首页 > 最新文献

Current research in ecological and social psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Feeling at home: An explorative field study of seasonal agricultural workers with different (dis) location backgrounds 家的感觉:不同(非)地点背景的季节性农业工人的探索性田野研究
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100170
Meral Gezici Yalçın , N. Ekrem Düzen , Furkan Bardak , Ayse K. Uskul , Murat Öztürk

The sense of feeling at home by people ‘on the move’ was inquired through an adaptation of the homemaking approach. Two groups of people who make their living by working in agricultural sites (internally mobile seasonal agricultural workers and internationally displaced migrant workers) were reached out to examine associations between feeling at home, social interactions, perceived degradation, and subjective well-being. Results showed that both worker groups (seasonal and displaced workers) felt at home despite precarious working and living conditions. Expectedly, feeling at home was predicted significantly by social interactions with others; however, the type of interactions also determined the direction of the effects. While within-group interaction (binding ties) predicted feeling at home positively, across-groups interaction (bridging ties) predicted it negatively for both groups. Additionally, perceived degradation and subjective well-being moderated the effect of feeling at home partially: the effect emerged for a cross-groups but not for within-group interactions. In conclusion, the notion of binding and bridging ties could help to attain an increased explanatory power rather than contact theory alone in understanding the patterns of feeling at home.

“在移动中”的人们对家的感觉是通过对家政方法的改编来询问的。通过在农业场所工作谋生的两组人(国内流动的季节性农业工人和国际流离失所的移徙工人)进行了接触,以检查家的感觉、社会互动、感知退化和主观幸福感之间的联系。结果显示,尽管工作和生活条件不稳定,但两个工人群体(季节性工人和流离失所工人)都有家的感觉。不出所料,与他人的社会互动显著地预测了宾至如归的感觉;然而,相互作用的类型也决定了效应的方向。群体内互动(绑定关系)对家的感觉有积极的预测,而群体间互动(桥接关系)对两组的家的感觉都有消极的预测。此外,感知退化和主观幸福感在一定程度上调节了宾至如归的影响:这种影响在跨群体中出现,但在群体内互动中没有。综上所述,结合和衔接关系的概念可以帮助获得更强的解释力,而不是单独的接触理论来理解在家的感觉模式。
{"title":"Feeling at home: An explorative field study of seasonal agricultural workers with different (dis) location backgrounds","authors":"Meral Gezici Yalçın ,&nbsp;N. Ekrem Düzen ,&nbsp;Furkan Bardak ,&nbsp;Ayse K. Uskul ,&nbsp;Murat Öztürk","doi":"10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100170","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100170","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The sense of feeling at home by people ‘on the move’ was inquired through an adaptation of the homemaking approach. Two groups of people who make their living by working in agricultural sites (internally mobile seasonal agricultural workers and internationally displaced migrant workers) were reached out to examine associations between feeling at home, social interactions, perceived degradation, and subjective well-being. Results showed that both worker groups (seasonal and displaced workers) felt at home despite precarious working and living conditions. Expectedly, feeling at home was predicted significantly by social interactions with others; however, the type of interactions also determined the direction of the effects. While within-group interaction (binding ties) predicted feeling at home positively, across-groups interaction (bridging ties) predicted it negatively for both groups. Additionally, perceived degradation and subjective well-being moderated the effect of feeling at home partially: the effect emerged for a cross-groups but not for within-group interactions. In conclusion, the notion of binding and bridging ties could help to attain an increased explanatory power rather than contact theory alone in understanding the patterns of feeling at home.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72748,"journal":{"name":"Current research in ecological and social psychology","volume":"5 ","pages":"Article 100170"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S2666622723000837/pdfft?md5=6e32ae142ed3a940618f2833bd777e48&pid=1-s2.0-S2666622723000837-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138466149","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A moth to a flame? Fulfilling connectedness needs through romantic relationships protects conspiracy theorists against COVID-19 misinformation 飞蛾变火焰?通过浪漫关系满足联系需求保护阴谋论者免受新冠肺炎错误信息的影响
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100111
Sandra L. Murray , Ji Xia , Veronica Lamarche , Mark D. Seery , James McNulty , Dale W. Griffin , Deborah E. Ward , Han Young Jung , Lindsey Hicks , David Dubois

