首页 > 最新文献

Animal Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Rats’ performance in a suboptimal choice procedure implemented in a natural-foraging analogue 大鼠在自然觅食模拟的次优选择程序中的表现。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01913-2
Fernanda González-Barriga, Vladimir Orduña

Rats and pigeons have shown striking differences in their behavior in the suboptimal choice procedure: while pigeons show a strong and consistent preference for the discriminative alternative, most studies performed with rats have found optimal preferences, and in the cases in which suboptimal preferences have been reported, those results have not been replicated. Currently, there is no consensus about the reasons for these discrepant results between species, but different explanations have been proposed either with an empirical base or exclusively in theoretical terms. In the latter category it has been proposed that the discrepancy might have arisen because of differences in the relationship between the natural foraging response of each species, and the response required in the laboratory. For analyzing this possibility, we conducted two experiments carried out within a maze that was specifically designed to allow rats to display behaviors related to their natural foraging. In experiment 1, we explored rats’ preferences when facing a discriminative alternative with probability of reinforcement (p) = 0.5, and a non-discriminative alternative with p = .75. In experiment 2, we evaluated preferences when the discriminative alternative had p = .20 and the non-discriminative had p = .50, rats were evaluated in a closed economy, with longer terminal links, and were allowed to escape from the outcome found. In both studies, rats showed a strong preference for the non-discriminative alternative and showed very high levels of discrimination between the positive and the negative outcomes of the discriminative alternative.

大鼠和鸽子在次优选择过程中的行为显示出惊人的差异:鸽子表现出强烈而一致的对鉴别性替代品的偏好,而对大鼠进行的大多数研究却发现了最优偏好,在报告了次优偏好的情况下,这些结果却没有得到重复。目前,对于不同物种之间出现这些差异结果的原因还没有达成共识,但已经提出了不同的解释,有的以经验为基础,有的则完全以理论为基础。在后一种情况下,有人认为出现差异的原因可能是每个物种的自然觅食反应与实验室要求的反应之间的关系不同。为了分析这种可能性,我们在一个迷宫中进行了两次实验,这个迷宫是专门为让大鼠表现出与自然觅食相关的行为而设计的。在实验 1 中,我们探讨了大鼠在面对强化概率 (p) = 0.5 的区别性选择和 p = 0.75 的非区别性选择时的偏好。在实验 2 中,我们评估了当辨别性替代品的强化概率 p = 0.20 和非辨别性替代品的强化概率 p = 0.50 时大鼠的偏好,大鼠在封闭的经济环境中接受评估,终端环节较长,并允许大鼠从发现的结果中逃脱。在这两项研究中,大鼠都表现出了对非歧视性选择的强烈偏好,并对歧视性选择的正反结果表现出了极高的辨别能力。
{"title":"Rats’ performance in a suboptimal choice procedure implemented in a natural-foraging analogue","authors":"Fernanda González-Barriga,&nbsp;Vladimir Orduña","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01913-2","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01913-2","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Rats and pigeons have shown striking differences in their behavior in the suboptimal choice procedure: while pigeons show a strong and consistent preference for the discriminative alternative, most studies performed with rats have found optimal preferences, and in the cases in which suboptimal preferences have been reported, those results have not been replicated. Currently, there is no consensus about the reasons for these discrepant results between species, but different explanations have been proposed either with an empirical base or exclusively in theoretical terms. In the latter category it has been proposed that the discrepancy might have arisen because of differences in the relationship between the natural foraging response of each species, and the response required in the laboratory. For analyzing this possibility, we conducted two experiments carried out within a maze that was specifically designed to allow rats to display behaviors related to their natural foraging. In experiment 1, we explored rats’ preferences when facing a discriminative alternative with probability of reinforcement (p) = 0.5, and a non-discriminative alternative with <i>p</i> = .75. In experiment 2, we evaluated preferences when the discriminative alternative had <i>p</i> = .20 and the non-discriminative had <i>p</i> = .50, rats were evaluated in a closed economy, with longer terminal links, and were allowed to escape from the outcome found. In both studies, rats showed a strong preference for the non-discriminative alternative and showed very high levels of discrimination between the positive and the negative outcomes of the discriminative alternative.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"27 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-024-01913-2.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142562497","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Odour generalisation and detection dog training 气味泛化和探测犬训练
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01907-0
Lyn Caldicott, Thomas W. Pike, Helen E. Zulch, Daniel S. Mills, Fiona J. Williams, Kevin R. Elliker, Bethany Hutchings, Anna Wilkinson

Detection dogs are required to search for and alert to specific odours of interest, such as drugs, cadavers, disease markers and explosives. However, the odour released from different samples of the same target substance will vary for a number of reasons, including the production method, evaporation, degradation, or by being mixed with extraneous odours. Generalisation, the tendency to respond in the same manner to stimuli which are different – but similar to – a conditioned stimulus, is therefore a crucial requirement for working detection dogs. Odour is a complex modality which poses unique challenges in terms of reliably predicting generalisation, when compared with auditory or visual stimuli. The primary aim of this review is to explore recent advances in our understanding of generalisation and the factors that influence it, and to consider these in light of detection dog training methods currently used in the field. We identify potential risks associated with certain training practices, and highlight areas where research is lacking and which warrant further investigation.

