首页 > 最新文献

Animal Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
First acoustic evidence of signature whistle production by spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris) 飞旋海豚(Stenella longirostris)发出特征性哨声的首个声学证据。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-07 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01824-8
Raul Rio

A dolphin’s signature whistle (SW) is a distinctive acoustic signal, issued in a bout pattern of unique frequency modulation contours; it allows individuals belonging to a given group to recognize each other and, consequently, to maintain contact and cohesion. The current study is the first scientific evidence that spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris) produce SWs. Acoustic data were recorded at a shallow rest bay called “Biboca”, in Fernando de Noronha Archipelago, Brazil. In total, 1902 whistles were analyzed; 40% (753/1,902) of them were classified as stereotyped whistles (STW). Based on the SIGID method, 63% (472/753) of all STWs were identified as SWs; subsequently, they were categorized into one of 18 SW types. SWs accounted for 25% (472/1,902) of the acoustic repertoire. External observers have shown near perfect agreement to classify whistles into the adopted SW categorization. Most acoustic and temporal variables measured for SWs showed mean values similar to those recorded in other studies with spinner dolphins, whose authors did not differentiate SWs from non-SWs. Principal component analysis has explained 78% of total SW variance, and it emphasized the relevance of shape/contour and frequency variables to SW variance. This scientific discovery helps improving bioacoustics knowledge about the investigated species. Future studies to be conducted in Fernando de Noronha Archipelago should focus on continuous investigations about SW development and use by S. longirostris, expanding individuals’ identifications (Photo ID and SW Noronha Catalog), assessing long-term whistle stability and emission rates, and making mother–offspring comparisons with sex-based differences.

海豚的标志性哨音(SW)是一种独特的声学信号,以独特的频率调制轮廓的阵列模式发出;它使属于特定群体的个体能够相互识别,从而保持联系和凝聚力。目前的研究首次从科学角度证明了飞旋海豚(Stenella longirostris)会发出 SWs。声学数据是在巴西费尔南多-德诺罗尼亚群岛一个名为 "比博卡 "的浅水休憩湾记录的。共分析了 1902 个哨音,其中 40% (753/1,902)被归类为定型哨音(STW)。根据 SIGID 方法,所有 STW 中的 63%(472/753)被确定为 SW;随后,它们被归入 18 种 SW 类型之一。SW 占声谱的 25%(472/1,902)。外部观察者在将口哨归入所采用的 SW 分类中显示出近乎完美的一致性。对 SW 类口哨测量的大多数声学和时间变量的平均值与其他针对飞旋海豚的研究中记录的平均值相似,而这些研究的作者并未将 SW 类口哨与非 SW 类口哨区分开来。主成分分析解释了 78% 的 SW 总变异,并强调了形状/轮廓和频率变量与 SW 变异的相关性。这一科学发现有助于提高有关所调查物种的生物声学知识。未来在费尔南多-德诺罗尼亚群岛开展的研究应侧重于继续调查长尾鳕(S. longirostris)的SW发展和使用情况,扩大个体识别范围(照片ID和诺罗尼亚西南群岛目录),评估长期哨声稳定性和发射率,并根据性别差异进行母子比较。
{"title":"First acoustic evidence of signature whistle production by spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris)","authors":"Raul Rio","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01824-8","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01824-8","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>A dolphin’s signature whistle (SW) is a distinctive acoustic signal, issued in a bout pattern of unique frequency modulation contours; it allows individuals belonging to a given group to recognize each other and, consequently, to maintain contact and cohesion. The current study is the first scientific evidence that spinner dolphins (<i>Stenella longirostris</i>) produce SWs. Acoustic data were recorded at a shallow rest bay called “Biboca”, in Fernando de Noronha Archipelago, Brazil. In total, 1902 whistles were analyzed; 40% (753/1,902) of them were classified as stereotyped whistles (STW). Based on the SIGID method, 63% (472/753) of all STWs were identified as SWs; subsequently, they were categorized into one of 18 SW types. SWs accounted for 25% (472/1,902) of the acoustic repertoire. External observers have shown near perfect agreement to classify whistles into the adopted SW categorization. Most acoustic and temporal variables measured for SWs showed mean values similar to those recorded in other studies with spinner dolphins, whose authors did not differentiate SWs from non-SWs. Principal component analysis has explained 78% of total SW variance, and it emphasized the relevance of shape/contour and frequency variables to SW variance. This scientific discovery helps improving bioacoustics knowledge about the investigated species. Future studies to be conducted in Fernando de Noronha Archipelago should focus on continuous investigations about SW development and use by <i>S. longirostris</i>, expanding individuals’ identifications (Photo ID and SW Noronha Catalog), assessing long-term whistle stability and emission rates, and making mother–offspring comparisons with sex-based differences.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"26 6","pages":"1915 - 1927"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-09-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10162327","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Grouping rule in tadpole: is quantity more or size assortment more important? 蝌蚪的分组规则:数量更重要还是大小分类更重要?
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-05 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01823-9
Kazuko Hase

