首页 > 最新文献

Behavior Research Methods最新文献

英文 中文
Factor retention in ordered categorical variables: Benefits and costs of polychoric correlations in eigenvalue-based testing. 有序分类变量中的因子保留:基于特征值的测试中多变量相关性的好处和代价。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-06 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02417-0
Nils Brandenburg

An essential step in exploratory factor analysis is to determine the optimal number of factors. The Next Eigenvalue Sufficiency Test (NEST; Achim, 2017) is a recent proposal to determine the number of factors based on significance tests of the statistical contributions of candidate factors indicated by eigenvalues of sample correlation matrices. Previous simulation studies have shown NEST to recover the optimal number of factors in simulated datasets with high accuracy. However, these studies have focused on continuous variables. The present work addresses the performance of NEST for ordinal data. It has been debated whether factor models - and thus also the optimal number of factors - for ordinal variables should be computed for Pearson correlation matrices, which are known to underestimate correlations for ordinal datasets, or for polychoric correlation matrices, which are known to be instable. The central research question is to what extent the problems associated with Pearson correlations and polychoric correlations deteriorate NEST for ordinal datasets. Implementations of NEST tailored to ordinal datasets by utilizing polychoric correlations are proposed. In a simulation, the proposed implementations were compared to the original implementation of NEST which computes Pearson correlations even for ordinal datasets. The simulation shows that substituting polychoric correlations for Pearson correlations improves the accuracy of NEST for binary variables and large sample sizes (N = 500). However, the simulation also shows that the original implementation using Pearson correlations was the most accurate implementation for Likert-type variables with four response categories when item difficulties were homogeneous.

探索性因子分析的一个重要步骤是确定因子的最佳数量。下一个特征值充分性检验(NEST;Achim,2017 年)是最近提出的一项建议,它基于对样本相关矩阵特征值所显示的候选因子统计贡献的显著性检验来确定因子数量。以往的模拟研究表明,NEST 能在模拟数据集中高精度地恢复最佳因子数。不过,这些研究主要针对连续变量。本研究将探讨 NEST 在序数数据中的表现。人们一直在争论,究竟是应该根据皮尔逊相关矩阵(已知会低估序数数据集的相关性)来计算序数变量的因子模型,进而计算因子的最佳数量,还是应该根据多变量相关矩阵(已知会不稳定)来计算序数变量的因子模型,进而计算因子的最佳数量。研究的核心问题是,与皮尔逊相关性和多元相关性相关的问题会在多大程度上恶化 NEST 对序数数据集的影响。本文提出了通过利用多变量相关性为序数数据集量身定制的 NEST 实现方法。在模拟中,将所提出的实现方法与 NEST 的原始实现方法进行了比较,后者甚至对序数数据集也能计算皮尔逊相关性。模拟结果表明,在二进制变量和大样本量(N = 500)情况下,用多变量相关性代替皮尔逊相关性提高了 NEST 的准确性。然而,模拟结果也表明,在项目难度相同的情况下,使用皮尔逊相关性的原始实施方案对于具有四个响应类别的李克特变量来说是最准确的实施方案。
{"title":"Factor retention in ordered categorical variables: Benefits and costs of polychoric correlations in eigenvalue-based testing.","authors":"Nils Brandenburg","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02417-0","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02417-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>An essential step in exploratory factor analysis is to determine the optimal number of factors. The Next Eigenvalue Sufficiency Test (NEST; Achim, 2017) is a recent proposal to determine the number of factors based on significance tests of the statistical contributions of candidate factors indicated by eigenvalues of sample correlation matrices. Previous simulation studies have shown NEST to recover the optimal number of factors in simulated datasets with high accuracy. However, these studies have focused on continuous variables. The present work addresses the performance of NEST for ordinal data. It has been debated whether factor models - and thus also the optimal number of factors - for ordinal variables should be computed for Pearson correlation matrices, which are known to underestimate correlations for ordinal datasets, or for polychoric correlation matrices, which are known to be instable. The central research question is to what extent the problems associated with Pearson correlations and polychoric correlations deteriorate NEST for ordinal datasets. Implementations of NEST tailored to ordinal datasets by utilizing polychoric correlations are proposed. In a simulation, the proposed implementations were compared to the original implementation of NEST which computes Pearson correlations even for ordinal datasets. The simulation shows that substituting polychoric correlations for Pearson correlations improves the accuracy of NEST for binary variables and large sample sizes (N = 500). However, the simulation also shows that the original implementation using Pearson correlations was the most accurate implementation for Likert-type variables with four response categories when item difficulties were homogeneous.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11362475/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140852527","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Connecting process models to response times through Bayesian hierarchical regression analysis. 通过贝叶斯分层回归分析将流程模型与响应时间联系起来。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-15 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02400-9
Thea Behrens, Adrian Kühn, Frank Jäkel

