首页 > 最新文献

acta ethologica最新文献

英文 中文
Water turbidity–induced alterations in coloration and courtship behavior of male guppies (Poecilia reticulata) 水浊度引起雄性孔雀鱼(Poecilia reticulata)颜色和求偶行为的改变
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2021-04-21 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-021-00369-8
Bruno Camargo-dos-Santos, Bruno Bastos Gonçalves, Marina Sanson Bellot, Isabela Inforzato Guermandi, Assaf Barki, Percília Cardoso Giaquinto

Water turbidity deteriorates visibility and thereby may change the physiology and behavior of aquatic animals that rely on vision. In the guppy fish (Poecilia reticulata), a key element in the mating behavior and reproductive success of males is female mate choice, which is predominantly based on visual signals. Females choose attractive males based on body coloration, and males court females by displaying their coloration. Here, we demonstrate that guppy males exhibit morphological and behavioral adjustments in response to changes in the visual environment. Males reared in turbid water had more conspicuous coloration than males reared in clear water, with higher intensity of carotenoid-based and ultraviolet colors, but not a larger area of red spots on the body. However, they performed less courtship displays in turbid water than males reared in clear water performed in clear water. Thus, increased coloration in turbid-water males was not accompanied by increased effort to display it. Although our findings demonstrated developmental plasticity in mating-related traits, turbidity-induced alteration in coloration did not match behavior change as could be predicted by favoring male attractiveness.

水浑浊恶化能见度,从而可能改变依赖视觉的水生动物的生理和行为。在孔雀鱼(Poecilia reticulata)中,雄性的交配行为和繁殖成功的一个关键因素是雌性的配偶选择,这主要是基于视觉信号。雌性根据身体的颜色选择有吸引力的雄性,雄性通过展示自己的颜色来追求雌性。在这里,我们证明雄性孔雀鱼表现出形态和行为的调整,以应对视觉环境的变化。在浑浊水中饲养的雄性比在清澈水中饲养的雄性有更明显的颜色,类胡萝卜素和紫外线的颜色强度更高,但身体上的红点面积较小。然而,它们在浑浊水中的求偶行为比在清水中饲养的雄鱼要少。因此,浑浊水雄鱼颜色的增加并不伴随着表现颜色的增加。尽管我们的研究结果证明了交配相关性状的发育可塑性,但浑浊度引起的颜色变化与倾向于雄性吸引力的行为变化并不匹配。
{"title":"Water turbidity–induced alterations in coloration and courtship behavior of male guppies (Poecilia reticulata)","authors":"Bruno Camargo-dos-Santos,&nbsp;Bruno Bastos Gonçalves,&nbsp;Marina Sanson Bellot,&nbsp;Isabela Inforzato Guermandi,&nbsp;Assaf Barki,&nbsp;Percília Cardoso Giaquinto","doi":"10.1007/s10211-021-00369-8","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-021-00369-8","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Water turbidity deteriorates visibility and thereby may change the physiology and behavior of aquatic animals that rely on vision. In the guppy fish (<i>Poecilia reticulata</i>), a key element in the mating behavior and reproductive success of males is female mate choice, which is predominantly based on visual signals. Females choose attractive males based on body coloration, and males court females by displaying their coloration. Here, we demonstrate that guppy males exhibit morphological and behavioral adjustments in response to changes in the visual environment. Males reared in turbid water had more conspicuous coloration than males reared in clear water, with higher intensity of carotenoid-based and ultraviolet colors, but not a larger area of red spots on the body. However, they performed less courtship displays in turbid water than males reared in clear water performed in clear water. Thus, increased coloration in turbid-water males was not accompanied by increased effort to display it. Although our findings demonstrated developmental plasticity in mating-related traits, turbidity-induced alteration in coloration did not match behavior change as could be predicted by favoring male attractiveness.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"24 2","pages":"127 - 136"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2021-04-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s10211-021-00369-8","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49353150","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
A non-vocal alarm? Effects of wing trill playbacks on antipredator responses in the scaled dove 非声音警报?翅膀颤音回放对斑鸠反捕食者反应的影响
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2021-04-15 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-021-00368-9
Paulo Sérgio Amorim, Raphael Igor Dias

Animals have evolved a variety of mechanisms to detect and avoid predation. The non-vocal sounds produced by some bird species during takeoff flights have been considered to function as an alarm call, because they may convey information about predation risk. Here, we experimentally investigated the effects of the non-vocal sound (wing trills) produced by the scaled dove (Columbina squammata) on antipredation behaviours of conspecifics. We evaluated the individual response to playbacks of the wing trill stimulus and compared it to the response to other two control stimuli (vocalizations of the scaled dove and the southern house wren). We found that doves’ probability to become vigilant or to display freezing behaviour was higher after a wing trills stimulus in comparison to the other playback stimuli. These results suggest that wing trill production in scaled doves communicate potential risks and are considered by the individuals in the decision-making process, but we cannot rule out the possibility that any takeoff flight sound might also promote antipredator responses.

