首页 > 最新文献

Animal Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Transitive reasoning in the adult domestic hen in a six-term series task. 成年家鸡在六学期系列任务中的转折推理。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-19 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01914-1
R Degrande, O Amichaud, B Piégu, F Cornilleau, P Jardat, V H B Ferreira, V Colson, L Lansade, L Calandreau

Transitive inference (TI) is a disjunctive syllogism that allows an individual to indirectly infer a relationship between two components, by knowing their respective relationship to a third component (if A > B and B > C, then A > C). The common procedure is the 5-term series task, in which individuals are tested on indirect, unlearned relations. Few bird species have been tested for TI to date, which limits our knowledge of the phylogenetic spread of such reasoning ability. Here we tested TI in adult laying hens using a more solid methodology, the 6-term series task, which has not been tested in poultry so far. Six hens were trained to learn direct relationships in a sequence of six arbitrary items (A > B > C > D > E > F) in a hybrid training procedure. Then, 12 testing sessions were run, comprising 3 non-rewarded inference trials each: BD, BE, and CE. All subjects showed TI within 12 inference trials and were capable of TI whatever the relative distance between the items in the series. We found that TI performance was not impacted by the reinforcement ratios of the items for most individuals, making it harder to support a purely associative-based resolution of the task. We suggest that TI is based on the same cognitive processes in poultry (Galloanserae) than in modern flying birds (Neoaves), and that the cognitive strategy to solve the task might be driven mainly by individual parameters within species. These results contribute to a better understanding of transitive inference processes in birds.

及物推理 (Transjunctive inference,TI) 是一种分条件三段论,它允许个体通过了解两个部分与第三个部分的关系,间接推断出这两个部分之间的关系(如果 A > B,B > C,那么 A > C)。常见的程序是五项序列任务,在这项任务中,个体要接受间接的、未学习过的关系测试。迄今为止,很少有鸟类物种接受过TI测试,这限制了我们对这种推理能力的系统发育分布的了解。在这里,我们使用一种更可靠的方法对成年蛋鸡的推理能力进行了测试,即迄今为止尚未在家禽中测试过的 6 项系列任务。六只母鸡在混合训练程序中学习了由六个任意项目组成的序列(A > B > C > D > E > F)中的直接关系。然后,进行了 12 次测试,每次包括 3 个无奖励推理试验:BD、BE 和 CE。所有受试者都在 12 次推理试验中表现出了 TI,而且无论系列项目之间的相对距离如何,他们都能进行 TI。我们发现,对于大多数人来说,TI 表现并不受项目强化比率的影响,因此很难支持纯粹基于联想的任务解决方案。我们认为,与现代飞禽(Neoaves)相比,家禽(Galloanserae)的TI基于相同的认知过程,而解决任务的认知策略可能主要受物种内个体参数的驱动。这些结果有助于更好地理解鸟类的传递推理过程。
{"title":"Transitive reasoning in the adult domestic hen in a six-term series task.","authors":"R Degrande, O Amichaud, B Piégu, F Cornilleau, P Jardat, V H B Ferreira, V Colson, L Lansade, L Calandreau","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01914-1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-024-01914-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Transitive inference (TI) is a disjunctive syllogism that allows an individual to indirectly infer a relationship between two components, by knowing their respective relationship to a third component (if A > B and B > C, then A > C). The common procedure is the 5-term series task, in which individuals are tested on indirect, unlearned relations. Few bird species have been tested for TI to date, which limits our knowledge of the phylogenetic spread of such reasoning ability. Here we tested TI in adult laying hens using a more solid methodology, the 6-term series task, which has not been tested in poultry so far. Six hens were trained to learn direct relationships in a sequence of six arbitrary items (A > B > C > D > E > F) in a hybrid training procedure. Then, 12 testing sessions were run, comprising 3 non-rewarded inference trials each: BD, BE, and CE. All subjects showed TI within 12 inference trials and were capable of TI whatever the relative distance between the items in the series. We found that TI performance was not impacted by the reinforcement ratios of the items for most individuals, making it harder to support a purely associative-based resolution of the task. We suggest that TI is based on the same cognitive processes in poultry (Galloanserae) than in modern flying birds (Neoaves), and that the cognitive strategy to solve the task might be driven mainly by individual parameters within species. These results contribute to a better understanding of transitive inference processes in birds.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"27 1","pages":"77"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-11-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142666918","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Basal cognition: shifting the center of gravity (again) 基本认知:重心的转移(再次)。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-11-06 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01832-8
Pamela Lyon, Ken Cheng
{"title":"Basal cognition: shifting the center of gravity (again)","authors":"Pamela Lyon,&nbsp;Ken Cheng","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01832-8","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01832-8","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"26 6","pages":"1743 - 1750"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-11-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71477255","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Hoarding titmice predominantly use Familiarity, and not Recollection, when remembering cache locations 囤积山雀在记忆缓存位置时主要使用熟悉度,而不是重新收集。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-21 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01829-3
Tom V. Smulders, Laura J. Douglas, Daniel Reza, Lucinda H. Male, Alexander Prysce, Amélie Alix, Alexander de Guzman Dodd, Jenny C. A. Read

