首页 > 最新文献

Behavior Research Methods最新文献

英文 中文
Survey measures of metacognitive monitoring are often false.
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-18 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-025-02621-6
Kit S Double

Metacognitive monitoring is an extremely important ability that predicts a wide range of outcomes. However, do people have insight into their own metacognitive monitoring capacity? This study measured participants' perceived metacognitive monitoring abilities using a novel psychometrically validated questionnaire (Study 1) and then examined how well survey responses aligned with online measures of metacognitive monitoring (resolution, discrimination, sensitivity, efficiency) taken from confidence ratings participants made while performing a perceptual decision-making task and Raven's Progressive Matrices (Study 2). We found a negative correlation between the questionnaire responses and many of the online measures of metacognitive monitoring - those who reported being better at metacognitive monitoring, in fact tended to be worse according to the online metacognitive ratings. This occurred because, in general, high self-perceptions of monitoring ability were, in fact, related to higher confidence and lower cognitive performance. These findings suggest that we may have inaccurate insights into our own metacognitive monitoring capacity and questionnaire-based measures of metacognitive abilities may be problematic as they may represent unrealistic self-perceptions.

{"title":"Survey measures of metacognitive monitoring are often false.","authors":"Kit S Double","doi":"10.3758/s13428-025-02621-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-025-02621-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Metacognitive monitoring is an extremely important ability that predicts a wide range of outcomes. However, do people have insight into their own metacognitive monitoring capacity? This study measured participants' perceived metacognitive monitoring abilities using a novel psychometrically validated questionnaire (Study 1) and then examined how well survey responses aligned with online measures of metacognitive monitoring (resolution, discrimination, sensitivity, efficiency) taken from confidence ratings participants made while performing a perceptual decision-making task and Raven's Progressive Matrices (Study 2). We found a negative correlation between the questionnaire responses and many of the online measures of metacognitive monitoring - those who reported being better at metacognitive monitoring, in fact tended to be worse according to the online metacognitive ratings. This occurred because, in general, high self-perceptions of monitoring ability were, in fact, related to higher confidence and lower cognitive performance. These findings suggest that we may have inaccurate insights into our own metacognitive monitoring capacity and questionnaire-based measures of metacognitive abilities may be problematic as they may represent unrealistic self-perceptions.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":"57 3","pages":"97"},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2025-02-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143447830","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Open-access network science: Investigating phonological similarity networks based on the SUBTLEX-US lexicon.
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-18 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-025-02610-9
John Alderete, Sarbjot Mann, Paul Tupper

Network science tools are becoming increasingly important to psycholinguistics, but few open-access data sets exist for exploring network properties of even well-studied languages like English. We constructed several phonological similarity networks (neighbors differ in exactly one consonant or vowel phoneme) using words from a lexicon based on the SUBTLEX-US English corpus, distinguishing networks by size and word representation (i.e., lemma vs. word form). The resulting networks are shown to exhibit many familiar characteristics, including small-world properties, broad degree distributions, and robustness to node removal, regardless of network size and word representation. We also validated the SUBTLEX phonological networks by showing that they exhibit contrasts in degree and clustering coefficient comparable to the same contrasts found in prior studies and exhibit familiar trends after extraction of a backbone network of nodes important to network centrality. The data release ( https://github.com/aldo-git-bit/phonological-similarity-networks-SUBTLEX ) includes 17 adjacency lists that can be further explored using the networkX package in Python, a package of files for building new adjacency lists from scratch, and several scripts that allow users to analyze and extend these results.

