首页 > 最新文献

Animal Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Does social motivation mitigate fear caused by a sudden sound in horses? 社会动机能减轻马突然发出的声音引起的恐惧吗?
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-07-14 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01805-x
Wiktoria Janicka, Izabela Wilk, Tomasz Próchniak

Living in a herd has multiple advantages for social species and is a primary survival strategy for prey. The presence of conspecifics, identified as a social buffer, may mitigate the individual stress response. Social isolation is, therefore, particularly stressful for horses, which are gregarious animals. However, they are not equally vulnerable to separation from the group. We tested whether more and less socially dependent horses and independent individuals would differ in their responses to novel and sudden sounds occurring in two contexts: non-social and social motivation. Twenty warmblood horses were first exposed to two social tests: to evaluate the level of social dependence (rate of restless behaviour; social isolation) and the quantity and the quality of interactions in which they were involved (stay on a paddock). Two fear audio tests were then performed to compare the responses to sudden sounds while feeding (non-social motivation; control trial) and while moving towards the herd (social motivation; experimental trial). Socially dependent horses showed more pronounced avoidance behaviour and needed much more time to resume feeding during the control trial. Hence, dependent individuals appeared to be more fearful. However, during an experimental trial, horses of both groups tended to ignore the sound or paid only limited attention to the stimulus, continuing to move forward towards their conspecifics. Thus, social motivation may mitigate fear caused by a frightening stimulus and make fearful and dependent horses more prone to face a potentially stressful event. This finding should be taken into account in horse training and management.

群居物种有多种优势,是猎物的主要生存策略。同种的存在,被认为是一种社会缓冲,可以减轻个体的压力反应。因此,社交孤立对马来说压力特别大,因为马是群居动物。然而,他们并非同样容易脱离群体。我们测试了越来越不依赖社会的马和独立的个体对两种情况下出现的新奇和突然的声音的反应是否会有所不同:非社会动机和社会动机。20匹温血马首先接受了两项社会测试:评估社会依赖程度(不安行为的比率;社会孤立)以及它们所参与的互动的数量和质量(待在围场上)。然后进行了两次恐惧音频测试,以比较喂食时(非社会动机;对照试验)和向牛群移动时(社会动机;实验试验)对突然声音的反应。在对照试验中,社交依赖的马表现出更明显的回避行为,需要更多的时间来恢复进食。因此,依赖他人的人似乎更害怕。然而,在一项实验试验中,两组马都倾向于忽视声音,或者只对刺激给予有限的关注,继续朝着它们的同类前进。因此,社会动机可以减轻由可怕的刺激引起的恐惧,并使恐惧和依赖的马更容易面临潜在的压力事件。这一发现应在马匹训练和管理中加以考虑。
{"title":"Does social motivation mitigate fear caused by a sudden sound in horses?","authors":"Wiktoria Janicka,&nbsp;Izabela Wilk,&nbsp;Tomasz Próchniak","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01805-x","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01805-x","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Living in a herd has multiple advantages for social species and is a primary survival strategy for prey. The presence of conspecifics, identified as a social buffer, may mitigate the individual stress response. Social isolation is, therefore, particularly stressful for horses, which are gregarious animals. However, they are not equally vulnerable to separation from the group. We tested whether more and less socially dependent horses and independent individuals would differ in their responses to novel and sudden sounds occurring in two contexts: non-social and social motivation. Twenty warmblood horses were first exposed to two social tests: to evaluate the level of social dependence (rate of restless behaviour; social isolation) and the quantity and the quality of interactions in which they were involved (stay on a paddock). Two fear audio tests were then performed to compare the responses to sudden sounds while feeding (non-social motivation; control trial) and while moving towards the herd (social motivation; experimental trial). Socially dependent horses showed more pronounced avoidance behaviour and needed much more time to resume feeding during the control trial. Hence, dependent individuals appeared to be more fearful. However, during an experimental trial, horses of both groups tended to ignore the sound or paid only limited attention to the stimulus, continuing to move forward towards their conspecifics. Thus, social motivation may mitigate fear caused by a frightening stimulus and make fearful and dependent horses more prone to face a potentially stressful event. This finding should be taken into account in horse training and management.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2023-07-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-023-01805-x.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10046580","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Injury shortens life expectancy in ants and affects some risk-related decisions of workers 伤害缩短了蚂蚁的预期寿命,并影响了工人的一些风险相关决策
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-07-14 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01810-0
Filip Turza, Krzysztof Miler

