首页 > 最新文献

Human Factors最新文献

英文 中文
Information Access Effort: The Role of Head Movements for Information Presented at Increasing Eccentricity on Flat Panel and Head-Mounted Displays. 信息访问工作:在平板显示器和头戴式显示器上以增加偏心率呈现的信息中,头部运动的作用。
IF 2.9 3区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-09 DOI: 10.1177/00187208231204567
Amelia C Warden, Christopher D Wickens, Benjamin A Clegg, Daniel Rehberg, Francisco R Ortega

Objective: This experiment examined performance costs when processing two sources of information positioned at increasing distances using a flat panel display and an augmented reality head-mounted display (AR-HMD).

Background: The AR-HMD enables positioning virtual information at various distances in space. However, the proximity compatibility principle suggests that closer separation when two sources of information require mental integration assists performance, whereas increased separation between two sources hurts integration performance more than when a single source requires focused attention. Previous studies have provided inconsistent findings regarding costs associated with increased separation. Few of these experiments have examined separation for both focused and integration tasks, compared vertical and lateral separation, or measured head movements.

Method: Three experiments collectively examined these issues using a flat panel display and a virtual display presented with an HMD, where the separation of information varied laterally or vertically during a focused attention (digit reading) task and an information integration (mental subtraction) task.

Results: There was no performance cost for either display when information was increasingly separated. However, head movements mitigated performance costs by preserving accuracy at larger separations without increasing response time.

Conclusion: Head movements appear to mitigate performance costs associated with presenting information increasingly far apart on flat panel displays and HMDs. Both eye scanning and head movements appear to be less effortful than expected.

Application: These findings have important implications for design guidelines regarding the placement of information presented on flat panel displays and, more specifically, HMDs, which can present information 360° around the user.

目的:本实验考察了使用平板显示器和增强现实头戴式显示器(AR-HMD)处理两个距离越来越远的信息源时的性能成本。背景:AR-HMD能够在空间中定位不同距离的虚拟信息。然而,邻近相容性原则表明,当两个信息源需要心理整合时,更紧密的分离有助于表现,而当两个来源之间的分离增加时,对整合表现的伤害比当单个来源需要集中注意力时更大。先前的研究提供了关于与分离增加相关的成本的不一致的结果。这些实验中很少有研究过集中任务和整合任务的分离,比较过垂直和横向分离,或者测量过头部运动。方法:三个实验使用平板显示器和带HMD的虚拟显示器共同研究了这些问题,其中在集中注意力(数字阅读)任务和信息整合(心理减法)任务期间,信息的分离横向或垂直变化。结果:当信息越来越分离时,这两种显示器都没有性能成本。然而,头部移动通过在不增加响应时间的情况下保持较大间距的精度来降低性能成本。结论:头部移动似乎可以降低与在平板显示器和HMD上呈现越来越远的信息相关的性能成本。眼睛扫描和头部移动似乎都没有预期的那么费力。应用:这些发现对平板显示器上显示信息的位置设计指南具有重要意义,更具体地说,HMD可以在用户周围360°显示信息。
{"title":"Information Access Effort: The Role of Head Movements for Information Presented at Increasing Eccentricity on Flat Panel and Head-Mounted Displays.","authors":"Amelia C Warden, Christopher D Wickens, Benjamin A Clegg, Daniel Rehberg, Francisco R Ortega","doi":"10.1177/00187208231204567","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00187208231204567","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>This experiment examined performance costs when processing two sources of information positioned at increasing distances using a flat panel display and an augmented reality head-mounted display (AR-HMD).</p><p><strong>Background: </strong>The AR-HMD enables positioning virtual information at various distances in space. However, the proximity compatibility principle suggests that closer separation when two sources of information require mental integration assists performance, whereas increased separation between two sources hurts integration performance more than when a single source requires focused attention. Previous studies have provided inconsistent findings regarding costs associated with increased separation. Few of these experiments have examined separation for both focused and integration tasks, compared vertical and lateral separation, or measured head movements.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Three experiments collectively examined these issues using a flat panel display and a virtual display presented with an HMD, where the separation of information varied laterally or vertically during a focused attention (digit reading) task and an information integration (mental subtraction) task.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>There was no performance cost for either display when information was increasingly separated. However, head movements mitigated performance costs by preserving accuracy at larger separations without increasing response time.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Head movements appear to mitigate performance costs associated with presenting information increasingly far apart on flat panel displays and HMDs. Both eye scanning and head movements appear to be less effortful than expected.</p><p><strong>Application: </strong>These findings have important implications for design guidelines regarding the placement of information presented on flat panel displays and, more specifically, HMDs, which can present information 360° around the user.</p>","PeriodicalId":56333,"journal":{"name":"Human Factors","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71523528","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Transparent Automated Advice to Mitigate the Impact of Variation in Automation Reliability. 减轻自动化可靠性差异影响的透明自动建议。
IF 2.9 3区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-27 DOI: 10.1177/00187208231196738
Isabella Gegoff, Monica Tatasciore, Vanessa Bowden, Jason McCarley, Shayne Loft

Objective: To examine the extent to which increased automation transparency can mitigate the potential negative effects of low and high automation reliability on disuse and misuse of automated advice, and perceived trust in automation.

Background: Automated decision aids that vary in the reliability of their advice are increasingly used in workplaces. Low-reliability automation can increase disuse of automated advice, while high-reliability automation can increase misuse. These effects could be reduced if the rationale underlying automated advice is made more transparent.

Methods: Participants selected the optimal UV to complete missions. The Recommender (automated decision aid) assisted participants by providing advice; however, it was not always reliable. Participants determined whether the Recommender provided accurate information and whether to accept or reject advice. The level of automation transparency (medium, high) and reliability (low: 65%, high: 90%) were manipulated between-subjects.

Results: With high- compared to low-reliability automation, participants made more accurate (correctly accepted advice and identified whether information was accurate/inaccurate) and faster decisions, and reported increased trust in automation. Increased transparency led to more accurate and faster decisions, lower subjective workload, and higher usability ratings. It also eliminated the increased automation disuse associated with low-reliability automation. However, transparency did not mitigate the misuse associated with high-reliability automation.