动物已经进化出各种各样的机制来探测和躲避捕食者。一些鸟类在起飞时发出的非声音被认为是一种警报,因为它们可能传递有关捕食风险的信息。本文通过实验研究了鳞鸽(Columbina squammata)发出的非声音(翅鸣)对同种生物反捕食行为的影响。我们评估了个体对翅膀颤音刺激回放的反应,并将其与其他两种对照刺激(鳞鸽和南方鹪鹩的声音)的反应进行了比较。我们发现,与其他回放刺激相比,受到翅膀颤音刺激后,鸽子变得警惕或表现出冻结行为的可能性更高。这些结果表明,翅鸣的产生传达了潜在的风险,并被个体在决策过程中考虑,但我们不能排除任何起飞飞行声音也可能促进反捕食者反应的可能性。
{"title":"A non-vocal alarm? Effects of wing trill playbacks on antipredator responses in the scaled dove","authors":"Paulo Sérgio Amorim,&nbsp;Raphael Igor Dias","doi":"10.1007/s10211-021-00368-9","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-021-00368-9","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Animals have evolved a variety of mechanisms to detect and avoid predation. The non-vocal sounds produced by some bird species during takeoff flights have been considered to function as an alarm call, because they may convey information about predation risk. Here, we experimentally investigated the effects of the non-vocal sound (wing trills) produced by the scaled dove (<i>Columbina squammata</i>) on antipredation behaviours of conspecifics. We evaluated the individual response to playbacks of the wing trill stimulus and compared it to the response to other two control stimuli (vocalizations of the scaled dove and the southern house wren). We found that doves’ probability to become vigilant or to display freezing behaviour was higher after a wing trills stimulus in comparison to the other playback stimuli. These results suggest that wing trill production in scaled doves communicate potential risks and are considered by the individuals in the decision-making process, but we cannot rule out the possibility that any takeoff flight sound might also promote antipredator responses.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"24 2","pages":"119 - 126"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2021-04-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s10211-021-00368-9","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49125128","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Correction to: Thanatological behavior of a female Leopard (Panthera pardus fusca) 修正:雌性豹(Panthera pardus fusca)的死亡行为
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2021-04-05 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-021-00367-w
Reuven Yosef, Hemant Dabi, Swapnil Kumbhojkar
{"title":"Correction to: Thanatological behavior of a female Leopard (Panthera pardus fusca)","authors":"Reuven Yosef,&nbsp;Hemant Dabi,&nbsp;Swapnil Kumbhojkar","doi":"10.1007/s10211-021-00367-w","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-021-00367-w","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"24 2","pages":"141 - 141"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2021-04-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s10211-021-00367-w","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50008848","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
To group or not to group: group size dynamics and intestinal parasites in Indian peafowl populations 分组或不分组:印度孔雀种群的群体大小动态和肠道寄生虫
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2021-03-22 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-021-00366-x
Priyanka Dange, Pranav Mhaisalkar, Dhanashree Paranjpe

Animals can form groups for various reasons including safety from predators, access to potential mates and benefits of allo-parental care. However, there are costs associated with living in a group such as competition for food and/or mates with other members of the group, higher chances of disease transmission, etc. Group size dynamics can change with the biotic and abiotic environment around individuals. In the current study, we explored the links between group size dynamics and intestinal parasites of Indian peafowl (Pavo cristatus) in the context of seasons and food provisioning. Data for group size was collected across three seasons (pre-monsoon, monsoon and post-monsoon) at three field sites (Morachi Chincholi, Nashik and Rajasthan). Individual and group sightings of peafowl were noted down along with group size and composition (no. of males, females, adults, juveniles and sub-adults). Faecal samples were collected from food provision and non-provision areas across the same three seasons at same field sites. Parasite load in the samples was quantified using microscopic examination. Group size was significantly higher in pre-monsoon season as compared with monsoon and post-monsoon seasons. Monsoon and post-monsoon seasons had higher intestinal parasite prevalence and load as compared with pre-monsoon season. Thus, group size and intestinal parasites of Indian peafowl have an inverse relationship across seasons. Parasite load was significantly greater at food provision sites as compared with non-provision sites while parasite prevalence was comparable. Aggregation of individuals at the food provision sites may influence the parasite transmission and group-size dynamics in Indian peafowl. In conclusion, Indian peafowl are behaviourally plastic and fission-fusion of social groups may allow them to tackle ecological pressures such as predation and parasite transmission in different seasons.

