首页 > 最新文献

Animal Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Hooded crows (Corvus cornix) manufacture objects relative to a mental template 戴帽乌鸦(Corvus cornix)根据心理模板制造物体
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-29 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01874-6
Anna A. Smirnova, Leia R. Bulgakova, Maria A. Cheplakova, Sarah A. Jelbert

It was recently found that not only tool-specialized New Caledonian crows, but also Goffin cockatoos can manufacture physical objects in accordance with a mental template. That is, they can emulate features of existing objects when they manufacture new items. Both species spontaneously ripped pieces of card into large strips if they had previously learned that a large template was rewarded, and small strips when they previously learned that a small template was rewarded. Among New Caledonian crows, this cognitive ability was suggested as a potential mechanism underlying the transmission of natural tool designs. Here, we tested for the same ability in another non-specialised tool user–Hooded crows (Corvus cornix). Crows were exposed to pre-made template objects, varying first in colour and then in size, and were rewarded only if they chose pre-made objects that matched the template. In subsequent tests, birds were given the opportunity to manufacture versions of these objects. All three crows ripped paper pieces from the same colour material as the rewarded template, and, crucially, also manufactured objects that were more similar in size to previously rewarded, than unrewarded, templates, despite the birds being rewarded at random in both tests. Therefore, we found the ability to manufacture physical objects relative to a mental template in yet another bird species not specialized in using or making foraging tools in the wild, but with a high level of brain and cognitive development.

最近研究发现,不仅擅长工具的新喀里多尼亚乌鸦,戈芬凤头鹦鹉也能根据心理模板制造实物。也就是说,它们在制造新物品时可以模仿现有物品的特征。如果这两种乌鸦以前就知道大的模板会得到奖励,那么它们就会自发地把卡片撕成大条状;如果它们以前就知道小的模板会得到奖励,那么它们就会自发地把卡片撕成小条状。在新喀里多尼亚乌鸦中,这种认知能力被认为是自然工具设计传播的潜在机制。在这里,我们测试了另一种非专门工具使用者--带帽乌鸦(Corvus cornix)的相同能力。乌鸦会接触到预先制作好的模板物体,首先是颜色,然后是大小,只有当乌鸦选择了与模板相匹配的预制物体时才会得到奖励。在随后的测试中,乌鸦有机会制作这些物体的模型。所有三只乌鸦都从与奖励模板相同颜色的材料上撕下了纸片,而且关键的是,尽管在两次测试中乌鸦都是随机获得奖励,但它们制造的物品在大小上与之前获得奖励的模板更相似,而不是未获得奖励的模板。因此,我们在另一种在野外不擅长使用或制作觅食工具,但大脑和认知能力发展水平较高的鸟类身上发现了相对于心理模板制造实物的能力。
{"title":"Hooded crows (Corvus cornix) manufacture objects relative to a mental template","authors":"Anna A. Smirnova,&nbsp;Leia R. Bulgakova,&nbsp;Maria A. Cheplakova,&nbsp;Sarah A. Jelbert","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01874-6","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01874-6","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>It was recently found that not only tool-specialized New Caledonian crows, but also Goffin cockatoos can manufacture physical objects in accordance with a mental template. That is, they can emulate features of existing objects when they manufacture new items. Both species spontaneously ripped pieces of card into large strips if they had previously learned that a large template was rewarded, and small strips when they previously learned that a small template was rewarded. Among New Caledonian crows, this cognitive ability was suggested as a potential mechanism underlying the transmission of natural tool designs. Here, we tested for the same ability in another non-specialised tool user–Hooded crows (<i>Corvus cornix</i>). Crows were exposed to pre-made template objects, varying first in colour and then in size, and were rewarded only if they chose pre-made objects that matched the template. In subsequent tests, birds were given the opportunity to manufacture versions of these objects. All three crows ripped paper pieces from the same colour material as the rewarded template, and, crucially, also manufactured objects that were more similar in size to previously rewarded, than unrewarded, templates, despite the birds being rewarded at random in both tests. Therefore, we found the ability to manufacture physical objects relative to a mental template in yet another bird species not specialized in using or making foraging tools in the wild, but with a high level of brain and cognitive development.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-04-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-024-01874-6.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140809688","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Proto-tool use for food processing in wild Arabian babblers: matching processing methods, substrates and prey types 野生阿拉伯狒狒使用原工具加工食物:加工方法、基质和猎物类型的匹配
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-24 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01866-6
Yitzchak Ben Mocha, Francesca Frisoni, Oded Keynan, Michael Griesser

Cognition is a powerful adaptation, enabling animals to utilise resources that are unavailable without manipulation. Tool use and food processing are examples of using cognition to overcome the protective mechanisms of food resources. Here, we describe and examine the flexibility of proto-tool use (defined as the alteration of an object through object-substrate manipulation) for food processing in a cooperatively breeding bird, the Arabian babbler (Argya squamiceps). Field observations demonstrate that the birds transport different caterpillar species to different substrate types depending on the processing method needed to prepare the caterpillar for eating. Species with toxic setae (e.g. Casama innotata) are transported to be rubbed on rough substrates (e.g. sand) before consumption, while other species (e.g. Hyles livornica) are transported to be pounded against hard substrates until their inner organs are removed and only their external body part is consumed. These results are among the few to describe flexible proto-tool use for food processing in wild animals. They thereby contribute to the taxonomic mapping of proto-tool use and food processing in non-human species, which is a fundamental step to advance comparative studies on the evolution of these behaviours and their underlying cognitive mechanisms.