Process models specify a series of mental operations necessary to complete a task. We demonstrate how to use process models to analyze response-time data and obtain parameter estimates that have a clear psychological interpretation. A prerequisite for our analysis is a process model that generates a count of elementary information processing steps (EIP steps) for each trial of an experiment. We can estimate the duration of an EIP step by assuming that every EIP step is of random duration, modeled as draws from a gamma distribution. A natural effect of summing several random EIP steps is that the expected spread of the overall response time increases with a higher EIP step count. With modern probabilistic programming tools, it becomes relatively easy to fit Bayesian hierarchical models to data and thus estimate the duration of a step for each individual participant. We present two examples in this paper: The first example is children's performance on simple addition tasks, where the response time is often well predicted by the smaller of the two addends. The second example is response times in a Sudoku task. Here, the process model contains some random decisions and the EIP step count thus becomes latent. We show how our EIP regression model can be extended to such a case. We believe this approach can be used to bridge the gap between classical cognitive modeling and statistical inference and will be easily applicable to many use cases.

过程模型指定了完成一项任务所需的一系列心理操作。我们演示了如何使用过程模型来分析反应时数据,并获得具有明确心理学解释的参数估计。我们分析的先决条件是一个过程模型,它能为实验的每次试验生成基本信息处理步骤(EIP 步骤)的计数。我们可以通过假设每个 EIP 步骤的持续时间都是随机的来估算 EIP 步骤的持续时间。将多个随机 EIP 步数相加的一个自然结果是,随着 EIP 步数的增加,整体响应时间的预期差值也会增加。利用现代概率编程工具,对数据拟合贝叶斯层次模型变得相对容易,从而估算出每个参与者的步骤持续时间。我们在本文中介绍了两个例子:第一个例子是儿童在简单加法任务中的表现,在这种任务中,两个加数中较小的加数往往能很好地预测反应时间。第二个例子是数独任务中的反应时间。在这里,过程模型包含了一些随机决定,因此 EIP 步数变得潜在。我们展示了如何将 EIP 回归模型扩展到这种情况。我们相信,这种方法可用于弥合经典认知建模与统计推断之间的差距,并将轻松适用于许多用例。
{"title":"Connecting process models to response times through Bayesian hierarchical regression analysis.","authors":"Thea Behrens, Adrian Kühn, Frank Jäkel","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02400-9","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02400-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Process models specify a series of mental operations necessary to complete a task. We demonstrate how to use process models to analyze response-time data and obtain parameter estimates that have a clear psychological interpretation. A prerequisite for our analysis is a process model that generates a count of elementary information processing steps (EIP steps) for each trial of an experiment. We can estimate the duration of an EIP step by assuming that every EIP step is of random duration, modeled as draws from a gamma distribution. A natural effect of summing several random EIP steps is that the expected spread of the overall response time increases with a higher EIP step count. With modern probabilistic programming tools, it becomes relatively easy to fit Bayesian hierarchical models to data and thus estimate the duration of a step for each individual participant. We present two examples in this paper: The first example is children's performance on simple addition tasks, where the response time is often well predicted by the smaller of the two addends. The second example is response times in a Sudoku task. Here, the process model contains some random decisions and the EIP step count thus becomes latent. We show how our EIP regression model can be extended to such a case. We believe this approach can be used to bridge the gap between classical cognitive modeling and statistical inference and will be easily applicable to many use cases.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11362371/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140943398","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Subgroup detection in linear growth curve models with generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) trees. 使用广义线性混合模型 (GLMM) 树检测线性增长曲线模型中的亚组。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-29 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02389-1
Marjolein Fokkema, Achim Zeileis

Growth curve models are popular tools for studying the development of a response variable within subjects over time. Heterogeneity between subjects is common in such models, and researchers are typically interested in explaining or predicting this heterogeneity. We show how generalized linear mixed-effects model (GLMM) trees can be used to identify subgroups with different trajectories in linear growth curve models. Originally developed for clustered cross-sectional data, GLMM trees are extended here to longitudinal data. The resulting extended GLMM trees are directly applicable to growth curve models as an important special case. In simulated and real-world data, we assess performance of the extensions and compare against other partitioning methods for growth curve models. Extended GLMM trees perform more accurately than the original algorithm and LongCART, and similarly accurate compared to structural equation model (SEM) trees. In addition, GLMM trees allow for modeling both discrete and continuous time series, are less sensitive to (mis-)specification of the random-effects structure and are much faster to compute.