The ability to perceive group size and discriminate the ontogeny of conspecifics would play a crucial role in the grouping behavior of animals. However, the relative importance of numerical quantity and size-assortative preferences in shaping grouping rules remains poorly understood. In this study, I examined the responses of Miyako toad (Bufo gargarizans miyakonis) tadpoles to number quantity and size discrimination by choice tests at different ontogenetic stages (small, medium, and large). The results revealed that small-sized tadpoles in early developmental stages significantly preferred larger numbers (4) compared to smaller ones (1). However, this preference was not observed in later developmental stages (medium and large). And interestingly, when there was no quantity bias, size discrimination was not observed in tadpoles, irrespective of their ontogeny. These findings suggest that Miyako toad tadpoles discern quantity, i.e., the number of conspecifics, but exhibit ontogeny-dependent utilization of this ability. Understanding the interplay between numerical quantity and size-assortative preferences in grouping behavior will provide esteemed insights into the adaptive value of number sense in vertebrates and shed light on evolutionary processes.

感知群体大小和辨别同种动物本体的能力在动物的群居行为中起着至关重要的作用。然而,人们对数量和大小排序偏好在形成分组规则中的相对重要性仍然知之甚少。在这项研究中,我通过选择测试,考察了宫古蟾蜍(Bufo gargarizans miyakonis)蝌蚪在不同发育阶段(小、中、大)对数量和大小分辨的反应。结果显示,与较小的蝌蚪(1)相比,早期发育阶段的小蝌蚪明显偏好较大的数量(4)。然而,在后期发育阶段(中型和大型)却没有观察到这种偏好。有趣的是,在没有数量偏好的情况下,无论蝌蚪的发育阶段如何,都没有观察到蝌蚪的大小分辨。这些研究结果表明,宫古蟾蜍蝌蚪能够辨别数量,即同种动物的数量,但这种能力的利用取决于个体发育过程。了解集群行为中数字数量和大小排序偏好之间的相互作用,将有助于深入了解数感在脊椎动物中的适应价值,并揭示进化过程。
{"title":"Grouping rule in tadpole: is quantity more or size assortment more important?","authors":"Kazuko Hase","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01823-9","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01823-9","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The ability to perceive group size and discriminate the ontogeny of conspecifics would play a crucial role in the grouping behavior of animals. However, the relative importance of numerical quantity and size-assortative preferences in shaping grouping rules remains poorly understood. In this study, I examined the responses of Miyako toad (<i>Bufo gargarizans miyakonis</i>) tadpoles to number quantity and size discrimination by choice tests at different ontogenetic stages (small, medium, and large). The results revealed that small-sized tadpoles in early developmental stages significantly preferred larger numbers (4) compared to smaller ones (1). However, this preference was not observed in later developmental stages (medium and large). And interestingly, when there was no quantity bias, size discrimination was not observed in tadpoles, irrespective of their ontogeny. These findings suggest that Miyako toad tadpoles discern quantity, i.e., the number of conspecifics, but exhibit ontogeny-dependent utilization of this ability. Understanding the interplay between numerical quantity and size-assortative preferences in grouping behavior will provide esteemed insights into the adaptive value of number sense in vertebrates and shed light on evolutionary processes.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"26 6","pages":"1905 - 1913"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-09-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10527719","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Active oscillations in microscale navigation 微尺度导航中的主动振荡。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-04 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01819-5
Kirsty Y. Wan

Living organisms routinely navigate their surroundings in search of better conditions, more food, or to avoid predators. Typically, animals do so by integrating sensory cues from the environment with their locomotor apparatuses. For single cells or small organisms that possess motility, fundamental physical constraints imposed by their small size have led to alternative navigation strategies that are specific to the microscopic world. Intriguingly, underlying these myriad exploratory behaviours or sensory functions is the onset of periodic activity at multiple scales, such as the undulations of cilia and flagella, the vibrations of hair cells, or the oscillatory shape modes of migrating neutrophils. Here, I explore oscillatory dynamics in basal microeukaryotes and hypothesize that these active oscillations play a critical role in enhancing the fidelity of adaptive sensorimotor integration.

