首页 > 最新文献

Animal Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Metacognition in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata): does impulsivity explain unnecessary looks in the tubes task? 日本猕猴(Macaca fuscata)的元认知:冲动能否解释浴缸任务中不必要的注视?
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-28 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01879-1
Lorraine Subias, Noriko Katsu, Kazunori Yamada

Potential metacognitive abilities, such as monitoring and controlling cognitive processes, have been revealed in some primate species. In the tubes task, apes and macaques showed higher content-checking behavior when unaware of a reward's location, but they also periodically inspected the tubes when aware, especially when a more appealing reward was involved. Some attribute this to the pleasure of looking at the reward. This study investigates whether the unnecessary tube-checking behavior observed in nine wild Japanese macaques, previously tested for metacognition using the tubes task, can be solely attributed to impulsivity. The macaques' propensity to look inside a single tube containing food they cannot immediately reach was measured and compared to their behavior in the tubes task. Results indicated that looking inside the baited tube increased as reward quality improved. However, macaques displaying unnecessary tube inspections in metacognitive tests showed less impulsivity to look. This intriguing result counters the notion that excessive looking in the tubes task is solely due to impulsive looking, prompting us to advocate for further research into the relationship between inhibition and metacognitive performance.

在一些灵长类动物身上发现了潜在的元认知能力,如监测和控制认知过程。在试管任务中,猿类和猕猴在不知道奖励位置时表现出较高的内容检查行为,但在知道奖励位置时,它们也会定期检查试管,尤其是当奖励更具吸引力时。一些人将此归因于查看奖励所带来的愉悦感。本研究调查了在九只野生日本猕猴身上观察到的不必要的检查管子行为(之前曾用管子任务对它们进行了元认知测试)是否完全归因于冲动。研究人员测量了猕猴查看其无法立即接触到的装有食物的单个试管内部的倾向,并将其与试管任务中的行为进行了比较。结果表明,随着奖赏质量的提高,猕猴往有诱饵的管子里看的行为也会增加。然而,在元认知测试中表现出不必要检查管子的猕猴看管的冲动性较低。这一有趣的结果反驳了在试管任务中过度看管完全是由于冲动性看管的观点,促使我们主张进一步研究抑制与元认知表现之间的关系。
{"title":"Metacognition in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata): does impulsivity explain unnecessary looks in the tubes task?","authors":"Lorraine Subias, Noriko Katsu, Kazunori Yamada","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01879-1","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01879-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Potential metacognitive abilities, such as monitoring and controlling cognitive processes, have been revealed in some primate species. In the tubes task, apes and macaques showed higher content-checking behavior when unaware of a reward's location, but they also periodically inspected the tubes when aware, especially when a more appealing reward was involved. Some attribute this to the pleasure of looking at the reward. This study investigates whether the unnecessary tube-checking behavior observed in nine wild Japanese macaques, previously tested for metacognition using the tubes task, can be solely attributed to impulsivity. The macaques' propensity to look inside a single tube containing food they cannot immediately reach was measured and compared to their behavior in the tubes task. Results indicated that looking inside the baited tube increased as reward quality improved. However, macaques displaying unnecessary tube inspections in metacognitive tests showed less impulsivity to look. This intriguing result counters the notion that excessive looking in the tubes task is solely due to impulsive looking, prompting us to advocate for further research into the relationship between inhibition and metacognitive performance.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11133117/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141157836","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perception of optical illusions in ungulates: insights from goats, sheep, guanacos and llamas. 有蹄类动物对视错觉的感知:山羊、绵羊、瓜纳科羊和美洲驼的启示。
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-24 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01878-2
Caterina Berardo, Ruben Holland, Alina Schaffer, Alvaro Lopez Caicoya, Katja Liebal, Paola Valsecchi, Federica Amici

Optical illusions have long been used in behavioural studies to investigate the perceptual mechanisms underlying vision in animals. So far, three studies have focused on ungulates, providing evidence that they may be susceptible to some optical illusions, in a way similar to humans. Here, we used two food-choice tasks to study susceptibility to the Müller-Lyer and Delboeuf illusions in 17 captive individuals belonging to four ungulate species (Lama guanicoe, Lama glama, Ovis aries, Capra hircus). At the group level, there was a significant preference for the longer/larger food over the shorter/smaller one in control trials. Additionally, the whole group significantly preferred the food stick between two inward arrowheads over an identical one between two outward arrowheads in experimental trials of the Müller-Lyer task, and also preferred the food on the smaller circle over an identical one on the larger circle in the experimental trials of the Delboeuf task. Group-level analyses further showed no significant differences across species, although at the individual level we found significant variation in performance. Our findings suggest that, in line with our predictions, ungulates are overall susceptible to the Müller-Lyer and the Delboeuf illusions, and indicate that the perceptual mechanisms underlying size estimation in artiodactyls might be similar to those of other species, including humans.