Conspiracy theorists’ unpopular opinions likely make them more apprehensive about interactions with others, frustrating their need to belong. Therefore, they may be susceptible to believing misinformation because evidence that others share their beliefs provides “social proof” that they can expect interactions with others to be positive and rewarding. The present research examined whether alternatively fulfilling the need for social connection through romantic relationships could protect conspiracy theorists against COVID-19 misinformation. In a 3-week daily diary study (N = 555), experimental participants implicitly learned to associate their romantic partners with positive experiences (by repeatedly pairing their partner with highly positive and approachable stimuli, McNulty et al., 2017). We then assessed how much participants trusted individuals they might normally distrust, as a manipulation check, and how much participants tuned their daily personal beliefs and behavior to match the U.S. public's daily susceptibility to COVID-19 misinformation. Participants high on conspiratorial thinking trusted fellow community members more in the experimental than control condition. Participants high on conspiratorial thinking in the experimental condition were also less likely to treat the U.S. public's greater daily susceptibility to COVID-19 misinformation as proof that they could discount the virus. The present findings suggest that rewarding romantic connections might be leveraged to limit conspiracy theorists’ susceptibility to believing public skepticism about COVID-19.

阴谋论者不受欢迎的观点可能会让他们对与他人的互动更加担忧,从而挫败他们对归属感的需求。因此,他们可能容易相信错误信息,因为其他人分享他们信仰的证据提供了“社会证明”,他们可以期望与他人的互动是积极和有益的。目前的研究考察了通过浪漫关系来满足社会联系的需求是否可以保护阴谋论者免受COVID-19错误信息的影响。在一项为期3周的每日日记研究中(N = 555),实验参与者隐性地学会了将他们的浪漫伴侣与积极的经历联系起来(通过反复将他们的伴侣与高度积极和可接近的刺激配对,McNulty et al., 2017)。然后,我们评估了参与者对他们通常可能不信任的个人的信任程度,作为操纵检查,以及参与者对他们的日常个人信仰和行为进行了多少调整,以匹配美国公众对COVID-19错误信息的日常敏感性。阴谋思维高的参与者在实验中比在控制条件下更信任社区成员。在实验条件下,阴谋论思维高的参与者也不太可能将美国公众每天对COVID-19错误信息的更大敏感性视为他们可以轻视病毒的证据。目前的研究结果表明,奖励浪漫关系可能会被用来限制阴谋论者相信公众对COVID-19的怀疑的敏感性。
{"title":"A moth to a flame? Fulfilling connectedness needs through romantic relationships protects conspiracy theorists against COVID-19 misinformation","authors":"Sandra L. Murray ,&nbsp;Ji Xia ,&nbsp;Veronica Lamarche ,&nbsp;Mark D. Seery ,&nbsp;James McNulty ,&nbsp;Dale W. Griffin ,&nbsp;Deborah E. Ward ,&nbsp;Han Young Jung ,&nbsp;Lindsey Hicks ,&nbsp;David Dubois","doi":"10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100111","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100111","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Conspiracy theorists’ unpopular opinions likely make them more apprehensive about interactions with others, frustrating their need to belong. Therefore, they may be susceptible to believing misinformation because evidence that others share their beliefs provides “social proof” that they can expect interactions with others to be positive and rewarding. The present research examined whether <em>alternatively</em> fulfilling the need for social connection through romantic relationships could protect conspiracy theorists against COVID-19 misinformation. In a 3-week daily diary study (<em>N</em> = 555), experimental participants implicitly learned to associate their romantic partners with positive experiences (by repeatedly pairing their partner with highly positive and approachable stimuli, McNulty et al., 2017). We then assessed how much participants trusted individuals they might normally distrust, as a manipulation check, and how much participants tuned their daily personal beliefs and behavior to match the U.S. public's daily susceptibility to COVID-19 misinformation. Participants <em>high</em> on conspiratorial thinking trusted fellow community members <em>more</em> in the experimental than control condition. Participants <em>high</em> on conspiratorial thinking in the experimental condition were also <em>less</em> likely to treat the U.S. public's greater daily susceptibility to COVID-19 misinformation as proof that they could discount the virus. The present findings suggest that rewarding romantic connections might be leveraged to limit conspiracy theorists’ susceptibility to believing public skepticism about COVID-19.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72748,"journal":{"name":"Current research in ecological and social psychology","volume":"4 ","pages":"Article 100111"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46073347","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Self-reported reasons for (not) being worried about climate change 自我报告担心(不担心)气候变化的原因
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100154
Thea Gregersen , Rouven Doran , Sina Storelv