生物经常在周围环境中穿梭,以寻找更好的环境、更多的食物或躲避捕食者。通常情况下,动物通过将来自环境的感官线索与运动装置相结合来实现这一目的。对于单细胞或具有运动能力的小型生物来说,由于其体积小而受到基本的物理限制,这就导致了微观世界特有的其他导航策略。耐人寻味的是,在这些无数的探索行为或感官功能的背后,是多种尺度的周期性活动的开始,例如纤毛和鞭毛的起伏、毛细胞的振动或迁移的中性粒细胞的振荡形状模式。在这里,我探讨了基底微真核细胞的振荡动力学,并假设这些活跃的振荡在提高适应性感觉运动整合的保真度方面发挥着关键作用。
{"title":"Active oscillations in microscale navigation","authors":"Kirsty Y. Wan","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01819-5","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01819-5","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Living organisms routinely navigate their surroundings in search of better conditions, more food, or to avoid predators. Typically, animals do so by integrating sensory cues from the environment with their locomotor apparatuses. For single cells or small organisms that possess motility, fundamental physical constraints imposed by their small size have led to alternative navigation strategies that are specific to the microscopic world. Intriguingly, underlying these myriad exploratory behaviours or sensory functions is the onset of periodic activity at multiple scales, such as the undulations of cilia and flagella, the vibrations of hair cells, or the oscillatory shape modes of migrating neutrophils. Here, I explore oscillatory dynamics in basal microeukaryotes and hypothesize that these active oscillations play a critical role in enhancing the fidelity of adaptive sensorimotor integration.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"26 6","pages":"1837 - 1850"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-09-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10769930/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10203187","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Photobehaviours guided by simple photoreceptor systems 由简单的光感受器系统引导的光行为。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-08-31 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01818-6
Emelie Brodrick, Gáspár Jékely

Light provides a widely abundant energy source and valuable sensory cue in nature. Most animals exposed to light have photoreceptor cells and in addition to eyes, there are many extraocular strategies for light sensing. Here, we review how these simpler forms of detecting light can mediate rapid behavioural responses in animals. Examples of these behaviours include photophobic (light avoidance) or scotophobic (shadow) responses, photokinesis, phototaxis and wavelength discrimination. We review the cells and response mechanisms in these forms of elementary light detection, focusing on aquatic invertebrates with some protist and terrestrial examples to illustrate the general principles. Light cues can be used very efficiently by these simple photosensitive systems to effectively guide animal behaviours without investment in complex and energetically expensive visual structures.

光在自然界中提供了广泛丰富的能量来源和宝贵的感官线索。大多数暴露在光下的动物都有感光细胞,除了眼睛之外,还有许多用于光感测的眼外策略。在这里,我们回顾了这些更简单的光检测形式如何介导动物的快速行为反应。这些行为的例子包括畏光(避光)或避暗(阴影)反应、光运动、趋光性和波长辨别。我们回顾了这些形式的基本光检测中的细胞和反应机制,重点关注水生无脊椎动物,并以一些原生生物和陆地为例来说明一般原理。这些简单的光敏系统可以非常有效地使用光提示来有效地引导动物行为,而无需投资于复杂且能量昂贵的视觉结构。
{"title":"Photobehaviours guided by simple photoreceptor systems","authors":"Emelie Brodrick,&nbsp;Gáspár Jékely","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01818-6","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01818-6","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Light provides a widely abundant energy source and valuable sensory cue in nature. Most animals exposed to light have photoreceptor cells and in addition to eyes, there are many extraocular strategies for light sensing. Here, we review how these simpler forms of detecting light can mediate rapid behavioural responses in animals. Examples of these behaviours include photophobic (light avoidance) or scotophobic (shadow) responses, photokinesis, phototaxis and wavelength discrimination. We review the cells and response mechanisms in these forms of elementary light detection, focusing on aquatic invertebrates with some protist and terrestrial examples to illustrate the general principles. Light cues can be used very efficiently by these simple photosensitive systems to effectively guide animal behaviours without investment in complex and energetically expensive visual structures.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"26 6","pages":"1817 - 1835"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-08-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10770211/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10178035","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of acute maternal stress induced by predator cues on spatial learning and memory of offspring in the subterranean rodent Ctenomys talarum 捕食者线索诱发的急性母性应激对地下啮齿动物栉鼠(Ctenomys talarum)后代空间学习和记忆的影响
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-08-26 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01822-w
J. Iribarne, V. Brachetta, M. Kittlein, C. Schleich, R. Zenuto

One of the main selection pressures to which animals are exposed in nature is predation, which affects a wide variety of biological traits. When the mother experiences this stressor during pregnancy and/or lactation, behavioral and physiological responses may be triggered in the offspring as well. Thus, in order to broaden and deepen knowledge on the transgenerational effects of predation stress, we evaluated how maternal stress experienced during pregnancy and/or lactation affects the spatial abilities of progeny at the onset of adulthood in the subterranean rodent Ctenomys talarum. The results showed that, contrary to what was observed in other rodent species, maternal exposure to predator cues during pregnancy and lactation did not negatively affect the spatial abilities of the offspring, even registering some minor positive effects. Concomitantly, no effects of predatory cues on physiological parameters associated with stress were observed in the progeny. This difference in results between the present study and previous works on maternal stress highlights the importance of considering the species to be evaluated (strain, age and origin—wild or captive—) and the type of stressor used (artificial or natural, intensity of exposure) in the evaluation of the possible transgenerational effects of maternal stress.