The Formica cinerea ants are known to be highly territorial and aggressively defend their nest and foraging areas against other ants. During the foraging, workers engage in large-scale battles with other colonies of ants and injuries often occur in the process. Such injuries open the body up to pathologies and can lead to costs expressed in lower survival. Here, we addressed the significance of injury in dictating decisions related to engagement in risky behavior in ants (i.e., rescue and aggression). We manipulated the life expectancies of F. cinerea workers by injury and found that the survival of injured workers was shorter compared to the intact individuals. Furthermore, we found that injured workers discriminated between the intact and injured nestmates and showed more rescue behavior toward intact individuals. These rescue actions were expressed as digging around the trapped ant in need of rescue, pulling at its body parts, transporting the sand covering it, and biting the thread entrapping it. In turn, intact and injured workers showed similar and high levels of aggression toward heterospecifics. Our findings highlight the role of behavioral context in the studies devoted to the decision-making processes among social insects and the importance of life expectancy in their behavioral patterns.

众所周知,灰蚁具有高度的领地性,并积极保护自己的巢穴和觅食区免受其他蚂蚁的攻击。在觅食过程中,工人们与其他蚂蚁群体进行大规模的战斗,在这个过程中经常会受伤。这种损伤会使身体出现病理,并可能导致生存率降低的成本。在这里,我们讨论了伤害在决定蚂蚁参与危险行为(即救援和攻击)相关决策中的重要性。我们通过伤害操纵了灰葡萄球菌工人的预期寿命,发现受伤工人的存活时间比完好工人更短。此外,我们发现受伤的工作人员区分了完整的和受伤的巢友,并对完整的个体表现出更多的救援行为。这些救援行动表现为在被困蚂蚁周围挖掘需要救援的蚂蚁,拉动它的身体部位,运送覆盖它的沙子,咬诱捕它的线。反过来,完好和受伤的工人对异性异性表现出相似且高度的攻击性。我们的研究结果强调了行为背景在社会昆虫决策过程研究中的作用,以及预期寿命在其行为模式中的重要性。
{"title":"Injury shortens life expectancy in ants and affects some risk-related decisions of workers","authors":"Filip Turza,&nbsp;Krzysztof Miler","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01810-0","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01810-0","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The <i>Formica cinerea</i> ants are known to be highly territorial and aggressively defend their nest and foraging areas against other ants. During the foraging, workers engage in large-scale battles with other colonies of ants and injuries often occur in the process. Such injuries open the body up to pathologies and can lead to costs expressed in lower survival. Here, we addressed the significance of injury in dictating decisions related to engagement in risky behavior in ants (i.e., rescue and aggression). We manipulated the life expectancies of <i>F. cinerea</i> workers by injury and found that the survival of injured workers was shorter compared to the intact individuals. Furthermore, we found that injured workers discriminated between the intact and injured nestmates and showed more rescue behavior toward intact individuals. These rescue actions were expressed as digging around the trapped ant in need of rescue, pulling at its body parts, transporting the sand covering it, and biting the thread entrapping it. In turn, intact and injured workers showed similar and high levels of aggression toward heterospecifics. Our findings highlight the role of behavioral context in the studies devoted to the decision-making processes among social insects and the importance of life expectancy in their behavioral patterns.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2023-07-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-023-01810-0.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10053161","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social information facilitates learning about novel food sources in adult flower-visiting bats 社会信息有助于了解成年访花蝙蝠的新食物来源
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-07-08 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01807-9
Andreas Rose, Marco Tschapka, Mirjam Knörnschild

Incorporating novel food sources into their diet is crucial for animals in changing environments. Although the utilization of novel food sources can be learned individually, learning socially from experienced conspecifics may facilitate this task and enable a transmission of foraging-related innovations across a population. In anthropogenically modified habitats, bats (Mammalia: Chiroptera) frequently adapt their feeding strategy to novel food sources, and corresponding social learning processes have been experimentally demonstrated in frugivorous and animalivorous species. However, comparable experiments are lacking for nectarivorous flower-visiting bats, even though their utilization of novel food sources in anthropogenically altered habitats is often observed and even discussed as the reason why bats are able to live in some areas. In the present study, we investigated whether adult flower-visiting bats may benefit from social information when learning about a novel food source. We conducted a demonstrator–observer dyad with wild Pallas’ long-tongued bats (Glossophaga soricina; Phyllostomidae: Glossophaginae) and hypothesized that naïve individuals would learn to exploit a novel food source faster when accompanied by an experienced demonstrator bat. Our results support this hypothesis and demonstrate flower-visiting bats to be capable of using social information to expand their dietary repertoire.

