首页 > 最新文献

Animal Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Evidence for the communicative function of human-directed gazing in 6- to 7-week-old dog puppies. 6-7周大的幼犬在人类引导下凝视的交流功能证据。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-23 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01898-y
Stefanie Riemer, Alina Bonorand, Lisa Stolzlechner

In human infants, the ability to show gaze alternations between an object of interest and another individual is considered fundamental to the development of complex social-cognitive abilities. Here we show that well-socialised dog puppies show gaze alternations in two contexts at an early age, 6-7 weeks. Thus, 69.4% of puppies in a novel object test and 45.59% of puppies during an unsolvable task alternated their gaze at least once between a person's face and the object. In both contexts, the frequency of gaze alternations was positively correlated with the duration of whimpering, supporting the communicative nature of puppies' gazing. Furthermore, the number of gaze alternations in the two contexts was correlated, indicating an underlying propensity for gazing at humans despite likely different motivations in the two contexts. Similar to humans, and unlike great apes or wolves, domestic dogs show gaze alternations from an early age if they are well-socialised. They appear to have a genetic preparedness to communicate with humans via gaze alternations early in ontogeny, but they may need close contact with humans for this ability to emerge, highlighting the interactive effects of domestication and environmental factors on behavioural development in dogs.

在人类婴儿中,在感兴趣的对象和另一个人之间表现出目光交替的能力被认为是发展复杂社会认知能力的基础。在这里,我们发现,社会化程度较高的幼犬在 6-7 周大时就能在两种情境中表现出目光交替。因此,69.4% 的幼犬在新物体测试中,45.59% 的幼犬在无法解决的任务中,至少有一次在人脸和物体之间交替注视。在这两种情况下,注视交替的频率与呜咽的持续时间呈正相关,这证明了幼犬注视的交流性质。此外,两种情境中目光交替的次数也有相关性,这表明尽管两种情境中的动机可能不同,但小狗都有凝视人类的潜在倾向。与人类相似,但与类人猿或狼不同的是,家犬如果社会化程度高,从幼年起就会表现出目光交替。它们似乎在本体发育早期就做好了通过目光交替与人类交流的遗传准备,但它们可能需要与人类密切接触才能出现这种能力,这凸显了驯化和环境因素对狗行为发展的交互影响。
{"title":"Evidence for the communicative function of human-directed gazing in 6- to 7-week-old dog puppies.","authors":"Stefanie Riemer, Alina Bonorand, Lisa Stolzlechner","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01898-y","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-024-01898-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In human infants, the ability to show gaze alternations between an object of interest and another individual is considered fundamental to the development of complex social-cognitive abilities. Here we show that well-socialised dog puppies show gaze alternations in two contexts at an early age, 6-7 weeks. Thus, 69.4% of puppies in a novel object test and 45.59% of puppies during an unsolvable task alternated their gaze at least once between a person's face and the object. In both contexts, the frequency of gaze alternations was positively correlated with the duration of whimpering, supporting the communicative nature of puppies' gazing. Furthermore, the number of gaze alternations in the two contexts was correlated, indicating an underlying propensity for gazing at humans despite likely different motivations in the two contexts. Similar to humans, and unlike great apes or wolves, domestic dogs show gaze alternations from an early age if they are well-socialised. They appear to have a genetic preparedness to communicate with humans via gaze alternations early in ontogeny, but they may need close contact with humans for this ability to emerge, highlighting the interactive effects of domestication and environmental factors on behavioural development in dogs.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142279385","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dogs distinguish authentic human emotions without being empathic. 狗狗能分辨人类的真实情感,但不会产生共鸣。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-21 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01899-x
Juliane Bräuer, Dorothea Eichentopf, Nomi Gebele, Louise Jandke, Veronique Mann, Katharina Schulte, Yana Bender

Several recent studies have investigated how dogs perceive human emotional expressions. They have measured the reactions of dogs when exposed to stimuli presented in different modalities, such as photographs, audio recordings or odor samples, or to humans simulating various emotional situations. In the current study, dog owners were manipulated to genuinely experience emotions of happiness, sadness, and neutrality. We measured how dogs responded to their owners' authentic emotions in two different natural situations: induction of the emotion through a video clip and training of a new task. Through a detailed analysis of dog behavior in these naturalistic settings, we investigated whether dogs show behavioral responses to genuine human emotions. We found that dogs behaved differently depending on the owner's emotional state: they gazed and jumped less at owners when they were sad, and their compliance with the 'sit' command was also diminished. When owners were happy, dogs performed better in the trained task. These results are discussed in light of how dogs perceive human emotional expressions and the adaptive value of this skill.