A national sample from Norway (N = 2001) was asked to report how much they worry about climate change (closed-ended question), and then to write down their reasons for (not) being worried (open-ended question). Answers to the open-ended question were content analyzed and compared across responses to the closed-ended question. The results showed that the most common reason for being at least somewhat worried was concern about the consequences of climate change. Respondents reporting high worry were in particular more likely to bring up consequences for humans than those reporting medium worry. Respondents who reported low worry referred to a broader range of reasons in their answers, such as believing in natural rather than human causes of climate change, expressing a sense of optimism towards potential solutions, or being discontent with political measures or public discourse on climate change. These findings add novel insights into understanding the subjective meaning associated with the degree to which people report being worried about climate change.

来自挪威的国家样本(N = 2001)被要求报告他们对气候变化的担忧程度(封闭式问题),然后写下他们担心(不担心)的原因(开放式问题)。对开放式问题的回答进行了内容分析,并与封闭式问题的回答进行了比较。结果显示,最常见的担忧原因是对气候变化的后果的担忧。报告高度担忧的受访者比报告中度担忧的受访者更有可能给人类带来后果。报告担忧程度较低的受访者在回答中提到了更广泛的原因,例如相信气候变化的自然原因而不是人为原因,对潜在的解决方案表示乐观,或者对气候变化的政治措施或公共话语感到不满。这些发现为理解与人们对气候变化的担忧程度相关的主观含义提供了新的见解。
{"title":"Self-reported reasons for (not) being worried about climate change","authors":"Thea Gregersen ,&nbsp;Rouven Doran ,&nbsp;Sina Storelv","doi":"10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100154","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100154","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>A national sample from Norway (<em>N</em> = 2001) was asked to report how much they worry about climate change (closed-ended question), and then to write down their reasons for (not) being worried (open-ended question). Answers to the open-ended question were content analyzed and compared across responses to the closed-ended question. The results showed that the most common reason for being at least somewhat worried was concern about the consequences of climate change. Respondents reporting high worry were in particular more likely to bring up consequences for humans than those reporting medium worry. Respondents who reported low worry referred to a broader range of reasons in their answers, such as believing in natural rather than human causes of climate change, expressing a sense of optimism towards potential solutions, or being discontent with political measures or public discourse on climate change. These findings add novel insights into understanding the subjective meaning associated with the degree to which people report being worried about climate change.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72748,"journal":{"name":"Current research in ecological and social psychology","volume":"5 ","pages":"Article 100154"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46967622","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Humanizing dehumanization research 人性化非人性化研究
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100102
Jonathan Leader Maynard , Aliza Luft

This essay contends that contemporary research should pay greater attention to three fundamentally human characteristics of dehumanization. First, we argue that scholars need to better consider and analyze the ideological context of dehumanization, specifically the multiple meanings that diverse cultural conceptions of the human and the dehumanized impart to dehumanizing concepts or attitudes. Second, we urge a greater emphasis on how social relationships influence dehumanization, recognizing that people are entangled in complex and intersecting social identities, relationships, and histories that affect how they respond to the dehumanizing intentions of others. Third, we argue that the institutional context of dehumanization must be investigated and theorized, as dehumanization’s effects are rarely the result of atomized individuals reacting to diffuse dehumanizing rhetoric but rather the result of collective action within more or less formal organizations. These tasks can be accomplished through increased interdisciplinarity, thereby enhancing the insights and applicability of dehumanization research to the numerous forms of conflict, brutality, and extremism that are so frequently associated with dehumanization.