在不断变化的环境中,将新的食物来源纳入它们的饮食对动物来说至关重要。尽管新食物来源的利用可以单独学习,但从经验丰富的同种动物那里进行社会学习可能会促进这项任务,并使觅食相关的创新能够在人群中传播。在人类改造的栖息地,蝙蝠(哺乳动物:翼手目)经常调整它们的进食策略以适应新的食物来源,相应的社会学习过程已在食草和嗜动物物种中得到实验证明。然而,对于以蜜腺为食的访花蝙蝠来说,缺乏可比的实验,尽管人们经常观察到它们在人类成因改变的栖息地中利用新的食物来源,甚至将其作为蝙蝠能够生活在某些地区的原因进行讨论。在本研究中,我们调查了成年访花蝙蝠在了解新的食物来源时是否会从社会信息中受益。我们与野生帕拉斯长舌蝙蝠(舌苔科;叶藻科:舌苔科)进行了一次演示者-观察者二人组,并假设天真的个体在有经验的演示蝙蝠的陪伴下会更快地学会利用新的食物来源。我们的研究结果支持了这一假设,并证明访花蝙蝠能够利用社会信息来扩大它们的饮食范围。
{"title":"Social information facilitates learning about novel food sources in adult flower-visiting bats","authors":"Andreas Rose,&nbsp;Marco Tschapka,&nbsp;Mirjam Knörnschild","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01807-9","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01807-9","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Incorporating novel food sources into their diet is crucial for animals in changing environments. Although the utilization of novel food sources can be learned individually, learning socially from experienced conspecifics may facilitate this task and enable a transmission of foraging-related innovations across a population. In anthropogenically modified habitats, bats (Mammalia: Chiroptera) frequently adapt their feeding strategy to novel food sources, and corresponding social learning processes have been experimentally demonstrated in frugivorous and animalivorous species. However, comparable experiments are lacking for nectarivorous flower-visiting bats, even though their utilization of novel food sources in anthropogenically altered habitats is often observed and even discussed as the reason why bats are able to live in some areas. In the present study, we investigated whether adult flower-visiting bats may benefit from social information when learning about a novel food source. We conducted a demonstrator–observer dyad with wild Pallas’ long-tongued bats (<i>Glossophaga soricina</i>; Phyllostomidae: Glossophaginae) and hypothesized that naïve individuals would learn to exploit a novel food source faster when accompanied by an experienced demonstrator bat. Our results support this hypothesis and demonstrate flower-visiting bats to be capable of using social information to expand their dietary repertoire.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2023-07-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-023-01807-9.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10055647","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Llamas use social information from conspecifics and humans to solve a spatial detour task Llamas利用来自同种和人类的社会信息来解决空间绕行任务
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-07-06 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01808-8
Annkatrin Pahl, Uta König von Borstel, Désirée Brucks

Learning by observing others (i.e. social learning) is an important mechanism to reduce the costs of individual learning. Social learning can occur between conspecifics but also heterospecifics. Domestication processes might have changed the animals’ sensitivity to human social cues and recent research indicates that domesticated species are particularly good in learning socially from humans. Llamas (Lama glama) are an interesting model species for that purpose. Llamas were bred as pack animals, which requires close contact and cooperative behaviour towards humans. We investigated whether llamas learn socially from trained conspecifics and humans in a spatial detour task. Subjects were required to detour metal hurdles arranged in a V-shape to reach a food reward. Llamas were more successful in solving the task after both a human and a conspecific demonstrated the task compared to a control condition with no demonstrator. Individual differences in behaviour (i.e. food motivation and distraction) further affected the success rate. Animals did not necessarily use the same route as the demonstrators, thus, indicating that they adopted a more general detour behaviour. These results suggest that llamas can extract information from conspecific and heterospecific demonstrations; hence, broadening our knowledge of domesticated species that are sensitive to human social behaviour.