最近有几项研究对狗如何感知人类的情绪表达进行了调查。这些研究测量了狗在受到照片、录音或气味样本等不同方式的刺激时,或在人类模拟各种情绪情景时的反应。在当前的研究中,狗主人被操纵真正体验快乐、悲伤和中立的情绪。我们测量了狗在两种不同的自然情况下对主人真实情绪的反应:通过视频短片诱导情绪和新任务训练。通过详细分析狗在这些自然环境中的行为,我们研究了狗是否会对人类的真实情感做出行为反应。我们发现,狗的行为会因主人的情绪状态而有所不同:当主人悲伤时,它们会减少对主人的注视和跳跃,对 "坐下 "命令的服从性也会降低。当主人高兴时,狗在训练任务中表现得更好。我们将根据狗狗如何感知人类的情绪表达以及这种技能的适应价值来讨论这些结果。
{"title":"Dogs distinguish authentic human emotions without being empathic.","authors":"Juliane Bräuer, Dorothea Eichentopf, Nomi Gebele, Louise Jandke, Veronique Mann, Katharina Schulte, Yana Bender","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01899-x","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01899-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Several recent studies have investigated how dogs perceive human emotional expressions. They have measured the reactions of dogs when exposed to stimuli presented in different modalities, such as photographs, audio recordings or odor samples, or to humans simulating various emotional situations. In the current study, dog owners were manipulated to genuinely experience emotions of happiness, sadness, and neutrality. We measured how dogs responded to their owners' authentic emotions in two different natural situations: induction of the emotion through a video clip and training of a new task. Through a detailed analysis of dog behavior in these naturalistic settings, we investigated whether dogs show behavioral responses to genuine human emotions. We found that dogs behaved differently depending on the owner's emotional state: they gazed and jumped less at owners when they were sad, and their compliance with the 'sit' command was also diminished. When owners were happy, dogs performed better in the trained task. These results are discussed in light of how dogs perceive human emotional expressions and the adaptive value of this skill.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-09-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11416375/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142279384","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cross-population variation in usage of a call combination: evidence of signal usage flexibility in wild bonobos. 呼叫组合使用的跨种群变异:野生倭黑猩猩信号使用灵活性的证据。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-30 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01884-4
Isaac Schamberg, Martin Surbeck, Simon W Townsend

The arbitrary relationship between signifier and signified is one of the features responsible for language's extreme lability, adaptability, and expressiveness. Understanding this arbitrariness and its emergence is essential in any account of the evolution of language. To shed light on the phylogeny of the phenomenon, comparative data examining the relationship between signal form and function in the communication systems of non-humans is central. Here we report the results of a study on the production and usage the whistle-high hoot call combination (W + HH) from two distant populations of wild bonobos (Pan paniscus): Lui Kotale, DRC, and Kokolopori, DRC. We find that the context in which bonobos produce the W + HHs varies systematically between populations. Our results suggest that variation in W + HH production may represent an example of signal-adjustment optionality, a key component of arbitrariness.

