首页 > 最新文献

acta ethologica最新文献

英文 中文
Heterospecific eavesdropping of jays (Garrulus glandarius) on blackbird (Turdus merula) mobbing calls 松鸦(Garrus glandarius)对黑鸟(Turdus merula)骚扰叫声的异源窃听
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2022-03-18 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-022-00391-4
Christoph Randler

Heterospecifics eavesdrop on mobbing calls and respond with appropriate behavior, but the functional aspects are less studied. Here, I studied whether jays (Garrulus glandarius) eavesdrop on blackbird (Turdus merula) mobbing calls in comparison to blackbird song. Furthermore, it was studied whether jays provided with extra information about predators differ in their response. Three different experimental designs were carried out: (1) control playback of blackbird song to control for the species’ presence, (2) experimental playback of different mobbing events of blackbirds towards different predators, (3) experimental playback similar to (2) but combined with different predator models. In the combined experiments, mobbing calls were tied to the respective visual stimuli. Comparing the experiments with and without predator presentation, a similar number of jays occurred during the playback-only experiment (n = 7) and the playback combined with model presentation (n = 6). However, during the playback-only experiment, jays approached the speaker closer and stayed for longer time in the nearer surrounding. These results show that jays need extra information to make an informed decision.

异种生物对蜂鸣声进行窃听,并做出相应的反应,但其功能方面的研究较少。在这里,我研究了松鸦(Garrulus glandarius)是否会偷听黑鸟(Turdus merula)的叫声,而不是黑鸟的歌声。此外,我们还研究了松鸦在获得捕食者的额外信息后,其反应是否会有所不同。采用三种不同的实验设计:(1)控制黑鹂鸣叫的播放以控制该物种的存在;(2)黑鹂对不同捕食者的不同围捕事件的实验播放;(3)与(2)相似但结合不同捕食者模型的实验播放。在联合实验中,骚扰呼叫与各自的视觉刺激联系在一起。在有捕食者呈现和没有捕食者呈现的实验中,只播放实验(n = 7)和播放与模型呈现相结合的实验(n = 6)中出现的松鸦数量相似。然而,在只播放的实验中,松鸦更靠近说话者,在更近的环境中停留的时间更长。这些结果表明,松鸦需要额外的信息来做出明智的决定。
{"title":"Heterospecific eavesdropping of jays (Garrulus glandarius) on blackbird (Turdus merula) mobbing calls","authors":"Christoph Randler","doi":"10.1007/s10211-022-00391-4","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-022-00391-4","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Heterospecifics eavesdrop on mobbing calls and respond with appropriate behavior, but the functional aspects are less studied. Here, I studied whether jays (<i>Garrulus glandarius</i>) eavesdrop on blackbird (<i>Turdus merula</i>) mobbing calls in comparison to blackbird song. Furthermore, it was studied whether jays provided with extra information about predators differ in their response. Three different experimental designs were carried out: (1) control playback of blackbird song to control for the species’ presence, (2) experimental playback of different mobbing events of blackbirds towards different predators, (3) experimental playback similar to (2) but combined with different predator models. In the combined experiments, mobbing calls were tied to the respective visual stimuli. Comparing the experiments with and without predator presentation, a similar number of jays occurred during the playback-only experiment (<i>n</i> = 7) and the playback combined with model presentation (<i>n</i> = 6). However, during the playback-only experiment, jays approached the speaker closer and stayed for longer time in the nearer surrounding. These results show that jays need extra information to make an informed decision.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"25 2","pages":"101 - 106"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-03-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10211-022-00391-4.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47069480","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The underground system of Clyomys laticeps changes in structure and composition according to climatic and vegetation variations 宽脊灰蝶地下系统的结构和组成随气候和植被的变化而变化
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2022-03-17 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-022-00389-y
L. C. Luchesi, B. C. Paula, P. F. Monticelli, G. Francescoli