通过观察他人进行学习(即社会学习)是降低个人学习成本的重要机制。社会学习可以发生在同种之间,也可以发生在异性之间。驯化过程可能改变了动物对人类社会线索的敏感性,最近的研究表明,驯化物种特别善于向人类进行社会学习。拉玛是一种有趣的模式物种。骆驼是作为驮畜饲养的,这需要与人类密切接触和合作。我们调查了美洲驼是否在空间迂回任务中从受过训练的同种动物和人类那里进行社会学习。受试者被要求绕过排列成V形的金属栏,以获得食物奖励。与没有演示者的对照条件相比,在人类和同种动物都演示了任务后,Llamas在解决任务方面更成功。行为上的个体差异(即食物动机和注意力分散)进一步影响了成功率。因此,动物不一定使用与示威者相同的路线,这表明它们采取了更普遍的绕行行为。这些结果表明,美洲驼可以从同种和异性的展示中提取信息;因此,拓宽了我们对人类社会行为敏感的驯化物种的了解。
{"title":"Llamas use social information from conspecifics and humans to solve a spatial detour task","authors":"Annkatrin Pahl,&nbsp;Uta König von Borstel,&nbsp;Désirée Brucks","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01808-8","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01808-8","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Learning by observing others (i.e. social learning) is an important mechanism to reduce the costs of individual learning. Social learning can occur between conspecifics but also heterospecifics. Domestication processes might have changed the animals’ sensitivity to human social cues and recent research indicates that domesticated species are particularly good in learning socially from humans. Llamas (<i>Lama glama</i>) are an interesting model species for that purpose. Llamas were bred as pack animals, which requires close contact and cooperative behaviour towards humans. We investigated whether llamas learn socially from trained conspecifics and humans in a spatial detour task. Subjects were required to detour metal hurdles arranged in a V-shape to reach a food reward. Llamas were more successful in solving the task after both a human and a conspecific demonstrated the task compared to a control condition with no demonstrator. Individual differences in behaviour (i.e. food motivation and distraction) further affected the success rate. Animals did not necessarily use the same route as the demonstrators, thus, indicating that they adopted a more general detour behaviour. These results suggest that llamas can extract information from conspecific and heterospecific demonstrations; hence, broadening our knowledge of domesticated species that are sensitive to human social behaviour.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2023-07-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-023-01808-8.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10112325","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Penguins perceive variations of source- and filter-related vocal parameters of species-specific vocalisations 企鹅感知特定物种发声的源和滤器相关发声参数的变化
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-07-04 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01806-w
Francesca Terranova, Luigi Baciadonna, Chiara Maccarone, Valentina Isaja, Marco Gamba, Livio Favaro

Animal vocalisations encode a wide range of biological information about the age, sex, body size, and social status of the emitter. Moreover, vocalisations play a significant role in signalling the identity of the emitter to conspecifics. Recent studies have shown that, in the African penguin (Spheniscus demersus), acoustic cues to individual identity are encoded in the fundamental frequency (F0) and resonance frequencies (formants) of the vocal tract. However, although penguins are known to produce vocalisations where F0 and formants vary among individuals, it remains to be tested whether the receivers can perceive and use such information in the individual recognition process. In this study, using the Habituation-Dishabituation (HD) paradigm, we tested the hypothesis that penguins perceive and respond to a shift of ± 20% (corresponding to the natural inter-individual variation observed in ex-situ colonies) of F0 and formant dispersion (ΔF) of species-specific calls. We found that penguins were more likely to look rapidly and for longer at the source of the sound when F0 and formants of the calls were manipulated, indicating that they could perceive variations of these parameters in the vocal signals. Our findings provide the first experimental evidence that, in the African penguin, listeners can perceive changes in F0 and formants, which can be used by the receiver as potential cues for the individual discrimination of the emitter.

动物的发声编码了关于发射器的年龄、性别、体型和社会地位的广泛生物信息。此外,发声在向同种发出发射器身份信号方面发挥着重要作用。最近的研究表明,在非洲企鹅中,个体身份的声学线索编码在声道的基频(F0)和共振频率(共振峰)中。然而,尽管已知企鹅会发出F0和共振峰因个体而异的声音,但受体能否在个体识别过程中感知和使用这些信息仍有待测试。在这项研究中,我们使用习惯化-去习惯化(HD)范式,检验了企鹅感知并对 ± 物种特异性呼叫的F0和共振峰分散度(ΔF)的20%(对应于在离地菌落中观察到的自然个体间变异)。我们发现,当F0和叫声的共振峰被操纵时,企鹅更有可能快速、更长时间地观察声音的来源,这表明它们可以感知声音信号中这些参数的变化。我们的发现提供了第一个实验证据,证明在非洲企鹅身上,听众可以感知F0和共振峰的变化,这可以被接受者用作个体辨别发射器的潜在线索。
{"title":"Penguins perceive variations of source- and filter-related vocal parameters of species-specific vocalisations","authors":"Francesca Terranova,&nbsp;Luigi Baciadonna,&nbsp;Chiara Maccarone,&nbsp;Valentina Isaja,&nbsp;Marco Gamba,&nbsp;Livio Favaro","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01806-w","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01806-w","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Animal vocalisations encode a wide range of biological information about the age, sex, body size, and social status of the emitter. Moreover, vocalisations play a significant role in signalling the identity of the emitter to conspecifics. Recent studies have shown that, in the African penguin (<i>Spheniscus demersus</i>), acoustic cues to individual identity are encoded in the fundamental frequency (<i>F</i><sub>0</sub>) and resonance frequencies (formants) of the vocal tract. However, although penguins are known to produce vocalisations where <i>F</i><sub>0</sub> and formants vary among individuals, it remains to be tested whether the receivers can perceive and use such information in the individual recognition process. In this study, using the Habituation-Dishabituation (HD) paradigm, we tested the hypothesis that penguins perceive and respond to a shift of ± 20% (corresponding to the natural inter-individual variation observed in ex-situ colonies) of <i>F</i><sub>0</sub> and formant dispersion (ΔF) of species-specific calls. We found that penguins were more likely to look rapidly and for longer at the source of the sound when <i>F</i><sub>0</sub> and formants of the calls were manipulated, indicating that they could perceive variations of these parameters in the vocal signals. Our findings provide the first experimental evidence that, in the African penguin, listeners can perceive changes in <i>F</i><sub>0</sub> and formants, which can be used by the receiver as potential cues for the individual discrimination of the emitter.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2023-07-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-023-01806-w.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10430711","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Inter-group alliance dynamics in Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus) 印度-太平洋宽吻海豚群体间联盟动态
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-06-30 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01804-y
Whitney R. Friedman, Michael Krützen, Stephanie L. King, Simon J. Allen, Livia Gerber, Samuel Wittwer, Richard C. Connor