缉毒犬需要搜索毒品、尸体、疾病标志物和爆炸物等特定气味并发出警报。然而,由于生产方法、蒸发、降解或与外来气味混合等多种原因,同一目标物质的不同样本释放出的气味会有所不同。因此,泛化,即对不同但类似于条件刺激的刺激做出相同反应的趋势,是工作犬检测的关键要求。气味是一种复杂的模式,与听觉或视觉刺激相比,它在可靠预测泛化方面提出了独特的挑战。本综述的主要目的是探讨我们在理解泛化和影响泛化的因素方面的最新进展,并结合该领域目前使用的探测犬训练方法来考虑这些因素。我们确定了与某些训练方法相关的潜在风险,并强调了缺乏研究和需要进一步调查的领域。
{"title":"Odour generalisation and detection dog training","authors":"Lyn Caldicott,&nbsp;Thomas W. Pike,&nbsp;Helen E. Zulch,&nbsp;Daniel S. Mills,&nbsp;Fiona J. Williams,&nbsp;Kevin R. Elliker,&nbsp;Bethany Hutchings,&nbsp;Anna Wilkinson","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01907-0","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01907-0","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Detection dogs are required to search for and alert to specific odours of interest, such as drugs, cadavers, disease markers and explosives. However, the odour released from different samples of the same target substance will vary for a number of reasons, including the production method, evaporation, degradation, or by being mixed with extraneous odours. Generalisation, the tendency to respond in the same manner to stimuli which are different – but similar to – a conditioned stimulus, is therefore a crucial requirement for working detection dogs. Odour is a complex modality which poses unique challenges in terms of reliably predicting generalisation, when compared with auditory or visual stimuli. The primary aim of this review is to explore recent advances in our understanding of generalisation and the factors that influence it, and to consider these in light of detection dog training methods currently used in the field. We identify potential risks associated with certain training practices, and highlight areas where research is lacking and which warrant further investigation.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"27 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-024-01907-0.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142562489","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Breeding stages affect egg recognition in azure-winged magpies (Cyanopica cyanus) 繁殖阶段对蔚蓝翅喜鹊(Cyanopica cyanus)卵识别的影响
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-30 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01915-0
Jianping Liu, Fudong Zhou, Hanlin Yan, Wei Liang

Egg rejection often involves a cognitive process of recognizing foreign eggs, which can vary not only between species or among different individuals of the same species, but also within the same individual during different breeding stages, leading to markedly different responses to parasitic eggs. We conducted a comparative study in Wuhan, Hubei, and Fusong, Jilin, China, on the recognition and rejection behavior of azure-winged magpies (Cyanopica cyanus) at different breeding stages (pre-egg-laying, one-host-egg, multi-host-egg and early incubation stages). In the Fusong population, there was a significant difference in the rejection rate of model eggs by azure-winged magpies at different stages of the egg-laying period. During the one-host-egg stage, the rejection rate (63.6%) was significantly lower than that during the pre-egg-laying stage (85.7%) and the multi-host-egg stage (100%). The population of azure-winged magpies in Wuhan exhibited a 100% rejection rate towards model eggs during the pre-egg-laying stage. Furthermore, during the incubation stage, azure-winged magpies were able to accurately recognize and reject foreign eggs even when those were in majority. This indicates that azure-winged magpies employ a template-based recognition mechanism rather than relying on discordance mechanism for recognition after the onset of incubation. This study suggests that while azure-winged magpies can truly recognize their own eggs, different breeding stages still influence their rejection response towards parasitic eggs, especially during the pre-egg-laying and egg laying stages.