Ecological factors may affect resource availability and distribution, impacting foraging and burrow construction behaviours. Clyomys laticeps is a caviomorph rodent with subterranean habits occurring on the Brazilian Cerrado domain (savanna-like) until the Paraguayan Chaco. We investigated their underground system’s architecture taking into account the vegetation and climate. We hypothesised that the sparse food distribution in the winter would promote longer tunnels and more complex architectures to connect more distant foraging areas, supposing that the species moved underground to avoid predators; moreover, the winter would promote food storage. We excavated eleven Clyomys underground systems and measured their size and internal parameters (tunnel and chamber width, length and depth) and complexity (linearity and convolution). We noticed that half of the systems were in the open landscape (OL) and half on vegetation covered (VL). If the anti-predation theory was right, we would find shorter tunnels on the VL systems. We found systems from 2 to 24 m2 and up to 22 m long. The deeper and biomass scarcer tunnels were on VL during the dry season, supposedly when animals would need underground water but not stocked food (the palm season). Also, they were more complex (higher circularity and convolution indexes) in OL, favouring our anti-predation hypothesis. Furthermore, Clyomys burrows offer refuge for other species such as arthropods, snakes, amphibians, and birds. We conclude that systems’ architecture is related to vegetation presence and seasonal foraging challenges. This rodent may construct its systems for shelter, food storage and as a safe trail among foraging areas.

生态因素可能影响资源的可用性和分布,影响觅食和挖洞行为。Clyomys laticeps是一种洞穴形啮齿动物,在巴西塞拉多地区(类似稀树草原)发生,直到巴拉圭查科。考虑到植被和气候,我们调查了他们地下系统的建筑。我们假设,冬季稀疏的食物分布会促进更长的隧道和更复杂的建筑,以连接更远的觅食区域,假设物种迁移到地下以躲避捕食者;此外,冬天会促进食物的储存。我们挖掘了11个Clyomys地下系统,并测量了它们的大小和内部参数(隧道和腔室宽度、长度和深度)以及复杂性(线性和卷积)。我们注意到一半的系统位于开放景观(OL)中,一半位于植被覆盖(VL)中。如果反捕食理论是正确的,我们会在VL系统上找到更短的隧道。我们发现系统从2到24平方米,最长可达22米。在干旱季节(棕榈季节),动物可能需要地下水,但没有储存食物,而更深、生物量更少的隧道则处于VL状态。此外,它们在OL中更复杂(更高的圆度和卷积指数),有利于我们的反捕食假设。此外,Clyomys的洞穴为节肢动物、蛇、两栖动物和鸟类等其他物种提供了避难所。我们得出结论,系统的结构与植被存在和季节性觅食挑战有关。这种啮齿动物可能会建立自己的系统,作为庇护所,食物储存和觅食区域之间的安全路线。
{"title":"The underground system of Clyomys laticeps changes in structure and composition according to climatic and vegetation variations","authors":"L. C. Luchesi,&nbsp;B. C. Paula,&nbsp;P. F. Monticelli,&nbsp;G. Francescoli","doi":"10.1007/s10211-022-00389-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-022-00389-y","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Ecological factors may affect resource availability and distribution, impacting foraging and burrow construction behaviours. <i>Clyomys laticeps</i> is a caviomorph rodent with subterranean habits occurring on the Brazilian Cerrado domain (savanna-like) until the Paraguayan Chaco. We investigated their underground system’s architecture taking into account the vegetation and climate. We hypothesised that the sparse food distribution in the winter would promote longer tunnels and more complex architectures to connect more distant foraging areas, supposing that the species moved underground to avoid predators; moreover, the winter would promote food storage. We excavated eleven <i>Clyomys</i> underground systems and measured their size and internal parameters (tunnel and chamber width, length and depth) and complexity (linearity and convolution). We noticed that half of the systems were in the open landscape (OL) and half on vegetation covered (VL). If the anti-predation theory was right, we would find shorter tunnels on the VL systems. We found systems from 2 to 24 m<sup>2</sup> and up to 22 m long. The deeper and biomass scarcer tunnels were on VL during the dry season, supposedly when animals would need underground water but not stocked food (the palm season). Also, they were more complex (higher circularity and convolution indexes) in OL, favouring our anti-predation hypothesis. Furthermore, <i>Clyomys</i> burrows offer refuge for other species such as arthropods, snakes, amphibians, and birds. We conclude that systems’ architecture is related to vegetation presence and seasonal foraging challenges. This rodent may construct its systems for shelter, food storage and as a safe trail among foraging areas.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"25 2","pages":"89 - 100"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-03-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49338299","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Pack members shape the acoustic structure of a wolf chorus 狼群成员塑造了狼合唱的声学结构
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2022-01-28 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-021-00388-5
Barbara Marti-Domken, Vicente Palacios Sanchez, Aurora Monzón