捕食是动物在自然界中面临的主要选择压力之一,它会影响动物的各种生物特征。当母体在妊娠期和/或哺乳期遇到这种压力时,后代的行为和生理反应也可能被触发。因此,为了扩大和加深对捕食应激的跨代影响的认识,我们评估了妊娠期和/或哺乳期经历的母体应激如何影响地下啮齿动物栉鼠(Ctenomys talarum)成年后后代的空间能力。结果表明,与在其他啮齿类动物中观察到的情况相反,孕期和哺乳期母体暴露于捕食者的暗示并不会对后代的空间能力产生负面影响,甚至会产生一些轻微的积极影响。同时,在后代身上也没有观察到捕食者线索对与应激有关的生理参数的影响。本研究与以往有关母性应激的研究结果存在差异,这凸显了在评估母性应激可能产生的跨代影响时,考虑评估物种(品系、年龄和来源--野生或人工饲养)和所用应激源类型(人工或自然、暴露强度)的重要性。
{"title":"Effects of acute maternal stress induced by predator cues on spatial learning and memory of offspring in the subterranean rodent Ctenomys talarum","authors":"J. Iribarne,&nbsp;V. Brachetta,&nbsp;M. Kittlein,&nbsp;C. Schleich,&nbsp;R. Zenuto","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01822-w","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01822-w","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>One of the main selection pressures to which animals are exposed in nature is predation, which affects a wide variety of biological traits. When the mother experiences this stressor during pregnancy and/or lactation, behavioral and physiological responses may be triggered in the offspring as well. Thus, in order to broaden and deepen knowledge on the transgenerational effects of predation stress, we evaluated how maternal stress experienced during pregnancy and/or lactation affects the spatial abilities of progeny at the onset of adulthood in the subterranean rodent <i>Ctenomys talarum</i>. The results showed that, contrary to what was observed in other rodent species, maternal exposure to predator cues during pregnancy and lactation did not negatively affect the spatial abilities of the offspring, even registering some minor positive effects. Concomitantly, no effects of predatory cues on physiological parameters associated with stress were observed in the progeny. This difference in results between the present study and previous works on maternal stress highlights the importance of considering the species to be evaluated (strain, age and origin—wild or captive—) and the type of stressor used (artificial or natural, intensity of exposure) in the evaluation of the possible transgenerational effects of maternal stress.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"26 6","pages":"1997 - 2008"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-08-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10078587","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
In paired preference tests, domestic chicks innately choose the colour green over red, and the shape of a frog over a sphere when both stimuli are green 在配对偏好测试中,当两个刺激物都是绿色时,家养小鸡天生会选择绿色而不是红色,选择青蛙的形状而不是球形。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-08-23 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01821-x
Francesca Protti-Sánchez, Uwe Mayer, Hannah M. Rowland

Many animals express unlearned colour preferences that depend on the context in which signals are encountered. These colour biases may have evolved in response to the signalling system to which they relate. For example, many aposematic animals advertise their unprofitability with red warning signals. Predators’ innate biases against these warning colours have been suggested as one of the potential explanations for the initial evolution of aposematism. It is unclear, however, whether unlearned colour preferences reported in a number of species is truly an innate behaviour or whether it is based on prior experience. We tested the spontaneous colour and shape preferences of dark-hatched, unfed, and visually naive domestic chicks (Gallus gallus). In four experiments, we presented chicks with a choice between either red (a colour typically associated with warning patterns) or green (a colour associated with palatable cryptic prey), volume-matched spheres (representing a generalised fruit shape) or frogs (representing an aposematic animal’s shape). Chicks innately preferred green stimuli and avoided red. Chicks also preferred the shape of a frog over a sphere when both stimuli were green. However, no preference for frogs over spheres was present when stimuli were red. Male chicks that experienced a bitter taste of quinine immediately before the preference test showed a higher preference for green frog-shaped stimuli. Our results suggest that newly hatched chicks innately integrate colour and shape cues during decision making, and that this can be augmented by other sensory experiences. Innate and experience-based behaviour could confer a fitness advantage to novel aposematic prey, and favour the initial evolution of conspicuous colouration.