Conclusion: Transparency protected against low-reliability automation disuse, but not against the increased misuse potentially associated with the reduced monitoring and verification of high-reliability automation.

Application: These outcomes can inform the design of transparent automation to improve human-automation teaming under conditions of varied automation reliability.

目的:研究提高自动化透明度在多大程度上可以减轻低自动化可靠性和高自动化可靠性对滥用和误用自动化建议以及自动化信任感的潜在负面影响:研究提高自动化透明度在多大程度上可以减轻低自动化可靠性和高自动化可靠性对不使用和滥用自动化建议以及对自动化的感知信任的潜在负面影响:背景:工作场所越来越多地使用自动决策辅助工具,这些工具提供的建议可靠性各不相同。低可靠性自动化会增加自动建议的废弃率,而高可靠性自动化则会增加误用率。如果自动建议的基本原理更加透明,就可以减少这些影响:方法:参与者选择最佳UV来完成任务。推荐器(自动决策辅助工具)通过提供建议来帮助参与者,但它并不总是可靠的。参与者决定推荐器是否提供了准确的信息,以及是否接受或拒绝建议。自动化透明度(中、高)和可靠性(低:65%,高:90%)在被试之间进行了调节:结果:与低可靠性自动化相比,高可靠性自动化使参与者做出的决策更准确(正确接受建议并识别信息是否准确/不准确)、更快速,并表示对自动化的信任度有所提高。透明度的提高使决策更准确、更迅速,主观工作量更低,可用性评分更高。它还消除了与低可靠性自动化相关的自动化滥用现象。然而,透明度并没有减少与高可靠性自动化相关的滥用现象:结论:透明度能防止低可靠性自动化的滥用,但不能防止与高可靠性自动化的监控和验证减少相关的滥用增加:这些结果可以为透明自动化的设计提供参考,从而在不同自动化可靠性条件下改善人机协作。
{"title":"Transparent Automated Advice to Mitigate the Impact of Variation in Automation Reliability.","authors":"Isabella Gegoff, Monica Tatasciore, Vanessa Bowden, Jason McCarley, Shayne Loft","doi":"10.1177/00187208231196738","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00187208231196738","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>To examine the extent to which increased automation transparency can mitigate the potential negative effects of low and high automation reliability on disuse and misuse of automated advice, and perceived trust in automation.</p><p><strong>Background: </strong>Automated decision aids that vary in the reliability of their advice are increasingly used in workplaces. Low-reliability automation can increase disuse of automated advice, while high-reliability automation can increase misuse. These effects could be reduced if the rationale underlying automated advice is made more transparent.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Participants selected the optimal UV to complete missions. The Recommender (automated decision aid) assisted participants by providing advice; however, it was not always reliable. Participants determined whether the Recommender provided accurate information and whether to accept or reject advice. The level of automation transparency (medium, high) and reliability (low: 65%, high: 90%) were manipulated between-subjects.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>With high- compared to low-reliability automation, participants made more accurate (correctly accepted advice <i>and</i> identified whether information was accurate/inaccurate) and faster decisions, and reported increased trust in automation. Increased transparency led to more accurate and faster decisions, lower subjective workload, and higher usability ratings. It also eliminated the increased automation disuse associated with low-reliability automation. However, transparency did not mitigate the misuse associated with high-reliability automation.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Transparency protected against low-reliability automation disuse, but not against the increased misuse potentially associated with the reduced monitoring and verification of high-reliability automation.</p><p><strong>Application: </strong>These outcomes can inform the design of transparent automation to improve human-automation teaming under conditions of varied automation reliability.</p>","PeriodicalId":56333,"journal":{"name":"Human Factors","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11141097/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10139257","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How Terminology Affects Users' Responses to System Failures. 术语如何影响用户对系统故障的响应。
IF 2.9 3区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-21 DOI: 10.1177/00187208231202572
Cindy Candrian, Anne Scherer

Objective: The objective of our research is to advance the understanding of behavioral responses to a system's error. By examining trust as a dynamic variable and drawing from attribution theory, we explain the underlying mechanism and suggest how terminology can be used to mitigate the so-called algorithm aversion. In this way, we show that the use of different terms may shape consumers' perceptions and provide guidance on how these differences can be mitigated.

Background: Previous research has interchangeably used various terms to refer to a system and results regarding trust in systems have been ambiguous.

Methods: Across three studies, we examine the effect of different system terminology on consumer behavior following a system failure.

Results: Our results show that terminology crucially affects user behavior. Describing a system as "AI" (i.e., self-learning and perceived as more complex) instead of as "algorithmic" (i.e., a less complex rule-based system) leads to more favorable behavioral responses by users when a system error occurs.

Conclusion: We suggest that in cases when a system's characteristics do not allow for it to be called "AI," users should be provided with an explanation of why the system's error occurred, and task complexity should be pointed out. We highlight the importance of terminology, as this can unintentionally impact the robustness and replicability of research findings.

Application: This research offers insights for industries utilizing AI and algorithmic systems, highlighting how strategic terminology use can shape user trust and response to errors, thereby enhancing system acceptance.

