首页 > 最新文献

Animal Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Great ape infants’ face touching and its role in social engagement
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-02-05 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-025-01931-8
Beatriz Felicio, Kim A. Bard

Touch has a key role in the social development of infant primates and in the regulation of social interactions, even so, there’s a rarity of studies on infants’ use of social touch. In this work, we document chimpanzee infants and human infants’ touching of other’s faces, a behavior already described in wild capuchin monkey infants, and investigate possible functions of infants’ social touch. A strength of this study is that we sampled chimpanzee and human infants from three different social ecologies each. Each infant was observed naturalistically, in their everyday environments. In 36 h of observation, we found 269 touch events, specifically 222 face touches and 47 head touches. We found significant differences between groups, within species. Face touching occurred preferentially with adult females in all groups, and preferentially in prosocial contexts, although the most preferred contexts differed across groups. A unifying concept was that almost all infant face touching occurred during joint attention events. We interpret this as the ability of 1-year-olds to use face touching as a behavioral marker of mutual engagement during bouts of triadic connectedness, that is when they engage together with a social partner about an object or an event. In this study, we document an understudied behavior of young chimpanzees and humans, one that is not only part of prosocial interactions, but one that may function to highlight infants’ active role in engagement with another, while they together engage in triadic connectedness.

{"title":"Great ape infants’ face touching and its role in social engagement","authors":"Beatriz Felicio,&nbsp;Kim A. Bard","doi":"10.1007/s10071-025-01931-8","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-025-01931-8","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Touch has a key role in the social development of infant primates and in the regulation of social interactions, even so, there’s a rarity of studies on infants’ use of social touch. In this work, we document chimpanzee infants and human infants’ touching of other’s faces, a behavior already described in wild capuchin monkey infants, and investigate possible functions of infants’ social touch. A strength of this study is that we sampled chimpanzee and human infants from three different social ecologies each. Each infant was observed naturalistically, in their everyday environments. In 36 h of observation, we found 269 touch events, specifically 222 face touches and 47 head touches. We found significant differences between groups, within species. Face touching occurred preferentially with adult females in all groups, and preferentially in prosocial contexts, although the most preferred contexts differed across groups. A unifying concept was that almost all infant face touching occurred during joint attention events. We interpret this as the ability of 1-year-olds to use face touching as a behavioral marker of mutual engagement during bouts of triadic connectedness, that is when they engage together with a social partner about an object or an event. In this study, we document an understudied behavior of young chimpanzees and humans, one that is not only part of prosocial interactions, but one that may function to highlight infants’ active role in engagement with another, while they together engage in triadic connectedness.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"28 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2025-02-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-025-01931-8.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143184708","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How bumblebees manage conflicting information seen on arrival and departure from flowers
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-02-05 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01926-x
Marie-Geneviève Guiraud, HaDi MaBouDi, Joe Woodgate, Olivia K. Bates, Oscar Ramos Rodriguez, Vince Gallo, Andrew B. Barron

Bees are flexible and adaptive learners, capable of learning stimuli seen on arrival and at departure from flowers where they have fed. This gives bees the potential to learn all information associated with a feeding event, but it also presents the challenge of managing information that is irrelevant, inconsistent, or conflicting. Here, we examined how presenting bumblebees with conflicting visual information before and after feeding influenced their learning rate and what they learned. Bees were trained to feeder stations mounted in front of a computer monitor. Visual stimuli were displayed behind each feeder station on the monitor. Positively reinforced stimuli (CS +) marked feeders offering sucrose solution. Negatively reinforced stimuli (CS−) marked feeders offering quinine solution. While alighted at the feeder station the stimuli were likely not visible to the bee. The “constant stimulus” training group saw the same stimulus throughout. For the “switched stimulus” training group, the CS + changed to the CS− during feeding. Learning was slower in the “switched stimulus” training group compared to the constant stimulus” group, but the training groups did not differ in their learning performance or the extent to which they generalised their learning. The information conflict in the “switched stimulus” group did not interfere with what had been learned. Differences between the “switched” and “constant stimulus” groups were greater for bees trained on a horizontal CS + than a vertical CS + suggesting bees differ in their processing of vertically and horizontally oriented stimuli. We discuss how bumblebees might resolve this type of information conflict so effectively, drawing on the known neurobiology of their visual learning system.