许多动物对颜色的偏好是后天形成的,这种偏好取决于遇到信号的环境。这些颜色偏好可能是根据与之相关的信号系统进化而来的。例如,许多会呼吸的动物会用红色警告信号来表明自己无利可图。捕食者对这些警告颜色的先天偏见被认为是 "窥视癖 "最初进化的潜在解释之一。然而,目前还不清楚在许多物种中报告的未学习的颜色偏好是否真的是一种先天行为,还是基于先前的经验。我们测试了深色孵化、未喂养和视觉幼稚的家养雏鸡(Gallus gallus)的自发颜色和形状偏好。在四次实验中,我们让雏鸟在红色(通常与警告图案相关的颜色)或绿色(与适口的隐蔽性猎物相关的颜色)、体积匹配的球体(代表一般水果的形状)或青蛙(代表动物的形状)之间进行选择。雏鸟天生喜欢绿色刺激,而回避红色刺激。当两个刺激物都是绿色时,小鸡也更喜欢青蛙的形状而不是球体。然而,当刺激物为红色时,雏鸟并不喜欢青蛙而不是球体。雄性雏鸟如果在偏好测试前立即尝过奎宁的苦味,就会对绿色青蛙形状的刺激表现出更高的偏好。我们的研究结果表明,刚孵出的雏鸟在做决定时会先天性地整合颜色和形状线索,而其他感官体验会增强这种整合。先天行为和基于经验的行为可能会给新的有表情的猎物带来适应优势,并有利于显眼颜色的最初进化。
{"title":"In paired preference tests, domestic chicks innately choose the colour green over red, and the shape of a frog over a sphere when both stimuli are green","authors":"Francesca Protti-Sánchez,&nbsp;Uwe Mayer,&nbsp;Hannah M. Rowland","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01821-x","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01821-x","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Many animals express unlearned colour preferences that depend on the context in which signals are encountered. These colour biases may have evolved in response to the signalling system to which they relate. For example, many aposematic animals advertise their unprofitability with red warning signals. Predators’ innate biases against these warning colours have been suggested as one of the potential explanations for the initial evolution of aposematism. It is unclear, however, whether unlearned colour preferences reported in a number of species is truly an innate behaviour or whether it is based on prior experience. We tested the spontaneous colour and shape preferences of dark-hatched, unfed, and visually naive domestic chicks (<i>Gallus gallus</i>). In four experiments, we presented chicks with a choice between either red (a colour typically associated with warning patterns) or green (a colour associated with palatable cryptic prey), volume-matched spheres (representing a generalised fruit shape) or frogs (representing an aposematic animal’s shape). Chicks innately preferred green stimuli and avoided red. Chicks also preferred the shape of a frog over a sphere when both stimuli were green. However, no preference for frogs over spheres was present when stimuli were red. Male chicks that experienced a bitter taste of quinine immediately before the preference test showed a higher preference for green frog-shaped stimuli. Our results suggest that newly hatched chicks innately integrate colour and shape cues during decision making, and that this can be augmented by other sensory experiences. Innate and experience-based behaviour could confer a fitness advantage to novel aposematic prey, and favour the initial evolution of conspicuous colouration.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"26 6","pages":"1973 - 1983"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-08-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10769926/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10108632","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Horses discriminate between human facial and vocal expressions of sadness and joy 马区分人类的面部和声音表达的悲伤和喜悦
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-08-06 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01817-7
Plotine Jardat, Océane Liehrmann, Fabrice Reigner, Céline Parias, Ludovic Calandreau, Léa Lansade

Communication of emotions plays a key role in intraspecific social interactions and likely in interspecific interactions. Several studies have shown that animals perceive human joy and anger, but few studies have examined other human emotions, such as sadness. In this study, we conducted a cross-modal experiment, in which we showed 28 horses two soundless videos simultaneously, one showing a sad, and one a joyful human face. These were accompanied by either a sad or joyful voice. The number of horses whose first look to the video that was incongruent with the voice was longer than their first look to the congruent video was higher than chance, suggesting that horses could form cross-modal representations of human joy and sadness. Moreover, horses were more attentive to the videos of joy and looked at them for longer, more frequently, and more rapidly than the videos of sadness. Their heart rates tended to increase when they heard joy and to decrease when they heard sadness. These results show that horses are able to discriminate facial and vocal expressions of joy and sadness and may form cross-modal representations of these emotions; they also are more attracted to joyful faces than to sad faces and seem to be more aroused by a joyful voice than a sad voice. Further studies are needed to better understand how horses perceive the range of human emotions, and we propose that future experiments include neutral stimuli as well as emotions with different arousal levels but a same valence.