Scatter-hoarding birds find their caches using spatial memory and have an enlarged hippocampus. Finding a cache site could be achieved using either Recollection (a discrete recalling of previously experienced information) or Familiarity (a feeling of “having encountered something before”). In humans, these two processes can be distinguished using receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves. ROC curves for olfactory memory in rats have shown the hippocampus is involved in Recollection, but not Familiarity. We test the hypothesis that food-hoarding birds, having a larger hippocampus, primarily use Recollection to find their caches. We validate a novel method of constructing ROC curves in humans and apply this method to cache retrieval by coal tits (Periparus ater). Both humans and birds mainly use Familiarity in finding their caches, with lower contribution of Recollection. This contribution is not significantly different from chance in birds, but a small contribution cannot be ruled out. Memory performance decreases with increasing retention interval in birds. The ecology of food-hoarding Parids makes it plausible that they mainly use Familiarity in the memory for caches. The larger hippocampus could be related to associating cache contents and temporal context with cache locations, rather than Recollection of the spatial information itself.

零散的囤积鸟类利用空间记忆找到它们的藏匿处,并有一个扩大的海马体。可以使用“回忆”(对以前经历过的信息的离散回忆)或“熟悉”(“以前遇到过什么”的感觉)来找到缓存站点。在人类中,这两个过程可以通过受试者工作特性(ROC)曲线来区分。大鼠嗅觉记忆的ROC曲线显示,海马体参与回忆,但不参与熟悉。我们检验了这样一种假设,即具有较大海马体的囤积食物的鸟类主要使用回忆来寻找它们的储藏处。我们验证了一种在人类中构建ROC曲线的新方法,并将该方法应用于山雀(Periparus ater)的缓存检索。人类和鸟类在寻找藏匿处时都主要使用熟悉度,而重新收集的贡献较小。这种贡献与鸟类的偶然性没有显著差异,但不能排除一小部分贡献。鸟类的记忆性能随着停留时间的增加而下降。食物囤积区的生态环境使得他们主要使用记忆中的熟悉度作为储藏室是合理的。较大的海马体可能与缓存内容和时间上下文与缓存位置的关联有关,而不是空间信息本身的重新收集。
{"title":"Hoarding titmice predominantly use Familiarity, and not Recollection, when remembering cache locations","authors":"Tom V. Smulders,&nbsp;Laura J. Douglas,&nbsp;Daniel Reza,&nbsp;Lucinda H. Male,&nbsp;Alexander Prysce,&nbsp;Amélie Alix,&nbsp;Alexander de Guzman Dodd,&nbsp;Jenny C. A. Read","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01829-3","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01829-3","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Scatter-hoarding birds find their caches using spatial memory and have an enlarged hippocampus. Finding a cache site could be achieved using either Recollection (a discrete recalling of previously experienced information) or Familiarity (a feeling of “having encountered something before”). In humans, these two processes can be distinguished using receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves. ROC curves for olfactory memory in rats have shown the hippocampus is involved in Recollection, but not Familiarity. We test the hypothesis that food-hoarding birds, having a larger hippocampus, primarily use Recollection to find their caches. We validate a novel method of constructing ROC curves in humans and apply this method to cache retrieval by coal tits (<i>Periparus ater</i>). Both humans and birds mainly use Familiarity in finding their caches, with lower contribution of Recollection. This contribution is not significantly different from chance in birds, but a small contribution cannot be ruled out. Memory performance decreases with increasing retention interval in birds. The ecology of food-hoarding Parids makes it plausible that they mainly use Familiarity in the memory for caches. The larger hippocampus could be related to associating cache contents and temporal context with cache locations, rather than Recollection of the spatial information itself.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"26 6","pages":"1929 - 1943"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-10-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10769918/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49673449","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Spot the odd one out: do snake pictures capture macaques’ attention more than other predators? 找出一个奇怪的问题:蛇的照片比其他捕食者更能吸引猕猴的注意力吗?
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-19 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01831-9
Karl Zeller, Sébastien Ballesta, Hélène Meunier, Julie Duboscq, Luca Morino, Adam Rimele, Xavier Bonnet, Audrey Maille, Guillaume Dezecache, Cécile Garcia

Detecting and identifying predators quickly is key to survival. According to the Snake Detection Theory (SDT), snakes have been a substantive threat to primates for millions of years, so that dedicated visual skills were tuned to detect snakes in early primates. Past experiments confronted the SDT by measuring how fast primate subjects detected snake pictures among non-dangerous distractors (e.g., flowers), but did not include pictures of primates’ other predators, such as carnivorans, raptors, and crocodilians. Here, we examined the detection abilities of N = 19 Tonkean macaques (Macaca tonkeana) and N = 6 rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) to spot different predators. By implementing an oddity task protocol, we recorded success rates and reaction times to locate a deviant picture among four pictures over more than 400,000 test trials. Pictures depicted a predator, a non-predator animal, or a simple geometric shape. The first task consisted of detecting a deviant picture among identical distractor pictures (discrimination) and the second task was designed to evaluate detection abilities of a deviant picture among different distractor pictures (categorization). The macaques detected pictures of geometric shapes better and faster than pictures of animals, and were better and faster at discriminating than categorizing. The macaques did not detect snakes better or faster than other animal categories. Overall, these results suggest that pictures of snakes do not capture visual attention more than other predators, questioning previous findings in favor of the SDT.