拒卵通常涉及识别外来卵的认知过程,不仅在物种之间或同一物种的不同个体之间存在差异,而且在同一个体的不同繁殖阶段也可能存在差异,从而导致对寄生卵的反应明显不同。我们在湖北武汉和吉林抚松对不同繁殖阶段(产卵前、单寄主卵、多寄主卵和孵化早期)的鹃翅喜鹊的识别和排斥行为进行了比较研究。在抚松种群中,不同产卵期鹃翅喜鹊对模式蛋的排斥率存在显著差异。单寄主蛋阶段的拒食率(63.6%)明显低于产蛋前期的拒食率(85.7%)和多寄主蛋阶段的拒食率(100%)。武汉的鹃翅喜鹊种群在产蛋前期对模式蛋的排斥率为 100%。此外,在孵化阶段,即使外来蛋占多数,鹃翅喜鹊也能准确识别并拒绝外来蛋。这表明,鹃翅喜鹊在孵化开始后采用的是基于模板的识别机制,而不是依靠不和谐机制进行识别。该研究表明,虽然鹃翅喜鹊能够真正识别自己的卵,但不同的繁殖阶段仍会影响其对寄生卵的排斥反应,尤其是在产卵前和产卵阶段。
{"title":"Breeding stages affect egg recognition in azure-winged magpies (Cyanopica cyanus)","authors":"Jianping Liu,&nbsp;Fudong Zhou,&nbsp;Hanlin Yan,&nbsp;Wei Liang","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01915-0","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01915-0","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Egg rejection often involves a cognitive process of recognizing foreign eggs, which can vary not only between species or among different individuals of the same species, but also within the same individual during different breeding stages, leading to markedly different responses to parasitic eggs. We conducted a comparative study in Wuhan, Hubei, and Fusong, Jilin, China, on the recognition and rejection behavior of azure-winged magpies (<i>Cyanopica cyanus</i>) at different breeding stages (pre-egg-laying, one-host-egg, multi-host-egg and early incubation stages). In the Fusong population, there was a significant difference in the rejection rate of model eggs by azure-winged magpies at different stages of the egg-laying period. During the one-host-egg stage, the rejection rate (63.6%) was significantly lower than that during the pre-egg-laying stage (85.7%) and the multi-host-egg stage (100%). The population of azure-winged magpies in Wuhan exhibited a 100% rejection rate towards model eggs during the pre-egg-laying stage. Furthermore, during the incubation stage, azure-winged magpies were able to accurately recognize and reject foreign eggs even when those were in majority. This indicates that azure-winged magpies employ a template-based recognition mechanism rather than relying on discordance mechanism for recognition after the onset of incubation. This study suggests that while azure-winged magpies can truly recognize their own eggs, different breeding stages still influence their rejection response towards parasitic eggs, especially during the pre-egg-laying and egg laying stages.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"27 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-024-01915-0.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142540509","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Measuring self-control in a wild songbird using a spatial discounting task 利用空间折扣任务测量野生鸣禽的自我控制能力
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-25 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01911-4
Ella McCallum, Rachael C. Shaw

Self-control allows animals to resist instant gratification and invest more time and/or energy in better outcomes. However, widespread temporal self-control tasks lack ecological validity for many species, and few studies have explored whether self-control can be measured in the wild. We used a spatial discounting task resembling natural foraging decisions to measure self-control in wild toutouwai (Petroica longipes), a songbird endemic to New Zealand. Birds chose between a near, low-quality food item and a high-quality food item further away. Toutouwai showed striking individual variation in their self-control abilities. Validation tests suggested that our task reliably measured self-control in a spatial foraging context. However, individual-level performance was confounded by food preferences and the satiation and/or learning effects associated with increasing trial number, limiting the applicability of this task as a measure of individual variation in self-control. Nonetheless, we found no correlation between an individual’s self-control and their inhibitory control measured using a detour task, suggesting that self-control is a distinct ability from the suppression of impulsive motor actions in toutouwai. This study demonstrates for the first time that a bird is capable of self-control in a spatial context and provides suggestions for how future researchers may robustly quantify individual differences in self-control in the wild.

