateral postures and low back pain were evaluated.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Mean pelvic obliquity (sit = 4.0 ± 2.8°, stand = 3.5 ± 1.7°) and lumbar lateral bending (sit = 4.5 ± 2.5°, stand = 4.1 ± 1.6°) were consistently asymmetrical. Pelvic obliquity range of motion was 4.7° larger in standing (13.6 ± 7.5°) than sitting (8.9 ± 8.7°). In sitting, 52% (pelvis) and 71% (lumbar) of participants, and in standing, 71% (pelvis and lumbar) of participants, were considered asymmetric for >90% of the protocol. Lateral postures displayed weak to low correlations with peak low back pain (<i>R</i> ≤ 0.388).</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The majority of participants displayed lateral asymmetries for the pelvis and lumbar spine within 5° of their upright standing posture.</p><p><strong>Application: </strong>In short-term sedentary exposures, associations between lateral postures and pain indicated that as the range in lateral postures increases there may be an increased possibility of pain.</p>","PeriodicalId":56333,"journal":{"name":"Human Factors","volume":" ","pages":"85-99"},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140859241","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sensorimotor Challenges in Minimally Invasive Surgery: A Theoretically-Oriented Review. 微创手术中的感觉运动挑战:以理论为导向的综述。
IF 2.9 3区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-22 DOI: 10.1177/00187208241263684
Christopher L Hewitson, Matthew J Crossley, John Cartmill, David M Kaplan

Objective: This review surveys the literature on sensorimotor challenges impacting performance in laparoscopic minimally invasive surgery (MIS).

Background: Despite its well-known benefits for patients, achieving proficiency in MIS can be challenging for surgeons due to many factors including altered visual perspectives and fulcrum effects in instrument handling. Research on these and other sensorimotor challenges has been hindered by imprecise terminology and the lack of a unified theoretical framework to guide research questions in the field.

Method: We conducted a systematic survey of the MIS literature, focusing on studies investigating sensorimotor challenges affecting laparoscopic performance. To provide a common foundation for cross-study comparisons, we propose a standardized taxonomy that distinguishes between different experimental paradigms used in the literature. We then show how the computational motor learning perspective provides a unifying theoretical framework for the field that can facilitate progress and motivate future research along clearer, hypothesis-driven lines.

Results: The survey identified diverse sensorimotor perturbations in MIS, which can be effectively categorized according to our proposed taxonomy. Studies investigating monitor-, camera-, and tool-based perturbations were systematically analyzed, elucidating their impact on surgical performance. We also show how the computational motor learning perspective provides deeper insights and potential strategies to mitigate challenges.

Conclusion: Sensorimotor challenges significantly impact MIS, necessitating a systematic, empirically informed approach. Our proposed taxonomy and theoretical framework shed light on the complexities involved, paving the way for more structured research and targeted training approaches to enhance surgical proficiency.

Application: Understanding the sensorimotor challenges inherent to MIS can guide the design of improved training curricula and inform the configuration of setups in the operating room to enhance surgeon performance and ultimately patient outcomes. This review offers key insights for surgeons, educators, and researchers in surgical performance and technology development.

目的:本综述调查了影响腹腔镜微创手术(MIS)表现的感觉运动挑战的文献:本综述调查了有关影响腹腔镜微创手术(MIS)表现的感觉运动挑战的文献:背景:尽管腹腔镜微创手术对患者的益处众所周知,但由于许多因素,包括器械操作中视觉视角的改变和支点效应,要熟练掌握腹腔镜微创手术对外科医生来说是一项挑战。由于术语不准确以及缺乏统一的理论框架来指导该领域的研究问题,有关这些及其他感觉运动挑战的研究一直受到阻碍:我们对管理信息系统文献进行了系统调查,重点关注影响腹腔镜手术表现的感觉运动挑战研究。为了给跨研究比较提供一个共同的基础,我们提出了一个标准化的分类标准,以区分文献中使用的不同实验范式。然后,我们展示了计算运动学习的观点如何为该领域提供了一个统一的理论框架,从而促进该领域的进展,并激励未来的研究沿着更清晰、假设驱动的路线前进:调查发现,在 MIS 中存在多种传感器运动干扰,可根据我们提出的分类法进行有效分类。我们系统分析了调查监视器、摄像头和工具干扰的研究,阐明了它们对手术表现的影响。我们还展示了计算运动学习观点如何提供更深入的见解和潜在的策略来缓解挑战:结论:感知运动挑战对 MIS 有着重大影响,因此有必要采用一种系统的、以经验为依据的方法。我们提出的分类法和理论框架揭示了其中的复杂性,为更有条理的研究和有针对性的培训方法铺平了道路,从而提高手术熟练程度:应用:了解 MIS 所固有的感觉运动挑战可以指导改进培训课程的设计,并为手术室的设置提供信息,从而提高外科医生的表现,最终改善患者的预后。这篇综述为外科医生、教育工作者以及手术性能和技术开发方面的研究人员提供了重要的见解。
{"title":"Sensorimotor Challenges in Minimally Invasive Surgery: A Theoretically-Oriented Review.","authors":"Christopher L Hewitson, Matthew J Crossley, John Cartmill, David M Kaplan","doi":"10.1177/00187208241263684","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00187208241263684","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>This review surveys the literature on sensorimotor challenges impacting performance in laparoscopic minimally invasive surgery (MIS).</p><p><strong>Background: </strong>Despite its well-known benefits for patients, achieving proficiency in MIS can be challenging for surgeons due to many factors including altered visual perspectives and fulcrum effects in instrument handling. Research on these and other sensorimotor challenges has been hindered by imprecise terminology and the lack of a unified theoretical framework to guide research questions in the field.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We conducted a systematic survey of the MIS literature, focusing on studies investigating sensorimotor challenges affecting laparoscopic performance. To provide a common foundation for cross-study comparisons, we propose a standardized taxonomy that distinguishes between different experimental paradigms used in the literature. We then show how the computational motor learning perspective provides a unifying theoretical framework for the field that can facilitate progress and motivate future research along clearer, hypothesis-driven lines.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The survey identified diverse sensorimotor perturbations in MIS, which can be effectively categorized according to our proposed taxonomy. Studies investigating monitor-, camera-, and tool-based perturbations were systematically analyzed, elucidating their impact on surgical performance. We also show how the computational motor learning perspective provides deeper insights and potential strategies to mitigate challenges.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Sensorimotor challenges significantly impact MIS, necessitating a systematic, empirically informed approach. Our proposed taxonomy and theoretical framework shed light on the complexities involved, paving the way for more structured research and targeted training approaches to enhance surgical proficiency.</p><p><strong>Application: </strong>Understanding the sensorimotor challenges inherent to MIS can guide the design of improved training curricula and inform the configuration of setups in the operating room to enhance surgeon performance and ultimately patient outcomes. This review offers key insights for surgeons, educators, and researchers in surgical performance and technology development.</p>","PeriodicalId":56333,"journal":{"name":"Human Factors","volume":" ","pages":"141-165"},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11626857/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141749838","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Human Factors
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1