认知是一种强大的适应能力,它使动物能够利用不加操作就无法获得的资源。工具使用和食物加工就是利用认知克服食物资源保护机制的例子。在这里,我们描述并研究了一种合作繁殖的鸟类--阿拉伯狒狒(Argya squamiceps)--使用原工具(定义为通过物体-基质操作改变物体)处理食物的灵活性。实地观察表明,鸟类会将不同种类的毛虫搬运到不同的基质上,这取决于毛虫食用前所需的处理方法。带有毒性刚毛的毛虫(如 Casama innotata)会被运到粗糙的基质(如沙)上摩擦后再食用,而其他种类的毛虫(如 Hyles livornica)则会被运到坚硬的基质上敲打,直到去除内脏,只食用外部身体部分。这些结果是为数不多的描述野生动物利用灵活的原生工具加工食物的结果。因此,它们有助于在分类学上绘制非人类物种使用原型工具和加工食物的图谱,这对于推进这些行为及其潜在认知机制的进化比较研究来说是至关重要的一步。
{"title":"Proto-tool use for food processing in wild Arabian babblers: matching processing methods, substrates and prey types","authors":"Yitzchak Ben Mocha,&nbsp;Francesca Frisoni,&nbsp;Oded Keynan,&nbsp;Michael Griesser","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01866-6","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01866-6","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Cognition is a powerful adaptation, enabling animals to utilise resources that are unavailable without manipulation. Tool use and food processing are examples of using cognition to overcome the protective mechanisms of food resources. Here, we describe and examine the flexibility of proto-tool use (defined as the alteration of an object through object-substrate manipulation) for food processing in a cooperatively breeding bird, the Arabian babbler (<i>Argya squamiceps</i>). Field observations demonstrate that the birds transport different caterpillar species to different substrate types depending on the processing method needed to prepare the caterpillar for eating. Species with toxic setae (e.g. <i>Casama innotata</i>) are transported to be rubbed on rough substrates (e.g. sand) before consumption, while other species (e.g. <i>Hyles livornica</i>) are transported to be pounded against hard substrates until their inner organs are removed and only their external body part is consumed. These results are among the few to describe flexible proto-tool use for food processing in wild animals. They thereby contribute to the taxonomic mapping of proto-tool use and food processing in non-human species, which is a fundamental step to advance comparative studies on the evolution of these behaviours and their underlying cognitive mechanisms.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-04-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-024-01866-6.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140661308","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perception of vocoded speech in domestic dogs 家犬对语音编码的感知
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-16 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01869-3
Amritha Mallikarjun, Emily Shroads, Rochelle S. Newman

Humans have an impressive ability to comprehend signal-degraded speech; however, the extent to which comprehension of degraded speech relies on human-specific features of speech perception vs. more general cognitive processes is unknown. Since dogs live alongside humans and regularly hear speech, they can be used as a model to differentiate between these possibilities. One often-studied type of degraded speech is noise-vocoded speech (sometimes thought of as cochlear-implant-simulation speech). Noise-vocoded speech is made by dividing the speech signal into frequency bands (channels), identifying the amplitude envelope of each individual band, and then using these envelopes to modulate bands of noise centered over the same frequency regions – the result is a signal with preserved temporal cues, but vastly reduced frequency information. Here, we tested dogs’ recognition of familiar words produced in 16-channel vocoded speech. In the first study, dogs heard their names and unfamiliar dogs’ names (foils) in vocoded speech as well as natural speech. In the second study, dogs heard 16-channel vocoded speech only. Dogs listened longer to their vocoded name than vocoded foils in both experiments, showing that they can comprehend a 16-channel vocoded version of their name without prior exposure to vocoded speech, and without immediate exposure to the natural-speech version of their name. Dogs’ name recognition in the second study was mediated by the number of phonemes in the dogs’ name, suggesting that phonological context plays a role in degraded speech comprehension.