动物形成群体的原因多种多样,包括躲避捕食者、获得潜在配偶以及获得非亲代照顾的好处。然而,在一个群体中生活是有代价的,例如与群体中的其他成员竞争食物和/或配偶,更高的疾病传播机会等。群体大小动态可以随着个体周围的生物和非生物环境而变化。在本研究中,我们在季节和食物供应的背景下探索了印度孔雀(Pavo cristatus)群体大小动态与肠道寄生虫之间的联系。在三个野外站点(Morachi Chincholi、Nashik和Rajasthan)收集了三个季节(季风前、季风和季风后)的群体规模数据。记录了孔雀的个体和群体的目击情况,以及群体的大小和组成。雄性、雌性、成虫、幼虫和亚成虫)。在相同的三个季节,在相同的野外地点,从食物供应区和非供应区收集粪便样本。用显微镜检查定量样品中的寄生虫载量。与季风和季风后季节相比,季风前季节的群体规模显著增加。与季风前季节相比,季风期和季风后季节肠道寄生虫患病率和负荷较高。因此,印度孔雀的群体规模和肠道寄生虫在不同季节呈反比关系。提供食物的地点与不提供食物的地点相比,寄生虫的负荷明显更大,而寄生虫的流行率是相当的。食物供应地点的个体聚集可能影响印度孔雀的寄生虫传播和群体大小动态。总之,印度孔雀在行为上具有可塑性,社会群体的分裂融合可能使它们能够应对不同季节的捕食和寄生虫传播等生态压力。
{"title":"To group or not to group: group size dynamics and intestinal parasites in Indian peafowl populations","authors":"Priyanka Dange,&nbsp;Pranav Mhaisalkar,&nbsp;Dhanashree Paranjpe","doi":"10.1007/s10211-021-00366-x","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-021-00366-x","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Animals can form groups for various reasons including safety from predators, access to potential mates and benefits of allo-parental care. However, there are costs associated with living in a group such as competition for food and/or mates with other members of the group, higher chances of disease transmission, etc. Group size dynamics can change with the biotic and abiotic environment around individuals. In the current study, we explored the links between group size dynamics and intestinal parasites of Indian peafowl (<i>Pavo cristatus</i>) in the context of seasons and food provisioning. Data for group size was collected across three seasons (pre-monsoon, monsoon and post-monsoon) at three field sites (Morachi Chincholi, Nashik and Rajasthan). Individual and group sightings of peafowl were noted down along with group size and composition (no. of males, females, adults, juveniles and sub-adults). Faecal samples were collected from food provision and non-provision areas across the same three seasons at same field sites. Parasite load in the samples was quantified using microscopic examination. Group size was significantly higher in pre-monsoon season as compared with monsoon and post-monsoon seasons. Monsoon and post-monsoon seasons had higher intestinal parasite prevalence and load as compared with pre-monsoon season. Thus, group size and intestinal parasites of Indian peafowl have an inverse relationship across seasons. Parasite load was significantly greater at food provision sites as compared with non-provision sites while parasite prevalence was comparable. Aggregation of individuals at the food provision sites may influence the parasite transmission and group-size dynamics in Indian peafowl. In conclusion, Indian peafowl are behaviourally plastic and fission-fusion of social groups may allow them to tackle ecological pressures such as predation and parasite transmission in different seasons.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"24 2","pages":"107 - 117"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2021-03-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s10211-021-00366-x","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41891015","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Spatial segregation and interspecific killing of common dolphins (Delphinus delphis) by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) 宽吻海豚(Tursiops truncatus)对普通海豚(Delphinus delphis)的空间隔离和种间杀戮
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2021-03-12 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-021-00363-0
Séverine Methion, Bruno Díaz López

We described the spatial segregation of two species of cetaceans, the common dolphin and the bottlenose dolphin. We also document the first direct observation of interspecific killing of a common dolphin by bottlenose dolphins and of interspecific necrophilia in cetaceans. The study was conducted from 2014 to 2019 in the Ría de Arousa (Northwest Spain). This study highlights that both species use this area as a foraging ground, although they show different patterns of occurrence (bottlenose dolphins were always observed in the ria and common dolphins were mostly observed outside). During the study period, bottlenose dolphins and common dolphins were only observed on five occasions at the same time and in the same area, including three occasions which led to the displacement of the common dolphin(s), and one lethal interaction. In this event, several bottlenose dolphins, including adults and calves, and males and females, aggressively herded, chased, and assaulted a common dolphin. After approximatively 10 min, the common dolphin corpse appeared floating at the surface, and several adult male bottlenose dolphins repeatedly pushed the body underneath the water surface and an (attempted) copulation was witnessed. We suggest that the common dolphin could have been killed for competition for food resources or practice for infanticide, and sexual arousal might have been triggered by expression of dominance. Further information about the occurrence of such behaviors, and the outcomes through specific studies on fitness would be crucial to further understand the implication of such events.

我们描述了两种鲸目动物,普通海豚和宽吻海豚的空间隔离。我们还记录了第一次直接观察到宽吻海豚的种间杀戮和鲸目动物的种间恋尸癖。该研究于2014年至2019年在Ría de阿拉瓦(西班牙西北部)进行。这项研究强调,这两个物种都将这一地区作为觅食地,尽管它们表现出不同的出现模式(宽吻海豚总是在ria中观察到,而普通海豚大多在室外观察到)。在研究期间,宽吻海豚和普通海豚在同一时间和同一区域只观察到五次,其中包括三次导致普通海豚流离失所的情况,以及一次致命的相互作用。在这次事件中,几只宽吻海豚,包括成年海豚和幼海豚,雄性和雌性,积极地聚集在一起,追逐并袭击了一只普通的海豚。大约10分钟后,普通海豚的尸体漂浮在水面上,几只成年雄性宽吻海豚反复将尸体推到水面下,并目睹了(试图)交配。我们认为,普通的海豚可能是为了争夺食物资源或杀婴而被杀死的,而性唤起可能是由统治地位的表达引发的。进一步了解此类行为的发生,以及通过特定的适应度研究得出的结果,对于进一步理解此类事件的含义至关重要。
{"title":"Spatial segregation and interspecific killing of common dolphins (Delphinus delphis) by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)","authors":"Séverine Methion,&nbsp;Bruno Díaz López","doi":"10.1007/s10211-021-00363-0","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-021-00363-0","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We described the spatial segregation of two species of cetaceans, the common dolphin and the bottlenose dolphin. We also document the first direct observation of interspecific killing of a common dolphin by bottlenose dolphins and of interspecific necrophilia in cetaceans. The study was conducted from 2014 to 2019 in the Ría de Arousa (Northwest Spain). This study highlights that both species use this area as a foraging ground, although they show different patterns of occurrence (bottlenose dolphins were always observed in the ria and common dolphins were mostly observed outside). During the study period, bottlenose dolphins and common dolphins were only observed on five occasions at the same time and in the same area, including three occasions which led to the displacement of the common dolphin(s), and one lethal interaction. In this event, several bottlenose dolphins, including adults and calves, and males and females, aggressively herded, chased, and assaulted a common dolphin. After approximatively 10 min, the common dolphin corpse appeared floating at the surface, and several adult male bottlenose dolphins repeatedly pushed the body underneath the water surface and an (attempted) copulation was witnessed. We suggest that the common dolphin could have been killed for competition for food resources or practice for infanticide, and sexual arousal might have been triggered by expression of dominance. Further information about the occurrence of such behaviors, and the outcomes through specific studies on fitness would be crucial to further understand the implication of such events.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"24 2","pages":"95 - 106"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2021-03-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s10211-021-00363-0","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47513633","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Thanatological behavior of a female Leopard (Panthera pardus fusca) 雌性豹(Panthera pardus fusca)的死亡行为
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2021-03-11 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-021-00364-z
Reuven Yosef, Hemant Dabi, Swapnil Kumbhojkar