本文认为,当代研究应更多地关注非人化的三个基本人性特征。首先,我们认为学者们需要更好地考虑和分析非人化的意识形态语境,特别是人与被非人化者的不同文化观念赋予非人化概念或态度的多重意义。其次,我们敦促更多地强调社会关系如何影响非人化,认识到人们纠缠在复杂和交叉的社会身份、关系和历史中,这些身份、关系和历史影响着他们如何回应他人的非人化意图。第三,我们认为必须对非人化的制度背景进行调查和理论化,因为非人化的影响很少是原子化的个人对扩散的非人化言论的反应的结果,而是在或多或少正式组织内集体行动的结果。这些任务可以通过增加跨学科来完成,从而提高非人性化研究对经常与非人性化相关的多种形式的冲突、暴行和极端主义的洞察力和适用性。
{"title":"Humanizing dehumanization research","authors":"Jonathan Leader Maynard ,&nbsp;Aliza Luft","doi":"10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100102","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100102","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This essay contends that contemporary research should pay greater attention to three fundamentally human characteristics of dehumanization. First, we argue that scholars need to better consider and analyze the ideological context of dehumanization, specifically the multiple meanings that diverse cultural conceptions of the human and the dehumanized impart to dehumanizing concepts or attitudes. Second, we urge a greater emphasis on how social relationships influence dehumanization, recognizing that people are entangled in complex and intersecting social identities, relationships, and histories that affect how they respond to the dehumanizing intentions of others. Third, we argue that the institutional context of dehumanization must be investigated and theorized, as dehumanization’s effects are rarely the result of atomized individuals reacting to diffuse dehumanizing rhetoric but rather the result of collective action within more or less formal organizations. These tasks can be accomplished through increased interdisciplinarity, thereby enhancing the insights and applicability of dehumanization research to the numerous forms of conflict, brutality, and extremism that are so frequently associated with dehumanization.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72748,"journal":{"name":"Current research in ecological and social psychology","volume":"4 ","pages":"Article 100102"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44315524","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Support for climate-driven migration in Australia: Testing an ideology-based threat model 对澳大利亚气候驱动移民的支持:基于意识形态的威胁模型的测试
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100119
Samantha K. Stanley , Zoe Leviston , Caroline Ng Tseung-Wong

We examine Australians’ preferences for resettling people displaced by climate change from overseas (‘climate refugees’), from within Australia (‘internal climate refugees’), and people displaced by war. Across three studies (Study 1 N = 467, Study 2 N = 1679, Study 3 N = 492), our findings reveal greater support for resettling refugee groups already residing in the nation: internal climate refugees and refugees of war. Although support for all three groups was reasonably high, participants were consistently and significantly less supportive of resettling international climate refugees. Both groups of international refugees (relocating due to war or climate changes) were viewed as posing greater threat than internally displaced Australians. Endorsement of right-wing ideological attitudes predicted lower support for climate refugees, which was mediated by symbolic and realistic threat perceptions. These findings highlight the potential of ideology, economic and cultural concerns to undermine support for resettling those displaced by climate change.