长期以来,光学幻觉一直被用于行为学研究,以探究动物视觉的感知机制。迄今为止,已有三项研究以有蹄类动物为研究对象,证明有蹄类动物可能会对某些光学幻觉产生易感性,其方式与人类类似。在这里,我们使用两种食物选择任务来研究四种有蹄类动物(驼科动物、驼属动物、羱羊、羯羊)中的 17 只圈养个体对 Müller-Lyer 和 Delboeuf 幻觉的易感性。在群体水平上,对照试验中长/大的食物明显优于短/小的食物。此外,在穆勒-莱尔任务的实验中,全组明显偏好两个向内箭头之间的食物棒,而不是两个向外箭头之间的相同食物棒;在德尔博夫任务的实验中,全组也明显偏好小圆圈上的食物,而不是大圆圈上的相同食物。群体水平的分析进一步表明,不同物种之间没有显著差异,但在个体水平上,我们发现了显著的表现差异。我们的研究结果表明,与我们的预测一致,有蹄类动物总体上容易受到缪勒-莱尔幻觉和德尔博夫幻觉的影响,并表明偶蹄类动物体型估计的感知机制可能与包括人类在内的其他物种类似。
{"title":"Perception of optical illusions in ungulates: insights from goats, sheep, guanacos and llamas.","authors":"Caterina Berardo, Ruben Holland, Alina Schaffer, Alvaro Lopez Caicoya, Katja Liebal, Paola Valsecchi, Federica Amici","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01878-2","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01878-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Optical illusions have long been used in behavioural studies to investigate the perceptual mechanisms underlying vision in animals. So far, three studies have focused on ungulates, providing evidence that they may be susceptible to some optical illusions, in a way similar to humans. Here, we used two food-choice tasks to study susceptibility to the Müller-Lyer and Delboeuf illusions in 17 captive individuals belonging to four ungulate species (Lama guanicoe, Lama glama, Ovis aries, Capra hircus). At the group level, there was a significant preference for the longer/larger food over the shorter/smaller one in control trials. Additionally, the whole group significantly preferred the food stick between two inward arrowheads over an identical one between two outward arrowheads in experimental trials of the Müller-Lyer task, and also preferred the food on the smaller circle over an identical one on the larger circle in the experimental trials of the Delboeuf task. Group-level analyses further showed no significant differences across species, although at the individual level we found significant variation in performance. Our findings suggest that, in line with our predictions, ungulates are overall susceptible to the Müller-Lyer and the Delboeuf illusions, and indicate that the perceptual mechanisms underlying size estimation in artiodactyls might be similar to those of other species, including humans.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11126503/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141092735","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Nest excavators' learning walks in the Australian desert ant Melophorus bagoti. 澳大利亚沙漠蚂蚁 Melophorus bagoti 的蚁巢挖掘者学习步行。
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-24 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01877-3
Sudhakar Deeti, Donald James McLean, Ken Cheng

The Australian red honey ant, Melophorus bagoti, stands out as the most thermophilic ant in Australia, engaging in all outdoor activities during the hottest periods of the day during summer months. This species of desert ants often navigates by means of path integration and learning landmark cues around the nest. In our study, we observed the outdoor activities of M. bagoti workers engaged in nest excavation, the maintenance of the nest structure, primarily by taking excess sand out of the nest. Before undertaking nest excavation, the ants conducted a single exploratory walk. Following their initial learning expedition, these ants then engaged in nest excavation activities. Consistent with previous findings on pre-foraging learning walks, after just one learning walk, the desert ants in our study demonstrated the ability to return home from locations 2 m away from the nest, although not from locations 4 m away. These findings indicate that even for activities like dumping excavated sand within a range of 5-10 cm outside the nest, these ants learn and utilize the visual landmark panorama around the nest.