{"title":"Open-access network science: Investigating phonological similarity networks based on the SUBTLEX-US lexicon.","authors":"John Alderete, Sarbjot Mann, Paul Tupper","doi":"10.3758/s13428-025-02610-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-025-02610-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Network science tools are becoming increasingly important to psycholinguistics, but few open-access data sets exist for exploring network properties of even well-studied languages like English. We constructed several phonological similarity networks (neighbors differ in exactly one consonant or vowel phoneme) using words from a lexicon based on the SUBTLEX-US English corpus, distinguishing networks by size and word representation (i.e., lemma vs. word form). The resulting networks are shown to exhibit many familiar characteristics, including small-world properties, broad degree distributions, and robustness to node removal, regardless of network size and word representation. We also validated the SUBTLEX phonological networks by showing that they exhibit contrasts in degree and clustering coefficient comparable to the same contrasts found in prior studies and exhibit familiar trends after extraction of a backbone network of nodes important to network centrality. The data release ( https://github.com/aldo-git-bit/phonological-similarity-networks-SUBTLEX ) includes 17 adjacency lists that can be further explored using the networkX package in Python, a package of files for building new adjacency lists from scratch, and several scripts that allow users to analyze and extend these results.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":"57 3","pages":"96"},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2025-02-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143447828","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
PREVIC: An adaptive parent report measure of expressive vocabulary in children between 3 and 8 years of age.
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-17 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-025-02615-4
Manuel Bohn, Julia Prein, Jonas Engicht, Daniel Haun, Natalia Gagarina, Tobias Koch

Parent report measures have proven to be a valuable research tool for studying early language development. Caregivers are given a list of words and are asked which of them their child has already used. However, most available measures are not suited for children beyond infancy, come with substantial licensing costs or lack a clear psychometric foundation. Here, we present the PREVIC (Parent Report of Expressive Vocabulary in Children), an open-access, high-quality vocabulary checklist for German-speaking children between 3 and 8 years of age. The PREVIC was constructed leveraging the advantages of item response theory: we designed a large initial item pool of 379 words and collected data from 1190 caregivers of children between 3 and 8 years of age. Based on these data, we computed a range of fit indices for each item (word) and used an automated item selection algorithm to compile a final pool that contains items that (a) vary in difficulty and (b) fit the Rasch (one-parameter logistic) model. The resulting task is highly reliable and shows convergent validity. The IRT-based construction allowed us to design an adaptive version of the task that substantially reduces the task duration while retaining measurement precision. The task - including the adaptive version - was implemented as a website and is freely accessible online ( https://ccp-odc.eva.mpg.de/previc-demo/ ). The PREVIC fills an important gap in the toolkit of researchers interested in language development and provides an ideal starting point for developing converging measures in other languages.

{"title":"PREVIC: An adaptive parent report measure of expressive vocabulary in children between 3 and 8 years of age.","authors":"Manuel Bohn, Julia Prein, Jonas Engicht, Daniel Haun, Natalia Gagarina, Tobias Koch","doi":"10.3758/s13428-025-02615-4","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-025-02615-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Parent report measures have proven to be a valuable research tool for studying early language development. Caregivers are given a list of words and are asked which of them their child has already used. However, most available measures are not suited for children beyond infancy, come with substantial licensing costs or lack a clear psychometric foundation. Here, we present the PREVIC (Parent Report of Expressive Vocabulary in Children), an open-access, high-quality vocabulary checklist for German-speaking children between 3 and 8 years of age. The PREVIC was constructed leveraging the advantages of item response theory: we designed a large initial item pool of 379 words and collected data from 1190 caregivers of children between 3 and 8 years of age. Based on these data, we computed a range of fit indices for each item (word) and used an automated item selection algorithm to compile a final pool that contains items that (a) vary in difficulty and (b) fit the Rasch (one-parameter logistic) model. The resulting task is highly reliable and shows convergent validity. The IRT-based construction allowed us to design an adaptive version of the task that substantially reduces the task duration while retaining measurement precision. The task - including the adaptive version - was implemented as a website and is freely accessible online ( https://ccp-odc.eva.mpg.de/previc-demo/ ). The PREVIC fills an important gap in the toolkit of researchers interested in language development and provides an ideal starting point for developing converging measures in other languages.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":"57 3","pages":"95"},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2025-02-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143439808","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Development and evaluation of Dona, a privacy-preserving donation platform for messaging data from WhatsApp, Facebook, and Instagram.
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-14 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02593-z
Olya Hakobyan, Paul-Julius Hillmann, Florian Martin, Erwin Böttinger, Hanna Drimalla