自我控制使动物能够抵制即时满足感,将更多时间和/或精力投入到更好的结果中。然而,对于许多物种来说,普遍的时间自我控制任务缺乏生态有效性,而且很少有研究探讨是否可以在野外测量自我控制。我们使用了一种类似于自然觅食决策的空间折扣任务来测量新西兰特有的鸣禽野生toutouwai(Petroica longipes)的自我控制能力。鸟类在近处的低质量食物和远处的高质量食物之间做出选择。Toutouwai在自我控制能力方面表现出惊人的个体差异。验证测试表明,我们的任务能够可靠地测量在空间觅食背景下的自我控制能力。然而,个体水平的表现受到食物偏好以及与增加试验次数相关的饱腹感和/或学习效应的影响,从而限制了这项任务作为自我控制能力个体差异测量方法的适用性。尽管如此,我们发现个体的自我控制能力与使用迂回任务测量的抑制控制能力之间没有相关性,这表明自我控制能力是一种有别于抑制冲动性动作的能力。这项研究首次证明了鸟类在空间环境中的自我控制能力,并为未来研究人员如何在野外对自我控制的个体差异进行稳健的量化提供了建议。
{"title":"Measuring self-control in a wild songbird using a spatial discounting task","authors":"Ella McCallum,&nbsp;Rachael C. Shaw","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01911-4","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01911-4","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Self-control allows animals to resist instant gratification and invest more time and/or energy in better outcomes. However, widespread temporal self-control tasks lack ecological validity for many species, and few studies have explored whether self-control can be measured in the wild. We used a spatial discounting task resembling natural foraging decisions to measure self-control in wild toutouwai (<i>Petroica longipes</i>), a songbird endemic to New Zealand. Birds chose between a near, low-quality food item and a high-quality food item further away. Toutouwai showed striking individual variation in their self-control abilities. Validation tests suggested that our task reliably measured self-control in a spatial foraging context. However, individual-level performance was confounded by food preferences and the satiation and/or learning effects associated with increasing trial number, limiting the applicability of this task as a measure of individual variation in self-control. Nonetheless, we found no correlation between an individual’s self-control and their inhibitory control measured using a detour task, suggesting that self-control is a distinct ability from the suppression of impulsive motor actions in toutouwai. This study demonstrates for the first time that a bird is capable of self-control in a spatial context and provides suggestions for how future researchers may robustly quantify individual differences in self-control in the wild.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"27 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11511709/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142493168","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Background predation risk induces anxiety-like behaviour and predator neophobia in zebrafish 背景捕食风险会诱发斑马鱼的焦虑行为和捕食者恐新症。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-23 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01908-z
Himal Thapa, Arash Salahinejad, Adam L. Crane, Ahmad Ghobeishavi, Maud C. O. Ferrari

Prey face a major challenge in balancing predator avoidance with other essential activities. In environments with high risk, prey may exhibit neophobia (fear of novelty) due to the increased likelihood of novel stimuli being dangerous. The zebrafish, Danio rerio, is an established model organism for many scientific studies. Although spatial and object neophobia in zebrafish have received previous attention, little is known about the role of background risk in inducing neophobia in zebrafish. Here, we present two experiments using zebrafish to explore whether background predation risk can induce anxiety-like behaviour in a novel environment and neophobic responses when exposed to a novel odour. Over five days, we repeatedly exposed zebrafish to either high background risk in the form of chemical alarm cues (i.e., injured conspecific cues that indicate a predator attack) or a low-risk water control stimulus. In Experiment 1, when tested in a novel tank, zebrafish exposed to high predation risk displayed anxiety-like responses (reduced activity and increased bottom time spent) compared to their low-risk counterparts. Moreover, high-risk individuals showed reduced intra-session habituation to the novel tank compared to low-risk individuals. In Experiment 2, high-risk individuals exhibited fear responses toward a novel odour, unlike low-risk individuals. These results reveal that short-term repeated exposures to high risk can induce anxiety-like behaviour and predator odour neophobia in zebrafish.