快速发现和识别捕食者是生存的关键。根据蛇探测理论(SDT),数百万年来,蛇一直是灵长类动物的实质性威胁,因此专门的视觉技能被调整为在早期灵长类动物中探测蛇。过去的实验通过测量灵长类动物受试者在非危险干扰物(如花朵)中检测到蛇图片的速度来对抗SDT,但不包括灵长类动物其他捕食者的图片,如食肉动物、猛禽和鳄鱼。在这里,我们检验了N = 19 Tonkean猕猴(Macaca tonkeana)和N = 6只恒河猴(猕猴)来发现不同的捕食者。通过实施一个奇怪的任务协议,我们记录了成功率和反应时间,在超过400000次的测试试验中,在四张图片中找到了一张异常图片。图片描绘了捕食者、非捕食者或简单的几何形状。第一个任务包括在相同的干扰物图片中检测异常图片(辨别),第二个任务旨在评估不同干扰物照片中异常图片的检测能力(分类)。猕猴比动物图片更好、更快地检测到几何形状的图片,并且在辨别方面比分类更好、更快。猕猴并没有比其他动物更好或更快地发现蛇。总的来说,这些结果表明,蛇的照片并没有比其他捕食者更能吸引视觉注意力,这对之前支持SDT的发现提出了质疑。
{"title":"Spot the odd one out: do snake pictures capture macaques’ attention more than other predators?","authors":"Karl Zeller,&nbsp;Sébastien Ballesta,&nbsp;Hélène Meunier,&nbsp;Julie Duboscq,&nbsp;Luca Morino,&nbsp;Adam Rimele,&nbsp;Xavier Bonnet,&nbsp;Audrey Maille,&nbsp;Guillaume Dezecache,&nbsp;Cécile Garcia","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01831-9","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01831-9","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Detecting and identifying predators quickly is key to survival. According to the Snake Detection Theory (SDT), snakes have been a substantive threat to primates for millions of years, so that dedicated visual skills were tuned to detect snakes in early primates. Past experiments confronted the SDT by measuring how fast primate subjects detected snake pictures among non-dangerous distractors (e.g., flowers), but did not include pictures of primates’ other predators, such as carnivorans, raptors, and crocodilians. Here, we examined the detection abilities of <i>N</i> = 19 Tonkean macaques (<i>Macaca tonkeana</i>) and <i>N</i> = 6 rhesus macaques (<i>Macaca mulatta</i>) to spot different predators. By implementing an oddity task protocol, we recorded success rates and reaction times to locate a deviant picture among four pictures over more than 400,000 test trials. Pictures depicted a predator, a non-predator animal, or a simple geometric shape. The first task consisted of detecting a deviant picture among identical distractor pictures (discrimination) and the second task was designed to evaluate detection abilities of a deviant picture among different distractor pictures (categorization). The macaques detected pictures of geometric shapes better and faster than pictures of animals, and were better and faster at discriminating than categorizing. The macaques did not detect snakes better or faster than other animal categories. Overall, these results suggest that pictures of snakes do not capture visual attention more than other predators, questioning previous findings in favor of the SDT.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"26 6","pages":"1945 - 1958"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-10-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49673450","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cognitive flexibility in a Tanganyikan bower-building cichlid, Aulonocranus dewindti 坦噶尼喀弓箭手建筑慈鲷Aulonocranus dewindti的认知灵活性。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-18 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01830-w
Maëlan Tomasek, Midori Stark, Valérie Dufour, Alex Jordan

Cognitive flexibility, the ability to modify one’s decision rules to adapt to a new situation, has been extensively studied in many species. In fish, though, data on cognitive flexibility are scarce, especially in the wild. We studied a lekking species of cichlid fish in Lake Tanganyika, Aulonocranus dewindti. Males create sand bowers as spawning sites and maintain them by removing any objects falling into it. In the first part of our experiment, we investigated the existence of spontaneous decision rules for the maintenance of the bowers. We showed that if a snail shell and a stone are placed in their bower, fish prefer to remove the shell first. In the second phase of our experiment, we took advantage of this spontaneous decision rule to investigate whether this rule was flexible. We tested five individuals in a choice against preference task, in which the fish had to modify their preference rule and remove the stone first to be allowed to then remove the shell and have a clean bower. While there was no overall trend towards flexibility in this task, there was variation at an individual level. Some individuals increased their preference for removing the shell first, deciding quickly and with little exploration of the objects. Others were more successful at choosing against preference and showed behaviours suggesting self-regulatory inhibition abilities. Bower-building cichlids could therefore be a promising model to study cognitive flexibility, and other aspects of animal cognition in the wild.