符号和被符号之间的任意关系是语言极易变通、适应性强和表现力强的特点之一。理解这种任意性及其出现对于任何语言进化论都至关重要。为了揭示这一现象的系统发育过程,研究非人类交流系统中信号形式与功能之间关系的比较数据至关重要。在此,我们报告了对两个远距离野生倭黑猩猩(Pan paniscus)种群的哨声-高叫声组合(W+HH)的产生和使用的研究结果:刚果(金)的 Lui Kotale 和刚果(金)的 Kokolopori。我们发现,倭黑猩猩产生 W + HHs 的环境在不同种群之间存在系统性差异。我们的研究结果表明,"W+HH "的产生可能是信号调整选择性的一个例子,而信号调整选择性是任意性的一个关键组成部分。
{"title":"Cross-population variation in usage of a call combination: evidence of signal usage flexibility in wild bonobos.","authors":"Isaac Schamberg, Martin Surbeck, Simon W Townsend","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01884-4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-024-01884-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The arbitrary relationship between signifier and signified is one of the features responsible for language's extreme lability, adaptability, and expressiveness. Understanding this arbitrariness and its emergence is essential in any account of the evolution of language. To shed light on the phylogeny of the phenomenon, comparative data examining the relationship between signal form and function in the communication systems of non-humans is central. Here we report the results of a study on the production and usage the whistle-high hoot call combination (W + HH) from two distant populations of wild bonobos (Pan paniscus): Lui Kotale, DRC, and Kokolopori, DRC. We find that the context in which bonobos produce the W + HHs varies systematically between populations. Our results suggest that variation in W + HH production may represent an example of signal-adjustment optionality, a key component of arbitrariness.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-08-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11364580/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142103706","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction to: Welldeveloped spatial reversal learning abilities in harbor seals (Phoca vitulina) : Issue : 25_5_Issue. Correction to:港海豹发育成熟的空间逆向学习能力 :期号 : 25_5_Issue.
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-30 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01893-3
Benedikt Niesterok, Shanie Martin, Lisa Hildebrand, Guido Dehnhardt, Frederike D Hanke
{"title":"Correction to: Welldeveloped spatial reversal learning abilities in harbor seals (Phoca vitulina) : Issue : 25_5_Issue.","authors":"Benedikt Niesterok, Shanie Martin, Lisa Hildebrand, Guido Dehnhardt, Frederike D Hanke","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01893-3","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-024-01893-3","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-08-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11364613/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142103705","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Surfacing of Latent Time Memories Supports the Representational Basis of Timing Behavior in Mice. 潜伏时间记忆的浮现支持小鼠计时行为的表征基础
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-29 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01889-z
Tutku Öztel, Fuat Balcı

Animals can adapt their reward expectancy to changes in delays to reward availability. When temporal relations are altered, associative models of interval timing predict that the original time memory is lost due to the updating of the underlying associative weights, whereas the representational models render the preservation of the original time memory (as previously demonstrated in the extinction of conditioned fear). The current study presents the critical test of these theoretical accounts by training mice with two different intervals in a consecutive fashion (short → long or long → short) and then testing timing behaviors during extinction where neither temporal relation is in effect. Mice that were trained with the long interval first clustered their anticipatory responses around the average of two intervals (indirect higher-order manifestation of two memories in the form of temporal averaging), whereas mice trained with the short interval first clustered their responses either around the short or long interval (direct manifestation of memory representations by their independent indexing). We assert that the original memory representation formed during training with the long interval "metrically affords" the integration of subsequent experiences with a shorter interval, allowing their co-activation during extinction. The original memory representation formed during training with the short interval would not metrically afford such integration and thus result in the formation of a new (mutually exclusive) time memory representation, which does not afford their co-activation during extinction. Our results provide strong support for the representational account of interval timing. We provide a new theoretical account of these findings based on the "metric affordances" of the original memory representation formed during training with the original intervals.