{"title":"How bumblebees manage conflicting information seen on arrival and departure from flowers","authors":"Marie-Geneviève Guiraud,&nbsp;HaDi MaBouDi,&nbsp;Joe Woodgate,&nbsp;Olivia K. Bates,&nbsp;Oscar Ramos Rodriguez,&nbsp;Vince Gallo,&nbsp;Andrew B. Barron","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01926-x","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01926-x","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Bees are flexible and adaptive learners, capable of learning stimuli seen on arrival and at departure from flowers where they have fed. This gives bees the potential to learn all information associated with a feeding event, but it also presents the challenge of managing information that is irrelevant, inconsistent, or conflicting. Here, we examined how presenting bumblebees with conflicting visual information before and after feeding influenced their learning rate and what they learned. Bees were trained to feeder stations mounted in front of a computer monitor. Visual stimuli were displayed behind each feeder station on the monitor. Positively reinforced stimuli (CS +) marked feeders offering sucrose solution. Negatively reinforced stimuli (CS−) marked feeders offering quinine solution. While alighted at the feeder station the stimuli were likely not visible to the bee. The “constant stimulus” training group saw the same stimulus throughout. For the “switched stimulus” training group, the CS + changed to the CS− during feeding. Learning was slower in the “switched stimulus” training group compared to the constant stimulus” group, but the training groups did not differ in their learning performance or the extent to which they generalised their learning. The information conflict in the “switched stimulus” group did not interfere with what had been learned. Differences between the “switched” and “constant stimulus” groups were greater for bees trained on a horizontal CS + than a vertical CS + suggesting bees differ in their processing of vertically and horizontally oriented stimuli. We discuss how bumblebees might resolve this type of information conflict so effectively, drawing on the known neurobiology of their visual learning system.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"28 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2025-02-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-024-01926-x.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143184752","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Antipredator behaviour in semi-feral horses: innate response and the influence of external factors
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-02-04 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-025-01933-6
Antoine Bercy, Francisco Ceacero, Martina Komárková

Rewilding can play a vital role in safeguarding biodiversity, with the grazing pressure exerted by large ungulates and controlled by their predators being a significant factor, particularly in European contexts. Domestic horses are becoming integral to such ungulates’ biomass, but they may differ from truly wild species due to their domesticated origin. This raises concerns about whether feral horses retain adequate antipredator behaviours, especially in the presence of expanding, large predators like wolves. The field of antipredator behaviour research is hampered by inconsistent results and a lack of standardisation, and the behaviour of free-ranging horses remains underexplored. To address this gap, we conducted a playback experiment on semi-feral Exmoor ponies (n = 97) in the Czech Republic, exposing them to wolf howls, deer rut calls, and static noise as a control. We assessed alert behaviour and herd grouping while accounting for variables such as herd size, sex, time of day, weather conditions, environment type, presence of other ungulates, and habituation effects. Over 70% of the ponies exhibited alert behaviour in response to both wolf and deer calls. Although the magnitude of responses did not differ significantly between wolf and deer calls, both elicited distinct reactions compared to the control. Most of the studied external factors significantly affected the observed alert responses, highlighting that they must be carefully considered in such studies since these may explain the conflicting results observed in previous studies. The significant behavioural differences in reaction to the sounds indicate that the horses can differentiate them and likely still possess some innate memory, as reported in other ungulates. This is a positive sign towards reintroduction. Future research should carefully consider the validity of the testing environment, habituation effects, and other external factors to ensure robust results.