认知灵活性,即修改决策规则以适应新情况的能力,在许多物种中得到了广泛的研究。然而,在鱼类中,关于认知灵活性的数据很少,尤其是在野外。我们研究了坦噶尼喀湖的一种慈鲷,Aulonocranus dewindti。雄性建造沙弓作为产卵场,并通过移除掉到沙弓中的任何物体来维护沙弓。在我们实验的第一部分,我们调查了维护沙弓的自发决策规则的存在。我们发现,如果把蜗牛壳和石头放在它们的弓里,鱼更喜欢先把壳取下来。在我们实验的第二阶段,我们利用这个自发的决策规则来研究这个规则是否灵活。我们在一项针对偏好的选择任务中测试了五个人,在该任务中,鱼必须修改他们的偏好规则,首先移除石头,然后才能移除外壳并拥有一个干净的弓。虽然这项任务总体上没有灵活性的趋势,但在个人层面上存在差异。一些人更喜欢先去掉外壳,快速做出决定,很少探索物体。其他人则更成功地进行了反偏好选择,并表现出暗示自我调节抑制能力的行为。因此,造肠慈鲷可能是研究野生动物认知灵活性和其他方面的一个很有前途的模型。
{"title":"Cognitive flexibility in a Tanganyikan bower-building cichlid, Aulonocranus dewindti","authors":"Maëlan Tomasek,&nbsp;Midori Stark,&nbsp;Valérie Dufour,&nbsp;Alex Jordan","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01830-w","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01830-w","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Cognitive flexibility, the ability to modify one’s decision rules to adapt to a new situation, has been extensively studied in many species. In fish, though, data on cognitive flexibility are scarce, especially in the wild. We studied a lekking species of cichlid fish in Lake Tanganyika, <i>Aulonocranus dewindti</i>. Males create sand bowers as spawning sites and maintain them by removing any objects falling into it. In the first part of our experiment, we investigated the existence of spontaneous decision rules for the maintenance of the bowers. We showed that if a snail shell and a stone are placed in their bower, fish prefer to remove the shell first. In the second phase of our experiment, we took advantage of this spontaneous decision rule to investigate whether this rule was flexible. We tested five individuals in a choice against preference task, in which the fish had to modify their preference rule and remove the stone first to be allowed to then remove the shell and have a clean bower. While there was no overall trend towards flexibility in this task, there was variation at an individual level. Some individuals increased their preference for removing the shell first, deciding quickly and with little exploration of the objects. Others were more successful at choosing against preference and showed behaviours suggesting self-regulatory inhibition abilities. Bower-building cichlids could therefore be a promising model to study cognitive flexibility, and other aspects of animal cognition in the wild.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"26 6","pages":"1959 - 1971"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-10-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10770232/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41231839","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Interspecific differences in developmental mode determine early cognitive abilities in teleost fish 发育模式的种间差异决定了硬骨鱼的早期认知能力。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-13 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01828-4
Giulia Montalbano, Cristiano Bertolucci, Angelo Bisazza, Tyrone Lucon-Xiccato

Most studies on developmental variation in cognition have suggested that individuals are born with reduced or absent cognitive abilities, and thereafter, cognitive performance increases with age during early development. However, these studies have been mainly performed in altricial species, such as humans, in which offspring are extremely immature at birth. In this work, we tested the hypothesis that species with other developmental modes might show different patterns of cognitive development. To this end, we analysed inhibitory control performance in two teleost species with different developmental modes, the zebrafish Danio rerio and the guppy Poecilia reticulata, exploiting a simple paradigm based on spontaneous behaviour and therefore applicable to subjects of different ages. Zebrafish hatch as larvae 3 days after fertilisation, and have an immature nervous system, a situation that mirrors extreme altriciality. We found that at the early stages of development, zebrafish displayed no evidence of inhibitory control, which only begun to emerge after one month of life. Conversely, guppies, which are born after approximately one month of gestation as fully developed and independent individuals, solved the inhibitory control task since their first days of life, although performance increased with sexual maturation. Our study suggests that the typical progression described during early ontogeny in humans and other species might not be the only developmental trend for animals’ cognition and that a species’ developmental mode might determine variation in cognition across subjects of different age.