动物可以根据奖励获得延迟的变化来调整它们的奖励预期。当时间关系发生改变时,时间间隔计时的联想模型预测,由于基本联想权重的更新,原始时间记忆会丢失,而表征模型则预测原始时间记忆会保留(正如之前在条件性恐惧的消退中证明的那样)。目前的研究对这些理论观点进行了关键性的测试,方法是用两种不同的时间间隔(短→长或长→短)连续训练小鼠,然后在两种时间关系都不生效的情况下测试消退过程中的计时行为。接受长间隔训练的小鼠首先将其预期反应集中在两个间隔的平均值周围(以时间平均的形式间接高阶表现出两种记忆),而接受短间隔训练的小鼠则首先将其反应集中在短间隔或长间隔周围(通过独立索引直接表现出记忆表征)。我们断言,在长间隔训练中形成的原始记忆表征 "度量性地提供 "了较短间隔的后续经验的整合,从而使它们在消退过程中被共同激活。而在短时间间隔训练中形成的原始记忆表征则无法进行这种整合,因此会形成一种新的(相互排斥的)时间记忆表征,这种表征无法在消亡过程中共同激活它们。我们的研究结果为时间间隔计时的表征理论提供了强有力的支持。我们根据在原始时间间隔训练过程中形成的原始记忆表征的 "度量能力",为这些发现提供了新的理论解释。
{"title":"Surfacing of Latent Time Memories Supports the Representational Basis of Timing Behavior in Mice.","authors":"Tutku Öztel, Fuat Balcı","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01889-z","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01889-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Animals can adapt their reward expectancy to changes in delays to reward availability. When temporal relations are altered, associative models of interval timing predict that the original time memory is lost due to the updating of the underlying associative weights, whereas the representational models render the preservation of the original time memory (as previously demonstrated in the extinction of conditioned fear). The current study presents the critical test of these theoretical accounts by training mice with two different intervals in a consecutive fashion (short → long or long → short) and then testing timing behaviors during extinction where neither temporal relation is in effect. Mice that were trained with the long interval first clustered their anticipatory responses around the average of two intervals (indirect higher-order manifestation of two memories in the form of temporal averaging), whereas mice trained with the short interval first clustered their responses either around the short or long interval (direct manifestation of memory representations by their independent indexing). We assert that the original memory representation formed during training with the long interval \"metrically affords\" the integration of subsequent experiences with a shorter interval, allowing their co-activation during extinction. The original memory representation formed during training with the short interval would not metrically afford such integration and thus result in the formation of a new (mutually exclusive) time memory representation, which does not afford their co-activation during extinction. Our results provide strong support for the representational account of interval timing. We provide a new theoretical account of these findings based on the \"metric affordances\" of the original memory representation formed during training with the original intervals.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-08-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11362479/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142103707","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Serial visual reversal learning in captive black-handed spider monkeys, Ateles geoffroyi. 人工饲养的黑手蜘蛛猴(Ateles geoffroyi)的序列视觉反转学习。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-13 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01897-z
Jules Dorschner, Laura Teresa Hernandez Salazar, Matthias Laska

Recent research suggests that socio-ecological factors such as dietary specialization and social complexity may be drivers of advanced cognitive skills among primates. Therefore, we assessed the ability of 12 black-handed spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi), a highly frugivorous platyrrhine primate with strong fission-fusion dynamics, to succeed in a serial visual reversal learning task. Using a two-alternative choice paradigm we first trained the animals to reliably choose a rewarded visual stimulus over a non-rewarded one. Upon reaching a pre-set learning criterion we then switched the reward values of the two stimuli and assessed if and how quickly the animals learned to reverse their choices, again to a pre-set learning criterion. This stimulus reversal procedure was then continued for a total of 80 sessions of 10 trials each. We found that the spider monkeys quickly learned to reliably discriminate between two simultaneously presented visual stimuli, that they succeeded in a visual reversal learning task, and that they displayed an increase in learning speed across consecutive reversals, suggesting that they are capable of serial reversal learning-set formation with visual cues. The fastest-learning individual completed five reversals within the 80 sessions. The spider monkeys outperformed most other primate and nonprimate mammal species tested so far on this type of cognitive task, including chimpanzees, with regard to their learning speed in both the initial learning task and in the first reversal task, suggesting a high degree of behavioral flexibility and inhibitory control. Our findings support the notion that socio-ecological factors such as dietary specialization and social complexity foster advanced cognitive skills in primates.