Wolf packs perform group vocalizations called chorus howls. These acoustic signals have a complex structure and could be involved in functions such as strengthening of social bonds, territory advertisement, or spacing between packs. We analyzed video recordings of 46 chorus howls emitted by 10 packs of wolves held in captivity, in order to investigate whether sex, age, social status, pack, or individual influence the way wolves participate in a chorus. We found that, during a chorus, wolves vocalized 63% of the time, with the howl being the most common vocalization (36% of the chorus duration), followed by woa (13.5%), other vocalizations (11.8%), and bark (1.7%). The main factor affecting the vocal behavior of wolves was age, since young wolves vocalized less and uttered shorter acoustic signals than adults. The discriminant analysis carried out with the wolves of Cañada Real pack assigned 89.3% of the cases to the correct individual, which is much better than the assignment expected by chance, suggesting that individuals could have a unique vocal usage during a chorus howl, mainly due to the use of howls and woa-woa howls. Based on our results, we propose that in the context of a chorus the woa-woa howl is important, although further research is needed to address this issue properly.

狼群会发出一种集体的叫声,叫做“狼嚎”。这些声音信号具有复杂的结构,可能涉及诸如加强社会纽带,领土广告或狼群之间的间隔等功能。我们分析了10群圈养狼发出的46声合唱嚎叫的视频记录,以调查性别、年龄、社会地位、狼群或个体是否会影响狼参与合唱的方式。我们发现,在合唱中,狼有63%的时间发出声音,其中嚎叫是最常见的声音(占合唱时间的36%),其次是woa(13.5%),其他声音(11.8%)和吠叫(1.7%)。影响狼发声行为的主要因素是年龄,因为年轻的狼比成年狼发声更少,发出的声音信号也更短。对Cañada Real狼群的狼进行的判别分析将89.3%的案例分配给了正确的个体,这比随机分配的预期要好得多,这表明个体在合唱嚎叫时可能有独特的声音使用,主要是由于嚎叫和哇哇嚎叫的使用。基于我们的研究结果,我们提出,在合唱的背景下,哇哇的嚎叫是重要的,尽管需要进一步的研究来适当地解决这个问题。
{"title":"Pack members shape the acoustic structure of a wolf chorus","authors":"Barbara Marti-Domken,&nbsp;Vicente Palacios Sanchez,&nbsp;Aurora Monzón","doi":"10.1007/s10211-021-00388-5","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-021-00388-5","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Wolf packs perform group vocalizations called chorus howls. These acoustic signals have a complex structure and could be involved in functions such as strengthening of social bonds, territory advertisement, or spacing between packs. We analyzed video recordings of 46 chorus howls emitted by 10 packs of wolves held in captivity, in order to investigate whether sex, age, social status, pack, or individual influence the way wolves participate in a chorus. We found that, during a chorus, wolves vocalized 63% of the time, with the howl being the most common vocalization (36% of the chorus duration), followed by woa (13.5%), other vocalizations (11.8%), and bark (1.7%). The main factor affecting the vocal behavior of wolves was age, since young wolves vocalized less and uttered shorter acoustic signals than adults. The discriminant analysis carried out with the wolves of Cañada Real pack assigned 89.3% of the cases to the correct individual, which is much better than the assignment expected by chance, suggesting that individuals could have a unique vocal usage during a chorus howl, mainly due to the use of howls and woa-woa howls. Based on our results, we propose that in the context of a chorus the woa-woa howl is important, although further research is needed to address this issue properly.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"25 2","pages":"79 - 87"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-01-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10211-021-00388-5.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48176508","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Correction to: Invasive Italian wall lizards outcompete native congeneric species in finding food in a Y‑maze 修正:入侵的意大利壁虎在Y型迷宫中寻找食物时胜过本土同类物种
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2021-11-03 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-021-00387-6
Anastasios Limnios, Chloe Adamopoulou, Miguel A. Carretero, Panayiotis Pafilis
{"title":"Correction to: Invasive Italian wall lizards outcompete native congeneric species in finding food in a Y‑maze","authors":"Anastasios Limnios,&nbsp;Chloe Adamopoulou,&nbsp;Miguel A. Carretero,&nbsp;Panayiotis Pafilis","doi":"10.1007/s10211-021-00387-6","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-021-00387-6","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"25 1","pages":"57 - 59"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2021-11-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45837506","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Hide and flirt: observed behavior of female jaguars (Panthera onca) to protect their young cubs from adult males 隐藏和调情:观察到雌性美洲虎(Panthera onca)保护幼崽不受成年雄性伤害的行为
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2021-10-22 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-021-00384-9
Diana C. Stasiukynas, Valeria Boron, Rafael Hoogesteijn, Jorge Barragán, Abigail Martin, Fernando Tortato, Samantha Rincón, Esteban Payán