大多数关于认知发展变化的研究表明,个体天生认知能力下降或缺失,此后,在早期发展过程中,认知表现会随着年龄的增长而增加。然而,这些研究主要在蝙蝠物种中进行,如人类,其后代在出生时极不成熟。在这项工作中,我们检验了具有其他发育模式的物种可能表现出不同的认知发展模式的假设。为此,我们分析了两种具有不同发育模式的硬骨鱼的抑制控制性能,即斑马鱼Danio rerio和网纹孔雀鱼Poecilia reticulata,利用了一种基于自发行为的简单范式,因此适用于不同年龄的受试者。斑马鱼在受精后3天孵化成幼虫,神经系统不成熟,这种情况反映了极端的变态。我们发现,在发育的早期阶段,斑马鱼没有表现出抑制性控制的证据,这种抑制性控制在一个月后才开始出现。相反,怀孕约一个月后出生的孔雀鱼是完全发育和独立的个体,从出生的第一天起就解决了抑制控制任务,尽管随着性成熟,它们的表现会提高。我们的研究表明,人类和其他物种早期个体发育过程中描述的典型进展可能不是动物认知的唯一发展趋势,一个物种的发展模式可能决定不同年龄受试者认知的变化。
{"title":"Interspecific differences in developmental mode determine early cognitive abilities in teleost fish","authors":"Giulia Montalbano,&nbsp;Cristiano Bertolucci,&nbsp;Angelo Bisazza,&nbsp;Tyrone Lucon-Xiccato","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01828-4","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01828-4","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Most studies on developmental variation in cognition have suggested that individuals are born with reduced or absent cognitive abilities, and thereafter, cognitive performance increases with age during early development. However, these studies have been mainly performed in altricial species, such as humans, in which offspring are extremely immature at birth. In this work, we tested the hypothesis that species with other developmental modes might show different patterns of cognitive development. To this end, we analysed inhibitory control performance in two teleost species with different developmental modes, the zebrafish <i>Danio rerio</i> and the guppy <i>Poecilia reticulata</i>, exploiting a simple paradigm based on spontaneous behaviour and therefore applicable to subjects of different ages. Zebrafish hatch as larvae 3 days after fertilisation, and have an immature nervous system, a situation that mirrors extreme altriciality. We found that at the early stages of development, zebrafish displayed no evidence of inhibitory control, which only begun to emerge after one month of life. Conversely, guppies, which are born after approximately one month of gestation as fully developed and independent individuals, solved the inhibitory control task since their first days of life, although performance increased with sexual maturation. Our study suggests that the typical progression described during early ontogeny in humans and other species might not be the only developmental trend for animals’ cognition and that a species’ developmental mode might determine variation in cognition across subjects of different age.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"26 6","pages":"1893 - 1903"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-10-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10769910/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41188883","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Use of the sun compass by monocularly occluded homing pigeons in a food localisation task in an outdoor arena 在户外竞技场的食物定位任务中,单眼遮挡的信鸽使用太阳罗盘。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-10 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01827-5
Sara Cioccarelli, Benedetta Bianchi, Dimitri Giunchi, Anna Gagliardo

Functional asymmetries of the avian visual system can be studied in monocularly occluded birds, as their hemispheres are largely independent. Right and left monocularly occluded homing pigeons and control birds under binocular view have been trained in a food localisation task in an octagonal outdoor arena provided with one coloured beacon on each wall. The three groups were tested after the removal of the visual beacons, so to assess their sun compass learning abilities. Pigeons using the left eye/right hemisphere system exhibited slower learning compared to the other monocular group. During the test in the arena void of visual beacons, the three groups of birds, regardless of their visual condition, were generally able to identify the training sector by exclusively relying on sun compass information. However, the directional choices of the pigeons with the left eye/right hemisphere in use were significantly affected by the removal of the beacons, while both control pigeons and birds with the right eye/left hemisphere in use displayed unaltered performances during the test. A subsample of pigeons of each group were re-trained in the octagonal arena with visual beacons present and tested after the removal of visual beacons after a 6 h fast clock-shift treatment. All birds displayed the expected deflection consistent to the sun compass use. While birds using either the left or the right visual systems were equally able to learn a sun compass-mediated spatial task, the left eye/right hemisphere visual system displayed an advantage in relying on visual beacons.