Social interactions are a fundamental aspect of human life, yet, their objective and naturalistic measurement remains challenging for scientific research. This challenge can be addressed using digital communication data. To this end, we have developed Dona, an open-source platform for donating messaging data from WhatsApp, Facebook, and Instagram. Given the highly sensitive nature of messaging data, we ensure participant privacy through rigorous data minimization. Dona removes all sensitive information on the user side prior to donation, retaining only de-identified meta-data such as message length and timestamps. This paper presents an overview of the platform, a deployment guide, and example use cases. In addition, we evaluate the informativeness of minimized messaging data for studying social interactions with two approaches. First, we conducted a user study in which 85 participants donated their data, received visualizations of their messaging behavior and evaluated the informativeness of this visual feedback. Second, we performed a quantitative analysis using over 1500 donated chats to confirm whether minimized messaging data captures known aspects of human interactions, such as interaction balance, heterogeneity, and burstiness. The results demonstrate that minimized, de-identified messaging data reflects informative interaction features as assessed by both self-reports and objective metrics. In conclusion, Dona is a donation platform well suited for sensitive contexts in which researchers aim to balance participant privacy with the acquisition of objective and informative data on social interactions.

{"title":"Development and evaluation of Dona, a privacy-preserving donation platform for messaging data from WhatsApp, Facebook, and Instagram.","authors":"Olya Hakobyan, Paul-Julius Hillmann, Florian Martin, Erwin Böttinger, Hanna Drimalla","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02593-z","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02593-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Social interactions are a fundamental aspect of human life, yet, their objective and naturalistic measurement remains challenging for scientific research. This challenge can be addressed using digital communication data. To this end, we have developed Dona, an open-source platform for donating messaging data from WhatsApp, Facebook, and Instagram. Given the highly sensitive nature of messaging data, we ensure participant privacy through rigorous data minimization. Dona removes all sensitive information on the user side prior to donation, retaining only de-identified meta-data such as message length and timestamps. This paper presents an overview of the platform, a deployment guide, and example use cases. In addition, we evaluate the informativeness of minimized messaging data for studying social interactions with two approaches. First, we conducted a user study in which 85 participants donated their data, received visualizations of their messaging behavior and evaluated the informativeness of this visual feedback. Second, we performed a quantitative analysis using over 1500 donated chats to confirm whether minimized messaging data captures known aspects of human interactions, such as interaction balance, heterogeneity, and burstiness. The results demonstrate that minimized, de-identified messaging data reflects informative interaction features as assessed by both self-reports and objective metrics. In conclusion, Dona is a donation platform well suited for sensitive contexts in which researchers aim to balance participant privacy with the acquisition of objective and informative data on social interactions.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":"57 3","pages":"94"},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2025-02-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11828832/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143413295","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reliability representativeness: How well does coefficient alpha summarize reliability across the score distribution?
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-10 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-025-02611-8
Daniel McNeish, Denis Dumas

Scale scores in psychology studies are commonly accompanied by a reliability coefficient like alpha. Coefficient alpha is an index that summarizes reliability across the entire score distribution, implying equal precision for all scores. However, an underappreciated fact is that reliability can be conditional such that scores in certain parts of the score distribution may be more reliable than others. This conditional perspective of reliability is common in item response theory (IRT), but psychologists are generally not well versed in IRT. Correspondingly, the representativeness of a single summary index like alpha across the entire score distribution can be unclear but is rarely considered. If conditional reliability is fairly homogeneous across the score distribution, coefficient alpha may be sufficiently representative and a useful summary. But, if conditional reliability is heterogeneous across the score distribution, alpha may be unrepresentative and may not align with the reliability of a typical score in the data or with a particularly important score like a cut point where decisions are made. This paper proposes a method, R package, and Shiny application to quantify the potential differences between coefficient alpha and conditional reliability across the score distribution. The goal is to facilitate comparisons between conditional reliability and reliability summary indices so that psychologists can contextualize the reliability of their scores more clearly and comprehensively.