Common across various taxa, infanticide is a highly variable phenomenon present from insects to birds to mammals. In felids, antagonistic sexual coevolution led to the development of female counterstrategies to infanticide spanning particular sexual behavior, physiology, and social strategies. Numerous protective behaviors are well documented for large felids such as lions, cheetahs, and pumas that rely on cooperative defenses and polyandrous mating to protect their cubs from infanticide. Nevertheless, little is known about other wildcat species adopting such behaviors. Solitary and enigmatic, jaguars (Panthera onca) are the largest cat existing in the Americas. Little is known about this big cats’ reproductive and rearing behavior, mainly due to its secretive nature. Here, field observations in two major wetland ecosystems of South America show new and unique findings on female jaguar counterstrategies towards male infanticide. Our findings suggest that, like their big cat relatives in Africa, jaguars have evolved behavioral counterstrategies to protect their young in response to antagonistic sexual coevolution.

在不同的分类群中,杀婴是一种高度可变的现象,从昆虫到鸟类再到哺乳动物。在猫科动物中,对抗性的性协同进化导致了雌性在特定的性行为、生理和社会策略方面对杀婴行为的对抗策略的发展。大型猫科动物,如狮子、猎豹和美洲狮,依靠合作防御和一夫多妻制交配来保护它们的幼崽免受杀婴行为,有许多保护行为被充分记录下来。然而,人们对其他野猫物种是否会有这种行为知之甚少。孤独而神秘的美洲虎(Panthera onca)是现存于美洲最大的猫科动物。人们对这种大型猫科动物的繁殖和养育行为知之甚少,主要是由于它的秘密性质。本文通过对南美两个主要湿地生态系统的实地观察,发现了雌性美洲虎对雄性杀婴行为的对策。我们的研究结果表明,就像它们在非洲的大型猫科动物亲戚一样,美洲虎已经进化出了行为对抗策略来保护它们的幼崽,以应对对抗性的性共同进化。
{"title":"Hide and flirt: observed behavior of female jaguars (Panthera onca) to protect their young cubs from adult males","authors":"Diana C. Stasiukynas,&nbsp;Valeria Boron,&nbsp;Rafael Hoogesteijn,&nbsp;Jorge Barragán,&nbsp;Abigail Martin,&nbsp;Fernando Tortato,&nbsp;Samantha Rincón,&nbsp;Esteban Payán","doi":"10.1007/s10211-021-00384-9","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-021-00384-9","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Common across various taxa, infanticide is a highly variable phenomenon present from insects to birds to mammals. In felids, antagonistic sexual coevolution led to the development of female counterstrategies to infanticide spanning particular sexual behavior, physiology, and social strategies. Numerous protective behaviors are well documented for large felids such as lions, cheetahs, and pumas that rely on cooperative defenses and polyandrous mating to protect their cubs from infanticide. Nevertheless, little is known about other wildcat species adopting such behaviors. Solitary and enigmatic, jaguars (<i>Panthera onca</i>) are the largest cat existing in the Americas. Little is known about this big cats’ reproductive and rearing behavior, mainly due to its secretive nature. Here, field observations in two major wetland ecosystems of South America show new and unique findings on female jaguar counterstrategies towards male infanticide. Our findings suggest that, like their big cat relatives in Africa, jaguars have evolved behavioral counterstrategies to protect their young in response to antagonistic sexual coevolution.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"25 3","pages":"179 - 183"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2021-10-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10211-021-00384-9.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42973426","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Invasive Italian wall lizards outcompete native congeneric species in finding food in a Y-maze 入侵的意大利壁虎在y形迷宫中寻找食物时胜过本土同类物种