{"title":"Antipredator behaviour in semi-feral horses: innate response and the influence of external factors","authors":"Antoine Bercy,&nbsp;Francisco Ceacero,&nbsp;Martina Komárková","doi":"10.1007/s10071-025-01933-6","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-025-01933-6","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Rewilding can play a vital role in safeguarding biodiversity, with the grazing pressure exerted by large ungulates and controlled by their predators being a significant factor, particularly in European contexts. Domestic horses are becoming integral to such ungulates’ biomass, but they may differ from truly wild species due to their domesticated origin. This raises concerns about whether feral horses retain adequate antipredator behaviours, especially in the presence of expanding, large predators like wolves. The field of antipredator behaviour research is hampered by inconsistent results and a lack of standardisation, and the behaviour of free-ranging horses remains underexplored. To address this gap, we conducted a playback experiment on semi-feral Exmoor ponies (<i>n</i> = 97) in the Czech Republic, exposing them to wolf howls, deer rut calls, and static noise as a control. We assessed alert behaviour and herd grouping while accounting for variables such as herd size, sex, time of day, weather conditions, environment type, presence of other ungulates, and habituation effects. Over 70% of the ponies exhibited alert behaviour in response to both wolf and deer calls. Although the magnitude of responses did not differ significantly between wolf and deer calls, both elicited distinct reactions compared to the control. Most of the studied external factors significantly affected the observed alert responses, highlighting that they must be carefully considered in such studies since these may explain the conflicting results observed in previous studies. The significant behavioural differences in reaction to the sounds indicate that the horses can differentiate them and likely still possess some innate memory, as reported in other ungulates. This is a positive sign towards reintroduction. Future research should carefully consider the validity of the testing environment, habituation effects, and other external factors to ensure robust results.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"28 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2025-02-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-025-01933-6.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143107891","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Ready, set, yellow! color preference of Indian free-ranging dogs
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-02-04 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01928-9
Anamitra Roy, Aesha Lahiri, Srijaya Nandi, Aayush Manchalwar, S. Siddharth, J. V. R. Abishek, Indira Bulhan, Shouvanik Sengupta, Sandeep Kumar, Tushnim Chakravarty, Anindita Bhadra

Most of the research on color vision related behaviors in dogs has involved training the dogs to perform visual discrimination tasks. We investigated the importance of color to untrained Indian free-ranging dogs (FRDs). Using one-time multi-option choice tests for color preference in 134 adult dogs, we found the dogs to prefer yellow objects over blue or gray ones while there was no preference between blue and gray. We next pitted a yellow object against a gray object that had food. Here, the dogs ignored the food (biscuit or chicken) to approach the yellow object first indicating the color preference to be quite strong. Color preference has previously been investigated in many other animals and has implications for behaviors like mate choice and foraging. Our study provides a new perspective into the ecology of Indian FRDs and might have implications for companion dogs as well, if they too show this preference.

{"title":"Ready, set, yellow! color preference of Indian free-ranging dogs","authors":"Anamitra Roy,&nbsp;Aesha Lahiri,&nbsp;Srijaya Nandi,&nbsp;Aayush Manchalwar,&nbsp;S. Siddharth,&nbsp;J. V. R. Abishek,&nbsp;Indira Bulhan,&nbsp;Shouvanik Sengupta,&nbsp;Sandeep Kumar,&nbsp;Tushnim Chakravarty,&nbsp;Anindita Bhadra","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01928-9","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01928-9","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Most of the research on color vision related behaviors in dogs has involved training the dogs to perform visual discrimination tasks. We investigated the importance of color to untrained Indian free-ranging dogs (FRDs). Using one-time multi-option choice tests for color preference in 134 adult dogs, we found the dogs to prefer yellow objects over blue or gray ones while there was no preference between blue and gray. We next pitted a yellow object against a gray object that had food. Here, the dogs ignored the food (biscuit or chicken) to approach the yellow object first indicating the color preference to be quite strong. Color preference has previously been investigated in many other animals and has implications for behaviors like mate choice and foraging. Our study provides a new perspective into the ecology of Indian FRDs and might have implications for companion dogs as well, if they too show this preference.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"28 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2025-02-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-024-01928-9.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143107890","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Female budgerigars prefer males with foraging skills that differ from their own
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-02-04 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01923-0
Yuqi Zou, Zitan Song, Jiani Chen, Yuehua Sun, Michael Griesser