We report an observation at Jhalana Leopard Reserve (JLR), Jaipur, India. On 16 March 2019, we saw a female walking up the mountain while calling her two, 4-month-old, male and female cubs. This allowed several safari jeeps to park in the shade of nearby Acacia trees. Two Striped Hyenas (Hyaena hyaena) approached and sniffed at the base of one of the trees near the parked jeeps. Upon seeing the Hyaenas, the female ran down the mountain, passed between the jeeps, and climbed into the tree. That is when we noticed the body of the female cub at a height of approximately 4.5 m. The female licked the body of the cub for several minutes and then picked it up in her mouth, climbed down from the tree, passed again between the jeeps, and walked up the mountain towards a dense stand of Thor (Euphorbia caducifolia), a thorny cactus. Our observations display how a mother leopard that has lost a cub refuses to abandon the carcass, cached it in a tree, and when discovered by scavengers removed it to a thicket of cacti. We consider the behavior towards her dead cub, and subsequent caching when discovered by other animals, to be the first evidence for thanatological expression in leopards.

我们报告了在印度斋浦尔Jhalana豹保护区(JLR)的一次观察。2019年3月16日,我们看到一只母熊一边爬上山,一边叫着她两个月大、四个月大的雄性和雌性幼崽。这使得几辆狩猎吉普车可以停在附近金合欢树的树荫下。两只条纹鬣狗(Hyaena Hyaena)走近停在吉普车附近的一棵树的底部,嗅了嗅。看到鬣狗后,母狼跑下山,穿过吉普车,爬上了树。就在那时,我们注意到了大约4.5米高的雌性幼崽的尸体。母熊舔了几分钟幼崽的身体,然后把它叼在嘴里,从树上爬下来,再次穿过吉普车,向山上长满荆棘仙人掌的托尔(Euphorbia caducifolia)走去。我们的观察显示,失去幼崽的母豹拒绝抛弃幼崽的尸体,将幼崽藏在树上,被食腐动物发现后,将幼崽移到仙人掌树丛中。我们认为,豹子对待死去幼崽的行为,以及被其他动物发现后的贮藏行为,是豹子死亡学表达的第一个证据。
{"title":"Thanatological behavior of a female Leopard (Panthera pardus fusca)","authors":"Reuven Yosef,&nbsp;Hemant Dabi,&nbsp;Swapnil Kumbhojkar","doi":"10.1007/s10211-021-00364-z","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-021-00364-z","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We report an observation at Jhalana Leopard Reserve (JLR), Jaipur, India. On 16 March 2019, we saw a female walking up the mountain while calling her two, 4-month-old, male and female cubs. This allowed several safari jeeps to park in the shade of nearby Acacia trees. Two Striped Hyenas (<i>Hyaena hyaena</i>) approached and sniffed at the base of one of the trees near the parked jeeps. Upon seeing the Hyaenas, the female ran down the mountain, passed between the jeeps, and climbed into the tree. That is when we noticed the body of the female cub at a height of approximately 4.5 m. The female licked the body of the cub for several minutes and then picked it up in her mouth, climbed down from the tree, passed again between the jeeps, and walked up the mountain towards a dense stand of Thor (<i>Euphorbia caducifolia</i>), a thorny cactus. Our observations display how a mother leopard that has lost a cub refuses to abandon the carcass, cached it in a tree, and when discovered by scavengers removed it to a thicket of cacti. We consider the behavior towards her dead cub, and subsequent caching when discovered by other animals, to be the first evidence for thanatological expression in leopards.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"24 2","pages":"137 - 140"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2021-03-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s10211-021-00364-z","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44500706","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Does experimentally simulated presence of a common cuckoo (Cuculus canorus) affect egg rejection and breeding success in the red-backed shrike (Lanius collurio)? 实验模拟普通布谷鸟(Cuculus canorus)的存在是否会影响红背伯劳鸟(Lanius collurio)的卵子排斥和繁殖成功?
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2021-03-08 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-021-00362-1
Piotr Tryjanowski, Artur Golawski, Mariusz Janowski, Tim H. Sparks