猎物要在躲避捕食者和其他基本活动之间取得平衡,面临着巨大的挑战。在高风险环境中,猎物可能会表现出新奇恐惧症(对新奇事物的恐惧),因为新奇刺激具有危险性的可能性增加了。斑马鱼(Danio rerio)是许多科学研究的成熟模式生物。虽然斑马鱼的空间和物体新恐惧症以前就受到过关注,但人们对背景风险在诱发斑马鱼新恐惧症中的作用知之甚少。在此,我们用斑马鱼进行了两项实验,以探讨背景捕食风险是否会诱发斑马鱼在新环境中的焦虑行为以及暴露于新气味时的新恐惧症反应。在五天时间里,我们反复让斑马鱼接触高背景风险的化学警报线索(即表示捕食者攻击的受伤的同种线索)或低风险的水控制刺激。在实验 1 中,与低风险的斑马鱼相比,暴露于高捕食风险的斑马鱼在新水箱中接受测试时表现出类似焦虑的反应(活动减少,在水底停留的时间增加)。此外,与低风险斑马鱼相比,高风险斑马鱼在实验过程中对新水箱的适应性降低。在实验 2 中,与低风险个体不同,高风险个体对新气味表现出恐惧反应。这些结果表明,短期重复暴露于高风险环境可诱发斑马鱼的焦虑样行为和捕食者气味恐惧症。
{"title":"Background predation risk induces anxiety-like behaviour and predator neophobia in zebrafish","authors":"Himal Thapa,&nbsp;Arash Salahinejad,&nbsp;Adam L. Crane,&nbsp;Ahmad Ghobeishavi,&nbsp;Maud C. O. Ferrari","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01908-z","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01908-z","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Prey face a major challenge in balancing predator avoidance with other essential activities. In environments with high risk, prey may exhibit neophobia (fear of novelty) due to the increased likelihood of novel stimuli being dangerous. The zebrafish, <i>Danio rerio</i>, is an established model organism for many scientific studies. Although spatial and object neophobia in zebrafish have received previous attention, little is known about the role of background risk in inducing neophobia in zebrafish. Here, we present two experiments using zebrafish to explore whether background predation risk can induce anxiety-like behaviour in a novel environment and neophobic responses when exposed to a novel odour. Over five days, we repeatedly exposed zebrafish to either high background risk in the form of chemical alarm cues (i.e., injured conspecific cues that indicate a predator attack) or a low-risk water control stimulus. In Experiment 1, when tested in a novel tank, zebrafish exposed to high predation risk displayed anxiety-like responses (reduced activity and increased bottom time spent) compared to their low-risk counterparts. Moreover, high-risk individuals showed reduced intra-session habituation to the novel tank compared to low-risk individuals. In Experiment 2, high-risk individuals exhibited fear responses toward a novel odour, unlike low-risk individuals. These results reveal that short-term repeated exposures to high risk can induce anxiety-like behaviour and predator odour neophobia in zebrafish.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"27 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11499451/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142493167","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When does metacognition evolve in the opt-out paradigm? 元认知在选择退出范式中何时发展?
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-22 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01910-5
Robin Watson

Metacognition (awareness of one’s own knowledge) is taken for granted in humans, but its evolution in non-human animals is not well understood. While there is experimental evidence of seemingly metacognitive judgements across species, studies rarely focus on why metacognition may have evolved. To address this, I present an evolutionary model of the opt-out paradigm, a common experiment used to assess animal’s metacognition. Individuals are repeatedly presented with a task or problem and must decide between opting-out and receiving a fixed payoff or opting-in and receiving a larger reward if they successfully solve the task. Two evolving traits – bias and metacognition – jointly determine whether individuals opt-in. The task’s reward, the mean probability of success and the variability in success across trials, and the cost of metacognition were varied. Results identify two scenarios where metacognition evolves: (1) environments where success variability is high; and (2) environments where mean success is low, but rewards are high. Overall, the results support predictions implicating uncertainty in the evolution of metacognition but suggest metacognition may also evolve in conditions where metacognition can be used to identify cases where an otherwise inaccessible high payoff is easy to acquire.

元认知(对自身知识的认知)在人类中被认为是理所当然的,但其在非人类动物中的进化却不甚明了。虽然有实验证据表明,不同物种之间似乎存在元认知判断,但研究很少关注元认知进化的原因。为了解决这个问题,我提出了一个选择退出范式的进化模型,这是一个用来评估动物元认知的常用实验。个体会反复面临一项任务或一个问题,他们必须在选择退出并获得固定回报,还是选择加入并在成功完成任务后获得更大回报之间做出决定。偏差和元认知这两种不断变化的特征共同决定了个体是否选择加入。任务的奖励、成功的平均概率和成功在不同试验中的变异性以及元认知的成本都是不同的。结果确定了元认知发展的两种情况:(1) 成功变异性高的环境;(2) 平均成功率低但奖励高的环境。总体而言,研究结果支持元认知进化过程中存在不确定性的预测,但也表明元认知也可能在这样的条件下进化,即元认知可以用来识别那些原本无法获得的高回报很容易获得的情况。
{"title":"When does metacognition evolve in the opt-out paradigm?","authors":"Robin Watson","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01910-5","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01910-5","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Metacognition (awareness of one’s own knowledge) is taken for granted in humans, but its evolution in non-human animals is not well understood. While there is experimental evidence of seemingly metacognitive judgements across species, studies rarely focus on why metacognition may have evolved. To address this, I present an evolutionary model of the opt-out paradigm, a common experiment used to assess animal’s metacognition. Individuals are repeatedly presented with a task or problem and must decide between opting-out and receiving a fixed payoff or opting-in and receiving a larger reward if they successfully solve the task. Two evolving traits – bias and metacognition – jointly determine whether individuals opt-in. The task’s reward, the mean probability of success and the variability in success across trials, and the cost of metacognition were varied. Results identify two scenarios where metacognition evolves: (1) environments where success variability is high; and (2) environments where mean success is low, but rewards are high. Overall, the results support predictions implicating uncertainty in the evolution of metacognition but suggest metacognition may also evolve in conditions where metacognition can be used to identify cases where an otherwise inaccessible high payoff is easy to acquire.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"27 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-024-01910-5.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142453103","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring horses’ (Equus caballus) gaze and asymmetric ear position in relation to human attentional cues 探索马匹(Equus caballus)的凝视和不对称耳位与人类注意力线索的关系
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-22 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01909-y
Gabriela Barrera, Anna Albiach-Serrano, Federico Guillén-Salazar