增长曲线模型是研究受试者体内反应变量随时间变化发展情况的常用工具。受试者之间的异质性在此类模型中很常见,研究人员通常对解释或预测这种异质性感兴趣。我们展示了如何利用广义线性混合效应模型(GLMM)树来识别线性生长曲线模型中具有不同轨迹的亚组。GLMM 树最初是针对聚类横截面数据开发的,在此扩展到纵向数据。扩展后的 GLMM 树作为一个重要的特例,可直接应用于生长曲线模型。在模拟数据和真实世界数据中,我们评估了扩展方法的性能,并与其他用于增长曲线模型的划分方法进行了比较。扩展的 GLMM 树比原始算法和 LongCART 更精确,与结构方程模型 (SEM) 树相比也同样精确。此外,GLMM 树既能对离散时间序列建模,也能对连续时间序列建模,对随机效应结构的(错误)规范不那么敏感,而且计算速度更快。
{"title":"Subgroup detection in linear growth curve models with generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) trees.","authors":"Marjolein Fokkema, Achim Zeileis","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02389-1","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02389-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Growth curve models are popular tools for studying the development of a response variable within subjects over time. Heterogeneity between subjects is common in such models, and researchers are typically interested in explaining or predicting this heterogeneity. We show how generalized linear mixed-effects model (GLMM) trees can be used to identify subgroups with different trajectories in linear growth curve models. Originally developed for clustered cross-sectional data, GLMM trees are extended here to longitudinal data. The resulting extended GLMM trees are directly applicable to growth curve models as an important special case. In simulated and real-world data, we assess performance of the extensions and compare against other partitioning methods for growth curve models. Extended GLMM trees perform more accurately than the original algorithm and LongCART, and similarly accurate compared to structural equation model (SEM) trees. In addition, GLMM trees allow for modeling both discrete and continuous time series, are less sensitive to (mis-)specification of the random-effects structure and are much faster to compute.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141173769","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Speech production and perception data collection in R: A tutorial for web-based methods using speechcollectr. 用 R 语言收集语音生成和感知数据:使用 speechcollectr 的网络方法教程。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-03 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02399-z
Abbey L Thomas, Peter F Assmann

This tutorial is designed for speech scientists familiar with the R programming language who wish to construct experiment interfaces in R. We begin by discussing some of the benefits of building experiment interfaces in R-including R's existing tools for speech data analysis, platform independence, suitability for web-based testing, and the fact that R is open source. We explain basic concepts of reactive programming in R, and we apply these principles by detailing the development of two sample experiments. The first of these experiments comprises a speech production task in which participants are asked to read words with different emotions. The second sample experiment involves a speech perception task, in which participants listen to recorded speech and identify the emotion the talker expressed with forced-choice questions and confidence ratings. Throughout this tutorial, we introduce the new R package speechcollectr, which provides functions uniquely suited to web-based speech data collection. The package streamlines the code required for speech experiments by providing functions for common tasks like documenting participant consent, collecting participant demographic information, recording audio, checking the adequacy of a participant's microphone or headphones, and presenting audio stimuli. Finally, we describe some of the difficulties of remote speech data collection, along with the solutions we have incorporated into speechcollectr to meet these challenges.

本教程专为熟悉 R 编程语言并希望用 R 构建实验界面的语音科学家设计。我们首先讨论了用 R 构建实验界面的一些好处,包括 R 现有的语音数据分析工具、平台独立性、适合基于网络的测试,以及 R 是开源的这一事实。我们解释了使用 R 进行反应式编程的基本概念,并通过详细介绍两个示例实验的开发过程来应用这些原则。第一个实验包括一个语音生成任务,要求参与者读出带有不同情绪的单词。第二个示例实验涉及语音感知任务,参与者在其中聆听录制的语音,并通过强迫选择题和置信度评级来识别说话者所表达的情绪。在本教程中,我们将介绍新的 R 软件包 speechcollectr,该软件包提供的功能非常适合基于网络的语音数据收集。该软件包简化了语音实验所需的代码,为记录参与者同意、收集参与者人口信息、录制音频、检查参与者的麦克风或耳机是否合适以及呈现音频刺激等常见任务提供了函数。最后,我们将介绍远程语音数据收集的一些困难,以及我们为应对这些挑战而纳入 speechcollectr 的解决方案。
{"title":"Speech production and perception data collection in R: A tutorial for web-based methods using speechcollectr.","authors":"Abbey L Thomas, Peter F Assmann","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02399-z","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02399-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This tutorial is designed for speech scientists familiar with the R programming language who wish to construct experiment interfaces in R. We begin by discussing some of the benefits of building experiment interfaces in R-including R's existing tools for speech data analysis, platform independence, suitability for web-based testing, and the fact that R is open source. We explain basic concepts of reactive programming in R, and we apply these principles by detailing the development of two sample experiments. The first of these experiments comprises a speech production task in which participants are asked to read words with different emotions. The second sample experiment involves a speech perception task, in which participants listen to recorded speech and identify the emotion the talker expressed with forced-choice questions and confidence ratings. Throughout this tutorial, we introduce the new R package speechcollectr, which provides functions uniquely suited to web-based speech data collection. The package streamlines the code required for speech experiments by providing functions for common tasks like documenting participant consent, collecting participant demographic information, recording audio, checking the adequacy of a participant's microphone or headphones, and presenting audio stimuli. Finally, we describe some of the difficulties of remote speech data collection, along with the solutions we have incorporated into speechcollectr to meet these challenges.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141198617","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Graded Incomplete Letters Test (GILT): a rapid test to detect cortical visual loss, with UK Biobank implementation. 分级不完整字母测试 (GILT):检测大脑皮层视力损失的快速测试,在英国生物银行实施。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-18 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02448-7
Kxx Yong, A Petzold, P Foster, A Young, S Bell, Y Bai, A P Leff, S Crutch, J A Greenwood