我们研究了澳大利亚人对重新安置因气候变化而流离失所的人的偏好,这些人来自海外(“气候难民”),来自澳大利亚境内(“内部气候难民”),以及因战争而流离失所的人。通过三项研究(研究1 N = 467,研究2 N = 1679,研究3 N = 492),我们的研究结果显示,对重新安置已经居住在该国的难民群体(国内气候难民和战争难民)的支持更大。尽管对这三个群体的支持率都相当高,但参与者对重新安置国际气候难民的支持程度一直明显较低。这两组国际难民(由于战争或气候变化而迁移)被认为比国内流离失所的澳大利亚人构成更大的威胁。对右翼意识形态态度的支持预示着对气候难民的支持率较低,这是由象征性和现实的威胁感知所介导的。这些发现强调了意识形态、经济和文化方面的担忧可能会削弱对因气候变化而流离失所者的重新安置的支持。
{"title":"Support for climate-driven migration in Australia: Testing an ideology-based threat model","authors":"Samantha K. Stanley ,&nbsp;Zoe Leviston ,&nbsp;Caroline Ng Tseung-Wong","doi":"10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100119","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100119","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We examine Australians’ preferences for resettling people displaced by climate change from overseas (‘climate refugees’), from within Australia (‘internal climate refugees’), and people displaced by war. Across three studies (Study 1 <em>N</em> = 467, Study 2 <em>N</em> = 1679, Study 3 <em>N</em> = 492), our findings reveal greater support for resettling refugee groups already residing in the nation: internal climate refugees and refugees of war. Although support for all three groups was reasonably high, participants were consistently and significantly less supportive of resettling international climate refugees. Both groups of international refugees (relocating due to war or climate changes) were viewed as posing greater threat than internally displaced Australians. Endorsement of right-wing ideological attitudes predicted lower support for climate refugees, which was mediated by symbolic and realistic threat perceptions. These findings highlight the potential of ideology, economic and cultural concerns to undermine support for resettling those displaced by climate change.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72748,"journal":{"name":"Current research in ecological and social psychology","volume":"4 ","pages":"Article 100119"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43934608","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
National inequality, social capital, and public goods decision-making 国家不平等、社会资本与公共产品决策
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100112
Joshua C. Skewes , Laila Nockur

Inequality affects how people make social decisions. Laboratory research has shown that when income inequality is simulated using cooperative economic games, groups with higher inequality often generate less wealth overall, with poorer group members receiving the worst outcomes. This study links these experimental findings to real world inequality and applies a decision model to explain the effects in terms of social decision-making dynamics. Using a pre-existing dataset from 255 groups playing a public goods game in thirteen economically diverse societies, we show that in nations with higher inequality, groups contribute less (Research question (RQ) 1). Further, we find that higher inequality is associated with lower optimism regarding others’ contributions at the outset of the game and increased sensitivity to others’ contributions, which accelerates the decay of cooperation (RQ2). These effects might be explained by national differences in social capital as expressed by trust and adherence to civic norms (RQ3). Using the European Values Survey, we replicate the negative association between inequality and contributions to a public good by examining national volunteering rates (RQ4).

不平等影响人们如何做出社会决策。实验室研究表明,当使用合作经济游戏模拟收入不平等时,不平等程度较高的群体总体上产生的财富往往较少,较贫穷的群体成员得到的结果最差。本研究将这些实验结果与现实世界的不平等联系起来,并应用决策模型来解释社会决策动态方面的影响。使用来自13个经济多样化社会中255个群体参与公共物品游戏的已有数据集,我们发现,在不平等程度较高的国家,群体贡献较少(研究问题(RQ) 1)。此外,我们发现,不平等程度越高,在游戏开始时对他人贡献的乐观程度越低,对他人贡献的敏感度越高,这加速了合作的衰减(RQ2)。这些影响可以用国家间社会资本的差异来解释,社会资本表现为信任和对公民规范的遵守(RQ3)。利用欧洲价值观调查,我们通过检查国家志愿服务率(RQ4),复制了不平等与对公共利益的贡献之间的负相关关系。
{"title":"National inequality, social capital, and public goods decision-making","authors":"Joshua C. Skewes ,&nbsp;Laila Nockur","doi":"10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100112","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100112","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Inequality affects how people make social decisions. Laboratory research has shown that when income inequality is simulated using cooperative economic games, groups with higher inequality often generate less wealth overall, with poorer group members receiving the worst outcomes. This study links these experimental findings to real world inequality and applies a decision model to explain the effects in terms of social decision-making dynamics. Using a pre-existing dataset from 255 groups playing a public goods game in thirteen economically diverse societies, we show that in nations with higher inequality, groups contribute less (Research question (RQ) 1). Further, we find that higher inequality is associated with lower optimism regarding others’ contributions at the outset of the game and increased sensitivity to others’ contributions, which accelerates the decay of cooperation (RQ2). These effects might be explained by national differences in social capital as expressed by trust and adherence to civic norms (RQ3). Using the European Values Survey, we replicate the negative association between inequality and contributions to a public good by examining national volunteering rates (RQ4).</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72748,"journal":{"name":"Current research in ecological and social psychology","volume":"4 ","pages":"Article 100112"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44546548","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Coherently arbitrary pro-environmental behavior 一贯武断的环保行为
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100094
Sebastian Berger, Daniel Bregulla