Studies have shown that horses are sensitive to human attentional cues. Also, there is some evidence that they might be able to infer the knowledge state of a human and communicate intentionally with us. However, this ability is not fully characterized and certain behaviors, like gaze, asymmetric ears position or vocalizations, which could work as attention-getting behaviors, have been scarcely studied in this context. The aim of the present study was to assess whether horses’ gaze toward a person, asymmetric ears position and vocalizations are subject to audience effects and adjust to human attentional cues, which would suggest a communicative function. For this purpose, we adapted a protocol used with domestic dogs [Kaminski et al. 2017] and observed horses’ spontaneous behavior (gaze, asymmetric ears position and vocalizations) in the presence of a human holding and not holding food in an attentive position (facing the horse with open eyes) and in a non-attentive position (with her back turned towards the horse). We found significant evidence of horses being sensitive to human attentional cues (reflected in the horses’ gaze duration and asymmetric position of the ears), but not of intentional communication towards humans.

研究表明,马对人类的注意力暗示很敏感。此外,还有一些证据表明,它们可能能够推断人类的知识状态,并有意与我们交流。然而,这种能力的特征还不完全清楚,而且某些行为,如凝视、耳朵位置不对称或发声等可能作为吸引注意力的行为,在这方面的研究还很少。本研究的目的是评估马匹对人的注视、不对称的耳朵位置和发声是否会受到观众效应的影响,是否会根据人类的注意暗示进行调整,从而表明马匹具有交流功能。为此,我们改编了一个用于家犬的方案[Kaminski 等人,2017 年],并观察了马匹在人类持有或未持有食物时的自发行为(凝视、不对称耳位和发声),包括专注姿势(睁眼面对马匹)和非专注姿势(背对马匹)。我们发现有重要证据表明马匹对人类的注意力暗示很敏感(反映在马匹的注视持续时间和耳朵的不对称位置上),但并不表明马匹有意与人类交流。
{"title":"Exploring horses’ (Equus caballus) gaze and asymmetric ear position in relation to human attentional cues","authors":"Gabriela Barrera,&nbsp;Anna Albiach-Serrano,&nbsp;Federico Guillén-Salazar","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01909-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01909-y","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Studies have shown that horses are sensitive to human attentional cues. Also, there is some evidence that they might be able to infer the knowledge state of a human and communicate intentionally with us. However, this ability is not fully characterized and certain behaviors, like gaze, asymmetric ears position or vocalizations, which could work as attention-getting behaviors, have been scarcely studied in this context. The aim of the present study was to assess whether horses’ gaze toward a person, asymmetric ears position and vocalizations are subject to audience effects and adjust to human attentional cues, which would suggest a communicative function. For this purpose, we adapted a protocol used with domestic dogs [Kaminski et al. 2017] and observed horses’ spontaneous behavior (gaze, asymmetric ears position and vocalizations) in the presence of a human holding and not holding food in an attentive position (facing the horse with open eyes) and in a non-attentive position (with her back turned towards the horse). We found significant evidence of horses being sensitive to human attentional cues (reflected in the horses’ gaze duration and asymmetric position of the ears), but not of intentional communication towards humans.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"27 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-024-01909-y.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142453122","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effect of pre-session discrimination training on performance in a judgement bias test in dogs 会前辨别力训练对狗的判断偏差测试成绩的影响
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-12 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01905-2
Joseph Krahn, Amin Azadian, Camila Cavalli, Julia Miller, Alexandra Protopopova