Impairments of object recognition are core features of neurodegenerative syndromes, in particular posterior cortical atrophy (PCA; the 'visual-variant Alzheimer's disease'). These impairments arise from damage to higher-level cortical visual regions and are often missed or misattributed to common ophthalmological conditions. Consequently, diagnosis can be delayed for years with considerable implications for patients. We report a new test for the rapid measurement of cortical visual loss - the Graded Incomplete Letters Test (GILT). The GILT is an optimised psychophysical variation of a test used to diagnose cortical visual impairment, which measures thresholds for recognising letters under levels of increasing visual degradation (decreasing "completeness") in a similar fashion to ophthalmic tests. The GILT was administered to UK Biobank participants (total n=2,359) and participants with neurodegenerative conditions characterised by initial cortical visual (PCA, n=18) or memory loss (typical Alzheimer's disease, n=9). UK Biobank participants, including both typical adults and those with ophthalmological conditions, were able to recognise letters under low levels of completeness. In contrast, participants with PCA consistently made errors with only modest decreases in completeness. GILT sensitivity to PCA was 83.3% for participants reaching the 80% accuracy cut-off, increasing to 88.9% using alternative cut-offs (60% or 100% accuracy). Specificity values were consistently over 94% when compared to UK Biobank participants without or with documented visual conditions, regardless of accuracy cut-off. These first-release UK Biobank and clinical verification data suggest the GILT has utility in both rapidly detecting visual perceptual losses following posterior cortical damage and differentiating perceptual losses from common eye-related conditions.

物体识别障碍是神经退行性综合症的核心特征,尤其是后皮质萎缩(PCA;"视觉变异性阿尔茨海默病")。这些障碍源于高级皮层视觉区域的损伤,常常被漏诊或误诊为常见的眼科疾病。因此,诊断可能会延迟数年,对患者造成相当大的影响。我们报告了一种快速测量大脑皮层视力损失的新测试--分级不完全字母测试(GILT)。GILT 是对用于诊断大脑皮层视力损伤的一项测试的心理物理学变体的优化,它以类似于眼科测试的方式测量在视觉退化程度不断增加("完整度 "不断降低)的情况下识别字母的阈值。英国生物库参与者(总人数=2,359 人)和患有神经退行性疾病的参与者均接受了 GILT 测试,这些疾病的特征是最初的皮层视觉(PCA,人数=18 人)或记忆丧失(典型的阿尔茨海默病,人数=9 人)。英国生物库的参与者,包括典型的成年人和患有眼科疾病的人,都能在低完整度的情况下识别字母。与此相反,患有 PCA 的参与者在完整度略有下降的情况下也会持续出错。在准确率达到 80% 临界值时,GILT 对 PCA 的灵敏度为 83.3%,而在使用其他临界值(准确率为 60% 或 100%)时,灵敏度则增至 88.9%。无论采用哪种准确度临界值,与英国生物样本库中未记录或记录有视力状况的参与者相比,特异性值始终超过 94%。这些首次发布的英国生物库和临床验证数据表明,GILT 在快速检测后部皮质损伤后的视觉知觉损失以及区分知觉损失与常见眼部相关疾病方面都很有用。
{"title":"The Graded Incomplete Letters Test (GILT): a rapid test to detect cortical visual loss, with UK Biobank implementation.","authors":"Kxx Yong, A Petzold, P Foster, A Young, S Bell, Y Bai, A P Leff, S Crutch, J A Greenwood","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02448-7","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02448-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Impairments of object recognition are core features of neurodegenerative syndromes, in particular posterior cortical atrophy (PCA; the 'visual-variant Alzheimer's disease'). These impairments arise from damage to higher-level cortical visual regions and are often missed or misattributed to common ophthalmological conditions. Consequently, diagnosis can be delayed for years with considerable implications for patients. We report a new test for the rapid measurement of cortical visual loss - the Graded Incomplete Letters Test (GILT). The GILT is an optimised psychophysical variation of a test used to diagnose cortical visual impairment, which measures thresholds for recognising letters under levels of increasing visual degradation (decreasing \"completeness\") in a similar fashion to ophthalmic tests. The GILT was administered to UK Biobank participants (total n=2,359) and participants with neurodegenerative conditions characterised by initial cortical visual (PCA, n=18) or memory loss (typical Alzheimer's disease, n=9). UK Biobank participants, including both typical adults and those with ophthalmological conditions, were able to recognise letters under low levels of completeness. In contrast, participants with PCA consistently made errors with only modest decreases in completeness. GILT sensitivity to PCA was 83.3% for participants reaching the 80% accuracy cut-off, increasing to 88.9% using alternative cut-offs (60% or 100% accuracy). Specificity values were consistently over 94% when compared to UK Biobank participants without or with documented visual conditions, regardless of accuracy cut-off. These first-release UK Biobank and clinical verification data suggest the GILT has utility in both rapidly detecting visual perceptual losses following posterior cortical damage and differentiating perceptual losses from common eye-related conditions.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11362218/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141417597","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A comparative evaluation of measures to assess randomness in human-generated sequences. 人类生成序列随机性评估措施的比较评估。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02456-7
Tim Angelike, Jochen Musch