最近的研究表明,饮食专门化和社会复杂性等社会生态因素可能是灵长类动物高级认知技能的驱动因素。因此,我们评估了12只黑手蜘蛛猴(Ateles geoffroyi)在连续视觉反转学习任务中的成功能力。我们首先使用双向选择范式训练动物可靠地选择有奖励的视觉刺激而不是无奖励的视觉刺激。在达到预先设定的学习标准后,我们将两个刺激物的奖励值进行调换,然后评估动物是否以及如何快速学会反向选择,并再次达到预先设定的学习标准。这种刺激逆转程序一共进行了 80 次,每次 10 个试验。我们发现,蜘蛛猴很快就学会了可靠地区分两个同时呈现的视觉刺激,它们在视觉反转学习任务中取得了成功,并且在连续反转中显示出学习速度的提高,这表明它们能够利用视觉线索形成连续的反转学习集。学习速度最快的个体在80次训练中完成了5次反转。在初始学习任务和第一次反转任务中,蜘蛛猴的学习速度超过了迄今为止在这类认知任务中测试过的大多数灵长类和非灵长类哺乳动物,包括黑猩猩,这表明它们具有高度的行为灵活性和抑制控制能力。我们的研究结果支持这样一种观点,即社会生态因素(如饮食专门化和社会复杂性)促进了灵长类动物高级认知技能的发展。
{"title":"Serial visual reversal learning in captive black-handed spider monkeys, Ateles geoffroyi.","authors":"Jules Dorschner, Laura Teresa Hernandez Salazar, Matthias Laska","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01897-z","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01897-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent research suggests that socio-ecological factors such as dietary specialization and social complexity may be drivers of advanced cognitive skills among primates. Therefore, we assessed the ability of 12 black-handed spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi), a highly frugivorous platyrrhine primate with strong fission-fusion dynamics, to succeed in a serial visual reversal learning task. Using a two-alternative choice paradigm we first trained the animals to reliably choose a rewarded visual stimulus over a non-rewarded one. Upon reaching a pre-set learning criterion we then switched the reward values of the two stimuli and assessed if and how quickly the animals learned to reverse their choices, again to a pre-set learning criterion. This stimulus reversal procedure was then continued for a total of 80 sessions of 10 trials each. We found that the spider monkeys quickly learned to reliably discriminate between two simultaneously presented visual stimuli, that they succeeded in a visual reversal learning task, and that they displayed an increase in learning speed across consecutive reversals, suggesting that they are capable of serial reversal learning-set formation with visual cues. The fastest-learning individual completed five reversals within the 80 sessions. The spider monkeys outperformed most other primate and nonprimate mammal species tested so far on this type of cognitive task, including chimpanzees, with regard to their learning speed in both the initial learning task and in the first reversal task, suggesting a high degree of behavioral flexibility and inhibitory control. Our findings support the notion that socio-ecological factors such as dietary specialization and social complexity foster advanced cognitive skills in primates.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-08-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11322210/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141970494","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sex-specific discrimination of familiar and unfamiliar mates in the Tokay gecko. 托卡伊壁虎对熟悉和不熟悉配偶的性别鉴别。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-07 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01896-0
Marie-Ornélia Verger, Maëlle Devillebichot, Eva Ringler, Birgit Szabo

Social animals need to keep track of other individuals in their group to be able to adjust their behaviour accordingly and facilitate group cohesion. This recognition ability varies across species and is influenced by cognitive capacities such as learning and memory. In reptiles, particularly Squamates (lizards, snakes, and worm lizards), chemical communication is pivotal for territoriality, reproduction, and other social interactions. However, the cognitive processes underlying these social interactions remain understudied. In our study, we examined the ability of male and female Tokay geckos (Gekko gecko) to chemically differentiate familiar and unfamiliar mating partners. Our findings suggest that both sexes can make this distinction, with males responding more to the odour of a familiar mate, and females responding more to unfamiliar mates. The lizards maintained their discriminatory abilities for two to three weeks but not up to six weeks after separation. This research highlights the efficacy of using odours as social stimuli for investigating social cognition in lizards, a promising avenue to better understand social cognition in these animals.