情感交流在种内社会互动中起着关键作用,可能在种间互动中也起着重要作用。几项研究表明,动物能感知人类的喜悦和愤怒,但很少有研究考察人类的其他情绪,如悲伤。在这项研究中,我们进行了一项跨模态实验,在实验中,我们向28匹马同时展示了两段无声的视频,一段是悲伤的,另一段是快乐的人脸。伴随着这些的不是悲伤就是喜悦的声音。第一次看与声音不一致的视频的时间比第一次看一致视频的时间长的马的数量高于偶然性,这表明马可以形成人类喜悦和悲伤的跨模态表达。此外,与悲伤的视频相比,马更关注快乐的视频,看它们的时间更长、频率更高、速度更快。当他们听到喜悦时,他们的心率往往会增加,而当他们听到悲伤时,心率则会下降。这些结果表明,马能够区分喜悦和悲伤的面部和声音表达,并可能形成这些情绪的跨模态表征;他们也更喜欢快乐的面孔而不是悲伤的面孔,并且似乎更喜欢被快乐的声音而非悲伤的声音所唤起。需要进一步的研究来更好地理解马是如何感知人类情绪的,我们建议未来的实验包括中性刺激以及具有不同唤醒水平但具有相同效价的情绪。
{"title":"Horses discriminate between human facial and vocal expressions of sadness and joy","authors":"Plotine Jardat,&nbsp;Océane Liehrmann,&nbsp;Fabrice Reigner,&nbsp;Céline Parias,&nbsp;Ludovic Calandreau,&nbsp;Léa Lansade","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01817-7","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01817-7","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Communication of emotions plays a key role in intraspecific social interactions and likely in interspecific interactions. Several studies have shown that animals perceive human joy and anger, but few studies have examined other human emotions, such as sadness. In this study, we conducted a cross-modal experiment, in which we showed 28 horses two soundless videos simultaneously, one showing a sad, and one a joyful human face. These were accompanied by either a sad or joyful voice. The number of horses whose first look to the video that was incongruent with the voice was longer than their first look to the congruent video was higher than chance, suggesting that horses could form cross-modal representations of human joy and sadness. Moreover, horses were more attentive to the videos of joy and looked at them for longer, more frequently, and more rapidly than the videos of sadness. Their heart rates tended to increase when they heard joy and to decrease when they heard sadness. These results show that horses are able to discriminate facial and vocal expressions of joy and sadness and may form cross-modal representations of these emotions; they also are more attracted to joyful faces than to sad faces and seem to be more aroused by a joyful voice than a sad voice. Further studies are needed to better understand how horses perceive the range of human emotions, and we propose that future experiments include neutral stimuli as well as emotions with different arousal levels but a same valence.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"26 5","pages":"1733 - 1742"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2023-08-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10095345","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On being a Hydra with, and without, a nervous system: what do neurons add? 有神经系统和没有神经系统的九头蛇:神经元能增加什么?
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-08-04 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01816-8
Alison Hanson

The small freshwater cnidarian Hydra has been the subject of scientific inquiry for over 300 years due to its remarkable regenerative capacities and apparent immortality. More recently, Hydra has been recognized as an excellent model system within neuroscience because of its small size, transparency, and simple nervous system, which allow high-resolution imaging of its entire nerve net while behaving. In less than a decade, studies of Hydra’s nervous system have yielded insights into the activity of neural circuits in vivo unobtainable in most other animals. In addition to these unique attributes, there is yet another lesser-known feature of Hydra that makes it even more intriguing: it does not require its neural hardware to live. The extraordinary ability to survive the removal and replacement of its entire nervous system makes Hydra uniquely suited to address the question of what neurons add to an extant organism. Here, I will review what early work on nerve-free Hydra reveals about the potential role of the nervous system in these animals and point towards future directions for this work.

300 多年来,小型淡水刺胞动物水螅一直是科学研究的主题,因为它具有非凡的再生能力和明显的永生性。最近,水螅因其体积小、透明度高和神经系统简单而被认为是神经科学领域的一个极好的模型系统。在不到十年的时间里,通过对水螅神经系统的研究,人们深入了解了体内神经回路的活动,这是其他大多数动物所无法企及的。除了这些独特的属性外,水螅还有一个鲜为人知的特点,使其更加引人入胜:它不需要神经硬件就能生存。水螅在整个神经系统被移除和替换后仍能存活的非凡能力,使它成为解决 "神经元能为现存生物体带来什么 "这一问题的独一无二的选择。在这里,我将回顾关于无神经水螅的早期研究揭示了神经系统在这些动物中的潜在作用,并指出这项工作的未来方向。
{"title":"On being a Hydra with, and without, a nervous system: what do neurons add?","authors":"Alison Hanson","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01816-8","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01816-8","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The small freshwater cnidarian <i>Hydra</i> has been the subject of scientific inquiry for over 300 years due to its remarkable regenerative capacities and apparent immortality. More recently, <i>Hydra</i> has been recognized as an excellent model system within neuroscience because of its small size, transparency, and simple nervous system, which allow high-resolution imaging of its entire nerve net while behaving. In less than a decade, studies of <i>Hydra’s</i> nervous system have yielded insights into the activity of neural circuits in vivo unobtainable in most other animals. In addition to these unique attributes, there is yet another lesser-known feature of <i>Hydra</i> that makes it even more intriguing: it does not require its neural hardware to live. The extraordinary ability to survive the removal and replacement of its entire nervous system makes <i>Hydra</i> uniquely suited to address the question of what neurons add to an extant organism. Here, I will review what early work on nerve-free <i>Hydra</i> reveals about the potential role of the nervous system in these animals and point towards future directions for this work.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"26 6","pages":"1799 - 1816"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-08-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10770230/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9935106","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Family first! Influence of parental investment in Guinea pigs (Cavia porcellus) prosocial choices 家庭至上!父母投资对豚鼠亲社会选择的影响
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-08-01 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01813-x
Mathilde Lalot, Aude Bourgeois, Michel Saint Jalme, Dalila Bovet