澳大利亚红蜜蚁(Melophorus bagoti)是澳大利亚最耐高温的蚂蚁,在夏季一天中最热的时段从事所有户外活动。这种沙漠蚂蚁通常通过整合路径和学习巢穴周围的地标线索来导航。在我们的研究中,我们观察了巴戈蒂蚁工蚁的户外活动,它们从事巢穴挖掘和巢穴结构维护工作,主要是清除巢穴中多余的沙子。在进行巢穴挖掘之前,蚂蚁先进行了一次探索性行走。在初步学习考察之后,这些蚂蚁又开始了巢穴挖掘活动。与之前关于觅食前学习行走的研究结果一致,在我们的研究中,沙漠蚂蚁只进行了一次学习行走后,就能从离巢穴2米远的地方返回家园,但不能从4米远的地方返回家园。这些发现表明,即使是在巢外5-10厘米的范围内倾倒挖出的沙子等活动,这些蚂蚁也能学习和利用巢周围的视觉地标全景。
{"title":"Nest excavators' learning walks in the Australian desert ant Melophorus bagoti.","authors":"Sudhakar Deeti, Donald James McLean, Ken Cheng","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01877-3","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01877-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The Australian red honey ant, Melophorus bagoti, stands out as the most thermophilic ant in Australia, engaging in all outdoor activities during the hottest periods of the day during summer months. This species of desert ants often navigates by means of path integration and learning landmark cues around the nest. In our study, we observed the outdoor activities of M. bagoti workers engaged in nest excavation, the maintenance of the nest structure, primarily by taking excess sand out of the nest. Before undertaking nest excavation, the ants conducted a single exploratory walk. Following their initial learning expedition, these ants then engaged in nest excavation activities. Consistent with previous findings on pre-foraging learning walks, after just one learning walk, the desert ants in our study demonstrated the ability to return home from locations 2 m away from the nest, although not from locations 4 m away. These findings indicate that even for activities like dumping excavated sand within a range of 5-10 cm outside the nest, these ants learn and utilize the visual landmark panorama around the nest.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11126504/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141092658","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
In "Tone" with dogs: exploring canine musicality. 与狗 "同音":探索犬类的音乐性。
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-16 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01875-5
Claudia Pinelli, Anna Scandurra, Cristina Giacoma, Alfredo Di Lucrezia, Biagio D'Aniello

This study investigates the musical perception skills of dogs through playback experiments. Dogs were trained to distinguish between two different target locations based on a sequence of four ascending or descending notes. A total of 16 dogs of different breeds, age, and sex, but all of them with at least basic training, were recruited for the study. Dogs received training from their respective owners in a suitable environment within their familiar home settings. The training sequence consisted of notes [Do-Mi-Sol#-Do (C7-E7-G7#-C8; Hz frequency: 2093, 2639, 3322, 4186)] digitally generated as pure sinusoidal tones. The training protocol comprised 3 sequential training levels, with each level consisting of 4 sessions with a minimum of 10 trials per session. In the test phase, the sequence was transposed to evaluate whether dogs used relative pitch when identifying the sequences. A correct response by the dog was recorded as 1, while an incorrect response, occurring when the dog chose the opposite zone of the bowl, was marked as 0. Statistical analyses were performed using a binomial test. Among 16 dogs, only two consistently performed above the chance level, demonstrating the ability to recognize relative pitch, even with transposed sequences. This study suggests that dogs may have the ability to attend to relative pitch, a critical aspect of human musicality.

本研究通过回放实验研究狗的音乐感知能力。研究人员训练狗根据四个升调或降调音符的序列来区分两个不同的目标位置。本研究共招募了 16 只不同品种、年龄和性别的狗,但它们都至少受过基本训练。狗狗们在各自熟悉的家庭环境中接受各自主人的训练。训练序列由数字生成的纯正弦波音符[Do-Mi-Sol#-Do(C7-E7-G7#-C8;赫兹频率:2093、2639、3322、4186)]组成。训练方案包括 3 个连续的训练级别,每个级别包括 4 个训练环节,每个环节至少进行 10 次试验。在测试阶段,序列被移调,以评估犬在识别序列时是否使用了相对音高。狗的正确反应记为 1,而当狗选择碗的相反区域时出现的错误反应记为 0。在 16 只狗中,只有两只狗的表现始终高于偶然水平,表明它们具有识别相对音高的能力,即使是转调序列也不例外。这项研究表明,狗可能具有注意相对音高的能力,而这正是人类音乐性的一个重要方面。
{"title":"In \"Tone\" with dogs: exploring canine musicality.","authors":"Claudia Pinelli, Anna Scandurra, Cristina Giacoma, Alfredo Di Lucrezia, Biagio D'Aniello","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01875-5","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01875-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study investigates the musical perception skills of dogs through playback experiments. Dogs were trained to distinguish between two different target locations based on a sequence of four ascending or descending notes. A total of 16 dogs of different breeds, age, and sex, but all of them with at least basic training, were recruited for the study. Dogs received training from their respective owners in a suitable environment within their familiar home settings. The training sequence consisted of notes [Do-Mi-Sol#-Do (C7-E7-G7#-C8; Hz frequency: 2093, 2639, 3322, 4186)] digitally generated as pure sinusoidal tones. The training protocol comprised 3 sequential training levels, with each level consisting of 4 sessions with a minimum of 10 trials per session. In the test phase, the sequence was transposed to evaluate whether dogs used relative pitch when identifying the sequences. A correct response by the dog was recorded as 1, while an incorrect response, occurring when the dog chose the opposite zone of the bowl, was marked as 0. Statistical analyses were performed using a binomial test. Among 16 dogs, only two consistently performed above the chance level, demonstrating the ability to recognize relative pitch, even with transposed sequences. This study suggests that dogs may have the ability to attend to relative pitch, a critical aspect of human musicality.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11096221/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140943819","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Changes in movement patterns in relation to sun conditions and spatial scales in wild western gorillas 野生西大猩猩运动模式的变化与日照条件和空间尺度的关系
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-29 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01871-9
B. Robira, S. Benhamou, E. Obeki Bayanga, T. Breuer, S. Masi