社会性动物需要跟踪群体中的其他个体,以便能够相应地调整自己的行为,促进群体的凝聚力。这种识别能力因物种而异,并受到学习和记忆等认知能力的影响。在爬行动物,尤其是有鳞类动物(蜥蜴、蛇和蚓蜥)中,化学交流对于领地划分、繁殖和其他社会互动至关重要。然而,这些社会交往背后的认知过程仍未得到充分研究。在我们的研究中,我们考察了雄性和雌性托凯壁虎(Gekko gecko)用化学方法区分熟悉和不熟悉的交配对象的能力。我们的研究结果表明,雌雄壁虎都能做出这种区分,雄性对熟悉配偶的气味反应更大,而雌性对陌生配偶的气味反应更大。蜥蜴的辨别能力可以维持两到三周,但在分离后的六周内则无法维持。这项研究强调了利用气味作为社会刺激来研究蜥蜴的社会认知的有效性,这是更好地了解这些动物的社会认知的一个很有前景的途径。
{"title":"Sex-specific discrimination of familiar and unfamiliar mates in the Tokay gecko.","authors":"Marie-Ornélia Verger, Maëlle Devillebichot, Eva Ringler, Birgit Szabo","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01896-0","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01896-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Social animals need to keep track of other individuals in their group to be able to adjust their behaviour accordingly and facilitate group cohesion. This recognition ability varies across species and is influenced by cognitive capacities such as learning and memory. In reptiles, particularly Squamates (lizards, snakes, and worm lizards), chemical communication is pivotal for territoriality, reproduction, and other social interactions. However, the cognitive processes underlying these social interactions remain understudied. In our study, we examined the ability of male and female Tokay geckos (Gekko gecko) to chemically differentiate familiar and unfamiliar mating partners. Our findings suggest that both sexes can make this distinction, with males responding more to the odour of a familiar mate, and females responding more to unfamiliar mates. The lizards maintained their discriminatory abilities for two to three weeks but not up to six weeks after separation. This research highlights the efficacy of using odours as social stimuli for investigating social cognition in lizards, a promising avenue to better understand social cognition in these animals.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-08-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11306719/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141896562","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A comparative evaluation of the role of olfaction in attachment. 比较评估嗅觉在依恋中的作用。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-30 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01891-5
Emma Cox, Courtney Collins-Pisano, Lane Montgomery, Jeffrey S Katz

Olfactory information plays an important role in the attachment and bonding processes for both humans and non-human animals. Odor cues obtained via individual body odor facilitate attachment and bonding processes across species with regard to both mate selection and mother-infant bonding. The purpose of the present paper is to summarize the role of odor as it pertains to bond formation and maintenance in the mother-infant bond for human infants and non-human animal infants, and for mate selection among human adults and non-human animals. We then synthesize this summary with literature on attachment and existing evidence for the relationships between olfaction and attachment processes. Finally, we suggest avenues for areas of future research.

嗅觉信息在人类和非人类动物的依恋和结合过程中发挥着重要作用。通过个体体味获得的气味线索促进了不同物种在配偶选择和母婴结合方面的依恋和结合过程。本文的目的是总结气味在人类婴儿和非人类动物婴儿的母婴纽带中,以及在人类成年人和非人类动物的配偶选择中,对纽带的形成和维持所起的作用。然后,我们将这一总结与有关依恋的文献以及嗅觉与依恋过程之间关系的现有证据进行综合。最后,我们提出了未来研究的方向。
{"title":"A comparative evaluation of the role of olfaction in attachment.","authors":"Emma Cox, Courtney Collins-Pisano, Lane Montgomery, Jeffrey S Katz","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01891-5","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01891-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Olfactory information plays an important role in the attachment and bonding processes for both humans and non-human animals. Odor cues obtained via individual body odor facilitate attachment and bonding processes across species with regard to both mate selection and mother-infant bonding. The purpose of the present paper is to summarize the role of odor as it pertains to bond formation and maintenance in the mother-infant bond for human infants and non-human animal infants, and for mate selection among human adults and non-human animals. We then synthesize this summary with literature on attachment and existing evidence for the relationships between olfaction and attachment processes. Finally, we suggest avenues for areas of future research.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-07-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11289241/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141854470","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Looks like home: numerosity, but not spatial frequency guides preference in zebrafish larvae (Danio rerio) 看起来像家:斑马鱼幼体(Danio rerio)的偏好受数量而非空间频率的影响
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-27 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01888-0
Elisabeth Adam, Mirko Zanon, Andrea Messina, Giorgio Vallortigara