Providing artificial eggs is a commonly used technique to understand brood parasitism, mainly by the common cuckoo (Cuculus canorus). However, the presence of a cuckoo egg in the host nest would also require an earlier physical presence of the common cuckoo within the host territory. During our study of the red-backed shrike (Lanius collurio), we tested two experimental approaches: (1) providing an artificial “cuckoo” egg in shrike nests and (2) additionally placing a stuffed common cuckoo with a male call close to the shrike nest. We expected that the shrikes subject to the additional common cuckoo call stimuli would be more sensitive to brood parasitism and demonstrate a higher egg rejection rate. In the years 2017–2018,  in two locations in Poland, a total of 130 red-backed shrike nests were divided into two categories: in 66 we added only an artificial egg, and in the remaining 64 we added not only the egg, but also presented a stuffed, calling common cuckoo. Shrikes reacted more strongly if the stuffed common cuckoo was present. However, only 13 incidences of egg acceptance were noted, with no significant differences between the locations, experimental treatments or their interaction. Analysis of breeding success revealed significant differences between the locations, between experimental treatments and their interaction, which suggests a strong location effect. The red-backed shrike is an efficient rejector of foreign eggs. It would be interesting to see how similar tests affect hosts that have much higher rates of brood parasitism and egg acceptance.

提供人工卵是一种常用的技术来了解幼虫寄生,主要是普通杜鹃(Cuculus canorus)。然而,在寄主巢中出现杜鹃蛋也需要在寄主领地内更早地出现普通杜鹃。在对红背伯劳鸟(Lanius collurio)的研究中,我们测试了两种实验方法:(1)在伯劳巢中提供人工“杜鹃”蛋;(2)在伯劳巢附近放置一个普通布谷鸟标本,并发出雄性叫声。结果表明,在布谷鸟叫声的刺激下,伯劳鸟对幼虫寄生的反应更敏感,排斥率更高。在2017-2018年,在波兰的两个地点,共有130个红背伯劳巢被分为两类:在66个我们只添加了一个人造蛋,在剩下的64个我们不仅添加了蛋,还提供了一个填充的,叫普通杜鹃。如果布谷鸟标本在场,伯劳鸟的反应会更强烈。然而,只有13个卵子被接受的发生率被注意到,在地点、实验处理或它们的相互作用之间没有显著差异。育种成功率分析显示,不同地点、不同处理间及其相互作用之间存在显著差异,表明存在较强的位置效应。红背伯劳鸟能有效地排斥外来的蛋。看看类似的测试如何影响那些有更高的幼虫寄生率和卵子接受率的宿主,这将是一件有趣的事情。
{"title":"Does experimentally simulated presence of a common cuckoo (Cuculus canorus) affect egg rejection and breeding success in the red-backed shrike (Lanius collurio)?","authors":"Piotr Tryjanowski,&nbsp;Artur Golawski,&nbsp;Mariusz Janowski,&nbsp;Tim H. Sparks","doi":"10.1007/s10211-021-00362-1","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-021-00362-1","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Providing artificial eggs is a commonly used technique to understand brood parasitism, mainly by the common cuckoo (<i>Cuculus canorus</i>). However, the presence of a cuckoo egg in the host nest would also require an earlier physical presence of the common cuckoo within the host territory. During our study of the red-backed shrike (<i>Lanius collurio</i>), we tested two experimental approaches: (1) providing an artificial “cuckoo” egg in shrike nests and (2) additionally placing a stuffed common cuckoo with a male call close to the shrike nest. We expected that the shrikes subject to the additional common cuckoo call stimuli would be more sensitive to brood parasitism and demonstrate a higher egg rejection rate. In the years 2017–2018,  in two locations in Poland, a total of 130 red-backed shrike nests were divided into two categories: in 66 we added only an artificial egg, and in the remaining 64 we added not only the egg, but also presented a stuffed, calling common cuckoo. Shrikes reacted more strongly if the stuffed common cuckoo was present. However, only 13 incidences of egg acceptance were noted, with no significant differences between the locations, experimental treatments or their interaction. Analysis of breeding success revealed significant differences between the locations, between experimental treatments and their interaction, which suggests a strong location effect. The red-backed shrike is an efficient rejector of foreign eggs. It would be interesting to see how similar tests affect hosts that have much higher rates of brood parasitism and egg acceptance.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"24 2","pages":"87 - 94"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2021-03-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s10211-021-00362-1","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42800727","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Separating effects of spatial location and microhabitat density on perceived predation risk in small mammals 空间位置和微栖息地密度对小型哺乳动物感知捕食风险的分离影响
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2021-03-05 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-021-00365-y
K. N. Denny, K. N. Bilodeau, C. A. Dumont, Z. H. Olson

Prey animals must balance the benefits of foraging in a particular area with their risk of predation in that area and often prioritize indirect cues to predation risk over direct cues of predators when making decisions about foraging under the risk of predation. Researchers using giving-up density (GUD) as a metric to assess perceived predation risk have found that it is affected by microhabitat, with certain animals willing to spend more time foraging (i.e., lower GUD) in relatively dense microhabitats and less time foraging (i.e., higher GUD) in relatively open microhabitats. This phenomenon has been attributed to those animals perceiving less predation risk when near or under shelter. However, the measurement has often confounded microhabitat density with distance from shelter in species without conspicuous dens. We measured GUD in foraging small mammals while experimentally manipulating microhabitat density and controlling for spatial location of the forager. Small mammals increased foraging (i.e., decreased GUD) as expected when we increased microhabitat density, but they did so despite those manipulations being randomly permuted spatially over the course of four rounds of data collection. Our results indicate that experimentally controlling for the potentially confounding effect of the forager’s distance to a safe location, such as dens, nests, or other shelters, did not remove the effect of microhabitat density on perceived predation risk. Thus, our results suggest that an animal’s perception of risk is likely mediated by both their location in their home range and the microhabitat at that location. We provide suggestions for further work.