心理学研究中的量表评分通常会附带一个信度系数,如 alpha。系数α是一个概括整个分数分布可靠性的指数,意味着所有分数的精确度相同。然而,一个未得到充分重视的事实是,信度可能是有条件的,比如分数分布中某些部分的分数可能比其他部分的分数更可靠。这种有条件的信度观点在项目反应理论(IRT)中很常见,但心理学家一般对 IRT 并不精通。相应地,像 alpha 这样的单一汇总指标在整个分数分布中的代表性可能并不明确,但却很少被考虑。如果条件信度在整个分数分布中相当均匀,α 系数可能具有足够的代表性,是一个有用的总结。但是,如果条件信度在整个分数分布中是异质的,α 可能就不具有代表性,也可能与数据中典型分数的信度不一致,或与特别重要的分数(如决策的切点)不一致。本文提出了一种方法、R 软件包和 Shiny 应用程序,用于量化整个分数分布中系数 alpha 和条件可靠性之间的潜在差异。本文的目的是促进条件信度与信度汇总指数之间的比较,从而使心理学家能够更清晰、更全面地了解分数的信度。
{"title":"Reliability representativeness: How well does coefficient alpha summarize reliability across the score distribution?","authors":"Daniel McNeish, Denis Dumas","doi":"10.3758/s13428-025-02611-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-025-02611-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Scale scores in psychology studies are commonly accompanied by a reliability coefficient like alpha. Coefficient alpha is an index that summarizes reliability across the entire score distribution, implying equal precision for all scores. However, an underappreciated fact is that reliability can be conditional such that scores in certain parts of the score distribution may be more reliable than others. This conditional perspective of reliability is common in item response theory (IRT), but psychologists are generally not well versed in IRT. Correspondingly, the representativeness of a single summary index like alpha across the entire score distribution can be unclear but is rarely considered. If conditional reliability is fairly homogeneous across the score distribution, coefficient alpha may be sufficiently representative and a useful summary. But, if conditional reliability is heterogeneous across the score distribution, alpha may be unrepresentative and may not align with the reliability of a typical score in the data or with a particularly important score like a cut point where decisions are made. This paper proposes a method, R package, and Shiny application to quantify the potential differences between coefficient alpha and conditional reliability across the score distribution. The goal is to facilitate comparisons between conditional reliability and reliability summary indices so that psychologists can contextualize the reliability of their scores more clearly and comprehensively.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":"57 3","pages":"93"},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2025-02-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143389901","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction: A large-scale database of Mandarin Chinese word associations from the Small World of Words Project.
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-07 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-025-02603-8
Bing Li, Ziyi Ding, Simon De Deyne, Qing Cai
{"title":"Correction: A large-scale database of Mandarin Chinese word associations from the Small World of Words Project.","authors":"Bing Li, Ziyi Ding, Simon De Deyne, Qing Cai","doi":"10.3758/s13428-025-02603-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-025-02603-8","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":"57 3","pages":"91"},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2025-02-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143370352","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
To interact or not to interact: The pros and cons of including interactions in linear regression models.
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-07 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-025-02613-6
Aljoscha Rimpler, Henk A L Kiers, Don van Ravenzwaaij

Interaction effects are very common in the psychological literature. However, interaction effects are typically very small and often fail to replicate. In this study, we conducted a simulation comparing the generalizability and estimability of two linear regression models: one correctly specified to account for interaction effects and one misspecified including simple effects only. We manipulated noise levels, predictor variable correlations, and different sets of regression weights, resulting in 9216 different conditions. From each dataset, we drew 1000 samples of N = 25, 50, 100, 250, 500, and 1000, resulting in a total of 55,296,000 analyses for each model. Our results show that misspecification can drastically bias regression estimates, sometimes leading to zero or reversed simple effects. Furthermore, we found that when models are generalized to the entire population, the difference between the explained variance in the sample and in the population is often smaller for the misspecified model than for the correctly specified model. However, the comparison between models shows that the correctly specified model explains the data at the population level better overall. These results emphasize the importance of theory in modeling choices and show that it is important to provide a rationale for why interactions are included or excluded in an analysis.