The social intelligence hypothesis holds that complex social relationships are the major selective force underlying the evolution of large brain size and intelligence. Complex social relationships are exemplified by coalitions and alliances that are mediated by affiliative behavior, resulting in differentiated but shifting relationships. Male Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins in Shark Bay, Australia, form three alliance levels or ‘orders’, primarily among non-relatives. Strategic alliance formation has been documented within both first- and second-order alliances and between second-order alliances (‘third-order alliances’), revealing that the formation of strategic inter-group alliances is not limited to humans. Here we conducted a fine-scale study on 22 adult males over a 6-year period to determine if third-order alliance relationships are differentiated, and mediated by affiliative interactions. We found third-order alliance relationships were strongly differentiated, with key individuals playing a disproportionate role in maintaining alliances. Nonetheless, affiliative interactions occurred broadly between third-order allies, indicating males maintain bonds with third-order allies of varying strength. We also documented a shift in relationships and formation of a new third-order alliance. These findings further our understanding of dolphin alliance dynamics and provide evidence that strategic alliance formation is found in all three alliance levels, a phenomenon with no peer among non-human animals.

社会智力假说认为,复杂的社会关系是大大脑大小和智力进化的主要选择力。复杂的社会关系以联盟和联盟为例,这些联盟和联盟由附属行为介导,导致有差异但不断变化的关系。澳大利亚鲨鱼湾的雄性印太宽吻海豚形成了三个联盟级别或“订单”,主要是在非亲属中。战略联盟的形成已被记录在一阶和二阶联盟内以及二阶联盟之间(“三阶联盟”),这表明战略集团间联盟的形成并不局限于人类。在这里,我们在6年的时间里对22名成年男性进行了一项精细的研究,以确定三阶联盟关系是否是有区别的,并由附属互动介导。我们发现,三阶联盟关系差异很大,关键个人在维持联盟方面发挥着不成比例的作用。尽管如此,三阶盟友之间普遍存在亲密互动,这表明雄性与实力不同的三阶盟友保持着联系。我们还记录了关系的转变和新的三阶联盟的形成。这些发现进一步加深了我们对海豚联盟动态的理解,并提供了证据,证明战略联盟的形成在所有三个联盟层面上都存在,这是非人类动物中没有同类的现象。
{"title":"Inter-group alliance dynamics in Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus)","authors":"Whitney R. Friedman,&nbsp;Michael Krützen,&nbsp;Stephanie L. King,&nbsp;Simon J. Allen,&nbsp;Livia Gerber,&nbsp;Samuel Wittwer,&nbsp;Richard C. Connor","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01804-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01804-y","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The social intelligence hypothesis holds that complex social relationships are the major selective force underlying the evolution of large brain size and intelligence. Complex social relationships are exemplified by coalitions and alliances that are mediated by affiliative behavior, resulting in differentiated but shifting relationships. Male Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins in Shark Bay, Australia, form three alliance levels or ‘orders’, primarily among non-relatives. Strategic alliance formation has been documented <i>within</i> both first- and second-order alliances and <i>between</i> second-order alliances (‘third-order alliances’), revealing that the formation of strategic inter-group alliances is not limited to humans. Here we conducted a fine-scale study on 22 adult males over a 6-year period to determine if third-order alliance relationships are differentiated, and mediated by affiliative interactions. We found third-order alliance relationships were strongly differentiated, with key individuals playing a disproportionate role in maintaining alliances. Nonetheless, affiliative interactions occurred broadly between third-order allies, indicating males maintain bonds with third-order allies of varying strength. We also documented a shift in relationships and formation of a new third-order alliance. These findings further our understanding of dolphin alliance dynamics and provide evidence that strategic alliance formation is found in all three alliance levels, a phenomenon with no peer among non-human animals.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2023-06-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-023-01804-y.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10053141","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction: Two-action task, testing imitative social learning in kea (Nestor notabilis) 更正:两项行动任务,测试kea中的模仿社会学习(Nestor notabilis)
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-06-21 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01803-z
Elisabeth Suwandschieff, Amelia Wein, Remco Folkertsma, Thomas Bugnyar, Ludwig Huber, Raoul Schwing
{"title":"Correction: Two-action task, testing imitative social learning in kea (Nestor notabilis)","authors":"Elisabeth Suwandschieff,&nbsp;Amelia Wein,&nbsp;Remco Folkertsma,&nbsp;Thomas Bugnyar,&nbsp;Ludwig Huber,&nbsp;Raoul Schwing","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01803-z","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01803-z","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2023-06-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-023-01803-z.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9801964","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Grey wolves (Canis lupus) discriminate between familiar and unfamiliar human voices 灰狼区分熟悉和陌生的人声
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-06-20 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01796-9
Beatrice Gammino, Vicente Palacios, Holly Root-Gutteridge, David Reby, Marco Gamba