An accurate understanding of pro-environmental behavior is a key research topic within environmental psychology and a prerequisite for an adequate psychological response to environmental issues. In this study, we present an experiment testing the degree to which decision makers’ pro-environmental behavior is “coherently arbitrary”. Coherent arbitrariness refers to the phenomenon that behavior in experimental models may only appear rational, as if supported by fixed preferences, despite being affected by arbitrary factors unrelated to preferences. Using the Carbon Emission Task, the present research extends this behavioral economic finding to pro-environmental behavior research. We find that (a) objectively identical trade-offs are evaluated substantially differently depending on the relative rather than absolute price level of comparative choices, and (b) biospheric values correlate robustly with behavior across conditions. This result may also help to explain findings documenting a motivation-impact gap in pro-environmental behavior, as people may find it difficult to objectively and globally assess the costs and benefits associated with their choices.

准确理解亲环境行为是环境心理学中的一个重要研究课题,也是对环境问题做出适当心理反应的前提。在这项研究中,我们提出了一个实验来测试决策者的亲环境行为是“连贯任意”的程度。相干随意性指的是这样一种现象,即实验模型中的行为可能只会显得理性,似乎受到固定偏好的支持,尽管受到与偏好无关的任意因素的影响。利用碳排放任务,本研究将这一行为经济学发现扩展到亲环境行为研究。我们发现(a)客观上相同的权衡会根据比较选择的相对价格水平而不是绝对价格水平而得到显著不同的评估;(b)生物圈价值与各种条件下的行为密切相关。这一结果也可能有助于解释记录亲环境行为的动机-影响差距的发现,因为人们可能会发现很难客观和全面地评估与他们的选择相关的成本和收益。
{"title":"Coherently arbitrary pro-environmental behavior","authors":"Sebastian Berger,&nbsp;Daniel Bregulla","doi":"10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100094","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100094","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>An accurate understanding of pro-environmental behavior is a key research topic within environmental psychology and a prerequisite for an adequate psychological response to environmental issues. In this study, we present an experiment testing the degree to which decision makers’ pro-environmental behavior is “coherently arbitrary”. Coherent arbitrariness refers to the phenomenon that behavior in experimental models may only appear rational, <em>as if</em> supported by fixed preferences, despite being affected by arbitrary factors unrelated to preferences. Using the <em>Carbon Emission Task</em>, the present research extends this behavioral economic finding to pro-environmental behavior research. We find that (a) <em>objectively</em> identical trade-offs are evaluated substantially differently depending on the relative rather than absolute price level of comparative choices, and (b) biospheric values correlate robustly with behavior across conditions. This result may also help to explain findings documenting a motivation-impact gap in pro-environmental behavior, as people may find it difficult to objectively and globally assess the costs and benefits associated with their choices.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72748,"journal":{"name":"Current research in ecological and social psychology","volume":"4 ","pages":"Article 100094"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48889045","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Trees are honest, bugs are creative, sunsets are hopeful - Identifying character strengths in nature: A structured tabular thematic analysis 树是诚实的,虫子是有创造力的,日落是有希望的——识别自然中的性格优势:一个结构化的表格主题分析
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100092
Ryan Lumber , Holli-Anne Passmore , Ryan Niemiec