被捕食的动物必须平衡在特定地区觅食的好处和在该地区被捕食的风险,在决定是否在被捕食的风险下觅食时,往往优先考虑捕食风险的间接线索而不是捕食者的直接线索。研究人员使用放弃密度(GUD)作为评估感知捕食风险的指标,发现它受到微生境的影响,某些动物愿意在相对密集的微生境中花费更多的时间觅食(即较低的GUD),而在相对开放的微生境中花费更少的时间觅食(即较高的GUD)。这一现象归因于那些动物在靠近或躲在避难所时感受到的被捕食风险较小。然而,在没有明显洞穴的物种中,测量经常混淆微栖息地密度和距离庇护所的距离。通过实验控制微生境密度和觅食者的空间位置,测量了觅食小型哺乳动物的GUD。当我们增加微生境密度时,小型哺乳动物的觅食量增加(即GUD降低),这与我们预期的一样,但在四轮数据收集过程中,这些操作在空间上是随机排列的。我们的研究结果表明,通过实验控制觅食者到安全地点(如洞穴、巢穴或其他庇护所)的距离的潜在混淆效应,并不能消除微生境密度对感知捕食风险的影响。因此,我们的研究结果表明,动物对风险的感知可能是由它们在其家园范围内的位置和该位置的微栖息地介导的。并对今后的工作提出建议。
{"title":"Separating effects of spatial location and microhabitat density on perceived predation risk in small mammals","authors":"K. N. Denny,&nbsp;K. N. Bilodeau,&nbsp;C. A. Dumont,&nbsp;Z. H. Olson","doi":"10.1007/s10211-021-00365-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-021-00365-y","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Prey animals must balance the benefits of foraging in a particular area with their risk of predation in that area and often prioritize indirect cues to predation risk over direct cues of predators when making decisions about foraging under the risk of predation. Researchers using giving-up density (GUD) as a metric to assess perceived predation risk have found that it is affected by microhabitat, with certain animals willing to spend more time foraging (i.e., lower GUD) in relatively dense microhabitats and less time foraging (i.e., higher GUD) in relatively open microhabitats. This phenomenon has been attributed to those animals perceiving less predation risk when near or under shelter. However, the measurement has often confounded microhabitat density with distance from shelter in species without conspicuous dens. We measured GUD in foraging small mammals while experimentally manipulating microhabitat density and controlling for spatial location of the forager. Small mammals increased foraging (i.e., decreased GUD) as expected when we increased microhabitat density, but they did so despite those manipulations being randomly permuted spatially over the course of four rounds of data collection. Our results indicate that experimentally controlling for the potentially confounding effect of the forager’s distance to a safe location, such as dens, nests, or other shelters, did not remove the effect of microhabitat density on perceived predation risk. Thus, our results suggest that an animal’s perception of risk is likely mediated by both their location in their home range and the microhabitat at that location. We provide suggestions for further work.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"24 2","pages":"79 - 85"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2021-03-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s10211-021-00365-y","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49654001","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Orientation of Belminus triatomines to cockroaches and cockroaches’ fecal volatiles: an ethological approach 三角蝽蝽对蟑螂和蟑螂粪便挥发物的取向:一种行为学方法
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2021-01-14 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-021-00361-2
Fernando Otálora-Luna, Oscar Páez-Rondón, Elis Aldana, Claudia Magaly Sandoval Ramírez

Most triatomine bugs (Hemiptera: Reduviidae: Triatominae) are hematophagous, though Belminus species can live off of cockroach hemolymph to complete their life cycle. In this work, we described the fixed action pattern (FAP) employed by B. ferroae to identify, approach, and suck on a living cockroach. The FAP described here is composed of the following stereotyped behaviors: (1) visual and/or olfactory detection of the cockroach, (2) reaching, (3) cautious approach, (4) antennal exploration, (5) extension of the proboscis, (6) piercing to sedate, (7) walking away and waiting (postsedation behavior), (8) second cautious approach, (9) extension of the proboscis, (10) piercing to suck hemolymph. The FAP sequence observed suggests that Belminus bugs are not predators like the rest of reduviids (assassin bugs)—but are kleptophagous ectoparasites, since they do not attack and kill a prey but rather steal hemolymph from its invertebrate host. Based on these ethological observations, we propose kleptophagy as a trait that naturally groups the Belminus species into the Triatominae subfamily. In order to identify chemicals cues that could elicit such FAP, we examined the behavior of B. corredori, B. ferroae, and B. herreri in response to the cockroaches’ odor, fresh cockroach feces and fresh rodent wastes. The last two sources were tested based on the assumption that abundant chemicals near host refuges could serve as cues for host orientation. We found that the cockroach odor emanating from a box significantly attracted B. corredori and B. herreri in a still air olfactometer. The three Belminus species approached the captive cockroach after 1 h, but avoided to climb the box. Odors emanating from the cockroach feces attracted B. corredori and B. ferroae in a Y-olfactometer. Triatomines and their hosts have intimately shared the same refuge for millions of years; certain molecules occur across invertebrate and vertebrate refuges and are recurrent in human abodes, thus plausibly explaining how these bugs can readily switch to the domestic habitat, while keeping with their kleptophagous nature.