The ability to discriminate between different individuals based on identity cues, which is important to support the social behaviour of many animal species, has mostly been investigated in conspecific contexts. A rare example of individual heterospecific discrimination is found in domestic dogs, who are capable of recognising their owners’ voices. Here, we test whether grey wolves, the nearest wild relative of dogs, also have the ability to distinguish familiar human voices, which would indicate that dogs’ ability is not a consequence of domestication. Using the habituation–dishabituation paradigm, we presented captive wolves with playback recordings of their keepers’ and strangers’ voices producing either familiar or unfamiliar phrases. The duration of their response was significantly longer when presented with keepers’ voices than with strangers’ voices, demonstrating that wolves discriminated between familiar and unfamiliar speakers. This suggests that dogs’ ability to discriminate between human voices was probably present in their common ancestor and may support the idea that this is a general ability of vertebrates to recognise heterospecific individuals. Our study also provides further evidence for familiar voice discrimination in a wild animal in captivity, indicating that this ability may be widespread across vertebrate species.

基于身份线索区分不同个体的能力,对支持许多动物物种的社会行为很重要,主要是在同种环境中进行研究的。在家养狗身上发现了一个罕见的个体异性歧视的例子,它们能够识别主人的声音。在这里,我们测试了灰狼,狗的近亲,是否也有能力分辨熟悉的人声,这表明狗的能力不是驯化的结果。使用习惯化-去习惯化范式,我们向圈养的狼提供了饲养员和陌生人声音的回放记录,这些声音产生了熟悉或陌生的短语。当用饲养员的声音呈现时,它们的反应持续时间明显长于陌生人的声音,这表明狼区分熟悉和不熟悉的说话者。这表明,狗区分人声的能力可能存在于它们的共同祖先身上,这可能支持脊椎动物识别异性个体的一般能力的观点。我们的研究还为圈养野生动物熟悉的声音辨别提供了进一步的证据,表明这种能力可能在脊椎动物中广泛存在。
{"title":"Grey wolves (Canis lupus) discriminate between familiar and unfamiliar human voices","authors":"Beatrice Gammino,&nbsp;Vicente Palacios,&nbsp;Holly Root-Gutteridge,&nbsp;David Reby,&nbsp;Marco Gamba","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01796-9","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01796-9","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The ability to discriminate between different individuals based on identity cues, which is important to support the social behaviour of many animal species, has mostly been investigated in conspecific contexts. A rare example of individual heterospecific discrimination is found in domestic dogs, who are capable of recognising their owners’ voices. Here, we test whether grey wolves, the nearest wild relative of dogs, also have the ability to distinguish familiar human voices, which would indicate that dogs’ ability is not a consequence of domestication. Using the habituation–dishabituation paradigm, we presented captive wolves with playback recordings of their keepers’ and strangers’ voices producing either familiar or unfamiliar phrases. The duration of their response was significantly longer when presented with keepers’ voices than with strangers’ voices, demonstrating that wolves discriminated between familiar and unfamiliar speakers. This suggests that dogs’ ability to discriminate between human voices was probably present in their common ancestor and may support the idea that this is a general ability of vertebrates to recognise heterospecific individuals. Our study also provides further evidence for familiar voice discrimination in a wild animal in captivity, indicating that this ability may be widespread across vertebrate species.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2023-06-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-023-01796-9.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10036530","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Learning and organization of within-session sequences by pigeons (Columba livia) 鸽子学习和组织会话内序列(Columba livia)
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-06-19 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01801-1
Muhammad A. J. Qadri, Robert G. Cook