目的:我们研究的目的是促进对系统错误的行为反应的理解。通过将信任视为一个动态变量并借鉴归因理论,我们解释了潜在的机制,并建议如何使用术语来缓解所谓的算法厌恶。通过这种方式,我们表明,使用不同的术语可能会影响消费者的认知,并为如何减轻这些差异提供指导。背景:先前的研究交替使用各种术语来指代系统,关于系统中的信任的结果一直不明确。方法:在三项研究中,我们考察了不同的系统术语对系统故障后消费者行为的影响。结果:我们的研究结果表明,术语对用户行为的影响至关重要。当系统错误发生时,将系统描述为“AI”(即自学习和感知更复杂),而不是“算法”(即不太复杂的基于规则的系统)会导致用户做出更有利的行为反应。结论:我们建议,在系统的特性不允许将其称为“AI”的情况下,应向用户解释系统错误发生的原因,并指出任务的复杂性。我们强调了术语的重要性,因为这可能会无意中影响研究结果的稳健性和可复制性。应用:这项研究为使用人工智能和算法系统的行业提供了见解,强调了战略性术语的使用如何影响用户信任和对错误的反应,从而提高系统的接受度。
{"title":"How Terminology Affects Users' Responses to System Failures.","authors":"Cindy Candrian, Anne Scherer","doi":"10.1177/00187208231202572","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00187208231202572","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>The objective of our research is to advance the understanding of behavioral responses to a system's error. By examining trust as a dynamic variable and drawing from attribution theory, we explain the underlying mechanism and suggest how terminology can be used to mitigate the so-called algorithm aversion. In this way, we show that the use of different terms may shape consumers' perceptions and provide guidance on how these differences can be mitigated.</p><p><strong>Background: </strong>Previous research has interchangeably used various terms to refer to a system and results regarding trust in systems have been ambiguous.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Across three studies, we examine the effect of different system terminology on consumer behavior following a system failure.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Our results show that terminology crucially affects user behavior. Describing a system as \"AI\" (i.e., self-learning and perceived as more complex) instead of as \"algorithmic\" (i.e., a less complex rule-based system) leads to more favorable behavioral responses by users when a system error occurs.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>We suggest that in cases when a system's characteristics do not allow for it to be called \"AI,\" users should be provided with an explanation of why the system's error occurred, and task complexity should be pointed out. We highlight the importance of terminology, as this can unintentionally impact the robustness and replicability of research findings.</p><p><strong>Application: </strong>This research offers insights for industries utilizing AI and algorithmic systems, highlighting how strategic terminology use can shape user trust and response to errors, thereby enhancing system acceptance.</p>","PeriodicalId":56333,"journal":{"name":"Human Factors","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11141081/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41168304","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An Eye-Fixation Related Electroencephalography Technique for Predicting Situation Awareness: Implications for Driver State Monitoring Systems. 一种用于预测态势感知的与眼睛固定相关的脑电图技术:对驾驶员状态监测系统的启示。
IF 2.9 3区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-18 DOI: 10.1177/00187208231204570
Jing Yang, Nade Liang, Brandon J Pitts, Kwaku Prakah-Asante, Reates Curry, Denny Yu

Objective: This study developed a fixation-related electroencephalography band power (FRBP) approach for situation awareness (SA) assessment in automated driving.

Background: Maintaining good SA in Level 3 automated vehicles is crucial to drivers' takeover performance when the automated system fails. A multimodal fusion approach that enables the analysis of the visual behavioral and cognitive processes of SA can facilitate real-time assessment of SA in future driver state monitoring systems.

Method: Thirty participants performed three simulated automated driving tasks. After each task, the Situation Awareness Global Assessment Technique (SAGAT) was deployed to capture their SA about key elements that could affect their takeover task performance. Participants eye movements and brain activities were recorded. Data on their brain activity after each eye fixation on the key elements were extracted and labeled according to the correctness of the SAGAT. Mixed-effects models were used to identify brain regions that were indicative of SA, and machine learning models for SA assessment were developed based on the identified brain regions.

Results: Participants' alpha and theta oscillation at frontal and temporal areas are indicative of SA. In addition, the FRBP technique can be used to predict drivers' SA with an accuracy of 88% using a neural network model.

Conclusion: The FRBP technique, which incorporates eye movements and brain activities, can provide more comprehensive evaluation of SA. Findings highlight the potential of utilizing FRBP to monitor drivers' SA in real-time.

Application: The proposed framework can be expanded and applied to driver state monitoring systems to measure human SA in real-world driving.

目的:本研究开发了一种用于自动驾驶中情境意识(SA)评估的注视相关脑电图带功率(FRBP)方法。背景:当自动化系统出现故障时,在3级自动化车辆中保持良好的SA对驾驶员的接管性能至关重要。能够分析SA的视觉行为和认知过程的多模式融合方法可以促进未来驾驶员状态监测系统中SA的实时评估。方法:30名参与者进行了三项模拟自动驾驶任务。在每项任务之后,都会部署态势感知全球评估技术(SAGAT)来获取他们关于可能影响接管任务绩效的关键要素的SA。记录参与者的眼球运动和大脑活动。根据SAGAT的正确性,提取并标记他们每次眼睛注视关键元素后的大脑活动数据。使用混合效应模型来识别指示SA的大脑区域,并基于识别的大脑区域开发用于SA评估的机器学习模型。结果:参与者在额叶和颞叶的α和θ振荡表明SA。此外,使用神经网络模型,FRBP技术可用于预测驾驶员的SA,准确率为88%。结论:结合眼动和大脑活动的FRBP技术可以对SA进行更全面的评估。研究结果突出了利用FRBP实时监测驾驶员SA的潜力。应用:所提出的框架可以扩展并应用于驾驶员状态监测系统,以测量真实驾驶中的人类SA。
{"title":"An Eye-Fixation Related Electroencephalography Technique for Predicting Situation Awareness: Implications for Driver State Monitoring Systems.","authors":"Jing Yang, Nade Liang, Brandon J Pitts, Kwaku Prakah-Asante, Reates Curry, Denny Yu","doi":"10.1177/00187208231204570","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00187208231204570","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>This study developed a fixation-related electroencephalography band power (FRBP) approach for situation awareness (SA) assessment in automated driving.</p><p><strong>Background: </strong>Maintaining good SA in Level 3 automated vehicles is crucial to drivers' takeover performance when the automated system fails. A multimodal fusion approach that enables the analysis of the visual behavioral and cognitive processes of SA can facilitate real-time assessment of SA in future driver state monitoring systems.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Thirty participants performed three simulated automated driving tasks. After each task, the Situation Awareness Global Assessment Technique (SAGAT) was deployed to capture their SA about key elements that could affect their takeover task performance. Participants eye movements and brain activities were recorded. Data on their brain activity after each eye fixation on the key elements were extracted and labeled according to the correctness of the SAGAT. Mixed-effects models were used to identify brain regions that were indicative of SA, and machine learning models for SA assessment were developed based on the identified brain regions.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Participants' alpha and theta oscillation at frontal and temporal areas are indicative of SA. In addition, the FRBP technique can be used to predict drivers' SA with an accuracy of 88% using a neural network model.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The FRBP technique, which incorporates eye movements and brain activities, can provide more comprehensive evaluation of SA. Findings highlight the potential of utilizing FRBP to monitor drivers' SA in real-time.</p><p><strong>Application: </strong>The proposed framework can be expanded and applied to driver state monitoring systems to measure human SA in real-world driving.</p>","PeriodicalId":56333,"journal":{"name":"Human Factors","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49685473","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Multilevel Confirmatory Factor Analysis Reveals Two Distinct Human-Automation Trust Constructs. 多层次确认因素分析揭示了两种不同的人机信任结构。
IF 2.9 3区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-23 DOI: 10.1177/00187208241263774
Yusuke Yamani, Shelby K Long, Tetsuya Sato, Abby L Braitman, Michael S Politowicz, Eric T Chancey