Whether and how well people can behave randomly is of interest in many areas of psychological research. The ability to generate randomness is often investigated using random number generation (RNG) tasks, in which participants are asked to generate a sequence of numbers that is as random as possible. However, there is no consensus on how best to quantify the randomness of responses in human-generated sequences. Traditionally, psychologists have used measures of randomness that directly assess specific features of human behavior in RNG tasks, such as the tendency to avoid repetition or to systematically generate numbers that have not been generated in the recent choice history, a behavior known as cycling. Other disciplines have proposed measures of randomness that are based on a more rigorous mathematical foundation and are less restricted to specific features of randomness, such as algorithmic complexity. More recently, variants of these measures have been proposed to assess systematic patterns in short sequences. We report the first large-scale integrative study to compare measures of specific aspects of randomness with entropy-derived measures based on information theory and measures based on algorithmic complexity. We compare the ability of the different measures to discriminate between human-generated sequences and truly random sequences based on atmospheric noise, and provide a systematic analysis of how the usefulness of randomness measures is affected by sequence length. We conclude with recommendations that can guide the selection of appropriate measures of randomness in psychological research.

人们的行为是否具有随机性以及随机性有多大,是许多心理学研究领域所关注的问题。随机数生成(RNG)任务通常是对随机性生成能力的研究,在这些任务中,参与者被要求生成尽可能随机的数字序列。然而,对于如何最好地量化人类生成序列中反应的随机性,目前还没有达成共识。传统上,心理学家使用的随机性测量方法可以直接评估 RNG 任务中人类行为的特定特征,例如避免重复或系统地生成近期选择历史中未生成过的数字的倾向,这种行为被称为循环行为。其他学科也提出了基于更严格数学基础的随机性测量方法,这些方法对随机性的特定特征(如算法复杂性)限制较少。最近,这些测量方法的变体被提出来评估短序列中的系统模式。我们报告了首次大规模综合研究,比较了随机性特定方面的测量方法、基于信息论的熵衍生测量方法和基于算法复杂性的测量方法。我们比较了不同测量方法在区分人类生成的序列和基于大气噪声的真正随机序列方面的能力,并对随机性测量方法的实用性如何受到序列长度的影响进行了系统分析。最后,我们提出了一些建议,以指导在心理学研究中选择适当的随机性测量方法。
{"title":"A comparative evaluation of measures to assess randomness in human-generated sequences.","authors":"Tim Angelike, Jochen Musch","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02456-7","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02456-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Whether and how well people can behave randomly is of interest in many areas of psychological research. The ability to generate randomness is often investigated using random number generation (RNG) tasks, in which participants are asked to generate a sequence of numbers that is as random as possible. However, there is no consensus on how best to quantify the randomness of responses in human-generated sequences. Traditionally, psychologists have used measures of randomness that directly assess specific features of human behavior in RNG tasks, such as the tendency to avoid repetition or to systematically generate numbers that have not been generated in the recent choice history, a behavior known as cycling. Other disciplines have proposed measures of randomness that are based on a more rigorous mathematical foundation and are less restricted to specific features of randomness, such as algorithmic complexity. More recently, variants of these measures have been proposed to assess systematic patterns in short sequences. We report the first large-scale integrative study to compare measures of specific aspects of randomness with entropy-derived measures based on information theory and measures based on algorithmic complexity. We compare the ability of the different measures to discriminate between human-generated sequences and truly random sequences based on atmospheric noise, and provide a systematic analysis of how the usefulness of randomness measures is affected by sequence length. We conclude with recommendations that can guide the selection of appropriate measures of randomness in psychological research.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11362514/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141490709","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An abbreviated Chinese dyslexia screening behavior checklist for primary school students using a machine learning approach. 利用机器学习方法编制简略的中国小学生阅读障碍筛查行为检查表。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-29 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02461-w
Yimin Fan, Yixun Li, Mingyue Luo, Jirong Bai, Mengwen Jiang, Yi Xu, Hong Li