人类具有令人印象深刻的理解信号降级语音的能力;然而,对降级语音的理解在多大程度上依赖于人类特有的语音感知特征,而不是更普遍的认知过程,这一点尚不清楚。由于狗与人类生活在一起并经常听到语音,因此可以将狗作为模型来区分这些可能性。噪音编码语音(有时被认为是人工耳蜗模拟语音)是一种经常被研究的降级语音类型。噪声编码语音是通过将语音信号划分为不同的频段(信道),识别每个频段的振幅包络,然后使用这些包络对以相同频率区域为中心的噪声段进行调制--其结果是信号保留了时间线索,但频率信息却大大减少。在这里,我们测试了狗对 16 频道声码化语音中熟悉单词的识别能力。在第一项研究中,狗在声码语音和自然语音中听到了自己的名字和陌生狗的名字(陪衬)。在第二项研究中,狗只听到 16 频道的声码语音。在这两项实验中,狗听自己声码名字的时间都比听声码陪衬的时间长,这表明狗可以在没有事先接触声码语音的情况下理解 16 频道声码版本的名字,也可以在没有立即接触自然语音版本的名字的情况下理解自然语音版本的名字。在第二项研究中,狗的名字识别能力受狗名字中音素数量的影响,这表明语音语境在退化的语音理解能力中起着一定的作用。
{"title":"Perception of vocoded speech in domestic dogs","authors":"Amritha Mallikarjun,&nbsp;Emily Shroads,&nbsp;Rochelle S. Newman","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01869-3","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01869-3","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Humans have an impressive ability to comprehend signal-degraded speech; however, the extent to which comprehension of degraded speech relies on human-specific features of speech perception vs. more general cognitive processes is unknown. Since dogs live alongside humans and regularly hear speech, they can be used as a model to differentiate between these possibilities. One often-studied type of degraded speech is noise-vocoded speech (sometimes thought of as cochlear-implant-simulation speech). Noise-vocoded speech is made by dividing the speech signal into frequency bands (channels), identifying the amplitude envelope of each individual band, and then using these envelopes to modulate bands of noise centered over the same frequency regions – the result is a signal with preserved temporal cues, but vastly reduced frequency information. Here, we tested dogs’ recognition of familiar words produced in 16-channel vocoded speech. In the first study, dogs heard their names and unfamiliar dogs’ names (foils) in vocoded speech as well as natural speech. In the second study, dogs heard 16-channel vocoded speech only. Dogs listened longer to their vocoded name than vocoded foils in both experiments, showing that they can comprehend a 16-channel vocoded version of their name without prior exposure to vocoded speech, and without immediate exposure to the natural-speech version of their name. Dogs’ name recognition in the second study was mediated by the number of phonemes in the dogs’ name, suggesting that phonological context plays a role in degraded speech comprehension.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-04-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-024-01869-3.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140613761","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Figure-ground segmentation based on motion in the archerfish 基于箭鱼运动的图地分割
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-15 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01873-7
Svetlana Volotsky, Ronen Segev

Figure-ground segmentation is a fundamental process in visual perception that involves separating visual stimuli into distinct meaningful objects and their surrounding context, thus allowing the brain to interpret and understand complex visual scenes. Mammals exhibit varying figure-ground segmentation capabilities, ranging from primates that can perform well on figure-ground segmentation tasks to rodents that perform poorly. To explore figure-ground segmentation capabilities in teleost fish, we studied how the archerfish, an expert visual hunter, performs figure-ground segmentation. We trained archerfish to discriminate foreground objects from the background, where the figures were defined by motion as well as by discontinuities in intensity and texture. Specifically, the figures were defined by grating, naturalistic texture, and random noise moving in counterphase with the background. The archerfish performed the task well and could distinguish between all three types of figures and grounds. Their performance was comparable to that of primates and outperformed rodents. These findings suggest the existence of a complex visual process in the archerfish visual system that enables the delineation of figures as distinct from backgrounds, and provide insights into object recognition in this animal.

图地分割是视觉感知的一个基本过程,它涉及将视觉刺激物分割成不同的有意义物体及其周围环境,从而使大脑能够解释和理解复杂的视觉场景。哺乳动物的图地分割能力各不相同,灵长类动物在图地分割任务中表现良好,而啮齿类动物则表现较差。为了探索远洋鱼类的图地分割能力,我们研究了箭鱼这种视觉猎手是如何进行图地分割的。我们训练箭鱼从背景中分辨出前景物体,其中的图形是由运动以及强度和纹理的不连续性定义的。具体来说,人物是由光栅、自然纹理和与背景反相运动的随机噪音定义的。箭鱼很好地完成了任务,并能区分所有三种类型的图形和地面。它们的表现可与灵长类动物媲美,并优于啮齿类动物。这些研究结果表明,在箭鱼视觉系统中存在一个复杂的视觉过程,能够将图形与背景区分开来,并为这种动物的物体识别提供了启示。
{"title":"Figure-ground segmentation based on motion in the archerfish","authors":"Svetlana Volotsky,&nbsp;Ronen Segev","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01873-7","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01873-7","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Figure-ground segmentation is a fundamental process in visual perception that involves separating visual stimuli into distinct meaningful objects and their surrounding context, thus allowing the brain to interpret and understand complex visual scenes. Mammals exhibit varying figure-ground segmentation capabilities, ranging from primates that can perform well on figure-ground segmentation tasks to rodents that perform poorly. To explore figure-ground segmentation capabilities in teleost fish, we studied how the archerfish, an expert visual hunter, performs figure-ground segmentation. We trained archerfish to discriminate foreground objects from the background, where the figures were defined by motion as well as by discontinuities in intensity and texture. Specifically, the figures were defined by grating, naturalistic texture, and random noise moving in counterphase with the background. The archerfish performed the task well and could distinguish between all three types of figures and grounds. Their performance was comparable to that of primates and outperformed rodents. These findings suggest the existence of a complex visual process in the archerfish visual system that enables the delineation of figures as distinct from backgrounds, and provide insights into object recognition in this animal.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-04-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-024-01873-7.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140560448","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How to solve novel problems: the role of associative learning in problem-solving performance in wild great tits Parus major 如何解决新问题:联想学习在野生大山雀(Parus major)解决问题过程中的作用
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-12 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01872-8
Laure Cauchard, Pierre Bize, Blandine Doligez