Spatial judgement bias tests (JBTs) can involve teaching animals that a bowl provides a reward in one location but does not in another. The animal is then presented with the bowl placed between the rewarded and the unrewarded locations (i.e., ambiguous locations) and their latency to approach reflects expectation of reward or ‘optimism’. Some suggest that greater ‘optimism’ indicates better welfare. Performance in JBTs, however, may also indicate a learning history independently from welfare determinants. We hypothesized that dogs’ ‘optimism’ in a follow-up JBT may be impacted by a learning treatment involving additional trials of a different discrimination task. Once enrolled, companion dogs (n = 16) were required to complete three study phases: (1) a pre-treatment JBT, (2) a learning treatment, and (3) a post-treatment JBT. During the JBTs, dogs were presented with five locations: one rewarded, one unrewarded, and three ambiguous (all unrewarded). Dogs were randomly assigned to a trial-based learning task—a nose-touch to the palm of the hand. In the Experimental discrimination treatment phase (n = 8), dogs were presented with two hands in each trial and only rewarded for touching one specific hand. In the Control treatment phase (n = 8), dogs were presented with one hand per trial in alternating sequence and were yoked to dogs in the Experimental group to receive the same number of rewarded and unrewarded trials (to control for possible frustration). Using a repeated measures mixed model with JBT repeated within dog, we found no difference in the change in approach latency to the ambiguous locations between the dogs across treatments. ‘Optimism’ as measured in this JBT was not altered by the additional discrimination trials used in our study.

空间判断偏差测试(JBTs)可以让动物知道,一个碗在某一位置有奖励,而在另一位置则没有。然后将碗放在有奖励和无奖励的地点(即模糊地点)之间,动物接近碗的潜伏期反映了它们对奖励的期望或 "乐观 "程度。有人认为,"乐观 "程度越高,表明动物的福利越好。然而,JBT 的表现也可能表明狗的学习历史,而与福利决定因素无关。我们假设,狗在后续 JBT 中的 "乐观情绪 "可能会受到涉及不同辨别任务的额外试验的学习处理的影响。伴侣犬(n = 16)一旦注册,就必须完成三个研究阶段:(1)治疗前 JBT;(2)学习治疗;(3)治疗后 JBT。在 JBT 期间,狗会看到五个地点:一个有奖励、一个无奖励和三个模糊地点(均无奖励)。狗被随机分配到一项基于试验的学习任务--鼻子触碰手掌。在实验辨别处理阶段(n = 8),狗在每次试验中都会看到两只手,只有触摸到一只特定的手才会得到奖励。在对照组处理阶段(n = 8),狗在每次试验中交替出现一只手,并与实验组的狗绑在一起,接受相同次数的奖励和无奖励试验(以控制可能出现的挫败感)。使用重复测量混合模型,在犬内重复 JBT,我们发现在不同处理中,犬接近模糊位置的潜伏期变化没有差异。在我们的研究中,额外的辨别试验并没有改变 JBT 所测量的 "乐观 "程度。
{"title":"Effect of pre-session discrimination training on performance in a judgement bias test in dogs","authors":"Joseph Krahn,&nbsp;Amin Azadian,&nbsp;Camila Cavalli,&nbsp;Julia Miller,&nbsp;Alexandra Protopopova","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01905-2","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01905-2","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Spatial judgement bias tests (JBTs) can involve teaching animals that a bowl provides a reward in one location but does not in another. The animal is then presented with the bowl placed between the rewarded and the unrewarded locations (i.e., ambiguous locations) and their latency to approach reflects expectation of reward or ‘optimism’. Some suggest that greater ‘optimism’ indicates better welfare. Performance in JBTs, however, may also indicate a learning history independently from welfare determinants. We hypothesized that dogs’ ‘optimism’ in a follow-up JBT may be impacted by a learning treatment involving additional trials of a different discrimination task. Once enrolled, companion dogs (<i>n</i> = 16) were required to complete three study phases: (1) a pre-treatment JBT, (2) a learning treatment, and (3) a post-treatment JBT. During the JBTs, dogs were presented with five locations: one rewarded, one unrewarded, and three ambiguous (all unrewarded). Dogs were randomly assigned to a trial-based learning task—a nose-touch to the palm of the hand. In the Experimental discrimination treatment phase (<i>n</i> = 8), dogs were presented with two hands in each trial and only rewarded for touching one specific hand. In the Control treatment phase (<i>n</i> = 8), dogs were presented with one hand per trial in alternating sequence and were yoked to dogs in the Experimental group to receive the same number of rewarded and unrewarded trials (to control for possible frustration). Using a repeated measures mixed model with JBT repeated within dog, we found no difference in the change in approach latency to the ambiguous locations between the dogs across treatments. ‘Optimism’ as measured in this JBT was not altered by the additional discrimination trials used in our study.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"27 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s10071-024-01905-2.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142411508","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction to: Are lateralized and bold fish optimistic or pessimistic? 更正:横向大胆的鱼是乐观还是悲观?
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-08 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01894-2
Flavia Berlinghieri, Gabriele Rizzuto, Lara Kruizinga, Bernd Riedstra, Ton G. G. Groothuis, Culum Brown
{"title":"Correction to: Are lateralized and bold fish optimistic or pessimistic?","authors":"Flavia Berlinghieri,&nbsp;Gabriele Rizzuto,&nbsp;Lara Kruizinga,&nbsp;Bernd Riedstra,&nbsp;Ton G. G. Groothuis,&nbsp;Culum Brown","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01894-2","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01894-2","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"27 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11461544/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142387386","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The problem with two-event sequence learning by pigeons 鸽子的双事件序列学习问题。
IF 1.9 2区 生物学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-10-03 DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01906-1
Thomas R. Zentall, Daniel N. Peng