For most primates living in tropical forests, food resources occur in patchworks of different habitats that vary seasonally in quality and quantity. Efficient navigation (i.e., spatial memory-based orientation) towards profitable food patches should enhance their foraging success. The mechanisms underpinning primate navigating ability remain nonetheless mostly unknown. Using GPS long-term tracking (596 days) of one group of wild western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla), we investigated their ability to navigate at long distances, and tested for how the sun was used to navigate at any scale by improving landmark visibility and/or by acting as a compass. Long episodic movements ending at a distant swamp, a unique place in the home range where gorillas could find mineral-rich aquatic plants, were straighter and faster than their everyday foraging movements relying on spatial memory. This suggests intentional targeting of the swamp based on long-distance navigation skills, which can thus be efficient over a couple of kilometres. Interestingly, for both long-distance movements towards the swamp and everyday foraging movements, gorillas moved straighter under sunlight conditions even under a dense vegetation cover. By contrast, movement straightness was not markedly different when the sun elevation was low (the sun azimuth then being potentially usable as a compass) or high (so providing no directional information) and the sky was clear or overcast. This suggests that gorillas navigate their home range by relying on visual place recognition but do not use the sun azimuth as a compass. Like humans, who rely heavily on vision to navigate, gorillas should benefit from better lighting to help them identify landmarks as they move through shady forests. This study uncovers a neglected aspect of primate navigation. Spatial memory and vision might have played an important role in the evolutionary success of diurnal primate lineages.