Although problem-solving tasks are frequently used to assess innovative ability, the extent to which problem-solving performance reflects variation in cognitive skills has been rarely formally investigated. Using wild breeding great tits facing a new non-food motivated problem-solving task, we investigated the role of associative learning in finding the solution, compared to multiple other non-cognitive factors. We first examined the role of accuracy (the proportion of contacts made with the opening part of a string-pulling task), neophobia, exploration, activity, age, sex, body condition and participation time on the ability to solve the task. To highlight the effect of associative learning, we then compared accuracy between solvers and non-solvers, before and after the first cue to the solution (i.e., the first time they pulled the string opening the door). We finally compared accuracy over consecutive entrances for solvers. Using 884 observations from 788 great tits tested from 2010 to 2015, we showed that, prior to initial successful entrance, solvers were more accurate and more explorative than non-solvers, and that females were more likely to solve the task than males. The accuracy of solvers, but not of non-solvers, increased significantly after they had the opportunity to associate string pulling with the movement of the door, giving them a first cue to the task solution. The accuracy of solvers also increased over successive entrances. Our results demonstrate that variations in problem-solving performance primarily reflect inherent individual differences in associative learning, and are also to a lesser extent shaped by sex and exploratory behaviour.

尽管解决问题的任务经常被用来评估创新能力,但解决问题的表现在多大程度上反映了认知技能的变化却很少被正式研究。我们利用野生繁殖的大山雀面对一个新的非食物动机的问题解决任务,研究了联想学习在找到解决方案中的作用,以及与其他多种非认知因素的比较。我们首先研究了准确性(在拉绳子任务中与开口部分接触的比例)、恐新症、探索、活动、年龄、性别、身体状况和参与时间对解决任务能力的影响。为了突出联想学习的效果,我们比较了解题者和非解题者在第一次提示解题之前和之后(即他们第一次拉动绳子打开门)的准确性。最后,我们还比较了解题者连续开门的准确率。通过对2010年至2015年期间测试的788只大山雀的884次观察,我们发现,在首次成功进入之前,求解者比非求解者更准确、更善于探索,而且雌性比雄性更有可能完成任务。在有机会将拉绳与门的移动联系起来后,解题者的准确率显著提高,而非解题者的准确率则没有提高。解题者的准确率也随着连续的入口而提高。我们的研究结果表明,解决问题能力的差异主要反映了联想学习中固有的个体差异,在较小程度上也受性别和探索行为的影响。
{"title":"How to solve novel problems: the role of associative learning in problem-solving performance in wild great tits Parus major","authors":"Laure Cauchard,&nbsp;Pierre Bize,&nbsp;Blandine Doligez","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01872-8","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01872-8","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Although problem-solving tasks are frequently used to assess innovative ability, the extent to which problem-solving performance reflects variation in cognitive skills has been rarely formally investigated. Using wild breeding great tits facing a new non-food motivated problem-solving task, we investigated the role of associative learning in finding the solution, compared to multiple other non-cognitive factors. We first examined the role of accuracy (the proportion of contacts made with the opening part of a string-pulling task), neophobia, exploration, activity, age, sex, body condition and participation time on the ability to solve the task. To highlight the effect of associative learning, we then compared accuracy between solvers and non-solvers, before and after the first cue to the solution (i.e., the first time they pulled the string opening the door). We finally compared accuracy over consecutive entrances for solvers. Using 884 observations from 788 great tits tested from 2010 to 2015, we showed that, prior to initial successful entrance, solvers were more accurate and more explorative than non-solvers, and that females were more likely to solve the task than males. The accuracy of solvers, but not of non-solvers, increased significantly after they had the opportunity to associate string pulling with the movement of the door, giving them a first cue to the task solution. The accuracy of solvers also increased over successive entrances. Our results demonstrate that variations in problem-solving performance primarily reflect inherent individual differences in associative learning, and are also to a lesser extent shaped by sex and exploratory behaviour.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-04-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-024-01872-8.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140560372","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social demonstration of colour preference improves the learning of associated demonstrated actions 色彩偏好的社交演示可提高对相关演示动作的学习效果
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-09 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01865-7
Noam Zurek, Na’ama Aljadeff, Donya Khoury, Lucy M. Aplin, Arnon Lotem

We studied how different types of social demonstration improve house sparrows' (Passer domesticus) success in solving a foraging task that requires both operant learning (opening covers) and discrimination learning (preferring covers of the rewarding colour). We provided learners with either paired demonstration (of both cover opening and colour preference), action-only demonstration (of opening white covers only), or no demonstration (a companion bird eating without covers). We found that sparrows failed to learn the two tasks with no demonstration, and learned them best with a paired demonstration. Interestingly, the action of cover opening was learned faster with paired rather than action-only demonstration despite being equally demonstrated in both. We also found that only with paired demonstration, the speed of operant (action) learning was related to the demonstrator’s level of activity. Colour preference (i.e. discrimination learning) was eventually acquired by all sparrows that learned to open covers, even without social demonstration of colour preference. Thus, adding a demonstration of colour preference was actually more important for operant learning, possibly as a result of increasing the similarity between the demonstrated and the learned tasks, thereby increasing the learner’s attention to the actions of the demonstrator. Giving more attention to individuals in similar settings may be an adaptive strategy directing social learners to focus on ecologically relevant behaviours and on tasks that are likely to be learned successfully.