大多数三角蝽(半翅目:红蝇科:三角蝽科)是食血的,尽管Belminus的种类可以依靠蟑螂的血淋巴来完成它们的生命周期。在这项工作中,我们描述了铁芽孢杆菌识别、接近和吮吸活蟑螂的固定动作模式(FAP)。这里描述的FAP由以下刻板行为组成:(1)视觉和/或嗅觉检测蟑螂,(2)到达,(3)谨慎接近,(4)触角探索,(5)伸出喙,(6)刺穿镇静,(7)走开并等待(镇静后行为),(8)第二次谨慎接近,(9)伸出喙,(10)刺穿吸血淋巴。观察到的FAP序列表明,Belminus虫不像其他的reduviids(刺客虫)那样是掠食性外寄生虫,因为它们不攻击和杀死猎物,而是从它的无脊椎宿主那里窃取血淋巴。基于这些行为学观察,我们提出盗食是一种自然将Belminus物种归为Triatominae亚科的特征。为了确定可能引发这种FAP的化学线索,我们研究了B. corredori、B. ferroae和B. herreri对蟑螂气味、新鲜蟑螂粪便和新鲜啮齿动物粪便的反应。最后两种来源的测试是基于这样一种假设,即宿主避难所附近的大量化学物质可以作为宿主定向的线索。在静空气嗅探仪中,我们发现盒子散发出的蟑螂气味对小蠊和小蠊有明显的吸引作用。1 h后,三种Belminus接近捕获的蟑螂,但避免爬上盒子。用y型嗅觉仪检测蟑螂粪便散发的气味,可吸引corredi芽孢杆菌和铁芽孢杆菌。千百年来,三角蝽和它们的宿主亲密地生活在同一个避难所;某些分子出现在无脊椎动物和脊椎动物的避难所,并在人类的住所中反复出现,因此合理地解释了这些虫子如何能够在保持其偷窃性的同时轻易地转向家养栖息地。
{"title":"Orientation of Belminus triatomines to cockroaches and cockroaches’ fecal volatiles: an ethological approach","authors":"Fernando Otálora-Luna,&nbsp;Oscar Páez-Rondón,&nbsp;Elis Aldana,&nbsp;Claudia Magaly Sandoval Ramírez","doi":"10.1007/s10211-021-00361-2","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-021-00361-2","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Most triatomine bugs (Hemiptera: Reduviidae: Triatominae) are hematophagous, though <i>Belminus</i> species can live off of cockroach hemolymph to complete their life cycle. In this work, we described the fixed action pattern (FAP) employed by <i>B. ferroae</i> to identify, approach, and suck on a living cockroach. The FAP described here is composed of the following stereotyped behaviors: (1) visual and/or olfactory detection of the cockroach, (2) reaching, (3) cautious approach, (4) antennal exploration, (5) extension of the proboscis, (6) piercing to sedate, (7) walking away and waiting (postsedation behavior), (8) second cautious approach, (9) extension of the proboscis, (10) piercing to suck hemolymph. The FAP sequence observed suggests that <i>Belminus</i> bugs are not predators like the rest of reduviids (assassin bugs)—but are kleptophagous ectoparasites, since they do not attack and kill a prey but rather steal hemolymph from its invertebrate host. Based on these ethological observations, we propose kleptophagy as a trait that naturally groups the <i>Belminus</i> species into the Triatominae subfamily. In order to identify chemicals cues that could elicit such FAP, we examined the behavior of <i>B. corredori</i>, <i>B. ferroae</i>, and <i>B. herreri</i> in response to the cockroaches’ odor, fresh cockroach feces and fresh rodent wastes. The last two sources were tested based on the assumption that abundant chemicals near host refuges could serve as cues for host orientation. We found that the cockroach odor emanating from a box significantly attracted <i>B. corredori</i> and <i>B. herreri</i> in a still air olfactometer. The three <i>Belminus</i> species approached the captive cockroach after 1 h, but avoided to climb the box. Odors emanating from the cockroach feces attracted <i>B. corredori</i> and <i>B. ferroae</i> in a Y-olfactometer. Triatomines and their hosts have intimately shared the same refuge for millions of years; certain molecules occur across invertebrate and vertebrate refuges and are recurrent in human abodes, thus plausibly explaining how these bugs can readily switch to the domestic habitat, while keeping with their kleptophagous nature.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"24 1","pages":"53 - 66"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2021-01-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s10211-021-00361-2","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43829020","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reproductive behavior of Spix’s Whiptails in the wild: understanding the costs and benefits of mate-guarding 斯皮克斯鞭尾在野外的繁殖行为:了解配偶保护的成本和收益
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2021-01-12 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-020-00360-9
Raul Fernandes Dantas Sales, Eliza Maria Xavier Freire