对于生活在热带森林中的大多数灵长类动物来说,食物资源存在于不同栖息地的斑块中,这些斑块的质量和数量随季节而变化。灵长类动物对有利可图的食物斑块的高效导航(即基于空间记忆的定向)应能提高它们的觅食成功率。然而,灵长类动物导航能力的基本机制仍然是未知的。我们利用全球定位系统对一群野生西部低地大猩猩(Gorilla gorilla gorilla)进行了长期跟踪(596天),研究了它们的远距离导航能力,并测试了太阳是如何通过提高地标能见度和/或充当指南针来进行任何规模的导航的。沼泽是大猩猩家园中一个独特的地方,在那里大猩猩可以找到富含矿物质的水生植物,在沼泽结束的长距离偶发运动比依靠空间记忆的日常觅食运动更直、更快。这表明,大猩猩是根据长距离导航技能有意瞄准沼泽的,因此这种技能可以在几公里的范围内发挥高效作用。有趣的是,无论是向沼泽地的长距离移动还是日常的觅食移动,大猩猩在阳光条件下,即使在茂密的植被覆盖下,移动的直线度也更高。相比之下,当太阳高度较低时(此时太阳方位角有可能用作指南针)或较高时(因此无法提供方向信息),以及天空晴朗或阴霾时,大猩猩的移动直线度并无明显差异。这表明,大猩猩依靠视觉识别地点,而不是把太阳方位角当作指南针来导航。与主要依靠视觉导航的人类一样,大猩猩也应该受益于更好的照明,以帮助它们在阴暗的森林中移动时识别地标。这项研究发现了灵长类动物导航中一个被忽视的方面。空间记忆和视觉可能在昼伏夜出的灵长类进化过程中发挥了重要作用。
{"title":"Changes in movement patterns in relation to sun conditions and spatial scales in wild western gorillas","authors":"B. Robira, S. Benhamou, E. Obeki Bayanga, T. Breuer, S. Masi","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01871-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-024-01871-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p>For most primates living in tropical forests, food resources occur in patchworks of different habitats that vary seasonally in quality and quantity. Efficient navigation (i.e., spatial memory-based orientation) towards profitable food patches should enhance their foraging success. The mechanisms underpinning primate navigating ability remain nonetheless mostly unknown. Using GPS long-term tracking (596 days) of one group of wild western lowland gorillas (<i>Gorilla gorilla gorilla)</i>, we investigated their ability to navigate at long distances, and tested for how the sun was used to navigate at any scale by improving landmark visibility and/or by acting as a compass. Long episodic movements ending at a distant swamp, a unique place in the home range where gorillas could find mineral-rich aquatic plants, were straighter and faster than their everyday foraging movements relying on spatial memory. This suggests intentional targeting of the swamp based on long-distance navigation skills, which can thus be efficient over a couple of kilometres. Interestingly, for both long-distance movements towards the swamp and everyday foraging movements, gorillas moved straighter under sunlight conditions even under a dense vegetation cover. By contrast, movement straightness was not markedly different when the sun elevation was low (the sun azimuth then being potentially usable as a compass) or high (so providing no directional information) and the sky was clear or overcast. This suggests that gorillas navigate their home range by relying on visual place recognition but do not use the sun azimuth as a compass. Like humans, who rely heavily on vision to navigate, gorillas should benefit from better lighting to help them identify landmarks as they move through shady forests. This study uncovers a neglected aspect of primate navigation. Spatial memory and vision might have played an important role in the evolutionary success of diurnal primate lineages.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140829715","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Hooded crows (Corvus cornix) manufacture objects relative to a mental template 戴帽乌鸦(Corvus cornix)根据心理模板制造物体
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-29 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01874-6
Anna A. Smirnova, Leia R. Bulgakova, Maria A. Cheplakova, Sarah A. Jelbert

It was recently found that not only tool-specialized New Caledonian crows, but also Goffin cockatoos can manufacture physical objects in accordance with a mental template. That is, they can emulate features of existing objects when they manufacture new items. Both species spontaneously ripped pieces of card into large strips if they had previously learned that a large template was rewarded, and small strips when they previously learned that a small template was rewarded. Among New Caledonian crows, this cognitive ability was suggested as a potential mechanism underlying the transmission of natural tool designs. Here, we tested for the same ability in another non-specialised tool user–Hooded crows (Corvus cornix). Crows were exposed to pre-made template objects, varying first in colour and then in size, and were rewarded only if they chose pre-made objects that matched the template. In subsequent tests, birds were given the opportunity to manufacture versions of these objects. All three crows ripped paper pieces from the same colour material as the rewarded template, and, crucially, also manufactured objects that were more similar in size to previously rewarded, than unrewarded, templates, despite the birds being rewarded at random in both tests. Therefore, we found the ability to manufacture physical objects relative to a mental template in yet another bird species not specialized in using or making foraging tools in the wild, but with a high level of brain and cognitive development.