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2021-10-06 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-021-00385-8
Anastasios Limnios, C. Adamopoulou, M. Carretero, P. Pafilis
{"title":"Invasive Italian wall lizards outcompete native congeneric species in finding food in a Y-maze","authors":"Anastasios Limnios, C. Adamopoulou, M. Carretero, P. Pafilis","doi":"10.1007/s10211-021-00385-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10211-021-00385-8","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"25 1","pages":"43 - 55"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2021-10-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"51958929","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Invasive Italian wall lizards outcompete native congeneric species in finding food in a Y-maze 入侵的意大利壁虎在y形迷宫中寻找食物时胜过本土同类物种
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2021-10-06 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-021-00385-8
Anastasios Limnios, Chloe Adamopoulou, Miguel A. Carretero, Panayiotis Pafilis

Though biological invasions constitute one of the biggest threats for global biodiversity, our understanding of the mechanisms that enable invasive species to outperform native species is still limited, especially, in terms of behavior. Most available studies have examined behavioral traits which favor invasive species on the later stages of invasion, however, our knowledge on earlier stages, namely, when alien species face novel environments and must exploit new resources, remains obscure. Here, we focus on one crucial behavioral trait, finding food. The Italian wall lizard (Podarcis siculus) has been widely introduced and established viable populations in S. Europe and N. America. We examined whether P. siculus has enhanced exploratory behavior and abilities to find food compared to two native congeneric species with which it may come in contact in the near future, an insular endemic (P. milensis) and a widely distributed lizard (P. erhardii). We performed a Y-maze experiment, in which we varied arm markings in a standard way to prevent learning. Podarcis siculus was more efficient than its congenerics in finding and consuming food. This exploitative superiority was persistent, more frequent and repetitive. Interesting behavioral differences were also detected within the native species. Some P. milensis individuals showed no interest in exploring the maze, while few P. erhardii individuals remained rather indifferent to food even after detecting it. Our results suggest that the invasive P. siculus displays behavioral traits that could provide better opportunities for survival in the new environment and thus facilitate establishment even in the presence of congenerics. This provides further support to the idea that behavior plays a crucial role in animal invasions.