Despite their young age, zebrafish larvae have a well-developed visual system and can distinguish between different visual stimuli. First, we investigated if the first visual surroundings the larvae experience during the first days after hatching shape their habitat preference. Indeed, these animals seem to “imprint” on the first surroundings they see and select visual stimuli accordingly at 7 days post fertilization (dpf). In particular, if zebrafish larvae experience a bar background just after hatching, they later on prefer bars over white stimuli, and vice versa. We then used this acquired preference for bars to investigate innate numerical abilities. We wanted to specifically test if the zebrafish larvae show real numerical abilities or if they rely on a lower-level mechanism—i.e. spatial frequency—to discriminate between two different numerosities. When we matched the spatial frequency in stimuli with different numbers of bars, the larvae reliably selected the higher numerosity. A previous study has ruled out that 7 dpf zebrafish larvae use convex hull, cumulative surface area and density to choose between two numerosities. Therefore, our results indicate that zebrafish larvae rely on real numerical abilities rather than other cues, including spatial frequency, when spontaneously comparing two sets with different numbers of bars.

尽管斑马鱼幼体年龄较小,但它们的视觉系统非常发达,能够区分不同的视觉刺激。首先,我们研究了斑马鱼幼体在孵化后最初几天所经历的第一个视觉环境是否会影响它们对栖息地的偏好。事实上,斑马鱼似乎在受精后 7 天(dpf)就会 "印记 "它们看到的第一个环境,并据此选择视觉刺激。特别是,如果斑马鱼幼体刚孵化出来就经历了条形背景,那么它们以后会更喜欢条形而不是白色刺激,反之亦然。然后,我们利用斑马鱼对条纹的这种后天偏好来研究先天的数字能力。我们想特别测试斑马鱼幼体是否表现出真正的数字能力,或者它们是否依赖较低级的机制(即空间频率)来区分两种不同的数字。当我们在不同条数的刺激中匹配空间频率时,斑马鱼幼体可靠地选择了数值较高的条数。之前的研究已经排除了斑马鱼7 dpf幼体利用凸壳、累积表面积和密度来选择两种数值的可能性。因此,我们的研究结果表明,斑马鱼幼体在自发比较两组不同条数的数字时,依靠的是真实的数字能力,而不是其他线索,包括空间频率。
{"title":"Looks like home: numerosity, but not spatial frequency guides preference in zebrafish larvae (Danio rerio)","authors":"Elisabeth Adam, Mirko Zanon, Andrea Messina, Giorgio Vallortigara","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01888-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-024-01888-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Despite their young age, zebrafish larvae have a well-developed visual system and can distinguish between different visual stimuli. First, we investigated if the first visual surroundings the larvae experience during the first days after hatching shape their habitat preference. Indeed, these animals seem to “imprint” on the first surroundings they see and select visual stimuli accordingly at 7 days post fertilization (dpf). In particular, if zebrafish larvae experience a bar background just after hatching, they later on prefer bars over white stimuli, and vice versa. We then used this acquired preference for bars to investigate innate numerical abilities. We wanted to specifically test if the zebrafish larvae show real numerical abilities or if they rely on a lower-level mechanism—i.e. spatial frequency—to discriminate between two different numerosities. When we matched the spatial frequency in stimuli with different numbers of bars, the larvae reliably selected the higher numerosity. A previous study has ruled out that 7 dpf zebrafish larvae use convex hull, cumulative surface area and density to choose between two numerosities. Therefore, our results indicate that zebrafish larvae rely on real numerical abilities rather than other cues, including spatial frequency, when spontaneously comparing two sets with different numbers of bars.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141771062","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Investigating the relationship between physical cognitive tasks and a social cognitive task in a wild bird. 研究野生鸟类的体能认知任务与社会认知任务之间的关系。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-26 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01892-4
Grace Blackburn, Benjamin J Ashton, Alex Thornton, Holly Hunter, Sarah Woodiss-Field, Amanda R Ridley