Literature often assumed that prosocial behaviours (behaviours that benefit others with or without a cost for the actor) would have evolved many species to improve the effectiveness of parental care (Decety and Cowell 2014). While this hypothesis is rarely questioned at a phylogenetic scale, it was never tested at an individual scale to the best of our knowledge. Therefore, we chose to study the impact of effective parental care on prosociality by comparing the prosocial tendencies of Guinea pigs before mating, during mating and after parturition. We conducted Prosocial Choice Tests on three groups of Guinea pigs (males, multiparous females, and nulliparous females). Subjects had to choose between three options: a prosocial option (subject and recipient being rewarded), a selfish option (only subject was rewarded), and a null option (no reward). Our results showed high prosociality towards their mating partner and their young both in male and in female subjects. Males became selfish towards other males after parturition. Among other interesting results, we found a direct reciprocity phenomenon. We also highlighted an ability in our subjects to consider both the identity and relationship shared with the recipient, such as tolerance (enhancing prosociality), dominance rank (being tested with a dominant recipient increasing selfish responses), and its behaviour (begging calls eliciting prosociality, while threatening ones decreasing it), to choose an option. These findings suggested that prosociality could be modulated by many factors and that the constraints and stakes induced by breeding would highly influence prosocial strategies.

文献通常认为,亲社会行为(无论行为人是否付出代价,都有利于他人的行为)会进化出许多物种,以提高父母照顾的有效性(Decity和Cowell,2014)。虽然这一假设很少在系统发育尺度上受到质疑,但据我们所知,它从未在个体尺度上进行过测试。因此,我们选择通过比较豚鼠在交配前、交配期间和分娩后的亲社会倾向来研究有效的父母照顾对亲社会性的影响。我们对三组豚鼠(雄性、多胎雌性和未产仔雌性)进行了亲社会选择测试。受试者必须在三个选项之间做出选择:亲社会选项(受试者和接受者得到奖励)、自私选项(只有受试者得到奖励)和无效选项(没有奖励)。我们的研究结果显示,在男性和女性受试者中,他们对伴侣和年轻人都有很高的亲社会性。雄性在分娩后对其他雄性变得自私。在其他有趣的结果中,我们发现了一个直接的互惠现象。我们还强调了我们的受试者有能力考虑与接受者共享的身份和关系,如容忍度(增强亲社会性)、支配地位(与占主导地位的接受者一起测试,增加自私反应)及其行为(乞讨电话引发亲社会性,而威胁电话则降低亲社会性。这些发现表明,亲社会性可能受到许多因素的调节,而繁殖引起的约束和利害关系将对亲社会策略产生很大影响。
{"title":"Family first! Influence of parental investment in Guinea pigs (Cavia porcellus) prosocial choices","authors":"Mathilde Lalot,&nbsp;Aude Bourgeois,&nbsp;Michel Saint Jalme,&nbsp;Dalila Bovet","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01813-x","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01813-x","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Literature often assumed that prosocial behaviours (behaviours that benefit others with or without a cost for the actor) would have evolved many species to improve the effectiveness of parental care (Decety and Cowell 2014). While this hypothesis is rarely questioned at a phylogenetic scale, it was never tested at an individual scale to the best of our knowledge. Therefore, we chose to study the impact of effective parental care on prosociality by comparing the prosocial tendencies of Guinea pigs before mating, during mating and after parturition. We conducted Prosocial Choice Tests on three groups of Guinea pigs (males, multiparous females, and nulliparous females). Subjects had to choose between three options: a prosocial option (subject and recipient being rewarded), a selfish option (only subject was rewarded), and a null option (no reward). Our results showed high prosociality towards their mating partner and their young both in male and in female subjects. Males became selfish towards other males after parturition. Among other interesting results, we found a direct reciprocity phenomenon. We also highlighted an ability in our subjects to consider both the identity and relationship shared with the recipient, such as tolerance (enhancing prosociality), dominance rank (being tested with a dominant recipient increasing selfish responses), and its behaviour (begging calls eliciting prosociality, while threatening ones decreasing it), to choose an option. These findings suggested that prosociality could be modulated by many factors and that the constraints and stakes induced by breeding would highly influence prosocial strategies.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"26 5","pages":"1713 - 1732"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2023-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10028369","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Increasingly cautious sampling, not the black colouration of unpalatable prey, is used by fish in avoidance learning 鱼类在回避学习中使用的是越来越谨慎的采样,而不是令人不快的猎物的黑色
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-07-28 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01815-9
Mikołaj Kaczmarski, Jan M. Kaczmarek, Krzysztof Kowalski, Karol Borowski, Jacek Kęsy, Janusz Kloskowski