Most animals engage in complex activities that are the combination of simpler actions expressed over a period of time. The mechanisms organizing such sequential behavior have been of long-standing biological and psychological interest. Previously, we observed pigeons’ anticipatory behavior with a within-session sequence involving four choice alternatives suggestive of a potential understanding of the overall order and sequence of the items within a session. In that task, each colored alternative was correct for 24 consecutive trials as presented in a predictable sequence (i.e., A first, then B, then C, then D). To test whether these four already-trained pigeons possessed a sequential and linked representation of the ABCD items, we added a second four-item sequence involving new and distinct colored choice alternatives (i.e., E first for 24 trials, then F, then G, then H) and then alternated these ABCD and EFGH sequences over successive sessions. Over three manipulations, we tested and trained trials composed of combinations of elements from both sequences. We determined that pigeons did not learn any within-sequence associations among the elements. Despite the availability and explicit utility of such sequence cues, the data suggest instead that pigeons learned the discrimination tasks as a series of temporal associations among independent elements. This absence of any sequential linkage is consistent with the hypothesis that such representations are difficult to form in pigeons. This pattern of data suggests that for repeated sequential activities in birds, and potentially other animals including humans, there are highly effective, but underappreciated, clock-like mechanisms that control the ordering of behaviors.

大多数动物从事复杂的活动,这些活动是在一段时间内表达的简单动作的组合。组织这种连续行为的机制一直受到生物学和心理学的关注。此前,我们观察到鸽子的预期行为,会话内序列包括四个选择选项,这暗示了对会话内项目的整体顺序和序列的潜在理解。在该任务中,按照可预测的顺序(即,首先是a,然后是B,然后是C,然后是D),每个彩色替代品在24个连续试验中都是正确的。为了测试这四只已经训练过的鸽子是否拥有ABCD项目的顺序和链接表示,我们添加了第二个四项目序列,涉及新的和不同颜色的选择选项(即,24次试验中首先是e,然后是F,然后是G,然后是H),然后在连续的试验中交替使用ABCD和EFGH序列。在三次操作中,我们测试并训练了由两个序列的元素组合组成的试验。我们确定鸽子没有学习到任何元素之间的序列内关联。尽管这种序列线索的可用性和明确的实用性,但数据表明,鸽子学习辨别任务是作为独立元素之间的一系列时间关联。这种没有任何顺序联系的情况与鸽子很难形成这种表征的假设是一致的。这种数据模式表明,对于鸟类以及可能包括人类在内的其他动物的重复顺序活动,有非常有效但未被充分重视的类似时钟的机制来控制行为的顺序。
{"title":"Learning and organization of within-session sequences by pigeons (Columba livia)","authors":"Muhammad A. J. Qadri,&nbsp;Robert G. Cook","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01801-1","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01801-1","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Most animals engage in complex activities that are the combination of simpler actions expressed over a period of time. The mechanisms organizing such sequential behavior have been of long-standing biological and psychological interest. Previously, we observed pigeons’ anticipatory behavior with a within-session sequence involving four choice alternatives suggestive of a potential understanding of the overall order and sequence of the items within a session. In that task, each colored alternative was correct for 24 consecutive trials as presented in a predictable sequence (i.e., A first, then B, then C, then D). To test whether these four already-trained pigeons possessed a sequential and linked representation of the ABCD items, we added a second four-item sequence involving new and distinct colored choice alternatives (i.e., E first for 24 trials, then F, then G, then H) and then alternated these ABCD and EFGH sequences over successive sessions. Over three manipulations, we tested and trained trials composed of combinations of elements from both sequences. We determined that pigeons did not learn any within-sequence associations among the elements. Despite the availability and explicit utility of such sequence cues, the data suggest instead that pigeons learned the discrimination tasks as a series of temporal associations among independent elements. This absence of any sequential linkage is consistent with the hypothesis that such representations are difficult to form in pigeons. This pattern of data suggests that for repeated sequential activities in birds, and potentially other animals including humans, there are highly effective, but underappreciated, clock-like mechanisms that control the ordering of behaviors.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2023-06-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-023-01801-1.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10034508","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Left or right, that is the question: use of egocentric frame of reference and the right-eye advantage for understanding gestural signs in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) 问题是左还是右:使用以自我为中心的参照系和右眼优势来理解宽吻海豚的手势
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-06-15 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01799-6
Masaki Tomonaga, Yuka Uwano-Ito, Toyoshi Saito, Natsuko Sakurai