To increase early identification and intervention of dyslexia, a prescreening instrument is critical to identifying children at risk. The present work sought to shorten and validate the 30-item Mandarin Dyslexia Screening Behavior Checklist for Primary School Students (the full checklist; Fan et al., , 19, 521-527, 2021). Our participants were 15,522 Mandarin-Chinese-speaking students and their parents, sampled from classrooms in grades 2-6 across regions in mainland China. A machine learning approach (lasso regression) was applied to shorten the full checklist (Fan et al., , 19, 521-527, 2021), constructing grade-specific brief checklists first, followed by a compilation of the common brief checklist based on the similarity across grade-specific checklists. All checklists (the full, grade-specific brief, and common brief versions) were validated and compared with data in our sample and an external sample (N = 114; Fan et al., , 19, 521-527, 2021). The results indicated that the six-item common brief checklist showed consistently high reliability (αs > .82) and reasonable classification performance (about 60% prediction accuracy and 70% sensitivity), comparable to that of the full checklist and all grade-specific brief checklists across our current sample and the external sample from Fan et al., , 19, 521-527, (2021). Our analysis showed that 2.42 (out of 5) was the cutoff score that helped classify children's reading status (children who scored higher than 2.42 might be considered at risk for dyslexia). Our final product is a valid, accessible, common brief checklist for prescreening primary school children at risk for Chinese dyslexia, which can be used across grades and regions in mainland China.

为了加强对阅读障碍的早期识别和干预,预检工具对于识别高危儿童至关重要。本研究试图缩短并验证 30 个项目的《小学生普通话阅读障碍筛查行为核对表》(完整核对表;Fan 等,19, 521-527, 2021)。我们的研究对象是从中国大陆各地区二至六年级的班级中抽取的15522名普通话学生及其家长。我们采用了一种机器学习方法(套索回归)来缩短完整的核对表(Fan 等,19, 521-527, 2021),首先构建了针对具体年级的简要核对表,然后根据各年级核对表的相似性汇编了通用简要核对表。所有核对表(完整版、特定年级简明版和通用简明版)都经过了验证,并与我们的样本和外部样本(N = 114;Fan 等,19,521-527,2021)的数据进行了比较。结果表明,六项目通用简明核对表显示出一贯的高可靠性(αs > .82)和合理的分类性能(约60%的预测准确率和70%的灵敏度),在我们目前的样本和Fan等人的外部样本中,与完整核对表和所有特定年级简明核对表相当。我们的分析表明,2.42(满分 5 分)是有助于对儿童的阅读状况进行分类的临界值(得分高于 2.42 的儿童可能被认为有阅读障碍的风险)。我们的最终成果是一份有效、易用、通用的简明核对表,用于对有阅读障碍风险的小学生进行预检,可在中国大陆的不同年级和地区使用。
{"title":"An abbreviated Chinese dyslexia screening behavior checklist for primary school students using a machine learning approach.","authors":"Yimin Fan, Yixun Li, Mingyue Luo, Jirong Bai, Mengwen Jiang, Yi Xu, Hong Li","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02461-w","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02461-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>To increase early identification and intervention of dyslexia, a prescreening instrument is critical to identifying children at risk. The present work sought to shorten and validate the 30-item Mandarin Dyslexia Screening Behavior Checklist for Primary School Students (the full checklist; Fan et al., , 19, 521-527, 2021). Our participants were 15,522 Mandarin-Chinese-speaking students and their parents, sampled from classrooms in grades 2-6 across regions in mainland China. A machine learning approach (lasso regression) was applied to shorten the full checklist (Fan et al., , 19, 521-527, 2021), constructing grade-specific brief checklists first, followed by a compilation of the common brief checklist based on the similarity across grade-specific checklists. All checklists (the full, grade-specific brief, and common brief versions) were validated and compared with data in our sample and an external sample (N = 114; Fan et al., , 19, 521-527, 2021). The results indicated that the six-item common brief checklist showed consistently high reliability (αs > .82) and reasonable classification performance (about 60% prediction accuracy and 70% sensitivity), comparable to that of the full checklist and all grade-specific brief checklists across our current sample and the external sample from Fan et al., , 19, 521-527, (2021). Our analysis showed that 2.42 (out of 5) was the cutoff score that helped classify children's reading status (children who scored higher than 2.42 might be considered at risk for dyslexia). Our final product is a valid, accessible, common brief checklist for prescreening primary school children at risk for Chinese dyslexia, which can be used across grades and regions in mainland China.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141791730","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Timed picture naming norms for 800 photographs of 200 objects in English. 对 800 张包含 200 件物品的英文照片进行图片命名计时。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-19 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02380-w
Rens van Hoef, Dermot Lynott, Louise Connell