Objective: This work examined the relationship of the constructs measured by the trust scales developed by Chancey et al. (2017) and Jian et al. (2000) using a multilevel confirmatory factor analysis (CFA).

Background: Modern theories of automation trust have been proposed based on data collected using trust scales. Chancey et al. (2017) adapted Madsen and Gregor's (2000) trust scale to align with Lee and See's (2004) human-automation trust framework. In contrast, Jian et al. (2000) developed a scale empirically with trust and distrust as factors. However, it remains unclear whether these two scales measure the same construct.

Method: We analyzed data collected from previous experiments to investigate the relationship between the two trust scales using a multilevel CFA.

Results: Data provided evidence that Jian et al. (2000) and Chancey et al. (2017) automation trust scales are only weakly related. Trust and distrust are found to be distinct factors in Jian et al.'s (2000) scale, whereas performance, process, and purpose are distinct factors in Chancey et al.'s (2017) trust scale.

Conclusion: The analysis suggested that the two scales purporting to measure human-automation trust are only weakly related.

Application: Trust researchers and automation designers may consider using Chancey et al. (2017) and Jian et al. (2000) scales to capture different characteristics of human-automation trust.

研究目的本研究采用多层次确证因子分析(CFA)方法,研究了 Chancey 等人(2017 年)和 Jian 等人(2000 年)开发的信任量表所测量的构造之间的关系:现代自动化信任理论是基于信任量表收集的数据提出的。Chancey 等人(2017 年)改编了 Madsen 和 Gregor(2000 年)的信任量表,使其与 Lee 和 See(2004 年)的人类-自动化信任框架相一致。相反,Jian 等人(2000 年)则根据经验开发了一个以信任和不信任为因子的量表。然而,这两个量表是否测量的是同一概念仍不清楚:方法:我们分析了从以前的实验中收集到的数据,使用多层次 CFA 方法研究了两个信任量表之间的关系:数据提供的证据表明,Jian 等人(2000 年)和 Chancey 等人(2017 年)的自动化信任量表只是弱相关。在 Jian 等人(2000 年)的量表中,信任和不信任是不同的因素,而在 Chancey 等人(2017 年)的信任量表中,性能、过程和目的是不同的因素:分析表明,这两个旨在衡量人与自动化信任度的量表只有微弱的关联性:信任研究人员和自动化设计人员可以考虑使用 Chancey 等人(2017 年)和 Jian 等人(2000 年)的量表来捕捉人机信任的不同特征。
{"title":"Multilevel Confirmatory Factor Analysis Reveals Two Distinct Human-Automation Trust Constructs.","authors":"Yusuke Yamani, Shelby K Long, Tetsuya Sato, Abby L Braitman, Michael S Politowicz, Eric T Chancey","doi":"10.1177/00187208241263774","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/00187208241263774","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>This work examined the relationship of the constructs measured by the trust scales developed by Chancey et al. (2017) and Jian et al. (2000) using a multilevel confirmatory factor analysis (CFA).</p><p><strong>Background: </strong>Modern theories of automation trust have been proposed based on data collected using trust scales. Chancey et al. (2017) adapted Madsen and Gregor's (2000) trust scale to align with Lee and See's (2004) human-automation trust framework. In contrast, Jian et al. (2000) developed a scale empirically with trust and distrust as factors. However, it remains unclear whether these two scales measure the same construct.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We analyzed data collected from previous experiments to investigate the relationship between the two trust scales using a multilevel CFA.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Data provided evidence that Jian et al. (2000) and Chancey et al. (2017) automation trust scales are only weakly related. Trust and distrust are found to be distinct factors in Jian et al.'s (2000) scale, whereas performance, process, and purpose are distinct factors in Chancey et al.'s (2017) trust scale.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The analysis suggested that the two scales purporting to measure human-automation trust are only weakly related.</p><p><strong>Application: </strong>Trust researchers and automation designers may consider using Chancey et al. (2017) and Jian et al. (2000) scales to capture different characteristics of human-automation trust.</p>","PeriodicalId":56333,"journal":{"name":"Human Factors","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-07-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141753504","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sensorimotor Challenges in Minimally Invasive Surgery: A Theoretically-Oriented Review. 微创手术中的感觉运动挑战:以理论为导向的综述。
IF 2.9 3区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-22 DOI: 10.1177/00187208241263684
Christopher L Hewitson, Matthew J Crossley, John Cartmill, David M Kaplan

Objective: This review surveys the literature on sensorimotor challenges impacting performance in laparoscopic minimally invasive surgery (MIS).

Background: Despite its well-known benefits for patients, achieving proficiency in MIS can be challenging for surgeons due to many factors including altered visual perspectives and fulcrum effects in instrument handling. Research on these and other sensorimotor challenges has been hindered by imprecise terminology and the lack of a unified theoretical framework to guide research questions in the field.