{"title":"To interact or not to interact: The pros and cons of including interactions in linear regression models.","authors":"Aljoscha Rimpler, Henk A L Kiers, Don van Ravenzwaaij","doi":"10.3758/s13428-025-02613-6","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-025-02613-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Interaction effects are very common in the psychological literature. However, interaction effects are typically very small and often fail to replicate. In this study, we conducted a simulation comparing the generalizability and estimability of two linear regression models: one correctly specified to account for interaction effects and one misspecified including simple effects only. We manipulated noise levels, predictor variable correlations, and different sets of regression weights, resulting in 9216 different conditions. From each dataset, we drew 1000 samples of N = 25, 50, 100, 250, 500, and 1000, resulting in a total of 55,296,000 analyses for each model. Our results show that misspecification can drastically bias regression estimates, sometimes leading to zero or reversed simple effects. Furthermore, we found that when models are generalized to the entire population, the difference between the explained variance in the sample and in the population is often smaller for the misspecified model than for the correctly specified model. However, the comparison between models shows that the correctly specified model explains the data at the population level better overall. These results emphasize the importance of theory in modeling choices and show that it is important to provide a rationale for why interactions are included or excluded in an analysis.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":"57 3","pages":"92"},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2025-02-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11805792/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143370354","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
ESTraS, an easy and simple tracking system for assessing rodent behavior.
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-03 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02537-7
A Morcuende, T Femenía, J Manzanares, A Gasparyan

Animal behavior analysis software has become an essential tool in the life sciences. However, the currently available tools have some significant shortcomings that limit their use by individuals without programming skills, access to higher informatics resources, or the capability to invest large sums of money. We have developed and validated an easy-to-use and straightforward tracking system named ESTraS to address this issue. This freeware software allows researchers to track and analyze rodent behaviors, offering additional options such as trajectory and angle analysis. Through ESTraS, researchers can utilize unsupervised clustering techniques, such as k-means or hierarchical clustering, to further explore the obtained results. This clustering enables the classification of results based on observed similarities among subjects. The data of this manuscript shows that ESTraS can prove to be extremely valuable, not only by providing essential behavioral analysis tools but also by offering specific data analysis options with just one click.

{"title":"ESTraS, an easy and simple tracking system for assessing rodent behavior.","authors":"A Morcuende, T Femenía, J Manzanares, A Gasparyan","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02537-7","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02537-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Animal behavior analysis software has become an essential tool in the life sciences. However, the currently available tools have some significant shortcomings that limit their use by individuals without programming skills, access to higher informatics resources, or the capability to invest large sums of money. We have developed and validated an easy-to-use and straightforward tracking system named ESTraS to address this issue. This freeware software allows researchers to track and analyze rodent behaviors, offering additional options such as trajectory and angle analysis. Through ESTraS, researchers can utilize unsupervised clustering techniques, such as k-means or hierarchical clustering, to further explore the obtained results. This clustering enables the classification of results based on observed similarities among subjects. The data of this manuscript shows that ESTraS can prove to be extremely valuable, not only by providing essential behavioral analysis tools but also by offering specific data analysis options with just one click.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":"57 3","pages":"90"},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2025-02-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11790769/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143121722","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A large-scale, gamified online assessment of first impressions: The Who Knows project.
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-03 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-025-02601-w
Richard Rau, Michael P Grosz, Mitja D Back

Interpersonal judgments play a central role in human social interactions, influencing decisions ranging from friendships to presidential elections. Despite extensive research on the accuracy of these judgments, an overreliance on broad personality traits and subjective judgments as criteria for accuracy has hindered progress in this area. Further, most individuals involved in past studies (either as judges or targets) came from ad-hoc student samples which hampers generalizability. This paper introduces Who Knows ( https://whoknows.uni-muenster.de ), an innovative smartphone application designed to address these limitations. Who Knows was developed with the aim to create a comprehensive and reliable database for examining first impressions. It utilizes a gamified approach where users judge personality-related characteristics of strangers based on short video introductions. The project incorporates multifaceted criteria to evaluate judgments, going beyond traditional self-other agreement. Additionally, the app draws on a large pool of highly specific and heterogenous items and allows users to judge a diverse array of targets on their smartphones. The app's design prioritizes user engagement through a responsive interface, feedback mechanisms, and gamification elements, enhancing their motivation to provide judgments. The Who Knows project is ongoing and promises to shed new light on interpersonal perception by offering a vast dataset with diverse items and a large number of participants (as of fall 2024, N = 9,671 users). Researchers are encouraged to access this resource for a wide range of empirical inquiries and to contribute to the project by submitting items or software features to be included in future versions of the app.