尽管生物入侵是全球生物多样性的最大威胁之一,但我们对入侵物种优于本地物种的机制的理解仍然有限,特别是在行为方面。大多数现有的研究都考察了在入侵后期有利于入侵物种的行为特征,然而,我们对入侵早期阶段,即外来物种面对新环境和必须开发新资源的阶段的认识仍然模糊不清。在这里,我们关注一个关键的行为特征,寻找食物。意大利壁虎(Podarcis siculus)已被广泛引进,并在欧洲南部和北美建立了可存活的种群。我们研究了与可能在不久的将来与之接触的两种本地同属物种——岛屿特有物种(P. milensis)和广泛分布的蜥蜴(P. erhardii)相比,P. siculus是否具有增强的探索行为和寻找食物的能力。我们进行了一个y形迷宫实验,在这个实验中,我们以一种标准的方式改变手臂上的标记,以防止学习。豆豆在寻找和消耗食物方面比其同类更有效。这种剥削的优势是持久的,更频繁和重复的。在本地物种中也发现了有趣的行为差异。一些milensis个体对探索迷宫没有兴趣,而少数p.r hardii个体即使在发现食物后仍然对食物漠不关心。我们的研究结果表明,入侵的小锥虫表现出的行为特征可以在新环境中提供更好的生存机会,从而在同源物种存在的情况下也能促进建立。这进一步支持了行为在动物入侵中起关键作用的观点。
{"title":"Invasive Italian wall lizards outcompete native congeneric species in finding food in a Y-maze","authors":"Anastasios Limnios,&nbsp;Chloe Adamopoulou,&nbsp;Miguel A. Carretero,&nbsp;Panayiotis Pafilis","doi":"10.1007/s10211-021-00385-8","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-021-00385-8","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Though biological invasions constitute one of the biggest threats for global biodiversity, our understanding of the mechanisms that enable invasive species to outperform native species is still limited, especially, in terms of behavior. Most available studies have examined behavioral traits which favor invasive species on the later stages of invasion, however, our knowledge on earlier stages, namely, when alien species face novel environments and must exploit new resources, remains obscure. Here, we focus on one crucial behavioral trait, finding food. The Italian wall lizard (<i>Podarcis siculus</i>) has been widely introduced and established viable populations in S. Europe and N. America. We examined whether <i>P. siculus</i> has enhanced exploratory behavior and abilities to find food compared to two native congeneric species with which it may come in contact in the near future, an insular endemic (<i>P. milensis</i>) and a widely distributed lizard (<i>P. erhardii</i>). We performed a Y-maze experiment, in which we varied arm markings in a standard way to prevent learning. <i>Podarcis siculus</i> was more efficient than its congenerics in finding and consuming food. This exploitative superiority was persistent, more frequent and repetitive. Interesting behavioral differences were also detected within the native species. Some <i>P. milensis</i> individuals showed no interest in exploring the maze, while few <i>P. erhardii</i> individuals remained rather indifferent to food even after detecting it. Our results suggest that the invasive <i>P. siculus</i> displays behavioral traits that could provide better opportunities for survival in the new environment and thus facilitate establishment even in the presence of congenerics. This provides further support to the idea that behavior plays a crucial role in animal invasions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"25 1","pages":"43 - 55"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2021-10-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10211-021-00385-8.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50011742","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Dominance hierarchy and social network in a captive group of white-lipped peccary males: what happens after the alpha male leaves? 圈养的白唇山核桃雄性群体的统治等级和社会网络:雄性首领离开后会发生什么?
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2021-09-29 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-021-00386-7
Laís Aline Grossel, Manoel Lucas Javorouski, Nei Moreira, Emygdio Leite de Araujo Monteiro-Filho