Despite considerable research into the structure of cognition in non-human animal species, there is still much debate as to whether animal cognition is organised as a series of discrete domains or an overarching general cognitive factor. In humans, the existence of general intelligence is widely accepted, but less work has been undertaken in animal psychometrics to address this question. The relatively few studies on non-primate animal species that do investigate the structure of cognition rarely include tasks assessing social cognition and focus instead on physical cognitive tasks. In this study, we tested 36 wild Western Australian magpies (Gymnorhina tibicen dorsalis) on a battery of three physical (associative learning, spatial memory, and numerical assessment) and one social (observational spatial memory) cognitive task, to investigate if cognition in this species fits a general cognitive factor model, or instead one of separate physical and social cognitive domains. A principal component analysis (PCA) identified two principal components with eigenvalues exceeding 1; a first component onto which all three physical tasks loaded strongly and positively, and a second component onto which only the social task (observational spatial memory) loaded strongly and positively. These findings provide tentative evidence for separate physical and social cognitive domains in this species, and highlight the importance of including tasks assessing both social and physical cognition in cognitive test batteries.

尽管对非人类动物物种的认知结构进行了大量研究,但关于动物认知是由一系列离散的领域组成,还是由一个总体性的认知因素组成,仍然存在很多争论。在人类中,一般智力的存在已被广泛接受,但在动物心理测量学中,针对这一问题的研究却较少。相对较少的非灵长类动物认知结构研究很少包括评估社会认知的任务,而是侧重于物理认知任务。在这项研究中,我们对 36 只野生西澳大利亚喜鹊(Gymnorhina tibicen dorsalis)进行了一系列测试,包括三项物理(联想学习、空间记忆和数字评估)和一项社会(观察性空间记忆)认知任务,以研究该物种的认知是否符合一般认知因子模型,还是符合独立的物理和社会认知领域模型。主成分分析(PCA)确定了两个特征值超过 1 的主成分;第一个成分对所有三项物理任务都有强烈的正向加载,第二个成分对只有社会任务(观察空间记忆)才有强烈的正向加载。这些发现初步证明了该物种具有独立的物理和社会认知领域,并强调了在认知测试中包含评估社会和物理认知任务的重要性。
{"title":"Investigating the relationship between physical cognitive tasks and a social cognitive task in a wild bird.","authors":"Grace Blackburn, Benjamin J Ashton, Alex Thornton, Holly Hunter, Sarah Woodiss-Field, Amanda R Ridley","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01892-4","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01892-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Despite considerable research into the structure of cognition in non-human animal species, there is still much debate as to whether animal cognition is organised as a series of discrete domains or an overarching general cognitive factor. In humans, the existence of general intelligence is widely accepted, but less work has been undertaken in animal psychometrics to address this question. The relatively few studies on non-primate animal species that do investigate the structure of cognition rarely include tasks assessing social cognition and focus instead on physical cognitive tasks. In this study, we tested 36 wild Western Australian magpies (Gymnorhina tibicen dorsalis) on a battery of three physical (associative learning, spatial memory, and numerical assessment) and one social (observational spatial memory) cognitive task, to investigate if cognition in this species fits a general cognitive factor model, or instead one of separate physical and social cognitive domains. A principal component analysis (PCA) identified two principal components with eigenvalues exceeding 1; a first component onto which all three physical tasks loaded strongly and positively, and a second component onto which only the social task (observational spatial memory) loaded strongly and positively. These findings provide tentative evidence for separate physical and social cognitive domains in this species, and highlight the importance of including tasks assessing both social and physical cognition in cognitive test batteries.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-07-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11281958/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141764896","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Animal Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1