Foraging skills influence food intake and could therefore also play a role in mate choice decision. Previous empirical work has shown that individuals benefit from being in groups that include individuals with a variety of foraging skills as this increases foraging success. This idea, formalized in the skill-pool hypothesis, may extend to mate choice. Diverse foraging skills can expand the foraging niche of a pair and benefit offspring through enhanced parental provisioning, and exposure to a broader foraging skillset. To test this idea, we trained captive female and male budgerigars to solve one of two different novel foraging puzzle boxes. Then, females simultaneously observed two males that could solve either the same or the other box, and assessed female preferences in a binary mate choice apparatus. Females preferred males with foraging skills that differed from their own, independent of the skill type and the number of times males solved the foraging puzzle. These findings show that foraging skills can influence social preferences, including in a mate choice context, and support intraspecific diversity in foraging skills.

{"title":"Female budgerigars prefer males with foraging skills that differ from their own","authors":"Yuqi Zou,&nbsp;Zitan Song,&nbsp;Jiani Chen,&nbsp;Yuehua Sun,&nbsp;Michael Griesser","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01923-0","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01923-0","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Foraging skills influence food intake and could therefore also play a role in mate choice decision. Previous empirical work has shown that individuals benefit from being in groups that include individuals with a variety of foraging skills as this increases foraging success. This idea, formalized in the skill-pool hypothesis, may extend to mate choice. Diverse foraging skills can expand the foraging niche of a pair and benefit offspring through enhanced parental provisioning, and exposure to a broader foraging skillset. To test this idea, we trained captive female and male budgerigars to solve one of two different novel foraging puzzle boxes. Then, females simultaneously observed two males that could solve either the same or the other box, and assessed female preferences in a binary mate choice apparatus. Females preferred males with foraging skills that differed from their own, independent of the skill type and the number of times males solved the foraging puzzle. These findings show that foraging skills can influence social preferences, including in a mate choice context, and support intraspecific diversity in foraging skills.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"28 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2025-02-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-024-01923-0.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143107892","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Rats and mice rapidly update timed behaviors
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-24 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-025-01930-9
N. Aggadi, S. Krikawa, T. A. Paine, P. Simen, C. D. Howard

Keeping track of time intervals is a crucial aspect of behavior and cognition. Many theoretical models of how the brain times behavior make predictions for steady-state performance of well-learned intervals, but the rate of learning intervals in these models varies greatly, ranging from one-shot learning to learning over thousands of trials. Here, we explored how quickly rats and mice adapt to changes in interval durations using a serial fixed-interval task. In the first experiment, animals experienced randomly selected fixed-intervals of 12, 24, 36, 48, or 60 s, for blocks ranging from 13 to 21 trials. Consistent with previous work, animals abruptly increased lever pressing as reward availability approached, and these ‘start times’ scaled with the interval duration for both species. We then quantified the rate of updating to new trial durations and found that rodents consistently updated their start times within 2–3 trials following a change in interval duration, before stabilizing their behavior by the third or fourth trial. To account for repeated exposures to fixed-interval durations, a second set of animals was tested with new fixed-intervals after being trained on the serial fixed-interval task described above. Next, a third group was trained on fixed-interval durations that were generated de novo in each day. In each of these contexts, rodents rapidly increased or decreased their start times to mirror new FI durations following exposure to 1–2 trials of new intervals following block transitions. This work adds to growing evidence for rapid duration learning across species, highlighting the need for timing models to be capable of rapid updating in dynamic temporal scenarios.