How do bottlenose dolphins visually perceive the space around them? In particular, what cues do they use as a frame of reference for left–right perception? To address this question, we examined the dolphin's responses to various manipulations of the spatial relationship between the dolphin and the trainer by using gestural signs for actions given by the trainer, which have different meanings in the left and right hands. When the dolphins were tested with their backs to the trainer (Experiment 1) or in an inverted position underwater (Experiments 2 and 3), correct responses from the trainer's perspective were maintained for signs related to movement direction instructions. In contrast, reversed responses were frequently observed for signs that required different sounds for the left and right hands. When the movement direction instructions were presented with symmetrical graphic signs such as " × " and "●", accuracy decreased in the inverted posture (Experiment 3). Furthermore, when the signs for sounds were presented from either the left or right side of the dolphin's body, performance was better when the side of the sign movement coincided with the body side on which it was presented than when it was mismatched (Experiment 4). In the final experiment, when one eye was covered with an eyecup, the results showed that, as in the case of body-side presentation, performance was better when the open eye coincided with the side on which the sign movement was presented. These results indicate that dolphins used the egocentric frame for visuospatial cognition. In addition, they showed better performances when the gestural signs were presented to the right eye, suggesting the possibility of a left-hemispheric advantage in the dolphin's visuospatial cognition.

宽吻海豚是如何从视觉上感知周围的空间的?特别是,他们使用什么线索作为左右感知的参考框架?为了解决这个问题,我们研究了海豚对海豚和驯兽师之间空间关系的各种操作的反应,方法是使用驯兽师给出的动作的手势,这些手势在左手和右手中具有不同的含义。当海豚背对训练者进行测试(实验1)或在水下倒立(实验2和3)时,从训练者的角度来看,与运动方向指示相关的迹象保持了正确的反应。相反,对于需要左手和右手发出不同声音的体征,经常观察到相反的反应。当运动方向指示带有对称的图形标志时,如“ × “和”●“,倒立姿势的准确性下降(实验3)。此外,当声音的手势从海豚身体的左侧或右侧呈现时,手势运动的一侧与呈现的身体一侧重合时的表现比不匹配时要好(实验4)在最后的实验中,当一只眼睛被眼罩覆盖时,结果表明,与身体侧面呈现的情况一样,当睁开的眼睛与呈现手势运动的一侧重合时,表现更好。这些结果表明,海豚使用以自我为中心的视觉空间认知框架。此外,当手势出现在右眼时,它们表现出更好的表现,这表明在海豚的视觉空间认知中,左半球可能具有优势。
{"title":"Left or right, that is the question: use of egocentric frame of reference and the right-eye advantage for understanding gestural signs in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)","authors":"Masaki Tomonaga,&nbsp;Yuka Uwano-Ito,&nbsp;Toyoshi Saito,&nbsp;Natsuko Sakurai","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01799-6","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01799-6","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>How do bottlenose dolphins visually perceive the space around them? In particular, what cues do they use as a frame of reference for left–right perception? To address this question, we examined the dolphin's responses to various manipulations of the spatial relationship between the dolphin and the trainer by using gestural signs for actions given by the trainer, which have different meanings in the left and right hands. When the dolphins were tested with their backs to the trainer (Experiment 1) or in an inverted position underwater (Experiments 2 and 3), correct responses from the trainer's perspective were maintained for signs related to movement direction instructions. In contrast, reversed responses were frequently observed for signs that required different sounds for the left and right hands. When the movement direction instructions were presented with symmetrical graphic signs such as \" × \" and \"●\", accuracy decreased in the inverted posture (Experiment 3). Furthermore, when the signs for sounds were presented from either the left or right side of the dolphin's body, performance was better when the side of the sign movement coincided with the body side on which it was presented than when it was mismatched (Experiment 4). In the final experiment, when one eye was covered with an eyecup, the results showed that, as in the case of body-side presentation, performance was better when the open eye coincided with the side on which the sign movement was presented. These results indicate that dolphins used the egocentric frame for visuospatial cognition. In addition, they showed better performances when the gestural signs were presented to the right eye, suggesting the possibility of a left-hemispheric advantage in the dolphin's visuospatial cognition.\u0000</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2023-06-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-023-01799-6.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10034499","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Animal Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1