Method: We conducted a systematic survey of the MIS literature, focusing on studies investigating sensorimotor challenges affecting laparoscopic performance. To provide a common foundation for cross-study comparisons, we propose a standardized taxonomy that distinguishes between different experimental paradigms used in the literature. We then show how the computational motor learning perspective provides a unifying theoretical framework for the field that can facilitate progress and motivate future research along clearer, hypothesis-driven lines.

Results: The survey identified diverse sensorimotor perturbations in MIS, which can be effectively categorized according to our proposed taxonomy. Studies investigating monitor-, camera-, and tool-based perturbations were systematically analyzed, elucidating their impact on surgical performance. We also show how the computational motor learning perspective provides deeper insights and potential strategies to mitigate challenges.

Conclusion: Sensorimotor challenges significantly impact MIS, necessitating a systematic, empirically informed approach. Our proposed taxonomy and theoretical framework shed light on the complexities involved, paving the way for more structured research and targeted training approaches to enhance surgical proficiency.

Application: Understanding the sensorimotor challenges inherent to MIS can guide the design of improved training curricula and inform the configuration of setups in the operating room to enhance surgeon performance and ultimately patient outcomes. This review offers key insights for surgeons, educators, and researchers in surgical performance and technology development.

目的:本综述调查了影响腹腔镜微创手术(MIS)表现的感觉运动挑战的文献:本综述调查了有关影响腹腔镜微创手术(MIS)表现的感觉运动挑战的文献:背景:尽管腹腔镜微创手术对患者的益处众所周知,但由于许多因素,包括器械操作中视觉视角的改变和支点效应,要熟练掌握腹腔镜微创手术对外科医生来说是一项挑战。由于术语不准确以及缺乏统一的理论框架来指导该领域的研究问题,有关这些及其他感觉运动挑战的研究一直受到阻碍:我们对管理信息系统文献进行了系统调查,重点关注影响腹腔镜手术表现的感觉运动挑战研究。为了给跨研究比较提供一个共同的基础,我们提出了一个标准化的分类标准,以区分文献中使用的不同实验范式。然后,我们展示了计算运动学习的观点如何为该领域提供了一个统一的理论框架,从而促进该领域的进展,并激励未来的研究沿着更清晰、假设驱动的路线前进:调查发现,在 MIS 中存在多种传感器运动干扰,可根据我们提出的分类法进行有效分类。我们系统分析了调查监视器、摄像头和工具干扰的研究,阐明了它们对手术表现的影响。我们还展示了计算运动学习观点如何提供更深入的见解和潜在的策略来缓解挑战:结论:感知运动挑战对 MIS 有着重大影响,因此有必要采用一种系统的、以经验为依据的方法。我们提出的分类法和理论框架揭示了其中的复杂性,为更有条理的研究和有针对性的培训方法铺平了道路,从而提高手术熟练程度:应用:了解 MIS 所固有的感觉运动挑战可以指导改进培训课程的设计,并为手术室的设置提供信息,从而提高外科医生的表现,最终改善患者的预后。这篇综述为外科医生、教育工作者以及手术性能和技术开发方面的研究人员提供了重要的见解。
{"title":"Sensorimotor Challenges in Minimally Invasive Surgery: A Theoretically-Oriented Review.","authors":"Christopher L Hewitson, Matthew J Crossley, John Cartmill, David M Kaplan","doi":"10.1177/00187208241263684","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00187208241263684","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>This review surveys the literature on sensorimotor challenges impacting performance in laparoscopic minimally invasive surgery (MIS).</p><p><strong>Background: </strong>Despite its well-known benefits for patients, achieving proficiency in MIS can be challenging for surgeons due to many factors including altered visual perspectives and fulcrum effects in instrument handling. Research on these and other sensorimotor challenges has been hindered by imprecise terminology and the lack of a unified theoretical framework to guide research questions in the field.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We conducted a systematic survey of the MIS literature, focusing on studies investigating sensorimotor challenges affecting laparoscopic performance. To provide a common foundation for cross-study comparisons, we propose a standardized taxonomy that distinguishes between different experimental paradigms used in the literature. We then show how the computational motor learning perspective provides a unifying theoretical framework for the field that can facilitate progress and motivate future research along clearer, hypothesis-driven lines.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The survey identified diverse sensorimotor perturbations in MIS, which can be effectively categorized according to our proposed taxonomy. Studies investigating monitor-, camera-, and tool-based perturbations were systematically analyzed, elucidating their impact on surgical performance. We also show how the computational motor learning perspective provides deeper insights and potential strategies to mitigate challenges.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Sensorimotor challenges significantly impact MIS, necessitating a systematic, empirically informed approach. Our proposed taxonomy and theoretical framework shed light on the complexities involved, paving the way for more structured research and targeted training approaches to enhance surgical proficiency.</p><p><strong>Application: </strong>Understanding the sensorimotor challenges inherent to MIS can guide the design of improved training curricula and inform the configuration of setups in the operating room to enhance surgeon performance and ultimately patient outcomes. This review offers key insights for surgeons, educators, and researchers in surgical performance and technology development.</p>","PeriodicalId":56333,"journal":{"name":"Human Factors","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-07-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141749838","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Visual Complexity of Head-Up Display in Automobiles Modulates Attentional Tunneling. 汽车抬头显示器的视觉复杂性会调节注意力隧道效应。
IF 2.9 3区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-08 DOI: 10.1177/00187208231181496
Jieun Lee, Nahyun Lee, Jangkyu Ju, Jihwan Chae, Jiyoon Park, Hoe Sung Ryu, Yang Seok Cho

Objective: To investigate how the visual complexity of head-up displays (HUDs) influence the allocation of driver's attention in two separate visual domains (near and far domains).

Background: The types and amount of information displayed on automobile HUDs have increased. With limited human attention capacity, increased visual complexity in the near domain may lead to interference in the effective processing of information in the far domain.