{"title":"Rats and mice rapidly update timed behaviors","authors":"N. Aggadi,&nbsp;S. Krikawa,&nbsp;T. A. Paine,&nbsp;P. Simen,&nbsp;C. D. Howard","doi":"10.1007/s10071-025-01930-9","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-025-01930-9","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Keeping track of time intervals is a crucial aspect of behavior and cognition. Many theoretical models of how the brain times behavior make predictions for steady-state performance of well-learned intervals, but the rate of learning intervals in these models varies greatly, ranging from one-shot learning to learning over thousands of trials. Here, we explored how quickly rats and mice adapt to changes in interval durations using a serial fixed-interval task. In the first experiment, animals experienced randomly selected fixed-intervals of 12, 24, 36, 48, or 60 s, for blocks ranging from 13 to 21 trials. Consistent with previous work, animals abruptly increased lever pressing as reward availability approached, and these ‘start times’ scaled with the interval duration for both species. We then quantified the rate of updating to new trial durations and found that rodents consistently updated their start times within 2–3 trials following a change in interval duration, before stabilizing their behavior by the third or fourth trial. To account for repeated exposures to fixed-interval durations, a second set of animals was tested with new fixed-intervals after being trained on the serial fixed-interval task described above. Next, a third group was trained on fixed-interval durations that were generated <i>de novo</i> in each day. In each of these contexts, rodents rapidly increased or decreased their start times to mirror new FI durations following exposure to 1–2 trials of new intervals following block transitions. This work adds to growing evidence for rapid duration learning across species, highlighting the need for timing models to be capable of rapid updating in dynamic temporal scenarios.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"28 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2025-01-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11759285/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143027938","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Current predation risk has opposing effects on social learning of foraging locations across two guppy populations 目前的捕食风险对两种孔雀鱼种群觅食地点的社会学习有相反的影响
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-07 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01929-8
Mélanie F. Guigueno, Adrian C. K. Foster, Simon M. Reader

Social learning, where animals learn from other individuals, occurs in many diverse species. The influential but debated ‘costly information’ hypothesis posits that animals will rely more on social information in high-risk contexts, such as under increased predation risk. We examined and compared the effects of perceived predation risk on social learning of foraging sites in female Trinidadian guppies from wild and domestic populations raised in common-garden environments. We used a demonstrator-observer pairing where a subject could observe conspecific ‘demonstrators’ feeding from one of two feeders, and measured whether the observer subsequently spent more time at a demonstrated or non-demonstrated feeder. We manipulated perceived predation risk using alarm cue (conspecific skin extract). Stress responses and social learning differed between the two populations. Most notably, high predation risk enhanced social learning in the wild-type guppies, but depressed it in the domestic guppies. Thus, fish from both populations were able to socially learn, but under opposing contexts. These results suggest social learning propensities are the product of multiple interacting systems, and biases to favour social learning can emerge dependent on evolutionary history and current conditions.

社会学习,即动物向其他个体学习,存在于许多不同的物种中。有影响力但存在争议的“昂贵信息”假说认为,动物在高风险环境下会更依赖社会信息,比如在捕食风险增加的情况下。我们研究并比较了感知捕食风险对特立尼达雌孔雀鱼觅食地点社会学习的影响,这些孔雀鱼分别来自野生种群和在普通花园环境中饲养的家养种群。我们使用了示范-观察者配对,受试者可以观察两个喂食器中的一个的“示范”喂食,并测量观察者随后在示范喂食器或非示范喂食器上花费的时间是否更长。我们使用警报提示(同种皮肤提取物)来操纵感知捕食风险。压力反应和社会学习在两个人群中有所不同。最值得注意的是,高捕食风险增强了野生型孔雀鱼的社会学习能力,但抑制了家养孔雀鱼的社会学习能力。因此,两个种群的鱼都能够在相反的环境下进行社会学习。这些结果表明,社会学习倾向是多个相互作用系统的产物,有利于社会学习的偏见可以根据进化历史和当前条件而出现。
{"title":"Current predation risk has opposing effects on social learning of foraging locations across two guppy populations","authors":"Mélanie F. Guigueno,&nbsp;Adrian C. K. Foster,&nbsp;Simon M. Reader","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01929-8","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01929-8","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Social learning, where animals learn from other individuals, occurs in many diverse species. The influential but debated ‘costly information’ hypothesis posits that animals will rely more on social information in high-risk contexts, such as under increased predation risk. We examined and compared the effects of perceived predation risk on social learning of foraging sites in female Trinidadian guppies from wild and domestic populations raised in common-garden environments. We used a demonstrator-observer pairing where a subject could observe conspecific ‘demonstrators’ feeding from one of two feeders, and measured whether the observer subsequently spent more time at a demonstrated or non-demonstrated feeder. We manipulated perceived predation risk using alarm cue (conspecific skin extract). Stress responses and social learning differed between the two populations. Most notably, high predation risk enhanced social learning in the wild-type guppies, but depressed it in the domestic guppies. Thus, fish from both populations were able to socially learn, but under opposing contexts. These results suggest social learning propensities are the product of multiple interacting systems, and biases to favour social learning can emerge dependent on evolutionary history and current conditions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"28 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2025-01-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-024-01929-8.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142938714","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Strange features are no better than no features: predator recognition by untrained birds 奇怪的特征并不比没有特征好:未经训练的鸟类对捕食者的识别
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-07 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01924-z
Ondřej Fišer, Irena Strnadová, Petr Veselý, Michaela Syrová, Michal Němec, Barbora Kamišová, Josef Šalom, Roman Fuchs