The present study presents picture-naming norms for a large set of 800 high-quality photographs of 200 natural objects and artefacts spanning a range of categories, with four unique images per object. Participants were asked to provide a single, most appropriate name for each image seen. We report recognition latencies for each image, and several normed variables for the provided names: agreement, H-statistic (i.e. level of naming uncertainty), Zipf word frequency and word length. Rather than simply focusing on a single name per image (i.e. the modal or most common name), analysis of recognition latencies showed that it is important to consider the diversity of labels that participants may ascribe to each pictured object. The norms therefore provide a list of candidate labels per image with weighted measures of word length and frequency per image that incorporate all provided names, as well as modal measures based on the most common name only.

本研究介绍了 800 张高质量照片的图片命名规范,这些照片包含 200 个自然物体和人工制品类别,每个物体有四张独特的图片。我们要求参与者为看到的每张图片提供一个最合适的名称。我们报告了每张图片的识别延迟以及所提供名称的几个标准变量:一致度、H 统计量(即命名不确定性水平)、Zipf 词频和词长。对识别潜伏期的分析表明,重要的是要考虑参与者可能赋予每个图像对象的标签的多样性,而不是简单地关注每个图像的单一名称(即模式名称或最常见的名称)。因此,规范提供了每幅图像的候选标签列表,并对每幅图像的词长和词频进行了加权测量,其中包含了所有提供的名称,以及仅基于最常见名称的模态测量。
{"title":"Timed picture naming norms for 800 photographs of 200 objects in English.","authors":"Rens van Hoef, Dermot Lynott, Louise Connell","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02380-w","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02380-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present study presents picture-naming norms for a large set of 800 high-quality photographs of 200 natural objects and artefacts spanning a range of categories, with four unique images per object. Participants were asked to provide a single, most appropriate name for each image seen. We report recognition latencies for each image, and several normed variables for the provided names: agreement, H-statistic (i.e. level of naming uncertainty), Zipf word frequency and word length. Rather than simply focusing on a single name per image (i.e. the modal or most common name), analysis of recognition latencies showed that it is important to consider the diversity of labels that participants may ascribe to each pictured object. The norms therefore provide a list of candidate labels per image with weighted measures of word length and frequency per image that incorporate all provided names, as well as modal measures based on the most common name only.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11362483/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140179254","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Enabling analytical power calculations for multilevel models with autocorrelated errors through deriving and approximating the precision matrix. 通过推导和近似精度矩阵,对具有自相关误差的多级模型进行分析功率计算。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-15 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02435-y
Ginette Lafit, Richard Artner, Eva Ceulemans

To unravel how within-person psychological processes fluctuate in daily life, and how these processes differ between persons, intensive longitudinal (IL) designs in which participants are repeatedly measured, have become popular. Commonly used statistical models for those designs are multilevel models with autocorrelated errors. Substantive hypotheses of interest are then typically investigated via statistical hypotheses tests for model parameters of interest. An important question in the design of such IL studies concerns the determination of the number of participants and the number of measurements per person needed to achieve sufficient statistical power for those statistical tests. Recent advances in computational methods and software have enabled the computation of statistical power using Monte Carlo simulations. However, this approach is computationally intensive and therefore quite restrictive. To ease power computations, we derive simple-to-use analytical formulas for multilevel models with AR(1) within-person errors. Analytic expressions for a model family are obtained via asymptotic approximations of all sample statistics in the precision matrix of the fixed effects. To validate this analytical approach to power computation, we compare it to the simulation-based approach via a series of Monte Carlo simulations. We find comparable performances making the analytic approach a useful tool for researchers that can drastically save them time and resources.