我们研究了不同类型的社会示范如何提高家雀(Passer domesticus)完成觅食任务的成功率,这项任务需要操作性学习(打开盖子)和辨别学习(偏好奖励颜色的盖子)。我们为学习者提供了配对示范(打开盖子和颜色偏好)、纯动作示范(只打开白色盖子)或无示范(同伴鸟在不盖盖子的情况下进食)。我们发现,麻雀在没有示范的情况下无法学会这两项任务,而在配对示范的情况下学得最好。有趣的是,在配对而非仅动作示范的情况下,麻雀学习打开盖子的动作更快,尽管两者的示范效果相同。我们还发现,只有在配对示范时,操作(动作)学习的速度才与示范者的活动水平有关。所有学会打开盖子的麻雀最终都会获得颜色偏好(即辨别学习),即使没有社会颜色偏好示范。因此,增加颜色偏好的示范对操作性学习实际上更为重要,这可能是由于增加了示范任务和学习任务之间的相似性,从而提高了学习者对示范者行为的关注。对相似环境中的个体给予更多关注可能是一种适应性策略,它引导社会学习者关注与生态相关的行为和有可能成功学会的任务。
{"title":"Social demonstration of colour preference improves the learning of associated demonstrated actions","authors":"Noam Zurek,&nbsp;Na’ama Aljadeff,&nbsp;Donya Khoury,&nbsp;Lucy M. Aplin,&nbsp;Arnon Lotem","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01865-7","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01865-7","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>We studied how different types of social demonstration improve house sparrows' (<i>Passer domesticus</i>) success in solving a foraging task that requires both operant learning (opening covers) and discrimination learning (preferring covers of the rewarding colour). We provided learners with either paired demonstration (of both cover opening and colour preference), action-only demonstration (of opening white covers only), or no demonstration (a companion bird eating without covers). We found that sparrows failed to learn the two tasks with no demonstration, and learned them best with a paired demonstration. Interestingly, the action of cover opening was learned faster with paired rather than action-only demonstration despite being equally demonstrated in both. We also found that only with paired demonstration, the speed of operant (action) learning was related to the demonstrator’s level of activity. Colour preference (i.e. discrimination learning) was eventually acquired by all sparrows that learned to open covers, even without social demonstration of colour preference. Thus, adding a demonstration of colour preference was actually more important for operant learning, possibly as a result of increasing the similarity between the demonstrated and the learned tasks, thereby increasing the learner’s attention to the actions of the demonstrator. Giving more attention to individuals in similar settings may be an adaptive strategy directing social learners to focus on ecologically relevant behaviours and on tasks that are likely to be learned successfully.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-04-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-024-01865-7.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140560773","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
No evidence tube entrapment distresses rodents in typical empathy tests 在典型的移情测试中,没有证据表明管子被夹住会使啮齿动物感到痛苦。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01863-9
Dwi Atmoko Agung Nugroho, Sri Kusrohmaniah, Emma Pilz, Clare Krikorian, David Kearns, Burton Slotnick, Maria Gomez, Alan Silberberg

In the first two experiments an empty tube open at one end was placed in different locations. Male hamsters, tested one at a time, tended to stay close to the tube or in it. During the first minute of the first 4 sessions of Experiment 3, the hamster was unrestrained. If it entered the tube, it was locked within the tube. If it did not enter the tube during the first min, it was placed in it, and the tube was locked. Fifteen min later, the tube was opened, and the hamster was unrestrained for a further 20 min. The tube remained open during Session 5. Hamsters spent more time near the tube than predicted by chance and continued to enter the tube although tube-occupancy duration did not differ from chance levels. In Experiment 4, male rats were tested in two groups: rats in one group had been previously trapped in a tube and rats in the other group allowed to freely explore the test space. For the first two min of each of four 20-min sessions, trapped-group subjects were permitted to move about the chamber unless they entered the tube. In that case, they were locked in for the remainder of the session. If, after two min, they did not enter the tube, they were locked in it for the remaining 18 min. Free rats were unrestricted in all sessions. In Session 5, when both groups were permitted to move freely in the chamber, trapped and free rats spent more time in and near the tube than predicted by chance. These data show tube restraint does not seem to distress either hamsters or rats.