Predator recognition is essential for prey survival, allowing for appropriate antipredator strategies. Some bird species, such as the red-backed shrike (Lanius collurio), distinguish not only between predators and non-threatening species but also between different predator species. Earlier studies have identified general predator “key features”, especially beak shape and talons, as critical for predator recognition. The question, though, still remains of whether exchanging predator key features with those of nonpredatory species or, alternatively, completely removing them, have different or equal impact on recognition. Here we tested to ascertain whether the presence of the “incorrect key features” of a harmless pigeon (Columba livia) placed on a common kestrel (Falco tinnunculus) body impairs predator recognition more efficiently than the absence of any key features. We presented an unmodified kestrel dummy and two modified kestrel dummies (one with pigeon key features, the other lacking key features) to wild red-backed shrikes defending their nest. The shrikes attacked the unmodified dummy kestrel more intensively than both kestrel modifications when defending the nest. However, shrikes did not show different responses to the kestrel with pigeon key features and the featureless kestrel. Our findings show that the absence and exchange of key features have the same effect in this case. These results are discussed in the context of recognition of a specific predator species and predators as a category in general.

识别捕食者对猎物的生存至关重要,从而制定适当的反捕食者策略。一些鸟类,如红背伯劳鸟(Lanius collurio),不仅能区分捕食者和非威胁物种,还能区分不同的捕食者物种。早期的研究已经确定了一般捕食者的“关键特征”,特别是喙形和爪子,对于识别捕食者至关重要。然而,问题仍然是,与非掠食性物种交换捕食者的关键特征,或者完全去除它们,对识别的影响是不同的还是相同的。在这里,我们进行了测试,以确定一只无害的鸽子(Columba livia)的“错误关键特征”是否存在于一只普通的红隼(Falco tinunculus)身上,比没有任何关键特征更有效地损害了捕食者的识别。我们将一个未修改的红隼假人和两个修改的红隼假人(一个具有鸽子的关键特征,另一个不具有鸽子的关键特征)用于保护其巢穴的野生红背卓鸟。在保卫巢穴时,伯雷鸟对未改造的假红隼的攻击比两种改造的红隼都要强烈。然而,伯雷鸟对具有鸽子关键特征的红隼和无特征的红隼没有表现出不同的反应。我们的研究结果表明,在这种情况下,关键特征的缺失和交换具有相同的效果。这些结果是在识别特定的捕食者物种和捕食者作为一般类别的背景下讨论的。
{"title":"Strange features are no better than no features: predator recognition by untrained birds","authors":"Ondřej Fišer,&nbsp;Irena Strnadová,&nbsp;Petr Veselý,&nbsp;Michaela Syrová,&nbsp;Michal Němec,&nbsp;Barbora Kamišová,&nbsp;Josef Šalom,&nbsp;Roman Fuchs","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01924-z","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01924-z","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Predator recognition is essential for prey survival, allowing for appropriate antipredator strategies. Some bird species, such as the red-backed shrike (<i>Lanius collurio</i>), distinguish not only between predators and non-threatening species but also between different predator species. Earlier studies have identified general predator “key features”, especially beak shape and talons, as critical for predator recognition. The question, though, still remains of whether exchanging predator key features with those of nonpredatory species or, alternatively, completely removing them, have different or equal impact on recognition. Here we tested to ascertain whether the presence of the “incorrect key features” of a harmless pigeon (<i>Columba livia</i>) placed on a common kestrel (<i>Falco tinnunculus</i>) body impairs predator recognition more efficiently than the absence of any key features. We presented an unmodified kestrel dummy and two modified kestrel dummies (one with pigeon key features, the other lacking key features) to wild red-backed shrikes defending their nest. The shrikes attacked the unmodified dummy kestrel more intensively than both kestrel modifications when defending the nest. However, shrikes did not show different responses to the kestrel with pigeon key features and the featureless kestrel. Our findings show that the absence and exchange of key features have the same effect in this case. These results are discussed in the context of recognition of a specific predator species and predators as a category in general.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"28 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2025-01-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-024-01924-z.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142938715","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Contrasting two versions of the 4-cup 2-item disjunctive syllogism task in great apes 类人猿4杯2项析取三段论任务的两个版本的对比
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-02 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01927-w
Benjamin Jones, Josep Call