Dominance hierarchies are typically stable, with dominants occupying central positions in social interaction networks. However, system perturbations, such as the removal of individuals, may cause instability, which varies according to the group’s resilience. If the hierarchy undergoes a restructuring, this can occur through a dynamic process of self-organization (Social Dynamics hypothesis) or through the influence of individuals’ attributes (Previous Attributes hypothesis). We analyzed the resilience of the white-lipped peccary hierarchy after the alpha’s death and observed how the rise of a new dominant occurred. Additionally, we evaluated the validity of these two hypotheses in the restructuring of the system. We observed the group of white-lipped peccary males of the Municipal Zoo of Curitiba, PR, Brazil, from May to October 2018. We recorded and analyzed the agonistic and affiliative interactions, and we collected data from the attributes: weight, testicle size, testosterone serum and age, before and after the dominant’s death (August). Due to this perturbation, the hierarchy started to show instability, but proved to be resilient. There was an increase in agonism and more than one individual pleading for the new dominant position, which was occupied by a subordinate male. Affiliative interactions were also important in the rise of the new alpha. The hierarchy was well ordered by the social dynamics among individuals, but weight and testicular volume were also correlated with the status of the individuals before, and with the serum testosterone after the alpha’s death. Thus, both hypotheses affected the restructuring of this system.

支配等级通常是稳定的,支配者在社会互动网络中占据中心位置。然而,系统扰动,例如个体的移除,可能会导致不稳定,这取决于群体的恢复能力。如果层级结构经历重构,这可能通过自我组织的动态过程(社会动力学假说)或通过个体属性的影响(先前属性假说)发生。我们分析了在首领死后白唇金钱阶层的恢复力,并观察了一个新的统治阶层是如何崛起的。此外,我们评估了这两个假设在系统重组中的有效性。2018年5月至10月,我们观察了巴西库里蒂巴市立动物园的一群白唇雄pecary。我们记录并分析了拮抗性和亲和性的相互作用,并收集了优势体死亡前后(8月)的属性数据:体重、睾丸大小、睾酮血清和年龄。由于这种扰动,层次结构开始表现出不稳定性,但被证明是有弹性的。竞争加剧,而且不止一个个体在争夺新的统治地位,而这一地位是由一个从属的雄性占据的。在新阿尔法的兴起中,亲和互动也很重要。个体之间的社会动态对等级排序很好,但体重和睾丸体积也与个体在阿尔法死亡前的地位相关,与阿尔法死亡后的血清睾酮水平相关。因此,这两种假设都影响了这一制度的重组。
{"title":"Dominance hierarchy and social network in a captive group of white-lipped peccary males: what happens after the alpha male leaves?","authors":"Laís Aline Grossel,&nbsp;Manoel Lucas Javorouski,&nbsp;Nei Moreira,&nbsp;Emygdio Leite de Araujo Monteiro-Filho","doi":"10.1007/s10211-021-00386-7","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-021-00386-7","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Dominance hierarchies are typically stable, with dominants occupying central positions in social interaction networks. However, system perturbations, such as the removal of individuals, may cause instability, which varies according to the group’s resilience. If the hierarchy undergoes a restructuring, this can occur through a dynamic process of self-organization (Social Dynamics hypothesis) or through the influence of individuals’ attributes (Previous Attributes hypothesis). We analyzed the resilience of the white-lipped peccary hierarchy after the alpha’s death and observed how the rise of a new dominant occurred. Additionally, we evaluated the validity of these two hypotheses in the restructuring of the system. We observed the group of white-lipped peccary males of the Municipal Zoo of Curitiba, PR, Brazil, from May to October 2018. We recorded and analyzed the agonistic and affiliative interactions, and we collected data from the attributes: weight, testicle size, testosterone serum and age, before and after the dominant’s death (August). Due to this perturbation, the hierarchy started to show instability, but proved to be resilient. There was an increase in agonism and more than one individual pleading for the new dominant position, which was occupied by a subordinate male. Affiliative interactions were also important in the rise of the new alpha. The hierarchy was well ordered by the social dynamics among individuals, but weight and testicular volume were also correlated with the status of the individuals before, and with the serum testosterone after the alpha’s death. Thus, both hypotheses affected the restructuring of this system.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"25 2","pages":"65 - 77"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2021-09-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45462405","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Time-activity budget of urban-adapted free-ranging dogs 适应城市的自由放养犬的时间活动预算
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2021-09-08 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-021-00379-6
Arunita Banerjee, A. Bhadra
{"title":"Time-activity budget of urban-adapted free-ranging dogs","authors":"Arunita Banerjee, A. Bhadra","doi":"10.1007/s10211-021-00379-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10211-021-00379-6","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"25 1","pages":"33 - 42"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2021-09-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"51958756","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Time-activity budget of urban-adapted free-ranging dogs 适应城市的自由放养犬的时间活动预算
IF 1.1 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2021-09-08 DOI: 10.1007/s10211-021-00379-6
Arunita Banerjee, Anindita Bhadra