最近研究发现,不仅擅长工具的新喀里多尼亚乌鸦,戈芬凤头鹦鹉也能根据心理模板制造实物。也就是说,它们在制造新物品时可以模仿现有物品的特征。如果这两种乌鸦以前就知道大的模板会得到奖励,那么它们就会自发地把卡片撕成大条状;如果它们以前就知道小的模板会得到奖励,那么它们就会自发地把卡片撕成小条状。在新喀里多尼亚乌鸦中,这种认知能力被认为是自然工具设计传播的潜在机制。在这里,我们测试了另一种非专门工具使用者--带帽乌鸦(Corvus cornix)的相同能力。乌鸦会接触到预先制作好的模板物体,首先是颜色,然后是大小,只有当乌鸦选择了与模板相匹配的预制物体时才会得到奖励。在随后的测试中,乌鸦有机会制作这些物体的模型。所有三只乌鸦都从与奖励模板相同颜色的材料上撕下了纸片,而且关键的是,尽管在两次测试中乌鸦都是随机获得奖励,但它们制造的物品在大小上与之前获得奖励的模板更相似,而不是未获得奖励的模板。因此,我们在另一种在野外不擅长使用或制作觅食工具,但大脑和认知能力发展水平较高的鸟类身上发现了相对于心理模板制造实物的能力。
{"title":"Hooded crows (Corvus cornix) manufacture objects relative to a mental template","authors":"Anna A. Smirnova, Leia R. Bulgakova, Maria A. Cheplakova, Sarah A. Jelbert","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01874-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-024-01874-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p>It was recently found that not only tool-specialized New Caledonian crows, but also Goffin cockatoos can manufacture physical objects in accordance with a mental template. That is, they can emulate features of existing objects when they manufacture new items. Both species spontaneously ripped pieces of card into large strips if they had previously learned that a large template was rewarded, and small strips when they previously learned that a small template was rewarded. Among New Caledonian crows, this cognitive ability was suggested as a potential mechanism underlying the transmission of natural tool designs. Here, we tested for the same ability in another non-specialised tool user–Hooded crows (<i>Corvus cornix</i>). Crows were exposed to pre-made template objects, varying first in colour and then in size, and were rewarded only if they chose pre-made objects that matched the template. In subsequent tests, birds were given the opportunity to manufacture versions of these objects. All three crows ripped paper pieces from the same colour material as the rewarded template, and, crucially, also manufactured objects that were more similar in size to previously rewarded, than unrewarded, templates, despite the birds being rewarded at random in both tests. Therefore, we found the ability to manufacture physical objects relative to a mental template in yet another bird species not specialized in using or making foraging tools in the wild, but with a high level of brain and cognitive development.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140809688","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Proto-tool use for food processing in wild Arabian babblers: matching processing methods, substrates and prey types 野生阿拉伯狒狒使用原工具加工食物:加工方法、基质和猎物类型的匹配
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-24 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01866-6
Yitzchak Ben Mocha, Francesca Frisoni, Oded Keynan, Michaela Griesser
{"title":"Proto-tool use for food processing in wild Arabian babblers: matching processing methods, substrates and prey types","authors":"Yitzchak Ben Mocha, Francesca Frisoni, Oded Keynan, Michaela Griesser","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01866-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-024-01866-6","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140661308","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perception of vocoded speech in domestic dogs 家犬对语音编码的感知
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-16 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01869-3
Amritha Mallikarjun, Emily Shroads, Rochelle S. Newman

Humans have an impressive ability to comprehend signal-degraded speech; however, the extent to which comprehension of degraded speech relies on human-specific features of speech perception vs. more general cognitive processes is unknown. Since dogs live alongside humans and regularly hear speech, they can be used as a model to differentiate between these possibilities. One often-studied type of degraded speech is noise-vocoded speech (sometimes thought of as cochlear-implant-simulation speech). Noise-vocoded speech is made by dividing the speech signal into frequency bands (channels), identifying the amplitude envelope of each individual band, and then using these envelopes to modulate bands of noise centered over the same frequency regions – the result is a signal with preserved temporal cues, but vastly reduced frequency information. Here, we tested dogs’ recognition of familiar words produced in 16-channel vocoded speech. In the first study, dogs heard their names and unfamiliar dogs’ names (foils) in vocoded speech as well as natural speech. In the second study, dogs heard 16-channel vocoded speech only. Dogs listened longer to their vocoded name than vocoded foils in both experiments, showing that they can comprehend a 16-channel vocoded version of their name without prior exposure to vocoded speech, and without immediate exposure to the natural-speech version of their name. Dogs’ name recognition in the second study was mediated by the number of phonemes in the dogs’ name, suggesting that phonological context plays a role in degraded speech comprehension.