Across animal taxa, females of non-territorial species have potential opportunities to mate with multiple partners; hence, the primary mechanism available for males to ensure paternity is to guard the receptive female after copulation and repel other males. Hypothetically, mate-guarding is costly for males in terms of energy acquisition and increased risk of injury, and beneficial for females in terms of decreased harassment by other males and an increase in available foraging time. Here, we provide a detailed description of mating behavior and test these hypotheses in the Spix’s Whiptail (Ameivula ocellifera). Mating behavior is characterized by the following events: (1) a male courts a female in the entrance of her burrow; (2) if courtship is accepted by the female, a consensual copulation occurs; (3) after copulation the male accompanies the female during her daily activity, being aggressive towards other males; (4) when the female returns to the burrow, the companion male remains vigilant at the entrance and repels rival males. Besides the mate-guarding strategy, alternative mating tactics are adopted by some males that do not guard females after courtship and consensual copulation, while others try to copulate opportunistically with a female without prior courtship. Companion males spent more time vigilant, less time actively foraging, and captured less prey when compared to solitary males. Accompanied females captured prey in a similar proportion to solitary females but spent more time vigilant and less time foraging. Companion males won 100% of their interactions with rival males, chasing them away from the females. Accompanied females hence did not suffer harassment from other males when companion males were close. Our results evidence energetic costs of mate-guarding for males but not increased risk of injuries. By accepting mate-guarding, females do not appear to have energetic gains and lose the advantage of cryptic mate choice but can benefit from access to high-quality males and protection from harassment.

在动物分类群中,非领地物种的雌性具有与多个伴侣交配的潜在机会;因此,雄性确保父权的主要机制是在交配后保护接受交配的雌性,并排斥其他雄性。假设,从获取能量和增加受伤风险的角度来看,雄性保护配偶是昂贵的,而从减少其他雄性骚扰和增加可用觅食时间的角度来看,雌性保护配偶是有益的。在这里,我们提供了交配行为的详细描述,并在Spix的鞭尾(Ameivula ocelllifera)中验证了这些假设。交配行为主要表现为:(1)雄性向雌性在地洞入口处求爱;(2)求偶被雌鸟接受的,发生双方自愿的交配;(3)交配后,雄性伴雌性日常活动,对其他雄性具有攻击性;(4)当雌性返回洞穴时,雄性同伴在洞口保持警惕,并击退敌对的雄性。除了保护配偶的策略外,一些雄性在求爱和双方同意的交配后不保护雌性,而另一些雄性则试图在没有求爱的情况下与雌性进行机会性交配。与孤独的雄性相比,伴侣雄性花更多的时间保持警惕,更少的时间主动觅食,捕获的猎物也更少。有同伴的雌性与单独的雌性捕获猎物的比例相似,但它们花更多的时间保持警惕,花更少的时间觅食。雄性伴侣在与竞争对手的互动中赢得了100%的胜利,把它们从雌性身边赶走。因此,当雄性伴侣靠近时,被陪伴的雌性不会受到其他雄性的骚扰。我们的研究结果证明了雄性保护配偶的能量消耗,但没有增加受伤的风险。通过接受配偶保护,雌性似乎不会获得精力上的收益,也不会失去选择配偶的优势,而是可以从获得高质量的雄性和免受骚扰的保护中获益。
{"title":"Reproductive behavior of Spix’s Whiptails in the wild: understanding the costs and benefits of mate-guarding","authors":"Raul Fernandes Dantas Sales,&nbsp;Eliza Maria Xavier Freire","doi":"10.1007/s10211-020-00360-9","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-020-00360-9","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Across animal taxa, females of non-territorial species have potential opportunities to mate with multiple partners; hence, the primary mechanism available for males to ensure paternity is to guard the receptive female after copulation and repel other males. Hypothetically, mate-guarding is costly for males in terms of energy acquisition and increased risk of injury, and beneficial for females in terms of decreased harassment by other males and an increase in available foraging time. Here, we provide a detailed description of mating behavior and test these hypotheses in the Spix’s Whiptail (<i>Ameivula ocellifera</i>). Mating behavior is characterized by the following events: (1) a male courts a female in the entrance of her burrow; (2) if courtship is accepted by the female, a consensual copulation occurs; (3) after copulation the male accompanies the female during her daily activity, being aggressive towards other males; (4) when the female returns to the burrow, the companion male remains vigilant at the entrance and repels rival males. Besides the mate-guarding strategy, alternative mating tactics are adopted by some males that do not guard females after courtship and consensual copulation, while others try to copulate opportunistically with a female without prior courtship. Companion males spent more time vigilant, less time actively foraging, and captured less prey when compared to solitary males. Accompanied females captured prey in a similar proportion to solitary females but spent more time vigilant and less time foraging. Companion males won 100% of their interactions with rival males, chasing them away from the females. Accompanied females hence did not suffer harassment from other males when companion males were close. Our results evidence energetic costs of mate-guarding for males but not increased risk of injuries. By accepting mate-guarding, females do not appear to have energetic gains and lose the advantage of cryptic mate choice but can benefit from access to high-quality males and protection from harassment.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"24 1","pages":"41 - 51"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2021-01-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s10211-020-00360-9","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50020997","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
acta ethologica
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1