Method: Near-domain and far-domain vision were separately tested using a dual-task paradigm. In a simulated road environment, 62 participants were to control the speed of the vehicle (SMT; near domain) and manually respond to probes (PDT; far domain) simultaneously. Five HUD complexity levels including a HUD-absent condition were presented block-wise.

Results: Near domain performance was not modulated by the HUD complexity levels. However, the far domain detection accuracies were impaired as the HUD complexity level increased, with greater accuracy differences observed between central and peripheral probes.

Conclusion: Increased HUD visual complexity leads to a biased deployment of driver attention toward the central visual field. Therefore, the formulation of HUD designs must be preceded by an in-depth investigation of the dynamics of human cognition.

Application: To ensure driving safety, HUD designs should be rendered with minimal visual complexity by incorporating only essential information relevant to driving and removing driving-irrelevant or additional visual details.

目的:研究平视显示器(HUD)的视觉复杂性如何影响驾驶员在两个不同视觉域(近域和远域)的注意力分配:研究平视显示器(HUD)的视觉复杂性如何影响驾驶员在两个不同视觉域(近域和远域)的注意力分配:背景:汽车平视显示器上显示的信息种类和数量不断增加。在人类注意力有限的情况下,近域视觉复杂性的增加可能会干扰远域信息的有效处理:方法:采用双任务范式分别测试近域和远域视觉。在模拟道路环境中,62 名参与者要同时控制车速(SMT;近域)和手动响应探针(PDT;远域)。五个 HUD 复杂程度(包括 HUD 不存在的条件)按区块排列:结果:近域性能不受 HUD 复杂程度的影响。然而,随着 HUD 复杂程度的增加,远域检测的准确性受到了影响,在中央和外围探测器之间观察到了更大的准确性差异:结论:增加 HUD 视觉复杂度会导致驾驶员的注意力偏向中央视野。因此,在设计 HUD 之前,必须深入研究人类认知的动态变化:应用:为确保驾驶安全,HUD 设计应尽量减少视觉复杂性,只纳入与驾驶相关的基本信息,去除与驾驶无关的或额外的视觉细节。
{"title":"Visual Complexity of Head-Up Display in Automobiles Modulates Attentional Tunneling.","authors":"Jieun Lee, Nahyun Lee, Jangkyu Ju, Jihwan Chae, Jiyoon Park, Hoe Sung Ryu, Yang Seok Cho","doi":"10.1177/00187208231181496","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00187208231181496","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>To investigate how the visual complexity of head-up displays (HUDs) influence the allocation of driver's attention in two separate visual domains (near and far domains).</p><p><strong>Background: </strong>The types and amount of information displayed on automobile HUDs have increased. With limited human attention capacity, increased visual complexity in the near domain may lead to interference in the effective processing of information in the far domain.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Near-domain and far-domain vision were separately tested using a dual-task paradigm. In a simulated road environment, 62 participants were to control the speed of the vehicle (SMT; near domain) and manually respond to probes (PDT; far domain) simultaneously. Five HUD complexity levels including a HUD-absent condition were presented block-wise.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Near domain performance was not modulated by the HUD complexity levels. However, the far domain detection accuracies were impaired as the HUD complexity level increased, with greater accuracy differences observed between central and peripheral probes.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Increased HUD visual complexity leads to a biased deployment of driver attention toward the central visual field. Therefore, the formulation of HUD designs must be preceded by an in-depth investigation of the dynamics of human cognition.</p><p><strong>Application: </strong>To ensure driving safety, HUD designs should be rendered with minimal visual complexity by incorporating only essential information relevant to driving and removing driving-irrelevant or additional visual details.</p>","PeriodicalId":56333,"journal":{"name":"Human Factors","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9590463","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Primacy Effects in Extended Cognitive Strategy Choice: Initial Speed Benefits Outweigh Later Speed Benefits. 扩展认知策略选择中的优先效应:初期速度优势大于后期速度优势。
IF 2.9 3区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-23 DOI: 10.1177/00187208231195747
Patrick P Weis, Wilfried Kunde

Background: Human performers often recruit environment-based assistance to acquire or process information, such as relying on a smartphone app, a search engine, or a conversational agent. To make informed choices between several of such extended cognitive strategies, performers need to monitor the performance of these options.

Objective: In the present study, we investigated whether participants monitor an extended cognitive strategy's performance-here, speed-more closely during initial as compared to later encounters.

Methods: In three experiments, 737 participants were asked to first observe speed differences between two competing cognitive strategies-here, two competing algorithms that can obtain answers to trivia questions-and eventually choose between both strategies based on the observations.

Results: Participants were sensitive to subtle speed differences and selected strategies accordingly. Most remarkably, even when participants performed identically with both strategies across all encounters, the strategy with superior speed in the initial encounters was preferred. Worded differently, participants exhibited a technology-use primacy effect. Contrarily, evidence for a recency effect was weak at best.

Conclusion: These results suggest that great care is required when performers are first acquainted with novel ways to acquire or process information. Superior initial performance has the potential to desensitize the performer for inferior later performance and thus prohibit optimal choice.

Application: Awareness of primacy enables users and designers of extended cognitive strategies to actively remediate suboptimal behavior originating in early monitoring episodes.