在前两次实验中,一根一端开口的空管被放置在不同的位置。每次测试一只雄性仓鼠,雄性仓鼠倾向于靠近管子或呆在管子里。在实验 3 前 4 个环节的前一分钟,仓鼠是不受约束的。如果它进入管子,就会被锁在管子里。如果仓鼠在第一分钟内没有进入试管,则将其放入试管内,试管被锁定。15 分钟后,管子被打开,仓鼠在接下来的 20 分钟内不受约束。在第 5 个环节中,管子一直是打开的。仓鼠在管子附近逗留的时间比偶然预测的要长,并且继续进入管子,尽管管子占用时间与偶然水平没有差异。在实验 4 中,雄性大鼠被分成两组进行测试:一组的大鼠之前被困在管子里,另一组的大鼠被允许自由探索测试空间。在四个 20 分钟的测试过程中,每个测试过程的前两分钟,被困组受试者都可以在测试室中活动,除非他们进入管子。在这种情况下,他们会在剩余的时间里被关在里面。如果两分钟后它们没有进入管子,则会在剩余的 18 分钟内被关在管子里。自由大鼠在所有环节中都不受限制。在第 5 个环节中,当两组大鼠都被允许在室内自由活动时,被困大鼠和自由大鼠在管道内和管道附近花费的时间比偶然预测的要多。这些数据表明,束缚管似乎不会使仓鼠或大鼠感到不安。
{"title":"No evidence tube entrapment distresses rodents in typical empathy tests","authors":"Dwi Atmoko Agung Nugroho,&nbsp;Sri Kusrohmaniah,&nbsp;Emma Pilz,&nbsp;Clare Krikorian,&nbsp;David Kearns,&nbsp;Burton Slotnick,&nbsp;Maria Gomez,&nbsp;Alan Silberberg","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01863-9","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01863-9","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In the first two experiments an empty tube open at one end was placed in different locations. Male hamsters, tested one at a time, tended to stay close to the tube or in it. During the first minute of the first 4 sessions of Experiment 3, the hamster was unrestrained. If it entered the tube, it was locked within the tube. If it did not enter the tube during the first min, it was placed in it, and the tube was locked. Fifteen min later, the tube was opened, and the hamster was unrestrained for a further 20 min. The tube remained open during Session 5. Hamsters spent more time near the tube than predicted by chance and continued to enter the tube although tube-occupancy duration did not differ from chance levels. In Experiment 4, male rats were tested in two groups: rats in one group had been previously trapped in a tube and rats in the other group allowed to freely explore the test space. For the first two min of each of four 20-min sessions, trapped-group subjects were permitted to move about the chamber unless they entered the tube. In that case, they were locked in for the remainder of the session. If, after two min, they did not enter the tube, they were locked in it for the remaining 18 min. Free rats were unrestricted in all sessions. In Session 5, when both groups were permitted to move freely in the chamber, trapped and free rats spent more time in and near the tube than predicted by chance. These data show tube restraint does not seem to distress either hamsters or rats.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10984888/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140334443","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A preliminary analysis of the effect of individual differences on cognitive performance in young companion dogs 个体差异对幼年伴侣犬认知能力影响的初步分析。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01868-4
Jordan G. Smith, Sarah Krichbaum, Lane Montgomery, Emma Cox, Jeffrey S. Katz

Many factors influence cognitive performance in dogs, including breed, temperament, rearing history, and training. Studies in working dog populations have demonstrated age-related improvements in cognitive task performance across the first years of development. However, the effect of certain factors, such as age, sex, and temperament, on cognitive performance in puppies has yet to be evaluated in a more diverse population of companion dogs. In this study, companion dogs under 12 months of age were tested once on two tasks purported to measure aspects of executive function: the delayed-search task (DST) and the detour reversal task (DRT). Owners also filled out the Canine Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire (C-BARQ) to evaluate how temperament influenced task performance. Contrary to prior research, performance did not improve with age on either task. However, the lack of age effects was likely the result of small sample sizes and individual differences across other factors influencing performance. Specifically, temperament differences as measured by the C-BARQ subscales for nonsocial fear and excitability predicted task performance on the DST, but the effect of temperament on task performance differed between males and females. Excitability also predicted performance on the DRT, but the effect depended on the age of the dog. In addition, no correlations were observed between task measures, indicating a lack of construct validity. Overall, these findings provide a preliminary analysis of factors that appear to influence cognitive task performance in young companion dogs and highlight suggestions for future research evaluating the impact of individual differences on cognitive performance.