{"title":"A large-scale, gamified online assessment of first impressions: The Who Knows project.","authors":"Richard Rau, Michael P Grosz, Mitja D Back","doi":"10.3758/s13428-025-02601-w","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-025-02601-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Interpersonal judgments play a central role in human social interactions, influencing decisions ranging from friendships to presidential elections. Despite extensive research on the accuracy of these judgments, an overreliance on broad personality traits and subjective judgments as criteria for accuracy has hindered progress in this area. Further, most individuals involved in past studies (either as judges or targets) came from ad-hoc student samples which hampers generalizability. This paper introduces Who Knows ( https://whoknows.uni-muenster.de ), an innovative smartphone application designed to address these limitations. Who Knows was developed with the aim to create a comprehensive and reliable database for examining first impressions. It utilizes a gamified approach where users judge personality-related characteristics of strangers based on short video introductions. The project incorporates multifaceted criteria to evaluate judgments, going beyond traditional self-other agreement. Additionally, the app draws on a large pool of highly specific and heterogenous items and allows users to judge a diverse array of targets on their smartphones. The app's design prioritizes user engagement through a responsive interface, feedback mechanisms, and gamification elements, enhancing their motivation to provide judgments. The Who Knows project is ongoing and promises to shed new light on interpersonal perception by offering a vast dataset with diverse items and a large number of participants (as of fall 2024, N = 9,671 users). Researchers are encouraged to access this resource for a wide range of empirical inquiries and to contribute to the project by submitting items or software features to be included in future versions of the app.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":"57 3","pages":"83"},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2025-02-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11790696/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143121717","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Viewing mock crimes in virtual reality increases presence without impacting memory.
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-03 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02575-1
Andrew D Green, Andrew Clark, Melanie Pitchford, Andy Guppy

Traditional methods of displaying stimuli in eyewitness memory research, such as mock crime videos, are often criticised for lacking ecological validity. To overcome this, researchers have suggested using virtual reality (VR) technology to display the stimuli as it can promote a sense of presence, leading to real-world responses. However, little research has compared VR with traditional methods to demonstrate this enhanced validity. In Study 1, 54 participants viewed a mock crime video on screen or in VR while their heart rate was recorded, then completed measures of presence and emotion, and had their recall tested after 10 min. In Study 2, 74 participants' recall was tested after a 7-day delay and included a more in-depth exploration of emotional experience. In both studies, participants in the VR group reported a statistically significant increase in their sense of general presence, spatial presence, and involvement in the scene; however, there was no statistically significant difference in recall between the groups. Participants in the VR group had a statistically significant increase in heart rate in Study 1 only, and emotional experience in Study 2 only. The findings of this research suggest that VR may provide a more ecologically valid eyewitness experience than videos, without impacting participant memory or wellbeing. The findings of the current research are discussed in relation to previous literature and implications for experimental eyewitness memory research.

{"title":"Viewing mock crimes in virtual reality increases presence without impacting memory.","authors":"Andrew D Green, Andrew Clark, Melanie Pitchford, Andy Guppy","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02575-1","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02575-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Traditional methods of displaying stimuli in eyewitness memory research, such as mock crime videos, are often criticised for lacking ecological validity. To overcome this, researchers have suggested using virtual reality (VR) technology to display the stimuli as it can promote a sense of presence, leading to real-world responses. However, little research has compared VR with traditional methods to demonstrate this enhanced validity. In Study 1, 54 participants viewed a mock crime video on screen or in VR while their heart rate was recorded, then completed measures of presence and emotion, and had their recall tested after 10 min. In Study 2, 74 participants' recall was tested after a 7-day delay and included a more in-depth exploration of emotional experience. In both studies, participants in the VR group reported a statistically significant increase in their sense of general presence, spatial presence, and involvement in the scene; however, there was no statistically significant difference in recall between the groups. Participants in the VR group had a statistically significant increase in heart rate in Study 1 only, and emotional experience in Study 2 only. The findings of this research suggest that VR may provide a more ecologically valid eyewitness experience than videos, without impacting participant memory or wellbeing. The findings of the current research are discussed in relation to previous literature and implications for experimental eyewitness memory research.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":"57 3","pages":"88"},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2025-02-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11790719/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143121740","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Behavior Research Methods
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1