人类具有令人印象深刻的理解信号降级语音的能力;然而,对降级语音的理解在多大程度上依赖于人类特有的语音感知特征,而不是更普遍的认知过程,这一点尚不清楚。由于狗与人类生活在一起并经常听到语音,因此可以将狗作为模型来区分这些可能性。噪音编码语音(有时被认为是人工耳蜗模拟语音)是一种经常被研究的降级语音类型。噪声编码语音是通过将语音信号划分为不同的频段(信道),识别每个频段的振幅包络,然后使用这些包络对以相同频率区域为中心的噪声段进行调制--其结果是信号保留了时间线索,但频率信息却大大减少。在这里,我们测试了狗对 16 频道声码化语音中熟悉单词的识别能力。在第一项研究中,狗在声码语音和自然语音中听到了自己的名字和陌生狗的名字(陪衬)。在第二项研究中,狗只听到 16 频道的声码语音。在这两项实验中,狗听自己声码名字的时间都比听声码陪衬的时间长,这表明狗可以在没有事先接触声码语音的情况下理解 16 频道声码版本的名字,也可以在没有立即接触自然语音版本的名字的情况下理解自然语音版本的名字。在第二项研究中,狗的名字识别能力受狗名字中音素数量的影响,这表明语音语境在退化的语音理解能力中起着一定的作用。
{"title":"Perception of vocoded speech in domestic dogs","authors":"Amritha Mallikarjun, Emily Shroads, Rochelle S. Newman","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01869-3","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-024-01869-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Humans have an impressive ability to comprehend signal-degraded speech; however, the extent to which comprehension of degraded speech relies on human-specific features of speech perception vs. more general cognitive processes is unknown. Since dogs live alongside humans and regularly hear speech, they can be used as a model to differentiate between these possibilities. One often-studied type of degraded speech is noise-vocoded speech (sometimes thought of as cochlear-implant-simulation speech). Noise-vocoded speech is made by dividing the speech signal into frequency bands (channels), identifying the amplitude envelope of each individual band, and then using these envelopes to modulate bands of noise centered over the same frequency regions – the result is a signal with preserved temporal cues, but vastly reduced frequency information. Here, we tested dogs’ recognition of familiar words produced in 16-channel vocoded speech. In the first study, dogs heard their names and unfamiliar dogs’ names (foils) in vocoded speech as well as natural speech. In the second study, dogs heard 16-channel vocoded speech only. Dogs listened longer to their vocoded name than vocoded foils in both experiments, showing that they can comprehend a 16-channel vocoded version of their name without prior exposure to vocoded speech, and without immediate exposure to the natural-speech version of their name. Dogs’ name recognition in the second study was mediated by the number of phonemes in the dogs’ name, suggesting that phonological context plays a role in degraded speech comprehension.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140613761","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Figure-ground segmentation based on motion in the archerfish 基于箭鱼运动的图地分割
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-15 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01873-7
Svetlana Volotsky, Ronen Segev

Figure-ground segmentation is a fundamental process in visual perception that involves separating visual stimuli into distinct meaningful objects and their surrounding context, thus allowing the brain to interpret and understand complex visual scenes. Mammals exhibit varying figure-ground segmentation capabilities, ranging from primates that can perform well on figure-ground segmentation tasks to rodents that perform poorly. To explore figure-ground segmentation capabilities in teleost fish, we studied how the archerfish, an expert visual hunter, performs figure-ground segmentation. We trained archerfish to discriminate foreground objects from the background, where the figures were defined by motion as well as by discontinuities in intensity and texture. Specifically, the figures were defined by grating, naturalistic texture, and random noise moving in counterphase with the background. The archerfish performed the task well and could distinguish between all three types of figures and grounds. Their performance was comparable to that of primates and outperformed rodents. These findings suggest the existence of a complex visual process in the archerfish visual system that enables the delineation of figures as distinct from backgrounds, and provide insights into object recognition in this animal.

图地分割是视觉感知的一个基本过程,它涉及将视觉刺激物分割成不同的有意义物体及其周围环境,从而使大脑能够解释和理解复杂的视觉场景。哺乳动物的图地分割能力各不相同,灵长类动物在图地分割任务中表现良好,而啮齿类动物则表现较差。为了探索远洋鱼类的图地分割能力,我们研究了箭鱼这种视觉猎手是如何进行图地分割的。我们训练箭鱼从背景中分辨出前景物体,其中的图形是由运动以及强度和纹理的不连续性定义的。具体来说,人物是由光栅、自然纹理和与背景反相运动的随机噪音定义的。箭鱼很好地完成了任务,并能区分所有三种类型的图形和地面。它们的表现可与灵长类动物媲美,并优于啮齿类动物。这些研究结果表明,在箭鱼视觉系统中存在一个复杂的视觉过程,能够将图形与背景区分开来,并为这种动物的物体识别提供了启示。
{"title":"Figure-ground segmentation based on motion in the archerfish","authors":"Svetlana Volotsky, Ronen Segev","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01873-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-024-01873-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Figure-ground segmentation is a fundamental process in visual perception that involves separating visual stimuli into distinct meaningful objects and their surrounding context, thus allowing the brain to interpret and understand complex visual scenes. Mammals exhibit varying figure-ground segmentation capabilities, ranging from primates that can perform well on figure-ground segmentation tasks to rodents that perform poorly. To explore figure-ground segmentation capabilities in teleost fish, we studied how the archerfish, an expert visual hunter, performs figure-ground segmentation. We trained archerfish to discriminate foreground objects from the background, where the figures were defined by motion as well as by discontinuities in intensity and texture. Specifically, the figures were defined by grating, naturalistic texture, and random noise moving in counterphase with the background. The archerfish performed the task well and could distinguish between all three types of figures and grounds. Their performance was comparable to that of primates and outperformed rodents. These findings suggest the existence of a complex visual process in the archerfish visual system that enables the delineation of figures as distinct from backgrounds, and provide insights into object recognition in this animal.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140560448","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How to solve novel problems: the role of associative learning in problem-solving performance in wild great tits Parus major 如何解决新问题:联想学习在野生大山雀(Parus major)解决问题过程中的作用
IF 2.7 2区 生物学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-12 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01872-8
Laure Cauchard, Pierre Bize, Blandine Doligez