Chimpanzees excel at inference tasks which require that they search for a single food item from partial information. Yet, when presented with 2-item tasks which test the same inference operation, chimpanzees show a consistent breakdown in performance. Here we test a diverse zoo-housed cohort (n = 24) comprising all 4 great ape species under the classic 4-cup 2-item task, previously administered to children and chimpanzees, and a modified task administered to baboons. The aim of this study is to delineate whether the divergent results reported from the literature are taxonomic differences or artefacts of their methodologies, while extending the literature to cover the remaining great ape species. We find that apes adaptively adjust their choice behaviour in both variants of the task, but that they perform better in trials where the information provided rules out a location rather than removes one of the food items. In a second experiment involving those subjects who passed the first, along with a group of naïve subjects, we test whether subjects were able to apply the logical operation selectively by including control trials where the correct response is reversed. Performance in standard trials breaks down with the addition of control trials, meaning that if apes did solve the first experiment logically, they are not capable of applying that logic flexibly. Considering this finding, we conclude that a 4-cup 2-item task may not be a suitable test of logical reasoning in great apes.

黑猩猩擅长于从部分信息中寻找单一食物的推理任务。然而,当面对测试相同推理操作的两项任务时,黑猩猩表现出一致的表现。在这里,我们测试了一个不同的动物园里的队列(n = 24),包括所有4种类人猿,在经典的4杯2项任务下,以前给儿童和黑猩猩,以及一个修改后的任务给狒狒。本研究旨在探讨文献中报道的不同结果是分类差异还是其方法的产物,同时将文献扩展到其他类人猿物种。我们发现,在两种不同的任务中,猿类都能自适应地调整自己的选择行为,但在提供排除某个位置的信息而不是移除其中一种食物的信息的试验中,它们表现得更好。在第二个实验中,通过第一个测试的受试者与一组naïve受试者一起参与,我们测试了受试者是否能够选择性地应用逻辑运算,其中包括正确反应相反的对照试验。标准试验中的表现随着控制试验的增加而下降,这意味着如果类人猿确实在逻辑上解决了第一个实验,它们就不能灵活地应用该逻辑。考虑到这一发现,我们得出结论,4杯2项任务可能不适合测试类人猿的逻辑推理能力。
{"title":"Contrasting two versions of the 4-cup 2-item disjunctive syllogism task in great apes","authors":"Benjamin Jones,&nbsp;Josep Call","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01927-w","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01927-w","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Chimpanzees excel at inference tasks which require that they search for a single food item from partial information. Yet, when presented with 2-item tasks which test the same inference operation, chimpanzees show a consistent breakdown in performance. Here we test a diverse zoo-housed cohort (<i>n</i> = 24) comprising all 4 great ape species under the classic 4-cup 2-item task, previously administered to children and chimpanzees, and a modified task administered to baboons. The aim of this study is to delineate whether the divergent results reported from the literature are taxonomic differences or artefacts of their methodologies, while extending the literature to cover the remaining great ape species. We find that apes adaptively adjust their choice behaviour in both variants of the task, but that they perform better in trials where the information provided rules out a location rather than removes one of the food items. In a second experiment involving those subjects who passed the first, along with a group of naïve subjects, we test whether subjects were able to apply the logical operation selectively by including control trials where the correct response is reversed. Performance in standard trials breaks down with the addition of control trials, meaning that if apes did solve the first experiment logically, they are not capable of applying that logic flexibly. Considering this finding, we conclude that a 4-cup 2-item task may not be a suitable test of logical reasoning in great apes.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"28 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2025-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-024-01927-w.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142912823","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Playing with the rope: a house sparrow behaviour related to its breeding activity 玩绳子:一种家雀的行为与它的繁殖活动有关
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-12-24 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01921-2
José Ignacio Huertas-Gómez, Juan Manuel Peralta-Sánchez, Manuel Soler