为了揭示日常生活中人与人之间的心理过程是如何波动的,以及这些过程在人与人之间是如何不同的,对参与者进行反复测量的密集纵向(IL)设计已变得非常流行。这些设计常用的统计模型是具有自相关误差的多层次模型。然后,通常通过对相关模型参数的统计假设检验来研究感兴趣的实质性假设。设计此类 IL 研究的一个重要问题是确定参与者人数和每人的测量次数,以便为这些统计检验获得足够的统计功率。计算方法和软件的最新进展使得使用蒙特卡罗模拟计算统计能力成为可能。然而,这种方法的计算量很大,因此限制性很强。为了简化统计量计算,我们为具有 AR(1) 人内误差的多层次模型推导出了简单易用的分析公式。模型族的分析表达式是通过固定效应精确矩阵中所有样本统计量的渐近近似得到的。为了验证这种幂计算的分析方法,我们通过一系列蒙特卡罗模拟将其与基于模拟的方法进行了比较。我们发现两者的性能相当,因此分析方法成为研究人员的有用工具,可以大大节省他们的时间和资源。
{"title":"Enabling analytical power calculations for multilevel models with autocorrelated errors through deriving and approximating the precision matrix.","authors":"Ginette Lafit, Richard Artner, Eva Ceulemans","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02435-y","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02435-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>To unravel how within-person psychological processes fluctuate in daily life, and how these processes differ between persons, intensive longitudinal (IL) designs in which participants are repeatedly measured, have become popular. Commonly used statistical models for those designs are multilevel models with autocorrelated errors. Substantive hypotheses of interest are then typically investigated via statistical hypotheses tests for model parameters of interest. An important question in the design of such IL studies concerns the determination of the number of participants and the number of measurements per person needed to achieve sufficient statistical power for those statistical tests. Recent advances in computational methods and software have enabled the computation of statistical power using Monte Carlo simulations. However, this approach is computationally intensive and therefore quite restrictive. To ease power computations, we derive simple-to-use analytical formulas for multilevel models with AR(1) within-person errors. Analytic expressions for a model family are obtained via asymptotic approximations of all sample statistics in the precision matrix of the fixed effects. To validate this analytical approach to power computation, we compare it to the simulation-based approach via a series of Monte Carlo simulations. We find comparable performances making the analytic approach a useful tool for researchers that can drastically save them time and resources.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141619152","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Statistical indices of masculinity-femininity: A theoretical and practical framework. 男性-女性的统计指数:理论与实践框架。
IF 4.6 2区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-04 DOI: 10.3758/s13428-024-02369-5
Marco Del Giudice

Statistical indices of masculinity-femininity (M-F) summarize multivariate profiles of sex-related traits as positions on a single continuum of individual differences, from masculine to feminine. This approach goes back to the early days of sex differences research; however, a systematic discussion of alternative M-F indices (including their meaning, their mutual relations, and their psychometric properties) has been lacking. In this paper I present an integrative theoretical framework for the statistical assessment of masculinity-femininity, and provide practical guidance to researchers who wish to apply these methods to their data. I describe four basic types of M-F indices: sex-directionality, sex-typicality, sex-probability, and sex-centrality. I examine their similarities and differences in detail, and consider alternative ways of computing them. Next, I discuss the impact of measurement error on the validity of these indices, and outline some potential remedies. Finally, I illustrate the concepts presented in the paper with a selection of real-world datasets on body morphology, brain morphology, and personality. An R function is available to easily calculate multiple M-F indices from empirical data (with or without correction for measurement error) and draw summary plots of their individual and joint distributions.

男性-女性(M-F)统计指数将与性别有关的特征的多变量概况概括为个体差异的单一连续统一体中从男性到女性的位置。这种方法可以追溯到性别差异研究的早期;然而,一直以来都缺乏对其他 M-F 指数(包括它们的含义、相互关系和心理测量学特性)的系统性讨论。在本文中,我提出了男性-女性统计评估的综合理论框架,并为希望将这些方法应用于其数据的研究人员提供了实用指导。我描述了四种基本的男性-女性指数:性别方向性、性别典型性、性别概率和性别中心性。我将详细研究它们的异同,并考虑计算它们的其他方法。接下来,我将讨论测量误差对这些指数有效性的影响,并概述一些可能的补救措施。最后,我选择了一些关于身体形态、大脑形态和性格的真实世界数据集来说明本文提出的概念。我们提供了一个 R 函数,可以轻松地从经验数据中计算出多个 M-F 指数(无论是否修正了测量误差),并绘制出这些指数的个体分布和联合分布的汇总图。
{"title":"Statistical indices of masculinity-femininity: A theoretical and practical framework.","authors":"Marco Del Giudice","doi":"10.3758/s13428-024-02369-5","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13428-024-02369-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Statistical indices of masculinity-femininity (M-F) summarize multivariate profiles of sex-related traits as positions on a single continuum of individual differences, from masculine to feminine. This approach goes back to the early days of sex differences research; however, a systematic discussion of alternative M-F indices (including their meaning, their mutual relations, and their psychometric properties) has been lacking. In this paper I present an integrative theoretical framework for the statistical assessment of masculinity-femininity, and provide practical guidance to researchers who wish to apply these methods to their data. I describe four basic types of M-F indices: sex-directionality, sex-typicality, sex-probability, and sex-centrality. I examine their similarities and differences in detail, and consider alternative ways of computing them. Next, I discuss the impact of measurement error on the validity of these indices, and outline some potential remedies. Finally, I illustrate the concepts presented in the paper with a selection of real-world datasets on body morphology, brain morphology, and personality. An R function is available to easily calculate multiple M-F indices from empirical data (with or without correction for measurement error) and draw summary plots of their individual and joint distributions.</p>","PeriodicalId":8717,"journal":{"name":"Behavior Research Methods","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.6,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11362193/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140027309","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Behavior Research Methods
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1