影响狗狗认知能力的因素有很多,包括品种、性情、饲养历史和训练。对工作犬群体的研究表明,在狗成长的最初几年中,认知能力的提高与年龄有关。然而,年龄、性别和性情等特定因素对幼犬认知能力的影响还有待于在更多样化的伴侣犬群体中进行评估。在这项研究中,我们对 12 个月以下的伴侣犬进行了一次测试,测试内容包括两项旨在测量执行功能的任务:延迟搜索任务(DST)和迂回逆转任务(DRT)。狗主人还填写了犬行为评估与研究问卷(C-BARQ),以评估性情对任务表现的影响。与之前的研究相反,在这两项任务中,犬的表现并没有随着年龄的增长而提高。然而,缺乏年龄效应很可能是由于样本量较小以及影响成绩的其他因素存在个体差异。具体来说,由 C-BARQ 中的非社会性恐惧和兴奋性子量表测量出的气质差异可以预测 DST 的任务表现,但气质对任务表现的影响在男性和女性之间存在差异。兴奋性也能预测DRT的成绩,但其影响取决于狗的年龄。此外,在任务测量之间没有观察到相关性,这表明缺乏建构效度。总之,这些研究结果初步分析了似乎影响年轻伴侣犬认知任务表现的因素,并强调了未来研究评估个体差异对认知表现影响的建议。
{"title":"A preliminary analysis of the effect of individual differences on cognitive performance in young companion dogs","authors":"Jordan G. Smith,&nbsp;Sarah Krichbaum,&nbsp;Lane Montgomery,&nbsp;Emma Cox,&nbsp;Jeffrey S. Katz","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01868-4","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01868-4","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Many factors influence cognitive performance in dogs, including breed, temperament, rearing history, and training. Studies in working dog populations have demonstrated age-related improvements in cognitive task performance across the first years of development. However, the effect of certain factors, such as age, sex, and temperament, on cognitive performance in puppies has yet to be evaluated in a more diverse population of companion dogs. In this study, companion dogs under 12 months of age were tested once on two tasks purported to measure aspects of executive function: the delayed-search task (DST) and the detour reversal task (DRT). Owners also filled out the Canine Behavioral Assessment and Research Questionnaire (C-BARQ) to evaluate how temperament influenced task performance. Contrary to prior research, performance did not improve with age on either task. However, the lack of age effects was likely the result of small sample sizes and individual differences across other factors influencing performance. Specifically, temperament differences as measured by the C-BARQ subscales for nonsocial fear and excitability predicted task performance on the DST, but the effect of temperament on task performance differed between males and females. Excitability also predicted performance on the DRT, but the effect depended on the age of the dog. In addition, no correlations were observed between task measures, indicating a lack of construct validity. Overall, these findings provide a preliminary analysis of factors that appear to influence cognitive task performance in young companion dogs and highlight suggestions for future research evaluating the impact of individual differences on cognitive performance.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10984887/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140334412","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do dogs preferentially encode the identity of the target object or the location of others’ actions? 狗是优先对目标对象的身份进行编码,还是优先对他人行动的位置进行编码?
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-30 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01870-w
Lucrezia Lonardo, Christoph J. Völter, Robert Hepach, Claus Lamm, Ludwig Huber

The ability to make sense of and predict others’ actions is foundational for many socio-cognitive abilities. Dogs (Canis familiaris) constitute interesting comparative models for the study of action perception due to their marked sensitivity to human actions. We tested companion dogs (N = 21) in two screen-based eye-tracking experiments, adopting a task previously used with human infants and apes, to assess which aspects of an agent’s action dogs consider relevant to the agent’s underlying intentions. An agent was shown repeatedly acting upon the same one of two objects, positioned in the same location. We then presented the objects in swapped locations and the agent approached the objects centrally (Experiment 1) or the old object in the new location or the new object in the old location (Experiment 2). Dogs’ anticipatory fixations and looking times did not reflect an expectation that agents should have continued approaching the same object nor the same location as witnessed during the brief familiarization phase; this contrasts with some findings with infants and apes, but aligns with findings in younger infants before they have sufficient motor experience with the observed action. However, dogs’ pupil dilation and latency to make an anticipatory fixation suggested that, if anything, dogs expected the agents to keep approaching the same location rather than the same object, and their looking times showed sensitivity to the animacy of the agents. We conclude that dogs, lacking motor experience with the observed actions of grasping or kicking performed by a human or inanimate agent, might interpret such actions as directed toward a specific location rather than a specific object. Future research will need to further probe the suitability of anticipatory looking as measure of dogs’ socio-cognitive abilities given differences between the visual systems of dogs and primates.

理解和预测他人行动的能力是许多社会认知能力的基础。由于狗(Canis familiaris)对人类行动具有明显的敏感性,因此构成了研究行动感知的有趣比较模型。我们在两个基于屏幕的眼动跟踪实验中对伴侣犬(21 只)进行了测试,采用了以前在人类婴儿和猿类身上使用过的任务,以评估狗认为代理人行动的哪些方面与代理人的潜在意图相关。实验中,我们反复展示一个代理对位于同一位置的两个物体中的同一个物体所做的动作。然后,我们将这两个物体放在不同的位置上,让狗在中心位置接近这两个物体(实验 1),或在新位置接近旧物体,或在旧位置接近新物体(实验 2)。狗的预期定点和注视时间并不反映它们期望行为主体在短暂的熟悉阶段继续接近相同的物体或相同的位置;这与婴儿和猿类的一些研究结果不同,但与年龄较小的婴儿在对观察到的动作有足够的运动经验之前的研究结果一致。然而,狗的瞳孔放大和做出预期固定动作的潜伏期表明,如果有的话,狗期望特工继续接近同一地点而不是同一物体,而且它们的注视时间显示了对特工动画的敏感性。我们的结论是,由于狗缺乏对人类或无生命物体所做的抓或踢动作的运动经验,它们可能会将这些动作解释为指向特定位置而非特定物体。鉴于狗的视觉系统与灵长类动物的视觉系统存在差异,未来的研究需要进一步探究狗的社会认知能力是否适合用期待性观察来衡量。
{"title":"Do dogs preferentially encode the identity of the target object or the location of others’ actions?","authors":"Lucrezia Lonardo,&nbsp;Christoph J. Völter,&nbsp;Robert Hepach,&nbsp;Claus Lamm,&nbsp;Ludwig Huber","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01870-w","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01870-w","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The ability to make sense of and predict others’ actions is foundational for many socio-cognitive abilities. Dogs (<i>Canis familiaris</i>) constitute interesting comparative models for the study of action perception due to their marked sensitivity to human actions. We tested companion dogs (<i>N</i> = 21) in two screen-based eye-tracking experiments, adopting a task previously used with human infants and apes, to assess which aspects of an agent’s action dogs consider relevant to the agent’s underlying intentions. An agent was shown repeatedly acting upon the same one of two objects, positioned in the same location. We then presented the objects in swapped locations and the agent approached the objects centrally (Experiment 1) or the old object in the new location or the new object in the old location (Experiment 2). Dogs’ anticipatory fixations and looking times did not reflect an expectation that agents should have continued approaching the same object nor the same location as witnessed during the brief familiarization phase; this contrasts with some findings with infants and apes, but aligns with findings in younger infants before they have sufficient motor experience with the observed action. However, dogs’ pupil dilation and latency to make an anticipatory fixation suggested that, if anything, dogs expected the agents to keep approaching the same location rather than the same object, and their looking times showed sensitivity to the animacy of the agents. We conclude that dogs, lacking motor experience with the observed actions of grasping or kicking performed by a human or inanimate agent, might interpret such actions as directed toward a specific location rather than a specific object. Future research will need to further probe the suitability of anticipatory looking as measure of dogs’ socio-cognitive abilities given differences between the visual systems of dogs and primates.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-03-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10980658/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140326262","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Inhibitory control in teleost fish: a methodological and conceptual review 远洋鱼类的抑制控制:方法和概念综述。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-03-26 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01867-5
Tyrone Lucon-Xiccato