鸟类视觉系统的功能不对称性可以在单目遮挡的鸟类中进行研究,因为它们的半球在很大程度上是独立的。在每面墙上都有一个彩色信标的八角形户外竞技场中,对双目视野下的左右单眼遮挡信鸽和对照鸟进行了食物定位任务的训练。这三组人在移除视觉信标后接受了测试,以评估他们的太阳罗盘学习能力。与其他单眼组相比,使用左眼/右半球系统的鸽子学习速度较慢。在没有视觉信标的竞技场上进行测试期间,这三组鸟类,无论其视觉状况如何,通常都能够完全依靠太阳罗盘信息来识别训练区域。然而,使用左眼/右半球的鸽子的方向选择受到信标移除的显著影响,而对照鸽子和使用右眼/左半球的鸽子在测试期间都表现出不变的表现。每组鸽子的子样本在有视觉信标的八角形竞技场中重新训练,并在6小时快速时钟移位处理后移除视觉信标后进行测试。所有鸟类都显示出与太阳罗盘使用一致的预期偏转。虽然使用左侧或右侧视觉系统的鸟类同样能够学习太阳罗盘介导的空间任务,但左眼/右半球视觉系统在依赖视觉信标方面显示出优势。
{"title":"Use of the sun compass by monocularly occluded homing pigeons in a food localisation task in an outdoor arena","authors":"Sara Cioccarelli,&nbsp;Benedetta Bianchi,&nbsp;Dimitri Giunchi,&nbsp;Anna Gagliardo","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01827-5","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01827-5","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Functional asymmetries of the avian visual system can be studied in monocularly occluded birds, as their hemispheres are largely independent. Right and left monocularly occluded homing pigeons and control birds under binocular view have been trained in a food localisation task in an octagonal outdoor arena provided with one coloured beacon on each wall. The three groups were tested after the removal of the visual beacons, so to assess their sun compass learning abilities. Pigeons using the left eye/right hemisphere system exhibited slower learning compared to the other monocular group. During the test in the arena void of visual beacons, the three groups of birds, regardless of their visual condition, were generally able to identify the training sector by exclusively relying on sun compass information. However, the directional choices of the pigeons with the left eye/right hemisphere in use were significantly affected by the removal of the beacons, while both control pigeons and birds with the right eye/left hemisphere in use displayed unaltered performances during the test. A subsample of pigeons of each group were re-trained in the octagonal arena with visual beacons present and tested after the removal of visual beacons after a 6 h fast clock-shift treatment. All birds displayed the expected deflection consistent to the sun compass use. While birds using either the left or the right visual systems were equally able to learn a sun compass-mediated spatial task, the left eye/right hemisphere visual system displayed an advantage in relying on visual beacons.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"26 6","pages":"1985 - 1995"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-10-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10769948/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41181838","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
“The song remains the same”: not really! Vocal flexibility in the song of the indris “歌曲不变”:不是真的!印度民歌中的声乐灵活性。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-04 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01826-6
Anna Zanoli, Teresa Raimondi, Chiara De Gregorio, Daria Valente, Filippo Carugati, Valeria Torti, Olivier Friard, Longondraza Miaretsoa, Cristina Giacoma, Marco Gamba

In studying communicative signals, we can think of flexibility as a necessary correlate of creativity. Flexibility enables animals to find practical solutions and appropriate behaviors in mutable situations. In this study, we aimed to quantify the degree of flexibility in the songs of indris (Indri indri), the only singing lemur, using three different metrics: Jaro Distance, normalized diversity, and entropy. We hypothesized that the degree and the co-variation of the flexibility of indris singing together would vary according to their status and sex. We found that dominant females were more flexible than dominant males when concatenating elements into strings (element concatenation). The number of different elements in a song contribution normalized by the contribution length (contribution diversity) of dominant individuals positively co-varied for seven duetting pairs. Non-dominant individuals were more variable in element concatenation than dominant individuals, and they were more diverse in phrase type than dominant females. Independently from sex and status, individual contributions did not differ in entropy (a measure of the predictability of contributions). These results corroborate previous findings regarding the dimorphism by sex and by status of individual contributions to songs. Thus, they shed light on the presence and expression of flexibility in the behavior of a non-human primate species. Indeed, they potentially show an effect of social features in shaping vocal flexibility, which underlies many communication systems, including human language. We speculate that this degree of flexibility may account for creativity.

在研究交际信号时,我们可以将灵活性视为创造力的必要关联。灵活性使动物能够在多变的情况下找到切实可行的解决方案和适当的行为。在这项研究中,我们旨在使用三种不同的指标:Jaro距离、归一化多样性和熵来量化唯一会唱歌的狐猴indris(Indri Indri)歌曲的灵活性。我们假设印度教徒在一起唱歌的灵活性的程度和共同变化会根据他们的地位和性别而变化。我们发现,当将元素连接成字符串(元素连接)时,占优势的雌性比占优势的雄性更灵活。由优势个体的贡献长度(贡献多样性)归一化的歌曲贡献中不同元素的数量在七对二重唱中呈正相关。非显性个体在元素串联方面比显性个体更具可变性,并且在短语类型方面比显性女性更具多样性。与性别和地位无关,个人贡献在熵(贡献可预测性的衡量标准)方面没有差异。这些结果证实了先前关于性别和个人对歌曲贡献状态的二态性的发现。因此,他们揭示了非人类灵长类动物行为中灵活性的存在和表达。事实上,它们可能显示出社会特征在塑造声音灵活性方面的影响,而声音灵活性是包括人类语言在内的许多沟通系统的基础。我们推测,这种程度的灵活性可能是创造力的原因。
{"title":"“The song remains the same”: not really! Vocal flexibility in the song of the indris","authors":"Anna Zanoli,&nbsp;Teresa Raimondi,&nbsp;Chiara De Gregorio,&nbsp;Daria Valente,&nbsp;Filippo Carugati,&nbsp;Valeria Torti,&nbsp;Olivier Friard,&nbsp;Longondraza Miaretsoa,&nbsp;Cristina Giacoma,&nbsp;Marco Gamba","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01826-6","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01826-6","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In studying communicative signals, we can think of flexibility as a necessary correlate of creativity. Flexibility enables animals to find practical solutions and appropriate behaviors in mutable situations. In this study, we aimed to quantify the degree of flexibility in the songs of indris (<i>Indri indri</i>), the only singing lemur, using three different metrics: Jaro Distance, normalized diversity, and entropy. We hypothesized that the degree and the co-variation of the flexibility of indris singing together would vary according to their status and sex. We found that dominant females were more flexible than dominant males when concatenating elements into strings (element concatenation). The number of different elements in a song contribution normalized by the contribution length (contribution diversity) of dominant individuals positively co-varied for seven duetting pairs. Non-dominant individuals were more variable in element concatenation than dominant individuals, and they were more diverse in phrase type than dominant females. Independently from sex and status, individual contributions did not differ in entropy (a measure of the predictability of contributions). These results corroborate previous findings regarding the dimorphism by sex and by status of individual contributions to songs. Thus, they shed light on the presence and expression of flexibility in the behavior of a non-human primate species. Indeed, they potentially show an effect of social features in shaping vocal flexibility, which underlies many communication systems, including human language. We speculate that this degree of flexibility may account for creativity.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"26 6","pages":"2009 - 2021"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-10-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10769932/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41115737","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Orientation by environmental geometry and feature cues in the green and black poison frog (Dendrobates auratus) 绿色和黑色毒蛙(Dendrobates auratus)通过环境几何和特征线索确定方向。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-12 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01820-y
Cody A. Sorrell, Sabrina S. Burmeister