背景:人类表演者经常需要环境辅助来获取或处理信息,例如依靠智能手机应用程序、搜索引擎或对话代理。为了在几种此类扩展认知策略之间做出明智的选择,表演者需要监控这些选项的表现:在本研究中,我们调查了参与者在最初接触时是否比后来接触时更密切地监控扩展认知策略的性能--这里指的是速度:在三项实验中,我们要求 737 名参与者首先观察两种相互竞争的认知策略之间的速度差异--这里是两种相互竞争的算法,它们都能获得琐事问题的答案--并最终根据观察结果在两种策略之间做出选择:结果:参与者对微妙的速度差异非常敏感,并据此选择策略。最值得注意的是,即使参与者在所有交锋中使用两种策略的表现完全相同,在最初交锋中速度更快的策略也更受青睐。换句话说,参与者表现出了技术使用优先效应。相反,追忆效应的证据充其量也很微弱:这些结果表明,当表演者初次接触获取或处理信息的新方法时,需要非常谨慎。结论:这些结果表明,当表演者第一次熟悉新的获取或处理信息的方式时,需要非常小心。最初的优异表现有可能使表演者对后来的低劣表现不再敏感,从而无法做出最佳选择:应用:对优先性的认识使扩展认知策略的使用者和设计者能够积极纠正源于早期监测事件的次优行为。
{"title":"Primacy Effects in Extended Cognitive Strategy Choice: Initial Speed Benefits Outweigh Later Speed Benefits.","authors":"Patrick P Weis, Wilfried Kunde","doi":"10.1177/00187208231195747","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00187208231195747","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Human performers often recruit environment-based assistance to acquire or process information, such as relying on a smartphone app, a search engine, or a conversational agent. To make informed choices between several of such extended cognitive strategies, performers need to monitor the performance of these options.</p><p><strong>Objective: </strong>In the present study, we investigated whether participants monitor an extended cognitive strategy's performance-here, speed-more closely during initial as compared to later encounters.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>In three experiments, 737 participants were asked to first observe speed differences between two competing cognitive strategies-here, two competing algorithms that can obtain answers to trivia questions-and eventually choose between both strategies based on the observations.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Participants were sensitive to subtle speed differences and selected strategies accordingly. Most remarkably, even when participants performed identically with both strategies across <i>all</i> encounters, the strategy with superior speed in the <i>initial</i> encounters was preferred. Worded differently, participants exhibited a technology-use primacy effect. Contrarily, evidence for a recency effect was weak at best.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>These results suggest that great care is required when performers are first acquainted with novel ways to acquire or process information. Superior initial performance has the potential to desensitize the performer for inferior later performance and thus prohibit optimal choice.</p><p><strong>Application: </strong>Awareness of primacy enables users and designers of extended cognitive strategies to actively remediate suboptimal behavior originating in early monitoring episodes.</p>","PeriodicalId":56333,"journal":{"name":"Human Factors","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11089827/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10049259","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Fostering Drivers' Trust in Automated Driving Styles: The Role of Driver Perception of Automated Driving Maneuvers. 培养驾驶员对自动驾驶方式的信任:驾驶员对自动驾驶操纵的感知所起的作用。
IF 2.9 3区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2023-07-25 DOI: 10.1177/00187208231189661
Zheng Ma, Yiqi Zhang

Objective: This study investigated the impact of driving styles of drivers and automated vehicles (AVs) on drivers' perception of automated driving maneuvers and quantified the relationships among drivers' perception of AV maneuvers, driver trust, and acceptance of AVs.

Background: Previous studies on automated driving styles focused on the impact of AV's global driving style on driver's attitude and driving performance. However, research on drivers' perception of automated driving maneuvers at the specific driving style level is still lacking.

Method: Sixteen aggressive drivers and sixteen defensive drivers were recruited to experience twelve driving scenarios in either an aggressive AV or a defensive AV on the driving simulator. Their perception of AV maneuvers, trust, and acceptance was measured via questionnaires, and driving performance was collected via the driving simulator.

Results: Results revealed that drivers' trust and acceptance of AVs would decrease significantly if they perceived AVs to have a higher speed, larger deceleration, smaller deceleration, or shorter stopping distance than expected. Moreover, defensive drivers perceived significantly greater inappropriateness of these maneuvers from aggressive AVs than defensive AVs, whereas aggressive drivers didn't differ significantly in their perceived inappropriateness of these maneuvers with different driving styles.

Conclusion: The driving styles of automated vehicles and drivers influenced drivers' perception of automated driving maneuvers, which influence their trust and acceptance of AVs.

Application: This study suggested that the design of AVs should consider drivers' perceptions of automated driving maneuvers to avoid undermining drivers' trust and acceptance of AVs.

研究目的本研究调查了驾驶员和自动驾驶汽车(AV)的驾驶风格对驾驶员自动驾驶操作感知的影响,并量化了驾驶员对自动驾驶汽车操作感知、驾驶员信任度和对自动驾驶汽车接受度之间的关系:背景:以往关于自动驾驶风格的研究主要集中在自动驾驶汽车的整体驾驶风格对驾驶员态度和驾驶表现的影响上。背景:以往关于自动驾驶风格的研究主要集中在自动驾驶汽车的整体驾驶风格对驾驶员态度和驾驶表现的影响上,但对驾驶员在具体驾驶风格层面对自动驾驶操作的感知还缺乏研究:方法:招募了 16 名激进型驾驶员和 16 名防御型驾驶员,让他们在驾驶模拟器上体验激进型自动驾驶汽车或防御型自动驾驶汽车的 12 个驾驶场景。结果:结果表明,驾驶员的信任度和接受度均高于其他驾驶员:结果显示,如果驾驶员认为自动驾驶汽车的速度比预期的高、减速比预期的大、减速比预期的小或停车距离比预期的短,那么他们对自动驾驶汽车的信任度和接受度就会明显下降。此外,防御性驾驶员认为攻击性自动驾驶汽车的这些操作的不适宜性明显高于防御性自动驾驶汽车,而攻击性驾驶员在不同驾驶风格下对这些操作的不适宜性的感知没有明显差异:结论:自动驾驶汽车和驾驶员的驾驶风格会影响驾驶员对自动驾驶操作的感知,从而影响他们对自动驾驶汽车的信任度和接受度:本研究建议,在设计自动驾驶汽车时应考虑驾驶员对自动驾驶操作的感知,以避免削弱驾驶员对自动驾驶汽车的信任和接受程度。
{"title":"Fostering Drivers' Trust in Automated Driving Styles: The Role of Driver Perception of Automated Driving Maneuvers.","authors":"Zheng Ma, Yiqi Zhang","doi":"10.1177/00187208231189661","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00187208231189661","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>This study investigated the impact of driving styles of drivers and automated vehicles (AVs) on drivers' perception of automated driving maneuvers and quantified the relationships among drivers' perception of AV maneuvers, driver trust, and acceptance of AVs.</p><p><strong>Background: </strong>Previous studies on automated driving styles focused on the impact of AV's global driving style on driver's attitude and driving performance. However, research on drivers' perception of automated driving maneuvers at the specific driving style level is still lacking.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Sixteen aggressive drivers and sixteen defensive drivers were recruited to experience twelve driving scenarios in either an aggressive AV or a defensive AV on the driving simulator. Their perception of AV maneuvers, trust, and acceptance was measured via questionnaires, and driving performance was collected via the driving simulator.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Results revealed that drivers' trust and acceptance of AVs would decrease significantly if they perceived AVs to have a higher speed, larger deceleration, smaller deceleration, or shorter stopping distance than expected. Moreover, defensive drivers perceived significantly greater inappropriateness of these maneuvers from aggressive AVs than defensive AVs, whereas aggressive drivers didn't differ significantly in their perceived inappropriateness of these maneuvers with different driving styles.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The driving styles of automated vehicles and drivers influenced drivers' perception of automated driving maneuvers, which influence their trust and acceptance of AVs.</p><p><strong>Application: </strong>This study suggested that the design of AVs should consider drivers' perceptions of automated driving maneuvers to avoid undermining drivers' trust and acceptance of AVs.</p>","PeriodicalId":56333,"journal":{"name":"Human Factors","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10228662","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Finding a City Name in a Traffic Sign: Effects of Word Case and Visual Motion. 在交通标志中查找城市名称:单词大小写和视觉运动的影响。
IF 2.9 3区 心理学 Q1 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-10 DOI: 10.1177/00187208231192756
Pilar Tejero, Laura Royo, Javier Roca