Inhibitory control (IC) plays a central role in behaviour control allowing an individual to resist external lures and internal predispositions. While IC has been consistently investigated in humans, other mammals, and birds, research has only recently begun to explore IC in other vertebrates. This review examines current literature on teleost fish, focusing on both methodological and conceptual aspects. I describe the main paradigms adopted to study IC in fish, identifying well-established tasks that fit various research applications and highlighting their advantages and limitations. In the conceptual analysis, I identify two well-developed lines of research with fish examining IC. The first line focuses on a comparative approach aimed to describe IC at the level of species and to understand the evolution of interspecific differences in relation to ecological specialisation, brain size, and factors affecting cognitive performance. Findings suggest several similarities between fish and previously studied vertebrates. The second line of research focuses on intraspecific variability of IC. Available results indicate substantial variation in fish IC related to sex, personality, genetic, age, and phenotypic plasticity, aligning with what is observed with other vertebrates. Overall, this review suggests that although data on teleosts are still scarce compared to mammals, the contribution of this group to IC research is already substantial and can further increase in various disciplines including comparative psychology, cognitive ecology, and neurosciences, and even in applied fields such as psychiatry research.

抑制控制(IC)在行为控制中起着核心作用,它使个体能够抵御外部诱惑和内部倾向。虽然抑制控制在人类、其他哺乳动物和鸟类中得到了持续的研究,但对其他脊椎动物的研究直到最近才开始。这篇综述探讨了当前有关远志鱼类的文献,重点关注方法和概念两个方面。我描述了研究鱼类集成电路所采用的主要范式,确定了适合各种研究应用的成熟任务,并强调了它们的优势和局限性。在概念分析中,我确定了两条发展成熟的鱼类集成电路研究路线。第一条研究路线侧重于比较方法,目的是在物种水平上描述集成电路,并了解与生态特化、大脑大小和影响认知能力的因素有关的种间差异的演变。研究结果表明,鱼类与之前研究过的脊椎动物之间存在一些相似之处。第二个研究方向侧重于种内集成电路的变异。现有结果表明,鱼类的集成电路与性别、个性、遗传、年龄和表型可塑性有关,存在很大差异,这与在其他脊椎动物身上观察到的结果一致。总之,这篇综述表明,虽然与哺乳动物相比,有关远洋鱼类的数据仍然很少,但这一群体对集成电路研究的贡献已经很大,而且在比较心理学、认知生态学和神经科学等不同学科,甚至在精神病学研究等应用领域,还可以进一步增加。
{"title":"Inhibitory control in teleost fish: a methodological and conceptual review","authors":"Tyrone Lucon-Xiccato","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01867-5","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01867-5","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Inhibitory control (IC) plays a central role in behaviour control allowing an individual to resist external lures and internal predispositions. While IC has been consistently investigated in humans, other mammals, and birds, research has only recently begun to explore IC in other vertebrates. This review examines current literature on teleost fish, focusing on both methodological and conceptual aspects. I describe the main paradigms adopted to study IC in fish, identifying well-established tasks that fit various research applications and highlighting their advantages and limitations. In the conceptual analysis, I identify two well-developed lines of research with fish examining IC. The first line focuses on a comparative approach aimed to describe IC at the level of species and to understand the evolution of interspecific differences in relation to ecological specialisation, brain size, and factors affecting cognitive performance. Findings suggest several similarities between fish and previously studied vertebrates. The second line of research focuses on intraspecific variability of IC. Available results indicate substantial variation in fish IC related to sex, personality, genetic, age, and phenotypic plasticity, aligning with what is observed with other vertebrates. Overall, this review suggests that although data on teleosts are still scarce compared to mammals, the contribution of this group to IC research is already substantial and can further increase in various disciplines including comparative psychology, cognitive ecology, and neurosciences, and even in applied fields such as psychiatry research.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10965611/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140292550","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Animal Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1