The ability to use environmental geometry when orienting in space reflects an animal’s ability to use a global, allocentric framework. Therefore, understanding when and how animal’s use geometry relative to other types of cues in the environment has interested comparative cognition researchers for decades. Yet, only two amphibians have been tested to date. We trained the poison frog Dendrobates auratus to find goal shelters in a rectangular arena, in the presence and absence of a feature cue, and assessed the relative influence of the two types of cues using probe trials. We chose D. auratus because the species has complex interactions with their physical and social environments, including parental care that requires navigating to and from distant locations. We found that, like many vertebrates, D. auratus are capable of using geometric information to relocate goals. In addition, the frogs preferentially used the more reliable feature cue when the location of the feature conflicted with the geometry of the arena. The frogs were equally successful at using the feature cue when it was proximal or distal to the goal shelter, consistent with prior studies that found that D. auratus can use distal cues in a flexible manner. Our results provide further evidence that amphibians can use environmental geometry during orientation. Future studies that examine when and how amphibians use geometry relative to other types of cues will contribute to a more complete picture of spatial cognition in this important, yet understudied, group.

在确定空间方向时使用环境几何图形的能力反映了动物使用全球分配中心框架的能力。因此,数十年来,比较认知研究人员一直对了解动物在何时以及如何使用几何图形与环境中的其他类型线索进行比较感兴趣。然而,迄今为止只有两种两栖动物接受过测试。我们训练毒蛙在有和没有特征线索的情况下在矩形场地中寻找目标避难所,并通过探究试验评估两种线索的相对影响。我们之所以选择无尾椎蛙,是因为该物种与它们的物理和社会环境有着复杂的相互作用,包括需要往返于遥远地点的父母照顾。我们发现,和许多脊椎动物一样,金蛙能够利用几何信息来重新定位目标。此外,当特征的位置与竞技场的几何形状发生冲突时,蛙类会优先使用更可靠的特征线索。当特征线索位于目标掩蔽物的近端或远端时,蛙类使用特征线索的成功率相同,这与之前的研究结果一致,这些研究发现蛙类可以灵活地使用远端线索。我们的研究结果进一步证明,两栖动物在定向过程中可以利用环境的几何特征。未来的研究将考察两栖动物在何时以及如何使用几何图形而不是其他类型的线索,这将有助于更全面地了解这一重要但研究不足的类群的空间认知。
{"title":"Orientation by environmental geometry and feature cues in the green and black poison frog (Dendrobates auratus)","authors":"Cody A. Sorrell,&nbsp;Sabrina S. Burmeister","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01820-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01820-y","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The ability to use environmental geometry when orienting in space reflects an animal’s ability to use a global, allocentric framework. Therefore, understanding when and how animal’s use geometry relative to other types of cues in the environment has interested comparative cognition researchers for decades. Yet, only two amphibians have been tested to date. We trained the poison frog <i>Dendrobates auratus</i> to find goal shelters in a rectangular arena, in the presence and absence of a feature cue, and assessed the relative influence of the two types of cues using probe trials. We chose <i>D. auratus</i> because the species has complex interactions with their physical and social environments, including parental care that requires navigating to and from distant locations. We found that, like many vertebrates, <i>D. auratus</i> are capable of using geometric information to relocate goals. In addition, the frogs preferentially used the more reliable feature cue when the location of the feature conflicted with the geometry of the arena. The frogs were equally successful at using the feature cue when it was proximal or distal to the goal shelter, consistent with prior studies that found that <i>D. auratus</i> can use distal cues in a flexible manner. Our results provide further evidence that amphibians can use environmental geometry during orientation. Future studies that examine when and how amphibians use geometry relative to other types of cues will contribute to a more complete picture of spatial cognition in this important, yet understudied, group.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"26 6","pages":"2023 - 2030"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10211703","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
First acoustic evidence of signature whistle production by spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris) 飞旋海豚(Stenella longirostris)发出特征性哨声的首个声学证据。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-07 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-023-01824-8
Raul Rio