The domestic dog (Canis lupus familiaris) is known to have evolved from gray wolves, about 15,000 years ago. They frequently exist as free-ranging populations across the world. They are typically scavengers and well adapted to living among humans. Most canids living in and around urban habitats tend to avoid humans and show crepuscular activity peaks. In this study, we carried out a detailed population-level survey on free-ranging dogs in West Bengal, India, to understand the activity patterns of free-ranging dogs in relation to human activity. Using 5669 sightings of dogs, over a period of 1 year, covering the 24 h of the day, we carried out an analysis of the time activity budget of free-ranging dogs to conclude that they are generalists in their habit. They remain active when humans are active. Their activity levels are affected significantly by age class and time of the day. In addition, we provide a detailed ethogram of free-ranging dogs. This, to our knowledge, is the first study of this kind, which might be used to further study the eco-ethology of these dogs.

家犬(Canis lupus familiaris)是在大约1.5万年前由灰狼进化而来的。它们经常以自由放养的方式生活在世界各地。它们是典型的食腐动物,非常适应与人类生活在一起。大多数生活在城市及其周围的犬科动物倾向于避开人类,并表现出黄昏活动高峰。在这项研究中,我们对印度西孟加拉邦的自由放养犬进行了详细的种群水平调查,以了解自由放养犬的活动模式与人类活动的关系。在一年的时间里,我们对5669只狗进行了一天24小时的观察,对自由放养的狗的时间活动预算进行了分析,得出结论:它们在习惯上是多面手。当人类活动时,它们也会保持活跃。他们的活动水平受到年龄、班级和一天中的时间的显著影响。此外,我们还提供了自由放养犬的详细族谱。据我们所知,这是第一次这样的研究,可以用来进一步研究这些狗的生态行为学。
{"title":"Time-activity budget of urban-adapted free-ranging dogs","authors":"Arunita Banerjee,&nbsp;Anindita Bhadra","doi":"10.1007/s10211-021-00379-6","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10211-021-00379-6","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The domestic dog (<i>Canis lupus familiaris</i>) is known to have evolved from gray wolves, about 15,000 years ago. They frequently exist as free-ranging populations across the world. They are typically scavengers and well adapted to living among humans. Most canids living in and around urban habitats tend to avoid humans and show crepuscular activity peaks. In this study, we carried out a detailed population-level survey on free-ranging dogs in West Bengal, India, to understand the activity patterns of free-ranging dogs in relation to human activity. Using 5669 sightings of dogs, over a period of 1 year, covering the 24 h of the day, we carried out an analysis of the time activity budget of free-ranging dogs to conclude that they are generalists in their habit. They remain active when humans are active. Their activity levels are affected significantly by age class and time of the day. In addition, we provide a detailed ethogram of free-ranging dogs. This, to our knowledge, is the first study of this kind, which might be used to further study the eco-ethology of these dogs.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":6879,"journal":{"name":"acta ethologica","volume":"25 1","pages":"33 - 42"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2021-09-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10211-021-00379-6.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50015408","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
期刊
acta ethologica
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1