The psychological construct of nature connectedness has been consistently linked to well-being and pro-nature behavioral outcomes, with a sense of self considered important for individuals to feel like they are part of nature. Interventions focusing on noticing good things in nature and the Five Pathways Framework have been utilized to help people reconnect with the more-than-human world although they have often overlooked incorporating nature within the self-concept and emphasizing similarity with nature despite its importance for the construct. We developed and tested a related, but alternative, approach to previous interventions to focus on similarity and sense of self through anthropomorphism: that of mindfully identifying how one's own character strengths are exhibited in nature. A Structured Tabular Thematic Analysis was conducted on 747 written observations (n = 93) of shared character strengths in nature. Five themes were generated: (1) finding representations of the self through seasonal change; (2) identifying with weather and the character strengths it possesses; (3) experiencing awe and wonder in nature through shared character strengths; (4) nature as an honest or dishonest entity; and (5) the inability to find similarity between oneself and nature. These themes provide insight into the ability of the intervention to enable participants to find a sense of self in the rest of nature when identifying shared strengths. Nature connectedness pathways of meaning, compassion, and beauty were also evident in the observations. Implications for using a character strengths-based approach to boost nature connectedness through a shared sense of self and similarity are discussed. The identification of personal character strengths shared with nature offers a new and meaningful way to reconnect with the more-than-human world to which we belong.

自然联系的心理结构一直与幸福和亲自然的行为结果联系在一起,自我意识被认为对个人来说很重要,因为他们觉得自己是自然的一部分。关注自然中的美好事物和五种途径框架的干预措施已被用来帮助人们重新与超越人类的世界联系起来,尽管他们经常忽视将自然纳入自我概念,并强调与自然的相似性,尽管它对构建很重要。我们开发并测试了一种相关的替代方法,通过拟人化来关注相似性和自我意识:即有意识地识别自己的性格优势是如何在自然界中表现出来的。对747份(n = 93)关于自然界共同性格优势的书面观察进行了结构化表格主题分析。研究产生了五个主题:(1)通过季节变化寻找自我的表征;(2)认同天气及其所具有的性格优势;(3)通过共同的性格力量体验大自然的敬畏和惊奇;(四)诚实或者不诚实的性质;(5)找不到自己与自然的相似之处。这些主题提供了对干预能力的洞察,使参与者在确定共同优势时能够在自然的其余部分中找到自我感。在观察中,意义、同情和美的自然联系途径也很明显。讨论了使用基于性格优势的方法通过共享的自我意识和相似性来促进自然联系的含义。识别与自然共享的个人性格优势提供了一种新的有意义的方式来重新连接我们所属的超越人类的世界。
{"title":"Trees are honest, bugs are creative, sunsets are hopeful - Identifying character strengths in nature: A structured tabular thematic analysis","authors":"Ryan Lumber ,&nbsp;Holli-Anne Passmore ,&nbsp;Ryan Niemiec","doi":"10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100092","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100092","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The psychological construct of nature connectedness has been consistently linked to well-being and pro-nature behavioral outcomes, with a sense of self considered important for individuals to feel like they are part of nature. Interventions focusing on noticing good things in nature and the Five Pathways Framework have been utilized to help people reconnect with the more-than-human world although they have often overlooked incorporating nature within the self-concept and emphasizing similarity with nature despite its importance for the construct. We developed and tested a related, but alternative, approach to previous interventions to focus on similarity and sense of self through anthropomorphism: that of mindfully identifying how one's own character strengths are exhibited in nature. A Structured Tabular Thematic Analysis was conducted on 747 written observations (<em>n</em> = 93) of shared character strengths in nature. Five themes were generated: (1) finding representations of the self through seasonal change; (2) identifying with weather and the character strengths it possesses; (3) experiencing awe and wonder in nature through shared character strengths; (4) nature as an honest or dishonest entity; and (5) the inability to find similarity between oneself and nature. These themes provide insight into the ability of the intervention to enable participants to find a sense of self in the rest of nature when identifying shared strengths. Nature connectedness pathways of meaning, compassion, and beauty were also evident in the observations. Implications for using a character strengths-based approach to boost nature connectedness through a shared sense of self and similarity are discussed. The identification of personal character strengths shared with nature offers a new and meaningful way to reconnect with the more-than-human world to which we belong.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72748,"journal":{"name":"Current research in ecological and social psychology","volume":"4 ","pages":"Article 100092"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49661623","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Like me or near me? Assessing which norm referents best promote energy conservation in the field 喜欢我还是靠近我?评估哪些规范指标最能促进现场节能
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100142
Angela Ziyan Xiao , Gregg Sparkman , Sara M. Constantino