The house sparrow (Passer domesticus) is a gregarious generalist species, which makes it a good model for studying play. However, play has not been described for this species so far. We describe play behaviour in house sparrows for the first time, quantifying all play and play-related behaviours, searching for differences between the different sexes and ages, the possible association with reproductive success and the diffusion of this behaviour in the population. All behaviours were recorded from the end of 2018 breeding season to the start of the new one in 2019. Behaviours were classified into four levels of interaction of increasing complexity and intensity. Results showed that play behaviour was restricted to the breeding season, adult males played more often than the rest of the groups, and their behaviours correlated with the number of recruits they produced. Moreover, “Maximum Level” of play of the mothers significantly and positively correlated with that of their offspring, and the “Maximum Level” of an individual with the proportion of playing siblings. Despite the limitations of the present study, our results point out the existence of benefits for the reproductive success of playing individuals.

家雀(Passer domesticus)是一种群居的通才物种,这使它成为研究玩耍的一个很好的模型。然而,到目前为止,还没有关于这个物种玩耍的描述。我们首次描述了家麻雀的游戏行为,量化了所有游戏和与游戏相关的行为,寻找不同性别和年龄之间的差异,与繁殖成功的可能联系以及这种行为在种群中的传播。记录了从2018年繁殖季节结束到2019年新繁殖季节开始的所有行为。行为被分为四个层次的相互作用,其复杂性和强度都在增加。结果表明,玩耍行为仅限于繁殖季节,成年雄性比其他群体更频繁地玩耍,它们的行为与它们产生的新成员数量有关。母亲的“最大游戏水平”与其子女的“最大游戏水平”显著正相关,个体的“最大游戏水平”与兄弟姐妹的游戏比例显著正相关。尽管目前的研究存在局限性,但我们的研究结果指出,玩游戏的个体对繁殖成功有好处。
{"title":"Playing with the rope: a house sparrow behaviour related to its breeding activity","authors":"José Ignacio Huertas-Gómez,&nbsp;Juan Manuel Peralta-Sánchez,&nbsp;Manuel Soler","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01921-2","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01921-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The house sparrow (<i>Passer domesticus</i>) is a gregarious generalist species, which makes it a good model for studying play. However, play has not been described for this species so far. We describe play behaviour in house sparrows for the first time, quantifying all play and play-related behaviours, searching for differences between the different sexes and ages, the possible association with reproductive success and the diffusion of this behaviour in the population. All behaviours were recorded from the end of 2018 breeding season to the start of the new one in 2019. Behaviours were classified into four levels of interaction of increasing complexity and intensity. Results showed that play behaviour was restricted to the breeding season, adult males played more often than the rest of the groups, and their behaviours correlated with the number of recruits they produced. Moreover, “Maximum Level” of play of the mothers significantly and positively correlated with that of their offspring, and the “Maximum Level” of an individual with the proportion of playing siblings. Despite the limitations of the present study, our results point out the existence of benefits for the reproductive success of playing individuals.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"28 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-12-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-024-01921-2.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142880525","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Animal Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1