The efficiency of aposematic colouration of prey is based on the innate bias or facilitation of avoidance learning of predators. In many toxic bufonids, larvae are uniformly black, which is considered a warning signal. We compared fish predation on normal (black) and ‘transient albino’ (greyish) common toad Bufo bufo tadpoles that did not differ in toxicity or activity. In a two-stage experiment, each fish was presented with tadpoles of one colour in the first trial and the other colour in a subsequent trial. While tadpoles sampled by fish were typically not ingested, some died from injuries. The attack rate did not differ between tadpole phenotypes nor trials, irrespective of which phenotype was the first exposed to the fish. However, during the second trial, the sampled tadpoles, independent of colouration, were mouthed by fish for shorter periods and tadpole mortality decreased. The duration of mouthing also declined with an increasing number of attacks during subsequent trials. We conclude that in single-species prey populations, black tadpole colouration is not a warning signal as it does not accelerate predator learning about prey unprofitability. Our results indicate that with growing experience, predators sample potentially toxic prey more cautiously. This may explain why natural selection does not eliminate aposematic morphs even if predators continuously sample conspicuous prey.

猎物象征性着色的效率是基于捕食者天生的偏见或回避学习的便利性。在许多有毒的蟾蜍中,幼虫都是黑色的,这被认为是一个警告信号。我们比较了正常(黑色)和“短暂白化”(灰色)普通蟾蜍蟾蜍蝌蚪对鱼类的捕食,这两种蝌蚪的毒性或活性没有差异。在一个两阶段的实验中,在第一次试验中,每条鱼都被呈现出一种颜色的蝌蚪,在随后的试验中呈现出另一种颜色。虽然鱼类采样的蝌蚪通常不会被摄入,但有些会因受伤而死亡。无论哪种表型是第一次接触这种鱼,蝌蚪表型和试验的攻击率都没有差异。然而,在第二次试验中,采样的蝌蚪与颜色无关,被鱼咬的时间更短,蝌蚪死亡率降低。在随后的试验中,随着攻击次数的增加,口齿的持续时间也有所下降。我们得出的结论是,在单一物种的猎物种群中,黑蝌蚪的颜色并不是一个警告信号,因为它不会加速捕食者了解猎物的不适应力。我们的研究结果表明,随着经验的积累,捕食者对潜在有毒猎物的采样会更加谨慎。这也许可以解释为什么即使捕食者不断地对明显的猎物进行采样,自然选择也不能消除警示性的形态。
{"title":"Increasingly cautious sampling, not the black colouration of unpalatable prey, is used by fish in avoidance learning","authors":"Mikołaj Kaczmarski,&nbsp;Jan M. Kaczmarek,&nbsp;Krzysztof Kowalski,&nbsp;Karol Borowski,&nbsp;Jacek Kęsy,&nbsp;Janusz Kloskowski","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01815-9","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01815-9","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The efficiency of aposematic colouration of prey is based on the innate bias or facilitation of avoidance learning of predators. In many toxic bufonids, larvae are uniformly black, which is considered a warning signal. We compared fish predation on normal (black) and ‘transient albino’ (greyish) common toad <i>Bufo bufo</i> tadpoles that did not differ in toxicity or activity. In a two-stage experiment, each fish was presented with tadpoles of one colour in the first trial and the other colour in a subsequent trial. While tadpoles sampled by fish were typically not ingested, some died from injuries. The attack rate did not differ between tadpole phenotypes nor trials, irrespective of which phenotype was the first exposed to the fish. However, during the second trial, the sampled tadpoles, independent of colouration, were mouthed by fish for shorter periods and tadpole mortality decreased. The duration of mouthing also declined with an increasing number of attacks during subsequent trials. We conclude that in single-species prey populations, black tadpole colouration is not a warning signal as it does not accelerate predator learning about prey unprofitability. Our results indicate that with growing experience, predators sample potentially toxic prey more cautiously. This may explain why natural selection does not eliminate aposematic morphs even if predators continuously sample conspicuous prey.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"26 5","pages":"1705 - 1711"},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2023-07-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-023-01815-9.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10412430","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Animal Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1