Messages about what others typically do are increasingly used to encourage sustainable behaviors. The effectiveness of such social norm interventions hinges on selecting an appropriate referent group. However, it is unclear which characteristics make norm referents effective. In a field experiment on energy conservation, in a non-WEIRD and under-studied setting, we examine the role of two referent characteristics: group identification and physical proximity. Students in university dorms (N = 584) were randomly assigned to a control group or to one of two treatment groups, in which they were given social norm information that varied only in whether it was about a group with whom they were likely to identify or a physically proximal group with whom they identify less. Tracking electricity meter reading data in the weeks before and after the intervention, we found that social norm information about the high identification group led to an 11 percent reduction in energy consumption relative to a control group. In the physical proximity condition, we did not find an overall reduction in energy use relative to controls, although the effect of the intervention was significantly moderated by identification with the referent group. We conclude that identification with a group is important for the efficacy of social norms interventions in this setting, and that physical proximity alone may be insufficient for an effective norm referent.

关于他人通常所做的事情的信息越来越多地被用来鼓励可持续的行为。这种社会规范干预措施的有效性取决于选择一个合适的参照群体。然而,目前尚不清楚哪些特征使规范指称有效。在一个关于能量守恒的现场实验中,在一个非WEIRD和研究不足的环境中,我们检验了两个指称特征的作用:群体识别和物理接近。大学宿舍里的学生(N=584)被随机分配到一个对照组或两个治疗组中的一个,在这些组中,他们得到的社会规范信息只是关于他们可能认同的群体,还是他们认同较少的身体接近群体。通过跟踪干预前后几周的电表读数数据,我们发现,与对照组相比,高识别率组的社会规范信息导致能耗降低了11%。在身体接近的情况下,我们没有发现与对照组相比,能量使用的总体减少,尽管干预的效果因与参照组的认同而显著减弱。我们得出的结论是,在这种情况下,认同一个群体对社会规范干预的有效性很重要,而仅凭身体上的接近可能不足以作为有效的规范参考。
{"title":"Like me or near me? Assessing which norm referents best promote energy conservation in the field","authors":"Angela Ziyan Xiao ,&nbsp;Gregg Sparkman ,&nbsp;Sara M. Constantino","doi":"10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100142","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100142","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Messages about what others typically do are increasingly used to encourage sustainable behaviors. The effectiveness of such social norm interventions hinges on selecting an appropriate referent group. However, it is unclear which characteristics make norm referents effective. In a field experiment on energy conservation, in a non-WEIRD and under-studied setting, we examine the role of two referent characteristics: group identification and physical proximity. Students in university dorms (N = 584) were randomly assigned to a control group or to one of two treatment groups, in which they were given social norm information that varied only in whether it was about a group with whom they were likely to identify or a physically proximal group with whom they identify less. Tracking electricity meter reading data in the weeks before and after the intervention, we found that social norm information about the high identification group led to an 11 percent reduction in energy consumption relative to a control group. In the physical proximity condition, we did not find an overall reduction in energy use relative to controls, although the effect of the intervention was significantly moderated by identification with the referent group. We conclude that identification with a group is important for the efficacy of social norms interventions in this setting, and that physical proximity alone may be insufficient for an effective norm referent.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":72748,"journal":{"name":"Current research in ecological and social psychology","volume":"5 ","pages":"Article 100142"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49749401","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Challenging extremism: A randomized control trial examining the impact of counternarratives in the Middle East and North Africa 挑战极端主义:一项研究中东和北非反叙事影响的随机对照试验
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100097
Jocelyn J. Bélanger , Daniel W. Snook , Domnica Dzitac , Abdelhak Cheppih
{"title":"Challenging extremism: A randomized control trial examining the impact of counternarratives in the Middle East and North Africa","authors":"Jocelyn J. Bélanger ,&nbsp;Daniel W. Snook ,&nbsp;Domnica Dzitac ,&nbsp;Abdelhak Cheppih","doi":"10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100097","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100097","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":72748,"journal":{"name":"Current research in ecological and social psychology","volume":"4 ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49858003","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
期刊
Current research in ecological and social psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1