A dolphin’s signature whistle (SW) is a distinctive acoustic signal, issued in a bout pattern of unique frequency modulation contours; it allows individuals belonging to a given group to recognize each other and, consequently, to maintain contact and cohesion. The current study is the first scientific evidence that spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris) produce SWs. Acoustic data were recorded at a shallow rest bay called “Biboca”, in Fernando de Noronha Archipelago, Brazil. In total, 1902 whistles were analyzed; 40% (753/1,902) of them were classified as stereotyped whistles (STW). Based on the SIGID method, 63% (472/753) of all STWs were identified as SWs; subsequently, they were categorized into one of 18 SW types. SWs accounted for 25% (472/1,902) of the acoustic repertoire. External observers have shown near perfect agreement to classify whistles into the adopted SW categorization. Most acoustic and temporal variables measured for SWs showed mean values similar to those recorded in other studies with spinner dolphins, whose authors did not differentiate SWs from non-SWs. Principal component analysis has explained 78% of total SW variance, and it emphasized the relevance of shape/contour and frequency variables to SW variance. This scientific discovery helps improving bioacoustics knowledge about the investigated species. Future studies to be conducted in Fernando de Noronha Archipelago should focus on continuous investigations about SW development and use by S. longirostris, expanding individuals’ identifications (Photo ID and SW Noronha Catalog), assessing long-term whistle stability and emission rates, and making mother–offspring comparisons with sex-based differences.

海豚的标志性哨音(SW)是一种独特的声学信号,以独特的频率调制轮廓的阵列模式发出;它使属于特定群体的个体能够相互识别,从而保持联系和凝聚力。目前的研究首次从科学角度证明了飞旋海豚(Stenella longirostris)会发出 SWs。声学数据是在巴西费尔南多-德诺罗尼亚群岛一个名为 "比博卡 "的浅水休憩湾记录的。共分析了 1902 个哨音,其中 40% (753/1,902)被归类为定型哨音(STW)。根据 SIGID 方法,所有 STW 中的 63%(472/753)被确定为 SW;随后,它们被归入 18 种 SW 类型之一。SW 占声谱的 25%(472/1,902)。外部观察者在将口哨归入所采用的 SW 分类中显示出近乎完美的一致性。对 SW 类口哨测量的大多数声学和时间变量的平均值与其他针对飞旋海豚的研究中记录的平均值相似,而这些研究的作者并未将 SW 类口哨与非 SW 类口哨区分开来。主成分分析解释了 78% 的 SW 总变异,并强调了形状/轮廓和频率变量与 SW 变异的相关性。这一科学发现有助于提高有关所调查物种的生物声学知识。未来在费尔南多-德诺罗尼亚群岛开展的研究应侧重于继续调查长尾鳕(S. longirostris)的SW发展和使用情况,扩大个体识别范围(照片ID和诺罗尼亚西南群岛目录),评估长期哨声稳定性和发射率,并根据性别差异进行母子比较。
{"title":"First acoustic evidence of signature whistle production by spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris)","authors":"Raul Rio","doi":"10.1007/s10071-023-01824-8","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-023-01824-8","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>A dolphin’s signature whistle (SW) is a distinctive acoustic signal, issued in a bout pattern of unique frequency modulation contours; it allows individuals belonging to a given group to recognize each other and, consequently, to maintain contact and cohesion. The current study is the first scientific evidence that spinner dolphins (<i>Stenella longirostris</i>) produce SWs. Acoustic data were recorded at a shallow rest bay called “Biboca”, in Fernando de Noronha Archipelago, Brazil. In total, 1902 whistles were analyzed; 40% (753/1,902) of them were classified as stereotyped whistles (STW). Based on the SIGID method, 63% (472/753) of all STWs were identified as SWs; subsequently, they were categorized into one of 18 SW types. SWs accounted for 25% (472/1,902) of the acoustic repertoire. External observers have shown near perfect agreement to classify whistles into the adopted SW categorization. Most acoustic and temporal variables measured for SWs showed mean values similar to those recorded in other studies with spinner dolphins, whose authors did not differentiate SWs from non-SWs. Principal component analysis has explained 78% of total SW variance, and it emphasized the relevance of shape/contour and frequency variables to SW variance. This scientific discovery helps improving bioacoustics knowledge about the investigated species. Future studies to be conducted in Fernando de Noronha Archipelago should focus on continuous investigations about SW development and use by <i>S. longirostris</i>, expanding individuals’ identifications (Photo ID and SW Noronha Catalog), assessing long-term whistle stability and emission rates, and making mother–offspring comparisons with sex-based differences.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"26 6","pages":"1915 - 1927"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-09-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10162327","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Animal Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1