Although problem-solving tasks are frequently used to assess innovative ability, the extent to which problem-solving performance reflects variation in cognitive skills has been rarely formally investigated. Using wild breeding great tits facing a new non-food motivated problem-solving task, we investigated the role of associative learning in finding the solution, compared to multiple other non-cognitive factors. We first examined the role of accuracy (the proportion of contacts made with the opening part of a string-pulling task), neophobia, exploration, activity, age, sex, body condition and participation time on the ability to solve the task. To highlight the effect of associative learning, we then compared accuracy between solvers and non-solvers, before and after the first cue to the solution (i.e., the first time they pulled the string opening the door). We finally compared accuracy over consecutive entrances for solvers. Using 884 observations from 788 great tits tested from 2010 to 2015, we showed that, prior to initial successful entrance, solvers were more accurate and more explorative than non-solvers, and that females were more likely to solve the task than males. The accuracy of solvers, but not of non-solvers, increased significantly after they had the opportunity to associate string pulling with the movement of the door, giving them a first cue to the task solution. The accuracy of solvers also increased over successive entrances. Our results demonstrate that variations in problem-solving performance primarily reflect inherent individual differences in associative learning, and are also to a lesser extent shaped by sex and exploratory behaviour.

尽管解决问题的任务经常被用来评估创新能力,但解决问题的表现在多大程度上反映了认知技能的变化却很少被正式研究。我们利用野生繁殖的大山雀面对一个新的非食物动机的问题解决任务,研究了联想学习在找到解决方案中的作用,以及与其他多种非认知因素的比较。我们首先研究了准确性(在拉绳子任务中与开口部分接触的比例)、恐新症、探索、活动、年龄、性别、身体状况和参与时间对解决任务能力的影响。为了突出联想学习的效果,我们比较了解题者和非解题者在第一次提示解题之前和之后(即他们第一次拉动绳子打开门)的准确性。最后,我们还比较了解题者连续开门的准确率。通过对2010年至2015年期间测试的788只大山雀的884次观察,我们发现,在首次成功进入之前,求解者比非求解者更准确、更善于探索,而且雌性比雄性更有可能完成任务。在有机会将拉绳与门的移动联系起来后,解题者的准确率显著提高,而非解题者的准确率则没有提高。解题者的准确率也随着连续的入口而提高。我们的研究结果表明,解决问题能力的差异主要反映了联想学习中固有的个体差异,在较小程度上也受性别和探索行为的影响。
{"title":"How to solve novel problems: the role of associative learning in problem-solving performance in wild great tits Parus major","authors":"Laure Cauchard, Pierre Bize, Blandine Doligez","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01872-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-024-01872-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Although problem-solving tasks are frequently used to assess innovative ability, the extent to which problem-solving performance reflects variation in cognitive skills has been rarely formally investigated. Using wild breeding great tits facing a new non-food motivated problem-solving task, we investigated the role of associative learning in finding the solution, compared to multiple other non-cognitive factors. We first examined the role of accuracy (the proportion of contacts made with the opening part of a string-pulling task), neophobia, exploration, activity, age, sex, body condition and participation time on the ability to solve the task. To highlight the effect of associative learning, we then compared accuracy between solvers and non-solvers, before and after the first cue to the solution (i.e., the first time they pulled the string opening the door). We finally compared accuracy over consecutive entrances for solvers. Using 884 observations from 788 great tits tested from 2010 to 2015, we showed that, prior to initial successful entrance, solvers were more accurate and more explorative than non-solvers, and that females were more likely to solve the task than males. The accuracy of solvers, but not of non-solvers, increased significantly after they had the opportunity to associate string pulling with the movement of the door, giving them a first cue to the task solution. The accuracy of solvers also increased over successive entrances. Our results demonstrate that variations in problem-solving performance primarily reflect inherent individual differences in associative learning, and are also to a lesser extent shaped by sex and exploratory behaviour.</p>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140560372","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Animal Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1