Objectives: To investigate the word recognition effects of the use of all-uppercase (e.g., VALENCIA) or titled-case (e.g., Valencia) for city names in traffic signs, controlling for word size, and comparing stationary and dynamic viewing situations.

Background: Prior studies provide mixed evidence regarding the effects of word case on the recognition of city names in traffic signs. Moreover, the evidence on the potential impact of visual motion on these effects is scarce.

Method: We carried out an experimental study using simulated traffic signs. The task was to indicate, for each sign, whether it contained a given city name or not (word search task, 50% positive trials). Visual motion of signs was manipulated as a between-participants factor: stationary (the sign was still) versus dynamic (the sign expanded as if the participant was approaching to it). Word case was manipulated as a within-participants factor: all-uppercase versus two titled-case conditions varying in font size: width-matched titled-case and point size-matched titled-case.

Results: In both the stationary and dynamic conditions, all-uppercase resulted in more incorrect responses and slower latencies than width-matched titled-case. When compared to point size-matched titled-case, all-uppercase produced slower correct responses in the stationary condition, whereas faster in the dynamic condition.

Conclusion: Other factors being equal, all-uppercase city names will be recognized worse than their titled-case versions in traffic signs, both in stationary and dynamic situations.

Application: Results in the current experimental study would be of interest in the design of traffic signs and other circumstances in which text is presented in motion.

研究目的研究交通标志中的城市名称使用全大写(如 VALENCIA)或有标题大写(如 Valencia)时的单词识别效果,同时控制单词大小,并比较静态和动态观察情况:背景:先前的研究提供了关于单词大小写对交通标志中城市名称识别影响的不同证据。此外,关于视觉运动对这些影响的潜在影响的证据也很少:我们使用模拟交通标志进行了一项实验研究。任务是指出每个标志是否包含给定的城市名称(单词搜索任务,50% 的肯定试验)。标志牌的视觉运动作为参与者之间的因素进行了处理:静止(标志牌静止不动)与动态(标志牌扩大,好像参与者正在靠近它)。单词大小写则作为参与者内部因素进行操纵:全大写与两种字体大小不同的标题大小写条件:宽度匹配的标题大小写和点大小匹配的标题大小写:在静态和动态条件下,全大写字母比宽度匹配的大写字母导致了更多的错误回答和更慢的延迟。在静态条件下,与点大小匹配的标题大小写相比,全大写字母的正确反应速度较慢,而在动态条件下则较快:结论:在其他因素相同的情况下,无论是在静态还是动态条件下,全大写字母的城市名称在交通标志中的识别率都比有标题大写字母的城市名称低:应用:本次实验研究的结果将对交通标志的设计和其他在运动中显示文字的环境产生影响。
{"title":"Finding a City Name in a Traffic Sign: Effects of Word Case and Visual Motion.","authors":"Pilar Tejero, Laura Royo, Javier Roca","doi":"10.1177/00187208231192756","DOIUrl":"10.1177/00187208231192756","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objectives: </strong>To investigate the word recognition effects of the use of all-uppercase (e.g., VALENCIA) or titled-case (e.g., Valencia) for city names in traffic signs, controlling for word size, and comparing stationary and dynamic viewing situations.</p><p><strong>Background: </strong>Prior studies provide mixed evidence regarding the effects of word case on the recognition of city names in traffic signs. Moreover, the evidence on the potential impact of visual motion on these effects is scarce.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We carried out an experimental study using simulated traffic signs. The task was to indicate, for each sign, whether it contained a given city name or not (word search task, 50% positive trials). Visual motion of signs was manipulated as a between-participants factor: stationary (the sign was still) versus dynamic (the sign expanded as if the participant was approaching to it). Word case was manipulated as a within-participants factor: all-uppercase versus two titled-case conditions varying in font size: width-matched titled-case and point size-matched titled-case.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>In both the stationary and dynamic conditions, all-uppercase resulted in more incorrect responses and slower latencies than width-matched titled-case. When compared to point size-matched titled-case, all-uppercase produced slower correct responses in the stationary condition, whereas faster in the dynamic condition.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Other factors being equal, all-uppercase city names will be recognized worse than their titled-case versions in traffic signs, both in stationary and dynamic situations.</p><p><strong>Application: </strong>Results in the current experimental study would be of interest in the design of traffic signs and other circumstances in which text is presented in motion.</p>","PeriodicalId":56333,"journal":{"name":"Human Factors","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10028089","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Human Factors
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1