Bonobos appear to show little evidence of learning to make one response (R1) to an AB sequence and a different response (R2) to sequences BB, AA, and BA (Lind et al. PLoS ONE 18(9):e0290546, 2023), yet under different conditions, pigeons can learn this (Weisman et al. Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 6(4):312, 1980). Aspects of the bonobo procedure may have contributed to this failure. Most important, no response was required in the presence of the stimuli to encourage attention to them. Furthermore, learning to make one response to the target sequence and another to the other sequences involves a bias that allows for better than chance responding. With the two-alternative forced-choice procedure used with the bonobos, the R1 response is correct for one sequence, whereas the R2 response is correct for three sequences. To correct for this, there are three times as many AB trials as each of the other sequences. However, this correction allows a bias to develop in which reinforcement often can be obtained by using only the last stimulus seen as the basis of choice (e.g., when the last stimulus is B respond R1 when the last stimulus is A respond R2). This solution yields reinforcement on five out of six, or 83%, of the trials. In the present experiment with pigeons, using this two-alternative forced choice procedure, most subjects tended to base their choice on the last-seen stimulus. This design allowed subjects to use a suboptimal but relatively effective choice strategy.

倭黑猩猩似乎很少有证据表明它们学会了对 AB 序列做出一个反应(R1),而对 BB、AA 和 BA 序列做出不同的反应(R2)(Lind 等人,PLoS ONE 18(9):e0290546,2023 年),但在不同条件下,鸽子也能学会这一点(Weisman 等人,Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 6(4):312,1980 年)。倭黑猩猩程序的某些方面可能导致了这一失败。最重要的是,在刺激物存在的情况下,倭黑猩猩不需要做出任何反应来鼓励对刺激物的注意。此外,学习对目标序列做出一种反应,而对其他序列做出另一种反应涉及到一种偏差,这种偏差允许比偶然反应更好的反应。在倭黑猩猩使用的双项强迫选择程序中,R1 反应对一个序列是正确的,而 R2 反应对三个序列是正确的。为了纠正这一点,AB 试验的次数是其他序列的三倍。但是,这种纠正会产生一种偏差,即通常只使用最后一个刺激作为选择基础就能获得强化(例如,当最后一个刺激是 B 时,做出 R1 反应;当最后一个刺激是 A 时,做出 R2 反应)。这种方法可以在六次试验中的五次,即 83% 的试验中获得强化。在目前的鸽子实验中,使用这种双备选强迫选择程序时,大多数被试倾向于根据最后看到的刺激做出选择。这种设计允许被试使用一种次优但相对有效的选择策略。
{"title":"The problem with two-event sequence learning by pigeons","authors":"Thomas R. Zentall,&nbsp;Daniel N. Peng","doi":"10.1007/s10071-024-01906-1","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10071-024-01906-1","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Bonobos appear to show little evidence of learning to make one response (R1) to an AB sequence and a different response (R2) to sequences BB, AA, and BA (Lind et al. PLoS ONE 18(9):e0290546, 2023), yet under different conditions, pigeons can learn this (Weisman et al. Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 6(4):312, 1980). Aspects of the bonobo procedure may have contributed to this failure. Most important, no response was required in the presence of the stimuli to encourage attention to them. Furthermore, learning to make one response to the target sequence and another to the other sequences involves a bias that allows for better than chance responding. With the two-alternative forced-choice procedure used with the bonobos, the R1 response is correct for one sequence, whereas the R2 response is correct for three sequences. To correct for this, there are three times as many AB trials as each of the other sequences. However, this correction allows a bias to develop in which reinforcement often can be obtained by using only the last stimulus seen as the basis of choice (e.g., when the last stimulus is B respond R1 when the last stimulus is A respond R2). This solution yields reinforcement on five out of six, or 83%, of the trials. In the present experiment with pigeons, using this two-alternative forced choice procedure, most subjects tended to base their choice on the last-seen stimulus. This design allowed subjects to use a suboptimal but relatively effective choice strategy.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7879,"journal":{"name":"Animal Cognition","volume":"27 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11450055/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142364032","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Animal Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1